Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T15:07:01.128Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Children’s disambiguation of novel words varies by the number and position of phonological contrasts

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 April 2022

Catanya G. STAGER*
Affiliation:
University of Alabama
Laura M. MORETT
Affiliation:
University of Alabama
Audrey STELMACH
Affiliation:
University of Alabama
Anna Grace PARENTE
Affiliation:
University of Alabama
Josh MICKLER
Affiliation:
University of Alabama
Jason SCOFIELD
Affiliation:
University of Alabama
*
*Corresponding author: Catanya G. Stager, 209 Child Development Research Center, Box 870-160, Tuscaloosa, AL 34587, Email: cgstager@crimson.ua.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Young children often make pragmatic assumptions when learning new words. For example, they assume that a speaker who uses different words intends to refer to different things – the so-called principle of contrast. We used a standard disambiguation task to explore whether children’s assumptions about contrast depend on how much words differ. Three- to 6-year-olds heard pairs of words that differed in terms of the number, position, and types of phonological contrasts. Results indicate that children were less likely to disambiguate words differing by one phoneme than words differing by two or more phonemes, particularly when those one-phoneme differences were located at the beginning or end of the words (as in fim/vim). Overall, the findings suggest that children’s pragmatic assumptions about two contrasting words depend not only on if words differ, but also on how they differ.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

The challenge of learning any new word comes in deciding what that word means. Because a new word could have any number of possible meanings (Quine, Reference Quine1960), deciding which meaning is the most likely can be harder than one might initially guess. To meet this challenge, even young children must find a way to consider less than the full set of possible meanings. In the experiment presented here, we explore children’s use of the pragmatic principle of contrast to limit the set of meanings considered for a new word. According to this principle, children understand that “different words mean different things” (Clark, Reference Clark1987) and assume that a speaker who uses different words intends to refer to different things. This assumption can help limit the number of candidate meanings children might otherwise consider for the word by allowing them to eliminate any referent for which they already have a word.

In our experiment, 3- to 6-year-olds were presented with pairs of novel words that varied in the number and position of phonological differences between the words. In some cases, the words were very similar, differing in only one phoneme (e.g., deen/geen or fim/vim). In other cases, the words were very dissimilar, differing on all three phonemes (e.g., waf/kud). Word pairs were always presented in the context of a disambiguation task where children saw two objects, one of which was labeled with a word and one of which was left unlabeled. Children then were asked to identify the referent of a second word. Our overall question was whether the amount and/or position of phonological differences between the two words would affect the likelihood that children would observe the pragmatics of contrast and assign the second word to the unlabeled object.

Previous studies routinely find that children do just that: when presented with two objects, one known and one novel, and asked for the referent of a novel word, children show a strong tendency to select the novel, unlabeled object. This tendency is known as the disambiguation effect (Markman & Wachtel, Reference Markman and Wachtel1988; Merriman, Bowman, & MacWhinney, Reference Merriman, Bowman and MacWhinney1989). This effect emerges in infancy (Graham, Poulin-Dubois, & Baker, Reference Graham, Poulin-Dubois and Baker1998; Halberda, Reference Halberda2003; Markman, Wasow, & Hansen, Reference Markman, Wasow and Hansen2003; Mervis & Bertrand, Reference Mervis and Bertrand1994) and increases gradually during the first few years of language development (Bion, Borovsky, & Fernald, Reference Bion, Borovsky and Fernald2013) (see also Clark, Reference Clark1990; Markman, Reference Markman, Gelman and Byrnes1991; Markman & Wachtel, Reference Markman and Wachtel1988; Merriman et a., 1989). Knowledge and size of the child’s vocabulary as well as their overall language experience help drive these increases (Byers-Heinlein & Werker, Reference Byers-Heinlein and Werker2009; Graham et al., Reference Graham, Poulin-Dubois and Baker1998; Grassmann, Schulze, & Tomasello, Reference Grassmann, Schulze and Tomasello2015; Kalashnikova, Mattock, & Monaghan, Reference Kalashnikova, Mattock and Monaghan2016).

Despite a strong tendency to disambiguate novel words, children do not always do it. For example, young children sometimes prioritize other cues, like a speaker’s gestures and gaze, and instead select the known object in response to a novel word (Grassmann & Tomasello, Reference Grassmann and Tomasello2010). Other times they disregard such cues and select the novel object (Jaswal & Hansen, Reference Jaswal and Hansen2006). Likewise, depending on age and modality (Scofield, Hernandez-Reif, & Keith, Reference Scofield, Hernandez-Reif and Keith2009), children sometimes expect speakers to communicate about the novel object (4-year-olds but not 3-year-olds, Henning & Merriman, Reference Henning and Merriman2019) and other times they expect them to communicate about the known object (4-year-olds but not 3-year-olds, Scofield, Merriman, & Wall, Reference Scofield, Merriman and Wall2018; Wall, Merriman, & Scofield, Reference Wall, Merriman and Scofield2015; Wall & Merriman, Reference Wall and Merriman2020).

There is also evidence that the degree of phonological similarity between words can affect the likelihood that a child disambiguates. When novel words are greater in similarity to familiar words, young children show a weaker disambiguation effect (Merriman & Schuster, Reference Merriman and Schuster1991). Young children also show greater sensitivity to larger phonological differences between familiar and novel words when differences occur at the beginning of the word (e.g., shoe/foo) (White & Morgan, Reference White and Morgan2008). For three-to-five-year-olds, phonological feature differences also resulted in varying novelty responses for atypically pronounced words, compared to familiar words (e.g., fesh/fish) (Creel, Reference Creel2012). One important antecedent to the disambiguation of any two words occurs when children successfully discriminate between those words, as Merriman and Marazita (Reference Merriman and Marazita1995) note in step 5 of their model of disambiguation.

Although discrimination is necessary for disambiguation, it may not be sufficient. For example, 3- to 5-year-olds detect phonological deviations when presented with accented pronunciations of familiar words as in apple/epple. However, their tendency is to assume that the alternate pronunciation is referring to the familiar rather than a novel object (Creel, Reference Creel2012; see Swingley, Reference Swingley2016 and Swingley & Aslin, Reference Swingley and Aslin2007 for evidence in 2-year-olds), perhaps signaling a willingness to accept a range of pronunciations for a given word (Krueger & Storkel, Reference Krueger and Storkel2020; Krueger, Storkel, & Minai, Reference Krueger, Storkel and Minai2018). This kind of flexibility would allow children to pivot to other potentially informative cues when interpreting a speaker’s intent, something that could benefit word learning when interacting with foreign or accented speakers or with a speaker prone to mispronunciations (like a child peer) (Creel, Reference Creel2012).

