Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T07:16:32.516Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stigma by association

Psychological factors in relatives of people with mental illness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Margareta Östman*
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Lund
Lars Kjellin
Affiliation:
Psychiatric Research Centre, Örebro, Sweden
*
Dr Margareta Östman, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Division of Psychiatry, University Hospital SE-221 85 Lund, Sweden. Tel: 46 173849; fax: 46 173884; e-mail: margareta.ostman@psykiatr.lu.se
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Stigma affects not only people with mental illnesses, but their families as well. Understanding how stigma affects family members in terms of both their psychological response to the ill person and their contacts with psychiatric services will improve interactions with the family.

Aims

To investigate factors of psychological significance related to stigma of the relatives.

Method

In a Swedish multi-centre study, 162 relatives of patients in acute psychiatric wards following both voluntary and compulsory admissions were interviewed concerning psychological factors related to stigma.

Results

A majority of relatives experienced psychological factors of stigma by association. Eighteen per cent of the relatives had at times thought that the patient would be better off dead, and 10% had experienced suicidal thoughts. Stigma by association was greater in relatives experiencing mental health problems of their own, and was unaffected by patient background characteristics.

Conclusions

Interventions are needed to reduce the negative effects of psychological factors related to stigma by association in relatives of people with mental illness.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2002 

Negative attitudes exist in society towards people with mental illness. Discrimination occurs across every aspect of social and economic existence. Research has documented stigmatisation and its negative consequences for people with mental illnesses (Reference Farina and MillerFarina, 1982; Reference Link, Cullen and FrankLink et al, 1987).

Stigma is defined as a sign of disgrace or discredit that sets a person apart from others. Goffman (Reference Goffman1963), a sociological researcher with an interest in psychiatric stigma, defined stigma in terms of undesirable ‘deeply discrediting’ attributes that ‘disqualify one from full social acceptance’ and motivate efforts by the stigmatised individual to hide the mark when possible. However, he also commented that the difference between a normal and a stigmatised person was a question of perspective, not reality, and that stigma is in the eye of the beholder. A more recent definition has been proposed by Link & Phelan (Reference Link and Phelan2001) in which stigma exists when elements of labelling, stereotyping, separating, status loss and discrimination co-occur in a power situation that allows these processes to unfold.

Stigma by association

Stigma affects not only people with mental illnesses, but their families as well. The process by which a person is stigmatised by virtue of association with another stigmatised individual has been referred to as ‘courtesy’ (Reference GoffmanGoffman, 1963) or ‘associative’ stigma (Reference Mehta and FarinaMehta & Farina, 1988). Stigma by association has received comparatively little attention from empirical researchers. According to Mehta & Farina (Reference Mehta and Farina1988), being a close relative of a person with severe mental illness creates ‘a particularly difficult and delicate position if they cannot remove themselves, for they are both marker and marked’. Other studies confirm the process of stigma by association in family members (Reference LefleyLefley, 1987; Reference Phelan, Bromet and LinkPhelan et al, 1998; Reference ByrneByrne, 2001; Reference Struening, Perlick and LinkStruening et al, 2001).

To widen the knowledge of stigma by association in families of patients with severe mental illness it might be valuable to measure aspects of psychological distress and psychological burden perceived by members of these families. Accordingly, understanding how the situation of stigma affects family members both in connection with psychological feelings towards the ill person and in connection with psychiatric services can increase the knowledge of the situation of these families. Different aspects of family burden and participation in care are important parts of a Swedish study of the quality of mental health services during the period 1997-1999. In this study relatives of both compulsorily and voluntarily admitted patients were interviewed about different aspects of their burden, the need for support and their participation in the care of the patient. In particular, the psychological effects of being a relative of a person with severe mental illness were assessed.

The aim of the part of the study reported here was to investigate factors of psychological significance related to stigma by association in the relatives. Further aims are to investigate differences in these factors according to background variables concerning both the patient and the relative, and the relationship between the relative's mental health and perceived associative stigma.

METHOD

Study design

The research reported here is part of an international study on the use of coercion in Scandinavian mental health care systems. This Swedish multi-centre study focuses on voluntary and compulsory psychiatric in-patient care.