However, a willingness to accept alternate pronunciations could also undermine word learning. For each case where a child correctly interprets a mispronunciation, there could be a corresponding case where they incorrectly interpret an accurate pronunciation (Swingley, Reference Swingley2016). Moreover, the assumption that phonological contrasts do not reflect important underlying differences between the words would contradict one of the most prominent and well-supported explanations for the disambiguation effect: the pragmatic principle of contrast (see Diesendruck & Markson, Reference Diesendruck and Markson2001). According to contrast, language learners assume that any two contrasting forms (such as two different words) correspond to contrasting meanings in the world (Clark, Reference Clark1987, Reference Clark1990). From a pragmatic standpoint, a speaker who uses different words does so intentionally to denote different referents. This readiness to infer a speaker’s referential intent is at the heart of the pragmatic explanation for the disambiguation effect (Clark, Reference Clark1990; Diesendruck, Reference Diesendruck2005; Diesendruck & Markson, Reference Diesendruck and Markson2001; Scofield & Behrend, Reference Scofield and Behrend2007).

The Current Experiment

When completing the disambiguation task, children usually observe the pragmatics of contrast and select the novel object as the referent of the novel word. But they will sometimes ignore the pragmatics of contrast and select the known object as the referent of the novel word. The contradictory nature of this evidence raises questions about the factors that lead children down one path or the other. Past research suggests that the degree of similarity between words and position of phonological differences within the words could each play a role. In the current experiment, we systematically manipulate these factors. 3- to 6-year-olds were presented with pairs of novel words. Each word contained three phonemes in a consonant-vowel-consonant sequence, as in fim. The words in each pair contrasted by 0, 1, 2, or 3 phonemes. Words also contrasted in terms of the position of phonemic differences. For example, one phoneme differences could be located on the first phoneme (i.e., the initial consonant or onset), on the middle phoneme (i.e., the vowel, or nucleus), or on the last phoneme (i.e., the final consonant, or coda). See Table 1 for examples of position differences for one phoneme trials as well as for two and three phoneme trials. Finally, word pairs contrasted in terms of the number of differing phonological features occurring on each phoneme. Word pairs were presented in a disambiguation task where children had to identify the referent of a novel word and, separately, in an auditory discrimination task where children simply had to decide whether the words differed.

Table 1. Sample Novel Word Pairs with Number, Position, and Features of Contrasting Phonemes

* Bold signifies contrasting phoneme. IC: Initial Consonant, V: Vowel, FC: Final

Method

Participants

Eighty-five 3- to 6-year-olds (42 females, 43 males; age: 57 mos.; 71 White, 12 Black, two other) were recruited to participate in this experiment through preschools located in a mid-sized university town in the southeastern United States. Over the course of the study, we developed two different versions of the discrimination task. In the original version, children were asked to judge whether the words they heard were “the same” or “different”. However, successfully communicating these instructions to children (especially those below five years of age) in a way that they understood proved exceedingly difficult. We therefore created a simplified version of the task (as described later), which proved much easier for children to understand, and we discarded all data prior to the simplified version.

Materials

Novel words

One hundred and ten unique novel words were created for use in this experiment. All novel words were monosyllabic, comprising three English phonemes, and frequently followed a standard consonant-vowel-consonant (CVC) sequence. Some novel words contained more orthographic consonants or vowels but phonetically all were CVC. Pairs of novel words were constructed from either two of the same novel word (same pairs; n = 54) or two different novel words (different pairs; n = 60) for use in this experiment. To ensure that a given word was presented only once per task, the word appeared in either a same trial or a different trial, but not both.

In different word pairs, novel words differed from each other in number of contrasting phonemes (one, two, three), position of contrasting phonemes (initial consonant, vowel, final consonant), and number of contrasting phonological features (one, two, three). Contrasting consonants differed in at least one of the following three phonological features: place of articulation, manner of articulation, and voicing. Contrasting vowels differed in at least one of the following three phonological features: tension, horizontal place of articulation, and vertical place of articulation. See Table 1 for examples of novel word pairs and phonological characteristics (please see Appendix A for all word pairs used and response proportions of the disambiguation task.)

Novel objects

Novel object images used in this experiment were selected from objects with low familiarity scores and low name-ability scores from the Novel Object Unusual Name (NOUN) database (Horst & Hout, Reference Horst and Hout2014, Reference Horst and Hout2016). Additional object images were selected from Internet searches of household objects (kitchen and tools) likely to be unfamiliar and unnameable to young children. All object images were overlaid on a white background, appeared to be small enough to fit in a hand, and were presented in color. During experimental task paradigm development, novel objects were randomly pre-paired with novel words, such that the same novel object was always presented with the same novel word during experimental testing. No novel objects were duplicated across a participant’s task.

Technology and software

Experimental task paradigms were developed and administered using SuperLab 5 (Cedrus Corporation, 2012). Audio recordings of an adult male saying novel words and framing phrases were captured for use in experimental tasks. Words and sentence frames were recorded in a soundproof booth. After recording, novel words were spliced into framing phrases (“This is a” and “Point to the”) to create full audio clips using Audacity® 2.3.0 (http://audacityteam.org/). Experimental tasks were presented using a small, lightweight, tablet-style computer with touchscreen responsiveness (Microsoft Surface Pro 6). During experimental sessions, participants wore a pair of noise-limiting headphones (BT2200 by Puro Sound Labs) plugged into the computer. Prior to experimental task administration, audio levels were set at a default of 75 decibels and were increased if participants indicated that they could not hear test audio sufficiently. Experimental tasks were initiated when participants reported that they were able to hear test audio sufficiently.

Procedure

Parents of participants completed consent forms approved by the host university’s Institutional Review Board, which they returned to participants’ preschools. The experimenter met with assenting participants individually in a quiet room outside of their normal preschool classroom and invited them to participate in a game. Participants were positioned in front of the computer and fitted with headphones. A laminated set of handprints was used to help participants start from the same position during experimental tasks. Once participants were comfortable, the experimenter initiated one of the two experimental tasks described below. The experimenter took notes during tasks as a back-up for participant responses. At the end of experimental sessions, participants received a sticker as a reward for participation. The presentation order of the two tasks was counterbalanced. No effect for task order was found.

Disambiguation task

At the onset of the disambiguation task, participants completed a brief set of four warm-up trials (two same and two different) designed to orient them to the disambiguation task and ensure that they could follow task procedures. In these trials, participants saw a familiar object (e.g., a ball) appear on the computer screen and heard a pre-recorded male voice label the object with a familiar name (e.g., “This is a ball”). A second familiar object then appeared on screen alongside the first (e.g., a shoe), and participants were asked to identify one of the objects. In “same” trials, participants were prompted to identify the original object (e.g., “Point to the ball”). The “point to” instructions were accompanied by the experimenter demonstrating how to properly touch one of the two images on the screen. Children’s choices were coded according to which object they touched. In “different” trials, participants were prompted to identify the novel object (e.g., “Point to the shoe”; see Figure 1). If a child was unsuccessful on two of the four warm-up trials, researchers provided one more set of four warm-up trials. If a child failed to correctly identify two of the four trials in the second attempt, the session was ended by the experimenter and no data were collected. All children successfully completed the warm-up trials, but due to lack of interest, two children failed to complete the disambiguation task (a 4-year-old and a 6-year-old), and their data were excluded from the study.

Figure 1. Schematic of a “Different” Trial in the Disambiguation Task

After successfully completing all warm-up trials, participants proceeded to experimental trials. Experimental trials were structured identically to warm-up trials except that they included novel words. Participants completed a total of 28 experimental trials, 14 of which were “same” trials and 14 of which were “different” trials.

Auditory discrimination task

At the onset of the auditory discrimination task, participants completed a brief set of warm-up trials designed to orient them to the task. In these trials, participants saw two images side-by-side on the computer screen. Each image featured a cartoon drawing of a male speaker from the shoulders up, as depicted in Figure 2. The task was described as a “copycat” game where the first speaker (on the left) had to say a word and the second speaker (on the right) had to repeat it. Participants were instructed to judge whether the two speakers said the same word. To begin the task, children first saw the first speaker open his mouth and (in a pre-recorded male voice) say a familiar word (e.g., “ball”). Children then saw the second speaker open his mouth and (in the same a pre-recorded male voice) say a familiar word that was either the same (e.g., “ball”) or different (e.g., “shoe”) from the first word. Children indicated their judgment by touching a green button (yes, they said the same word) or a red button (no, they said the same word) on the screen. The button placement was randomized to the right or left side of the screen, as was the speaker placement. As in the disambiguation task, participants were excluded if they failed two of the four warm-up trials on the two consecutive warm up sets. One child (four-year-old) failed the warm-up trials and three children (two three-year-olds and one four-year-old) did not complete the task due to lack of interest. Their data were excluded from the study. The auditory discrimination task was included as a secondary task, used for insight into trials on which children failed to disambiguate.

Figure 2. Schematic of a “Different” Trial in the Auditory Discrimination Task

After successfully completing warm-up trials, participants proceeded to the experimental trials. The experimental trials were structured identically to warm-up trials except that they included novel rather than familiar words. As in the disambiguation task, participants completed a total of 28 experimental trials, 14 of which were “same” trials and 14 of which were “different” trials.

Results

Analyses

All data from the disambiguation task were analyzed using mixed effect models, which permit analysis of non-aggregated trial level data without assumptions of linearity, normality, or homoscedasticity. These data were analyzed using logit mixed effects models, which model the log odds (or logit) of a correct response on each trial (1 = correct; 0 = incorrect). In these models, number of phoneme or feature differences were included as fixed effects, and participants and word pairs were included as random effects. These models were fit using the lme4 package (Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015) in the R statistical programming language.

In what follows, we report the results of analyses of the number of phoneme differences and, for single phoneme changes, the position of differences, number of feature differences in those positions, and types of feature differences. For these analyses, only comparisons reaching or trending towards significance are reported; all other comparisons failed to reach significance. All models used to obtain these results are specified in Appendix B. As a supplementary analysis, we conducted one-sample t tests on the discrimination of word pairs incorrectly disambiguated. Our goal here was to gain insight into whether failed disambiguation might result from children not hearing the differences between words (and thus not discriminating them) or from children hearing but then ignoring the differences between words. For these analyses, only comparisons failing to reach significance (indicating at-chance accuracy) are reported; all other conditions reached significance (indicating above-chance accuracy).

Number of differing phonemes

We first examined whether disambiguation accuracy might vary based on the number of differing phonemes between words. We observed a main effect for same words compared to words differing in one, two, or three phonemes (see Table 2). Tukey-adjusted post-hoc tests revealed that disambiguation was less accurate for words that differed by one phoneme (M = 0.67, SD = 0.47) compared to those that differed by two (M = 0.79; SD = 0.41) or three phonemes (M = 0.83; SD = 0.38) or to words that were the same (M = 0.80, SD = 0.40; see Table 3 and Figure 3). This indicates that children are less likely to disambiguate words that differ by a single phoneme.

Table 2. Fixed Effect Estimates (Top) and Variance Estimates (Bottom) for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance by Number of Differing Phonemes and Age (Observations = 2378, Log Likelihood = –1190.0)

Table 3. Tukey HSD-Corrected Post-Hoc Tests for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance by Number of Differing Phonemes

Figure 3. Disambiguation Task Performance of 3-6 Yr.-Olds for Word Pairs with (A) Three Differing Phonemes (different = accurate); (B) Two Differing Phonemes (different = accurate); (C) One Differing Phoneme (different = accurate); (D) No Differing Phonemes (same = accurate). Data Points Jittered Horizontally and Vertically; Means Indicated by Black Dots and Values; Dashed Lines Represent Chance.

Position of differing phonemes

We next examined whether disambiguation accuracy for words that differed by a single phoneme might vary based on the position of that difference (i.e., the initial consonant, vowel, or final consonant). We observed main effects for the initial as well as the final consonants (see Table 4). Compared to trials that featured the same words, Tukey-adjusted post-hoc tests show that disambiguation was marginally less accurate for words differing on the initial consonant and significantly less accurate for words differing on the final consonant (see Table 5 and Figure 4).Footnote 1 In fact, disambiguation rates for words that differed on the final consonant were at chance (M = 0.56, SD = 0.16; t = 1.52, p = .13). This indicates that children are less likely to disambiguate words that differ on the first or the last phoneme.