A consecutive sample of committed patients and a random sample of patients voluntarily admitted to acute psychiatric wards were included in the study. People aged under 18 years and over 70 years, people with a main diagnosis of alcohol or substance misuse, severe learning difficulties or severe dementia, mentally disordered offenders, and individuals not speaking Swedish were excluded. The remaining patients were contacted by a psychiatrist within 5 days of their admission and invited to participate in the study. A psychiatrist assessed the patients' psychosocial functioning and psychopathology, and assigned a diagnosis according to DSM—IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Three weeks after admission to hospital a clinical psychologist or a psychiatrist interviewed the patients and asked their permission to interview a close relative, nominated by the patient.

Relatives were identified as spouses, parents, children or ‘other’ (mainly siblings), or as non-relatives. The interview with the relative was performed about a month after the patient's admission to hospital, by a trained psychiatric social worker. None of the interviewers was involved in the treatment of the patient. Some information concerning the patient was collected from case notes and an interview with the patient. The study was approved by the research ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of the University of Uppsala.

Setting

The study has as its subject the in-patient psychiatric services of four different Swedish centres, each with comprehensive responsibility for a geographically defined catchment area comprising both urban and rural areas, and with a total population of 90 000-260 000 inhabitants. The psychiatric departments in the counties had 3.13-5.13 beds per 10 000 inhabitants for short-term psychiatric care.

Participants

A consecutive sample of 196 committed patients and a random sample of 179 voluntarily admitted patients were asked to take part. At the first interview 138 committed and 144 voluntarily admitted patients participated, and at the follow-up interview (around 3 weeks after admission) 118 and 117 patients, respectively, took part. At the second interview the patients were asked for permission to contact a relative. Altogether 162 relatives — 73 relatives of the committed patients and 89 relatives of the voluntarily admitted patients — were interviewed. Drop-out occurred at two different stages in the investigation: when patients refused contact with a relative or stated that they lacked a relative to interview, and when the relative refused an interview or when contact failed. Dropout occurred twice as often at the first stage. Of the relatives asked to participate, 13% refused or were unable to perform the interview.

Patients whose close relatives were interviewed did not differ from the entire sample in terms of gender, age, diagnosis and level of functioning. Thirty-eight per cent of the patients were men. The mean age was 43 years (range 19-69 years). Thirty-one per cent of both the committed and the voluntarily admitted patients had a psychosis diagnosis according to DSM—IV, including schizophrenia, delusional disorders, schizoaffective and schizophreniform disorders and atypical psychoses; 44% had a diagnosis of affective mood disorder and 25% had other diagnoses. Their psychosocial function was measured using the Global Assessment Scale (American Psychiatric Association, 1987); the mean score was 37 (range 10-71). Background characteristics of the participating relatives are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Background characteristics of the relatives participating in the study

Men (n=78) (%) Women (n=84) (%) Total (n=162) (%)
Age (years)
19 5 0 3
20-39 32 23 27
40-59 43 51 48
60+ 20 26 23
Relationship to the patient
Spouse 471 *** 12 29
Parent 10 422 *** 27
Son/daughter 16 18 13
Other, sibling 21 33 27
Non-relative 6 2 4
Living in the same household 513 ** 25 38
Duration of relationship (years)
0-19 27 14 20
20-39 62 69 66
40-59 11 17 14

Interview with the relatives

The instrument used was a semi-structured questionnaire, asking relatives about their situation as the relative of a person with severe mental illness and their experiences in relation to both compulsory and voluntary psychiatric care. The questionnaire was developed from clinical experience and focuses on the burden of relatives, their need for support, and participation in the care. It contains 95 questions, measuring the relative's own objective feelings. The instrument includes eight dimensions of burden and participation in care, as well as measures of family attitudes towards mental health care in a general hospital. The instrument takes 60-90 min to administer, of which the ‘burden’ items take about 45 min. The time frame for the question is in most cases the month before the patient's admission to hospital. The instrument was developed for face-to-face interviews, but is also suitable for use over the telephone. The interrater reliability has been calculated and found satisfactory, with Cohen's κ=0.98 and an absolute correspondence of ratings in 96% of the questions. The test—retest reliability concerning burden and participation in care has been found to be generally satisfactory, measured both as a percentage of concordance and as Cohen's κ (Reference Östman and HanssonÖstman & Hansson, 2000a ). The instrument is also available in an English version and has been further described by Schene et al (Reference Schene, Tessler and Gamache1994).