Table 4. Fixed Effect Estimates (Top) and Variance Estimates (Bottom) for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs with One Differing Phoneme by Position of Phoneme Difference (Observations = 1681, Log Likelihood = −871.2)

Table 5. Tukey HSD-Corrected Post-Hoc Tests for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs with One Differing Phoneme by Position of Phoneme Difference

Figure 4. Disambiguation Task Performance of 3-6 Yr.-Olds for Word Pairs the Same or Differing in (A) Initial Consonant; (B) Vowel; and (C) Final Consonant. Data Points Jittered Horizontally and Vertically; Means Indicated by Black Dots and Values; Dashed Lines Represent Chance.

Number of differing phonological features

We next examined whether disambiguation accuracy varied by the number of differing phonological features between words. Given the prior analysis, we focused only on those words that differed in their initial or final consonants. Regarding initial consonants, we observed a main effect for words differing in no features vs. one, two, or three features. Tukey-adjusted post-hoc tests revealed that disambiguation was less accurate for words in which initial consonants differed in one phonological feature compared to no feature differences (i.e., in which initial consonants were the same). Moreover, disambiguation accuracy was at chance for words in which initial consonants differed in one phonological feature (M = 0.58, SD = 0.50; t = 1.42, p = .16). Regarding final consonants, we observed a main effect for words differing in no features vs. one, two, or three features. In addition, we observed a main effect for words in which final consonants differed in zero or one features compared to those differing in two or three features. Tukey-adjusted post-hoc tests revealed that disambiguation was less accurate for words in which final consonants differed in one phonological feature compared to those in which final consonants were the same and words in which final consonants differed in two or three phonological features compared to words in which final consonants were the same (see Tables 6 and 7 and Figure 5)Footnote 2. Disambiguation accuracy was at chance for words in which final consonants differed in one feature (M = 0.59, SD = 0.50; t = 1.64, p = .10) as well as words in which final consonants differed in two or three features (M = 0.54, SD = 0.50; t = 0.76, p = .45). In all, these patterns indicate that children are less likely to disambiguate words that differ in a single phonological feature on the initial consonant or any number of feature differences on the final consonant.

Table 6. Fixed Effect Estimates (Top) and Variance Estimates (Bottom) for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs with One Differing Phoneme by Number and Position of Feature Differences (Observations = 1681, Log Likelihood = −875.4)

Table 7. Tukey HSD-Corrected Post-Hoc Tests for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs with One Differing Phoneme by Number and Position of Feature Differences

Figure 5. Disambiguation Task Performance of 3-6 Yr.-Olds for Word Pairs with Zero (same = accurate) and One (different = accurate) or Two or Three (different = accurate) Differing Features in (A) Initial Consonants and (B) Final Consonants. Data Points Jittered Horizontally and Vertically; Means Indicated by Black Dots and Values; Dashed Lines Represent Chance.

Types of differing features

We next examined whether disambiguation accuracy might vary by the specific types of phonological features that differ between words. As before, we focused only on those words that differed on the initial or final consonants. For initial consonants, we observed a main effect of voicing (see Table 8). Tukey-adjusted post-hoc tests revealed that disambiguation was less accurate for word pairs in which initial consonants differed only in voicing (M = 0.65, SD = 0.48) compared to those that were the same in voicing (M = 0.77, SD = 0.42) or that differed in place, voicing, and manner (M = 0.78, SD = 0.42; see Table 9). For final consonants, we observed main effects of place and voicing and a place by voicing interaction (see Table 10). Tukey-corrected post-hoc tests revealed that disambiguation was less accurate for words in which final consonants differed only in voicing (M = 0.49, SD = 0.50) compared to those that were the same in voicing (M = 0.78, SD = 0.42) and those that differed only in manner (M = 0.84, SD = 0.37; see Table 11). Moreover, disambiguation was at chance for words in which the final consonant differed only in place (M = 0.55, SD = 0.48; t = 0.90, p = .37) and voicing (M = 0.49, SD = 0.50; t = 0.12, p = .91) and trended towards chance for word pairs in which the final consonant differed only in manner (M = 0.59, SD = 0.45; t = 1.92, p = .06).

Table 8. Fixed Effect Estimates (Top) and Variance Estimates (Bottom) for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs Differing in Initial Consonants by Feature Differences (Observations = 1681, Log Likelihood = −896.6)

Table 9. Tukey HSD-Corrected Post-Hoc Tests for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs Differing in Initial Consonants by Feature Differences

Table 10. Fixed Effect Estimates (Top) and Variance Estimates (Bottom) for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs Differing in Final Consonants by Feature Differences (Observations = 1681, Log Likelihood = −878.5)

Table 11. Tukey HSD-Corrected Post-Hoc Tests for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs Differing in Final Consonants by Feature Differences

Auditory discrimination

Finally, we examined auditory discrimination for word pairs that children failed to disambiguate reliably. Here we compared discrimination rates to chance. For words that differed by voicing on the initial consonant (M = 0.63, SD = 0.46; t = 1.54, p = .14) or on the final consonant (M = 0.63, SD = 0.45; t = 1.66, p = .11), discrimination rates did not differ from chance. By contrast, auditory discrimination rates were above chance for words that differed by place on the final consonant (M = 0.72, SD = 0.38; t = 2.90, p = .008).

Discussion

In this experiment, we asked whether 3- to 6-year-olds’ disambiguation of novel words would vary based on the number, position, or type of phonological differences between words. The results revealed that it did – when two words were more phonologically similar, children’s disambiguation performance was less accurate. Specifically, disambiguation of words that differed by one phoneme was less accurate than disambiguation of words that differed by two or more phonemes. Further, among word pairs that differed by one phoneme, disambiguation was least accurate for differences located at the initial (e.g., fim/vim) or final (e.g., val/vaz) consonant position. Feature-level analysis further suggested that position differences relying on voicing and place resulted in the lowest rates of disambiguation. Taken together, these findings indicate that 3- to 6-year-olds were less likely to disambiguate words that sounded more similar than words that sounded more different and suggest that what children assume about contrasting words may depend on how much those words differ.