The following items describing psychological factors related to associated stigma were investigated, and the respondents' answers were classified as yes/no:

  1. (a) Do you find the staff of the psychiatric services to be supportive in carrying the burden of being a relative of a person with severe mental illness?

  2. (b) Do you feel inferior to the staff of the psychiatric services in conversations?

  3. (c) Has the person's mental illness affected the possibilities of your having company of your own?

  4. (d) Do you feel supported by anyone in carrying the burden of having a relative with mental illness?

  5. (e) Has the person's mental illness impaired the relationship between you and that person?

  6. (f) Are there times when you wish that the person with mental illness had never been born, or that you and the person had never met?

  7. (g) Has the person's mental illness led to any mental health problems of your own?

  8. (h) Is the burden of the situation of being a relative so heavy that you have thought of suicide?

  9. (i) Are there times when you think that the ill person would be better off dead?

Statistical analysis

The chi-squared test was used to test for differences in proportions. Comparisons of answers to items describing psychological factors between subgroups were carried out with non-parametric tests, the Kruskal-Wallis test and the Mann—Whitney U-test. A P level of 0.05 was considered significant.

RESULTS

Relationship between assessed psychological factors and background characteristics of the participants

Eighty-three per cent of the relatives experienced a burden in one or more of the assessed psychological factors of stigma by association.

When psychological factors related to stigma assessed in this study were compared with the patients' diagnosis (Table 2) only one difference was found. Relatives of patients with an affective disorder were less likely to believe that the patient would be better off dead (9% v. 24% and 27%; χ2=7.8, d.f.=2, P=0.020). No other difference was found in relation to diagnosis. No significant differences were found due to the patient's age, gender or global assessment score, or whether the patient's admission was compulsory or voluntary.

Table 2 Differences in psychological factors related to stigma in relatives according to patient diagnosis

Patient diagnosis Total
Psychosis (n=50) (%) Affective disorder (n=71) (%) Other diagnosis (n=41) (%) (n=162) (%)
The staff of the psychiatric services were supportive in carrying the burden of being a relative of a person with severe mental illness 18 22 32 24
The relative felt inferior to the staff of the psychiatric services in conversations 20 32 32 28
The patient's mental illness had affected the relative's possibilities of having company of his/her own 35 35 27 34
The relative had support from someone in carrying the burden of being a relative of a person with mental illness 70 66 59 65
The patient's mental illness had impaired the relationship between the relative and the patient 24 30 17 25
There were times when the relative wished that the patient had never been born or that the relative and the patient had never met 22 21 20 21
The patient's mental illness had led to mental health problems in the relative 40 44 34 40
The burden on the relative was so heavy that the relative had had suicidal thoughts 6 11 12 10
The relative sometimes believed that the ill person would be better off dead 24 9* 27 18

When controlling for background variables of the relatives, there was no difference among the measured factors of stigma in relation to the relative's age. Depending on the relative's gender there was a difference in one respect: a greater proportion of women had thought that the ill relative would be better off dead (72% v. 28%; χ2=6.1, d.f.=2, P=0.048).

When the nature of the relationship was considered (Table 3), some differences were found: spouses were more often negatively affected in their possibilities of having company of their own (55% v. 21-33%; χ2=12.5, d.f.=3, P= 0.006), and more often had times of wishing that the patient had never been born or that the relative and the patient had never met (32% v. 5-23%; χ2=8.2, d.f.=3, P=0.043). Spouses also more seldom believed that the ill relative would be better off dead (4% v. 21-33%; χ2=10.3, d.f.=3, P=0.016).