Overall, our results are consistent with previous work, but they also extend it in important ways. As in previous studies, we found that the amount of difference between two words can affect whether children disambiguate a novel word. Overall, children appear to be sensitive to graded similarity between words (Creel, Reference Creel2012; Gerken, Murphy, & Aslin, Reference Gerken, Murphy and Aslin1995; Havy, Bertoncini, & Nazzi, Reference Havy, Bertoncini and Nazzi2011; Merriman & Schuster, Reference Merriman and Schuster1991; White & Morgan, Reference White and Morgan2008). As the number of sound and feature differences increases, particularly early in a word, so does the likelihood that the second word is interpreted as referring to the second object (e.g., Jarvis, Merriman, Barnett, Hanba & Van Haitsma, Reference Jarvis, Merriman, Barnett, Hanba and Van Haitsma2004). In our experiment, two and three phoneme differences produced significantly higher rates of disambiguation for 3- to 6-year-olds than did single phoneme differences. One additional note, though: not all single phoneme differences in our experiment resulted in lower levels of disambiguation. We found that word pairs featuring changes to the medial (vowel) position did not lead to lower disambiguation rates as compared to word pairs that differed by two or three phonemes. Instead, children reliably used vowel differences to trigger the disambiguation process (see also White & Aslin, Reference White and Aslin2011 for findings with toddlers; but see Havy et al., Reference Havy, Bertoncini and Nazzi2011 for contradictory results). Given that vowels play a critical role in studies on accent variation, we might have expected children to demonstrate higher overall tolerance for changes in vowel sounds as compared to changes in consonants. Although children accept changes to vowels under some situations, they did not here. Therefore, children might show a flexible interpretation of vowels that allows them to avoid confusing similar words in some contexts, yet also accept variable pronunciations of the same word in other contexts (White & Aslin, Reference White and Aslin2011).

Our experiment also extends this prior work in a couple ways. First, many studies have focused on contrasting a known, familiar word (apple) with an altered pronunciation of that same word (epple) (e.g., Creel, Reference Creel2012; Merriman & Schuster, Reference Merriman and Schuster1991; Swingley, Reference Swingley2016; though see Havy et al., Reference Havy, Bertoncini and Nazzi2011). These studies tend to show that even toddlers will interpret the altered pronunciation as referring to the known or familiar object. However, because children are familiar with the known words, it is not clear whether their tolerance for the altered pronunciation is based on a prior history of having heard these words pronounced in a variety of ways or on an inference about what the speaker in a given situation is intending to communicate. In our experiment, we focused exclusively on novel words. By doing so, we clarify that when children accept an altered pronunciation of a word, it is not simply because they have done so previously. Second, many prior studies have emphasized subtle contrasts between words (Creel, Reference Creel2012). In these studies, children often hear words that differ only in one segment (e.g., onset sound) and perhaps even in just a single phonological feature (e.g., White & Morgan, Reference White and Morgan2008). In our experiment, we systematically varied the amount of contrast, from words that differed in a single feature of a single phoneme to words that differed in three features of all three phonemes. As a result, we were able to assess whether the number, position, or even type of differences could shift children’s focus to the novel referent.

For children 3 years of age and older, the most robust effect on disambiguation in our study came from the amount or number of phonological differences between words. In fact, disambiguation rates were consistently high when word pairs differed by at least two contrasting phonemes. When words differed by only one phoneme, rates were often lower. However, we found that other factors – for example, the position of phonological differences between two words – also can affect whether children disambiguate a novel word. This is consistent with previous studies. For example, Merriman and Schuster (Reference Merriman and Schuster1991) found that 2- and 4-year-olds were more likely to select a familiar (rather than novel) object when a novel label differed in onset sound(s) but shared a syllabic ending (rime) with the familiar word (e.g., japple/apple). Further, Creel (Reference Creel2012) has shown that when atypical word pronunciations (like those from accented speakers) differ in onset sounds, they are more often interpreted by 3- to 5-year-olds as references to a known rather than a novel referent. This was true despite children detecting those differences. There is additional evidence that younger children also show a high sensitivity to phonological differences when they occur early in a word. White and Morgan (Reference White and Morgan2008) found that 19-month-olds more often interpreted novel words that differed from familiar words in one phonological feature (e.g., shoe/foo) as mispronunciations of the known label rather than as labels for novel objects. In our experiment, 3- to 6-year-olds were also less likely to disambiguate words that differed only in initial or onset sounds (e.g., fim/vim).

Interestingly, however, in addition to initial or onset differences, we also observed lower disambiguation rates on final or coda sounds (e.g., val/vaz). Perhaps this is not surprising given that children make more errors when producing codas in their own speech (e.g., Bernhardt & Stemberger, 1998; Stoel-Gammon & Dunn, 1985). Codas are processed last in the word sequence, often after initial hypotheses about the meaning of a word have already begun, and children tend to have more trouble identifying mispronunciations when they occur at the end of the word (Cole, Reference Cole1981; Creel, Reference Creel2012), perhaps due to higher rates of devoicing on final sounds. In fact, children in our experiment did not reliably discriminate words that differed only in voicing on the final phoneme, suggesting that the lower disambiguation rates could be a product of poorer discrimination. For many other phoneme position differences (e.g., words that differed on both the vowel and final consonant, words that differed on both the initial and final consonants, etc.), we did not find a negative effect on disambiguation.

Taken as a whole, the findings from our experiment suggest that not all contrasts are treated equally, and that the likelihood of disambiguation can vary based on how two words differ. Our results do show that factors like the number, position, and type of phonological differences between words can affect disambiguation. But they do not point to a simple explanation for why certain combinations of these factors undermine disambiguation while others do not. We can explore a couple of possible reasons though. One possibility is that contrast operates more like a correlate than a cause of disambiguation. Children assume that a speaker who uses different words is likely referring to different things. When two words are highly dissimilar, as they often are, this assumption may appear fairly automatic and accurate. The speaker’s intent is uncomplicated and the second word can be reliably interpreted as referring to a novel thing. But, when two words are highly similar, because they have fewer differences, because the differences are located on certain positions, or because some sound features more closely resemble each other, children may be less certain about the speaker’s intent and hesitate to quickly conclude that the second word was meant for a novel thing. They may instead consider that some differences between words could be unintentional, as in the case of mispronunciations, that the differences represent acceptable variations in normal pronunciation, such as in the case of slurred speech or an accent, or that more information is needed.

This line of reasoning could be interpreted as contradicting the principle of contrast – it suggests children may not automatically assume that two contrasting forms correspond to contrasting meanings in the world (Clark, Reference Clark1987, Reference Clark1988). However, it is worth noting that contrast is just one of the many pragmatic or contextual cues that children might consider when trying to determine the target of a speaker’s intentions (Grassmann, Stracke, & Tomasello, Reference Grassmann, Stracke and Tomasello2009; Grassmann & Tomasello, Reference Grassmann and Tomasello2010; Markman, Reference Markman1990; Smith, Jones, & Landau, Reference Smith, Jones and Landau1996; Tomasello & Akhtar, Reference Tomasello and Akhtar1995). The value of each cue, including contrast, might change depending on the context and what other cues are present (Jaswal, Reference Jaswal2004). This would allow children to apply the pragmatics of contrast flexibly, supporting their disambiguation of contrasting words in some instances but not others, depending on the information most relevant to understanding a speaker’s intentions (Krueger & Storkel, Reference Krueger and Storkel2020). For highly dissimilar words, contrast might be the most relevant and thus the most valuable cue. However, as word contrast decreases children may attend more closely, and assign more value, to the contextual cues most likely to indicate a speaker’s intentions. In the end, at least from a pragmatic standpoint, it is this intention-reading piece that is the most critical (Grassmann, Reference Grassmann and Matthews2014).

Another possible reason for lower rates of disambiguation for some contrasts but not others is that the disambiguation task itself is cognitively demanding. Unlike a more traditional word learning task, the disambiguation task requires children to attend to multiple words and objects, detect differences between the words and objects, map multiple words to multiple objects, and to do all this simultaneously. The task creates multiple pressure points on a child’s attentional, memory, language, and decision-making systems (Merriman & Marazita, Reference Merriman and Marazita1995) and in doing so consumes considerable cognitive resources. Of course, this would be true for any instance of disambiguation, so task difficulty alone would not explain why disambiguation rates might differ across word pairs. However, cognitive resources are also needed to process phonological differences. During a cognitively complex task like disambiguation, fewer resources would be available to the child for processing these differences. Indeed, Werker and colleagues have shown that task difficulty is greater when mapping novel labels to novel objects than when mapping known object-labels (Fennell & Werker, Reference Fennell and Werker2003; Stager & Werker, Reference Stager and Werker1997; Werker, Fennell, Corcoran, & Stager, Reference Werker, Fennell, Corcoran and Stager2002), especially for tasks involving minimally contrastive syllables (Pater, Stager, & Werker, Reference Pater, Stager and Werker2004). Lower rates of disambiguation might then occur because the additional resources needed to process subtle phonological changes between words are not available, or are less available, when solving the disambiguation task.

Both possibilities imply that disambiguation rates might fluctuate in step with changes in word similarity. Our data provide mixed support for this. We do find that words with single phoneme differences generate lower disambiguation rates than words with multiple phoneme differences showing that decreasing the contrast between words lowers disambiguation. But we do not find that disambiguation rates decline with every decrease in contrast – for example, two phoneme differences do not yield lower rates than three phoneme differences.

A final possible reason for lower disambiguation rates is that children simply did not hear the differences between certain words pairs. If the presentation of contrasting words activates assumptions about a speaker’s intent, then we would not expect those assumptions to be active in cases where children did not think the words contrasted. There is some evidence that this may have occurred in our experiment. For example, children did not reliably disambiguate words that differed only in voicing. Correspondingly, when these same word pairs were presented in an auditory discrimination task, children did not reliably discriminate them at above-chance rates. This leaves open the possibility that children did not disambiguate because they did not perceive these as different words. However, we did find that children were able to discriminate words that differed by place on the final consonant even though they did not reliably disambiguate them. These findings suggest that children may find it difficult to detect differences in words whose initial and final consonants differ by voicing and that this can negatively affect their ability to reliably disambiguate such words. However, for words that differ in place on the final consonant, disambiguation performance is not the result of an inability to reliably detect differences between the words.

In summary, the current research examined how differences between words might affect 3- to 6-year-olds’ use of the principle of contrast within a disambiguation task. The results suggest that the number, position, and types of phonological differences could all affect the likelihood that children would assign a novel word to a novel object. In general, as the differences between words decreased the likelihood of disambiguation also decreased. Together, these findings support the possibility that young children’s pragmatic assumptions about two contrasting words depend not only on if the words differ, but also on how they differ.

Acknowledgements

Portions of these findings were presented at the 2019 meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development and the 2020 meeting of the International Congress on Infant Studies.

Supplementary Material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper, visit http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000922000125

Appendix A

Table A1. List of Words Used for Same Trials in the Disambiguation Task for 3- to 6-year-olds, Number of Occurrences in the Sample, and Proportion of Novel Objects Chosen

Table A2. List of Words Used for Different Trials in the Disambiguation Task for 3- to 6-year-old Featuring One Phoneme Differences, Number of Occurrences in the Sample, and Proportion of Novel Objects Chosen

Table A3. List of Words Used for Different Trials in the Disambiguation Task for 3- to 6-year-olds Featuring Two Phoneme Differences, Number of Occurrences in the Sample, and Proportion of Novel Objects Chosen

Table A4. List of Words Used for Different Trials in the Disambiguation Task for 3- to 6-year-olds Featuring Three Phoneme Differences, Number of Occurrences in the Sample, and Proportion of Novel Objects Chosen

Appendix B

  • B1. Logit mixed effects model for number of phonological differences fit to disambiguation data of 3-6-yr.-oldsglmer(Correct ~ PD + (1|Participant) + (1 + PD|Trial)

  • B2. Logit mixed effects model for phonological difference by position fit to disambiguation data of 3-6-yr.-olds for same word pairs and word pairs differing in a single phonemeglmer(Correct ~ P1_SameDiff + P2_SameDiff + P3_SameDiff + (1|Participant) + (1 + P1_SameDiff + P2_SameDiff + P3_SameDiff|Trial)

  • B3. Logit mixed effects model for number of feature differences by position fit to disambiguation data of 3-6-yr.-olds for same word pairs and word pairs differing in a single phonemeglmer(Correct ~ P1_FD * P3_FD + (1|Participant) + (1|Trial)

  • B4. Logit mixed effects model for specific feature differences for the initial consonant fit to disambiguation data of 3-6-yr.-olds for same word pairs and word pairs differing in a single phonemeglmer(Correct ~ P1_FD_P * P1_FD_V * P1_FD_M + (1|Participant) + (1|Trial)

  • B5. Logit mixed effects model for specific feature differences for the final consonant fit to disambiguation data of 3-6-yr.-olds for same word pairs and word pairs differing in a single phonemeglmer(Correct ~ P3_FD_P * P3_FD_V * P3_FD_M + (1|Participant) + (1|Trial)

Footnotes

1 We note that each participant received only 2 trials with a single phonological difference in each position, which limits the possible accuracy values for trials of this type, whereas they received 18 trials with no phonological differences in each position, which provides much more possibilities for trials of this type.