Table 3 Differences in psychological factors related to stigma in subgroups of relatives

Subgroup
Spouses (n=47) (%) Parents (n=43) (%) Grown-up children (n=21) (%) Others, mostly siblings (n=51) (%)
The staff of the psychiatric services were supportive in carrying the burden of being a relative of a person with severe mental illness 30 28 24 14
The relative felt inferior to the staff of the psychiatric services in conversations 21 26 43 29
The patient's mental illness had affected the relative's possibilities of having company of his/her own 55** 33 29 21
The relative had support from someone in carrying the burden of being a relative of a person with mental illness 68 72 76 58
The patient's mental illness had impaired the relationship between the relative and the patient 32 14 38 21
There were times when the relative wished that the patient had never been born or that the relative and the patient had never met 32* 14 5 23
The patient's mental illness had led to mental health problems in the relative 49 47 43 28
The burden on the relative was so heavy that the relative had had suicidal thoughts 13 16 5 5
The relative sometimes believed that the ill person would be better off dead 4* 21 33 23

When the relative lived with the patient, a greater proportion reported that the patient's mental illness had affected their possibilities of having company of their own (51% v. 24%; χ2=12.4, d.f.=1, P<0.001). Furthermore, there was a smaller proportion who sometimes believed that the patient would be better off dead (3% v. 27%; χ2=14.2, d.f.=1, P<0.001) and a greater proportion of relatives who sometimes wished that the patient had never been born or that the relative and the patient had never met (30% v. 16%; χ2=4.3, d.f.=1, P=0.038).

Psychological factors of stigma related to relatives' own mental health problems

Among the relatives who felt that the patient's mental illness had caused mental health problems in themselves — 40% of the total group of relatives — a greater proportion sometimes believed that the ill person would be better off dead (26% v. 12%; χ2=5.03, d.f.=0.025). This group was also more likely to have had suicidal thoughts (20% v. 3%; χ2=12.5, d.f.=1, P<0.001).

DISCUSSION

Despite increasing awareness and discussion of stigma by association or family stigma, empirical researchers in mental health care have paid little attention to the topic compared with the broader topic of family burden. When relatives of people with severe mental illness are interviewed about their situation there is no easy delimitation between the topics of family burden, concerning subjective psychological burdens, and family stigma. However, assessing different aspects of associated stigma, measured both as family members' perceptions of treatment by others, and as psychological reactions to the situation of being related to a person with severe mental illness, may shed further light on the topic.

Psychological factors related to stigma

The results showed that a relatively high proportion of relatives considered that the patient's mental illness had affected the possibilities of having company of their own or had influenced relations with others, and had also led to mental health problems in the relatives themselves. A striking finding was that, for one group of relatives, these circumstances had seriously affected their thoughts about life and death, both in connection with their ill relative and in terms of suicidal thoughts of their own. Additionally, these relatives believed that the ill relative would be better off dead, and/or wished that the patient and the relative had never met and that the patient had never been born.

A majority of the relatives obtained support in carrying the burden of being related to a person with severe mental illness, mostly from other family members or their network of close friends, and more seldom from employees of the psychiatric services. Relatives' feelings of inferiority to staff in conversation, which is a stigmatising experience, may be an explanation for the low levels of cooperation between relatives and professionals.

Patient background factors

Minimal differences were found between psychological factors related to stigma among relatives and the background characteristics of the patient, whether age, gender, form of diagnosis or psychosocial functioning. This may illustrate that the situation of being a close relative of a person with severe mental illness is in itself a factor of importance, and contradicts the conventional wisdom of anti-stigma initiatives, that members of the public differentiate between illnesses. Furthermore, our results indicate differences in psychological factors of stigma according to the relative's gender and relationship to the patient. Female relatives were more prone to believe that the patient would be better off dead, and spouses (of whom a majority were men) were more affected in their possibilities of having company of their own and more often had times wishing that the relative and the patient had never met. In contrast to other relatives, spouses almost never believed that the patient would be better off dead. Our results may indicate, as seen in an earlier study by Noh & Avison (Reference Noh and Avison1988), specific gender differences among relatives in coping with their burdensome situation.

Relationship between stigma by association and the relatives' mental health

The findings of a high level of occurrence of psychological distress among relatives of people with severe mental illness are in accordance with earlier studies of relatives where the patient had been admitted to hospital (Scottish Schizophrenia Research Group, 1987, 1988). An earlier study by Ö stman & Hansson (Reference Östman and Hansson2000b ) reported a relationship between the relative's mental health and family burden, as well as participation in care and the relative's own need for support. In all areas, relatives who had no mental health problems seemed to live a life more of their own, were more satisfied with the patient's treatment and more often had a positive view of the quality of the psychiatric services. These earlier findings agree reasonably well with our findings of more psychological factors of stigma when the relatives experience mental health problems themselves. That relatives with their own mental health problems more often think that the patient would be better off dead and have more suicidal thoughts of their own ought to lead to new efforts to decrease the psychological costs in these families. Furthermore, a well-functioning and supportive network around a person with mental illness has been shown to reduce relapse (Reference Bebbington and KuipersBebbington & Kuipers, 1994; Reference Cornwall and ScottCornwall & Scott, 1996).