2 We note that each participant received only 1 trial with 1 or 2 or 3 feature differences in the initial and final positions, which limits the possible accuracy values for trials of this type, whereas they received 18 trials with no phonological differences in each position, which provides much more possibilities for trials of this type.

* 51 occurrences of vome (voʊm) due to an additional same trial set

References

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 148. http://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernhardt, B. H., & Stemberger, J. P. (1998). Handbook of phonological development: From the perspective of constraint-based nonlinear phonology. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bion, R. A. H., Borovsky, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word–object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30 months. Cognition, 126(1), 3953. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2009). Monolingual, bilingual, trilingual: infants’ language experience influences the development of a word-learning heuristic. Developmental Science, 12(5), 815823. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00902.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cedrus Corporation (2012). SuperLab Pro 5.0 Stimulation Presentation Software (Version 5.0) [Computer software and manual]. San Pedro, CA: Cedrus Corporation.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1987). The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. Mechanisms of language acquisition, 1, 33.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1988). On the logic of contrast. Journal of Child Language, 15(2), 317336. https://doi:10.1017/s0305000900012393CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V. (1990). On the pragmatics of contrast. Journal of Child Language, 17(2), 417431. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900013842CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, R. A. (1981). Perception of fluent speech by children and adults. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 379, 92109. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1981.tb42001.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Creel, S. C. (2012). Phonological similarity and mutual exclusivity: on‐line recognition of atypical pronunciations in 3–5‐year‐olds. Developmental Science, 15(5), 697713. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01173.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diesendruck, G. (2005). The principles of conventionality and contrast in word learning: An empirical examination. Developmental Psychology, 41(3), 451463. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.3.451CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diesendruck, G., & Markson, L. (2001). Children’s avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 630641. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fennell, C. T., & Werker, J. F. (2003). Early word learners’ ability to access phonetic detail in well-known words. Language and Speech, 46(2-3), 245264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerken, L. A., Murphy, W. D., & Aslin, R. N. (1995). 3- and 4-year-olds’ perceptual confusions for spoken words. Perception & Psychophysics, 57(4), 475486. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03213073CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, S. A., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Baker, R. K. (1998). Infants’ disambiguation of novel object words. First Language, 18(53), 149164. https://doi.org/10.1177/014272379801805302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grassmann, S. (2014). The pragmatics of word learning. In Matthews, D. (Ed.). Pragmatic Development in First Language Acquisition, (pp. 139160). Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Grassmann, S., Schulze, C., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Children’s level of word knowledge predicts their exclusion of familiar objects as referents of novel words. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01200CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grassmann, S., Stracke, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). 2-year-olds exclude novel objects as potential referents of novel words based on pragmatics. Cognition, 112(3), 488493. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grassmann, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young children follow pointing over words in interpreting acts of reference. Developmental Science, 13(1), 252263. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00871.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halberda, J. (2003). The development of a word-learning strategy. Cognition, 87(1), B23B34. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00186-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Havy, M., Bertoncini, J., & Nazzi, T. (2011). Word learning and phonetic processing in preschool-age children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(1), 2543. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henning, K. J., & Merriman, W. E. (2019). The disambiguation prediction effect. Journal of Cognition and Development, 20(3), 334353. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2019.1578224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horst, J. S., & Hout, M. C. (2014). The Novel Object and Unusual Name (NOUN) Database: A collection of novel images for use in experimental research. unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Horst, J. S., & Hout, M. C. (2016). The Novel Object and Unusual Name (NOUN) Database: A collection of novel images for use in experimental research. Behavior Research Methods, 48(4), 13931409. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0647-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarvis, L. H., Merriman, W. E., Barnett, M., Hanba, J., & Van Haitsma, K. S. (2004). Input that contradicts young children’s strategy for mapping novel words affects their phonological and semantic interpretation of other novel words. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 47(2), 392406. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/032)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaswal, V. K. (2004). Don’t believe everything you hear: Preschoolers’ sensitivity to speaker intent in category induction. Child Development, 75(6), 18711885. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00822.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaswal, V. K., & Hansen, W. B. (2006). Learning words: Children disregard some pragmatic information that conflicts with mutual exclusivity. Developmental Science, 9(2), 158165. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00475.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalashnikova, M., Mattock, K., & Monaghan, P. (2016). Mutual exclusivity develops as a consequence of abstract rather than particular vocabulary knowledge. First Language, 36(5), 451464. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723716648850CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krueger, B. I., Storkel, H. L., & Minai, U. (2018). The influence of misarticulations on children’s word identification and processing. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(4), 820836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, B. I., & Storkel, H. L. (2020). Children’s response bias and identification of misarticulated wordsJournal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research63(1), 259273. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-19-00140CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markman, E. M. (1990). Constraints children place on word meanings. Cognitive Science, 14(1), 5777. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1401_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, E. (1991). The whole-object, taxonomic, and mutual exclusivity assumptions as initial constraints on word meanings. In Gelman, S. & Byrnes, J. (Eds.), Perspectives on Language and Thought: Interrelations in Development (pp. 72106). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511983689.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 121157. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(88)90017-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markman, E. M., Wasow, J. L., & Hansen, M. B. (2003). Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners. Cognitive Psychology, 47(3), 241275. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0285(03)00034-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merriman, W., Bowman, L., & MacWhinney, B. (1989). The mutual exclusivity bias in children’s word learningMonographs of the Society for Research in Child Development54(3/4), I-129. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166130CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merriman, W. E., & Marazita, J. M. (1995). The effect of hearing similar-sounding words on young 2-year-olds’ disambiguation of novel reference. Developmental Psychology, 31(6), 973984. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.31.6.973CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merriman, W. E., & Schuster, J. M. (1991). Young children’s disambiguation of object name reference. Child Development, 62(6), 12881301. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01606.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mervis, C. B., & Bertrand, J. (1994). Acquisition of the novel name–nameless category (N3C) principle. Child Development, 65(6), 16461662. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00840.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pater, J., Stager, C., & Werker, J. (2004). The perceptual acquisition of phonological contrasts. Language, 384–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quine, W. V. O. (1960). Word and Object. Cambridge, Massachusets: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Scofield, J., & Behrend, D. A. (2007). 2-year-olds differentially disambiguate novel words and facts. Journal of Child Language, 34(4), 875889. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000907008100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scofield, J., Hernandez-Reif, M., & Keith, A. B. (2009). Preschool children’s multimodal word learning. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10(4), 306333. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248370903417662CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scofield, J., Merriman, W. E., & Wall, J. L. (2018). The effect of a tactile-to-visual shift on young children’s tendency to map novel labels onto novel objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 172, 112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.03.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. B., Jones, S. S., & Landau, B. (1996). Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism?. Cognition, 60(2), 143171. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(96)00709-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stager, C. L., & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word-learning tasks. Nature, 388(6640), 381382. https://doi.org/10.1038/41102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoel-Gammon, C., & Dunn, C. (1985). Normal and disordered phonology in children. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Swingley, D. (2016). 2-year-olds interpret novel phonological neighbors as familiar words. Developmental Psychology, 52(7), 1011. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2007). Lexical competition in young children’s word learning. Cognitive Psychology, 54(2), 99132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.05.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., & Akhtar, N. (1995). 2-year-olds use pragmatic cues to differentiate reference to objects and actions. Cognitive Development, 10(2), 201224. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(95)90009-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wall, J. L., & Merriman, W. E. (2020). The pragmatics of discovery constrains children’s tendency to map novel labels onto novel objects. First Language, 40(3), 251274. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723720915414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wall, J. L., Merriman, W. E., & Scofield, J. (2015). Young children’s disambiguation across the senses. Cognitive Development, 35, 163177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.06.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. F., Fennell, C. T., Corcoran, K. M., & Stager, C. L. (2002). Infants’ ability to learn phonetically similar words: Effects of age and vocabulary size. Infancy, 3(1), 130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, K. S., & Aslin, R. N. (2011). Adaptation to novel accents by toddlers. Developmental Science, 14(2), 372384. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00986.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, K. S., & Morgan, J. L. (2008). Sub-segmental detail in early lexical representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(1), 114132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.03.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample Novel Word Pairs with Number, Position, and Features of Contrasting Phonemes

Figure 1

Figure 1. Schematic of a “Different” Trial in the Disambiguation Task

Figure 2

Figure 2. Schematic of a “Different” Trial in the Auditory Discrimination Task

Figure 3

Table 2. Fixed Effect Estimates (Top) and Variance Estimates (Bottom) for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance by Number of Differing Phonemes and Age (Observations = 2378, Log Likelihood = –1190.0)

Figure 4

Table 3. Tukey HSD-Corrected Post-Hoc Tests for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance by Number of Differing Phonemes

Figure 5

Figure 3. Disambiguation Task Performance of 3-6 Yr.-Olds for Word Pairs with (A) Three Differing Phonemes (different = accurate); (B) Two Differing Phonemes (different = accurate); (C) One Differing Phoneme (different = accurate); (D) No Differing Phonemes (same = accurate). Data Points Jittered Horizontally and Vertically; Means Indicated by Black Dots and Values; Dashed Lines Represent Chance.

Figure 6

Table 4. Fixed Effect Estimates (Top) and Variance Estimates (Bottom) for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs with One Differing Phoneme by Position of Phoneme Difference (Observations = 1681, Log Likelihood = −871.2)

Figure 7

Table 5. Tukey HSD-Corrected Post-Hoc Tests for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs with One Differing Phoneme by Position of Phoneme Difference

Figure 8

Figure 4. Disambiguation Task Performance of 3-6 Yr.-Olds for Word Pairs the Same or Differing in (A) Initial Consonant; (B) Vowel; and (C) Final Consonant. Data Points Jittered Horizontally and Vertically; Means Indicated by Black Dots and Values; Dashed Lines Represent Chance.

Figure 9

Table 6. Fixed Effect Estimates (Top) and Variance Estimates (Bottom) for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs with One Differing Phoneme by Number and Position of Feature Differences (Observations = 1681, Log Likelihood = −875.4)

Figure 10

Table 7. Tukey HSD-Corrected Post-Hoc Tests for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs with One Differing Phoneme by Number and Position of Feature Differences

Figure 11

Figure 5. Disambiguation Task Performance of 3-6 Yr.-Olds for Word Pairs with Zero (same = accurate) and One (different = accurate) or Two or Three (different = accurate) Differing Features in (A) Initial Consonants and (B) Final Consonants. Data Points Jittered Horizontally and Vertically; Means Indicated by Black Dots and Values; Dashed Lines Represent Chance.

Figure 12

Table 8. Fixed Effect Estimates (Top) and Variance Estimates (Bottom) for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs Differing in Initial Consonants by Feature Differences (Observations = 1681, Log Likelihood = −896.6)

Figure 13

Table 9. Tukey HSD-Corrected Post-Hoc Tests for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs Differing in Initial Consonants by Feature Differences

Figure 14

Table 10. Fixed Effect Estimates (Top) and Variance Estimates (Bottom) for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs Differing in Final Consonants by Feature Differences (Observations = 1681, Log Likelihood = −878.5)

Figure 15

Table 11. Tukey HSD-Corrected Post-Hoc Tests for Multi-Level Logit Model of 3-6 Yr.-Olds’ Disambiguation Task Performance for Same Word Pairs and Word Pairs Differing in Final Consonants by Feature Differences

Figure 16

Table A1. List of Words Used for Same Trials in the Disambiguation Task for 3- to 6-year-olds, Number of Occurrences in the Sample, and Proportion of Novel Objects Chosen

Figure 17

Table A2. List of Words Used for Different Trials in the Disambiguation Task for 3- to 6-year-old Featuring One Phoneme Differences, Number of Occurrences in the Sample, and Proportion of Novel Objects Chosen

Figure 18

Table A3. List of Words Used for Different Trials in the Disambiguation Task for 3- to 6-year-olds Featuring Two Phoneme Differences, Number of Occurrences in the Sample, and Proportion of Novel Objects Chosen

Figure 19

Table A4. List of Words Used for Different Trials in the Disambiguation Task for 3- to 6-year-olds Featuring Three Phoneme Differences, Number of Occurrences in the Sample, and Proportion of Novel Objects Chosen

Supplementary material: File

Stager et al. supplementary material

Stager et al. supplementary material

Download Stager et al. supplementary material(File)
File 2.9 MB