Limitations of the study

Although the study has limitations in its use of a semi-structured interview with single questions for assessment of different factors of burden and of psychological distress, it also has the strength of investigating themes not previously approached. The semi-structured interview method makes it possible to obtain information and assess topics of a psychological nature, questions of life and death, and factors related to associated stigma, in a research design with a relatively large sample size.

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. Stigma by association in relatives of people with mental illness is itself a cause of psychological distress, and this is more pronounced when relatives themselves experience mental health problems.

  2. There are minimal gender differences in coping with the burdensome situation of having a relative with severe mental illness, although women tend to express more inner thoughts of death.

  3. For some people, having a relative with severe mental illness leads to serious thoughts about life and death, both in connection to the ill person and in terms of suicidal thoughts.

LIMITATIONS

  1. Almost a third of eligible relatives were not interviewed, because either the patient refused contact with relatives or the relative refused to participate.

  2. The use of a semi-structured interview with single questions for assessment of different factors of burden and of psychological distress may limit comparisons with other results.

  3. Owing to the exclusion criteria the results cannot be generalised to relatives of all people with mental illness.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest The study was supported by grants from the Vardal Foundation, Sweden (V2001 141).

References

American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn, revised) (DSM–III–R). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM–IV). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Bebbington, P. & Kuipers, L. (1994) The predictive utility of expressed emotion in schizophrenia: an aggregate analysis. Psychological Medicine, 24, 707718.Google Scholar
Byrne, P. (2001) Psychiatric stigma. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 281284.Google Scholar
Cornwall, P. & Scott, J. (1996) Burden of care, psychological distress and satisfaction with services in the relatives of acutely mentally disordered adults. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 31, 345348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farina, A. (1982) The stigma of mental disorders. In In The Eye of the Beholder (ed. Miller, A.), pp. 305363. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1963) Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Lefley, H. (1987) Impact of mental illness in families of mental health professionals. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 613619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Link, B. & Phelan, J. (2001) Conceptualizing stigma. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 363385.Google Scholar
Link, B., Cullen, F., Frank, J., et al (1987) The social rejection of ex-mental patients: understanding why labels matter. American Journal of Sociology, 92, 14611500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehta, S. & Farina, A. (1988) Associative stigma: perceptions of the difficulties of college-aged children of stigmatized fathers. Journal of Social Clinical Psychology, 7, 192202.Google Scholar
Noh, S. & Avison, W. (1988) Spouses of discharged psychiatric patients: factors associated with their experience of burden. Journal of Marriage and Family, 50, 377389.Google Scholar
Östman, M. & Hansson, L. (2000a) Family burden and care participation. A test–retest reliability study of an interview instrument concerning families with a severely mentally ill family member. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 54, 327332.Google Scholar
Östman, M. & Hansson, L. (2000b) Family burden, participation in care and mental health – an 11-year comparison of the situation of relatives to compulsorily and voluntarily admitted patients. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 46, 191200.Google Scholar
Phelan, J., Bromet, E. & Link, B. (1998) Psychiatric illness and family stigma. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 24, 115126.Google Scholar
Schene, A., Tessler, R. & Gamache, G. (1994) Instruments measuring family or caregiver burden in severe mental illness. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 29, 228240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scottish Schizophrenia Research Group (1987) The Scottish First Episode Schizophrenia Study. IV. Psychiatric and social impacton relatives. British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 340344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scottish Schizophrenia Research Group (1988) The Scottish First Episode Schizophrenia Study. V. One-year follow-up. British Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 470476.Google Scholar
Struening, E., Perlick, D., Link, B., et al (2001) The extent to which caregivers believe most people devalue consumers and their families. Psychiatric Services, 52, 16331638.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Background characteristics of the relatives participating in the study

Figure 1

Table 2 Differences in psychological factors related to stigma in relatives according to patient diagnosis

Figure 2

Table 3 Differences in psychological factors related to stigma in subgroups of relatives

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.