Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T20:16:43.123Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Perceptual alterations in the relationship between sensory reactivity, intolerance of uncertainty, and anxiety in autistic children with and without ADHD

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 November 2023

Helen J. Powell*
Affiliation:
Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
Jason L. He
Affiliation:
Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
Nermin Khalil
Affiliation:
Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
Ericka L. Wodka
Affiliation:
Center for Autism and Related Disorders, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD, USA Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Sciences, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
Alyssa DeRonda
Affiliation:
Center for Neurodevelopmental and Imaging Research, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD, USA
Richard A. E. Edden
Affiliation:
Department of Radiology and Radiological Science, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA F.M. Kirby Research Center for Functional Brain Imaging, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD, USA
Roma A. Vasa
Affiliation:
Center for Autism and Related Disorders, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD, USA Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Sciences, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
Stewart H. Mostofsky
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Sciences, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA Center for Neurodevelopmental and Imaging Research, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD, USA Department of Neurology, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
Nicolaas A. Puts
Affiliation:
Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK MRC Centre for Neurodevelopmental Disorders, King’s College London, London, UK
*
Corresponding author: Helen J. Powell; Email: helen.powell@kcl.ac.uk.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Sensory differences and anxiety disorders are highly prevalent in autistic individuals with and without ADHD. Studies have shown that sensory differences and anxiety are associated and that intolerance of uncertainty (IU) plays an important role in this relationship. However, it is unclear as to how different levels of the sensory processing pathway (i.e., perceptual, affective, or behavioral) contribute. Here, we used psychophysics to assess how alterations in tactile perception contribute to questionnaire measures of sensory reactivity, IU, and anxiety. Thirty-eight autistic children (aged 8-12 years; 27 with co-occurring ADHD) were included. Consistent with previous findings, mediation analyses showed that child-reported IU fully mediated an association between parent-reported sensory reactivity and parent-reported anxiety and that anxiety partially mediated an association between sensory reactivity and IU. Of the vibrotactile thresholds, only simultaneous frequency discrimination (SFD) thresholds correlated with sensory reactivity. Interestingly, we found that sensory reactivity fully mediated an association between SFD threshold and anxiety, and between SFD threshold and IU. Taken together, those findings suggest a mechanistic pathway whereby tactile perceptual alterations contribute to sensory reactivity at the affective level, leading in turn to increased IU and anxiety. This stepwise association can inform potential interventions for IU and anxiety in autism.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the same Creative Commons licence is used to distribute the re-used or adapted article and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Autism is a life-long, neurodevelopmental condition that is clinically defined by differences in social communication and a range of non-social characteristics, including restrictive, repetitive behaviors and atypical responses to sensory stimuli (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Sensory differences are estimated to be present in up to 97% of autistic individualsFootnote 1 (Baranek et al., Reference Baranek, David, Poe, Stone and Watson2006; Crane et al., Reference Crane, Goddard and Pring2009; Dellapiazza et al., Reference Dellapiazza, Vernhet, Blanc, Miot, Schmidt and Baghdadi2018; Leekam et al., Reference Leekam, Nieto, Libby, Wing and Gould2007). The importance of sensory differences as a defining feature of autism was highlighted following its addition to the DSM-5 criteria in 2013 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). As with other core features of autism, sensory processing can vary widely, both among and within individuals. Sensory differences in autism have been observed from as early as the first year of life and are known to persist throughout adulthood (Baranek et al., Reference Baranek, David, Poe, Stone and Watson2006; Crane et al., Reference Crane, Goddard and Pring2009; Leekam et al., Reference Leekam, Nieto, Libby, Wing and Gould2007). While some sensory experiences can be positive, others can be distressing, and can negatively impact school performance, social and adaptive behaviors, and developmental outcomes (Butera et al., Reference Butera, Ring, Sideris, Jayashankar, Kilroy, Harrison, Cermak and Aziz-Zadeh2020; Dellapiazza et al., Reference Dellapiazza, Vernhet, Blanc, Miot, Schmidt and Baghdadi2018; Howe & Stagg, Reference Howe and Stagg2016; Kojovic et al., Reference Kojovic, Hadid, Franchini and Schaer2019; Lane et al., Reference Lane, Young, Baker and Angley2010).

It is estimated that around 70% of autistic individuals will be diagnosed with at least one other co-occurring psychiatric disorder, of which ADHD and anxiety disorders are among the most common (Joshi et al., Reference Joshi, Wozniak, Petty, Martelon, Fried, Bolfek, Kotte, Stevens, Furtak, Bourgeois, Caruso, Caron and Biederman2012; Lai et al., Reference Lai, Kassee, Besney, Bonato, Hull, Mandy, Szatmari and Ameis2019; Leyfer et al., Reference Leyfer, Folstein, Bacalman, Davis, Dinh, Morgan, Tager-Flusberg and Lainhart2006; Simonoff et al., Reference Simonoff, Pickles, Charman, Chandler, Loucas and Baird2008). Anxiety disorders present unique challenges to autistic individuals and can be a great source of distress and impairment (T. E. Davis et al., Reference Davis, Hess, Moree, Fodstad, Dempsey, Jenkins and Matson2011; Keefer et al., Reference Keefer, Kreiser, Singh, Blakeley-Smith Judy Reaven and Vasa2018; Mazurek et al., Reference Mazurek, Vasa, Kalb, Kanne, Rosenberg, Keefer, Murray, Freedman and Ann Lowery2012; McVey et al., Reference McVey, Schiltz, Haendel, Dolan, Willar, Pleiss, Karst, Carlson, Krueger, Murphy, Casnar, Yund and van Hecke2018; Rodgers & Ofield, Reference Rodgers and Ofield2018; Vasa et al., Reference Vasa, Keefer, McDonald, Hunsche and Kerns2020). Studies of sensory differences in autistic individuals with co-occurring ADHD are comparatively sparse in relation to those studying the conditions separately. However, evidence suggests that those with a dual diagnosis may experience more sensory problems and have higher rates of other psychiatric conditions compared with either condition alone (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Wei, Chen, Su, Bai, Hsu, Huang, Chang, Chen and Chen2015; Mattard-Labrecque erg et al., Reference Mattard-Labrecque erg, ben Amor and Couture erg2013; Sanz-Cervera et al., Reference Sanz-Cervera, Pastor-Cerezuela, González-Sala, Tárraga-Mínguez and Fernández-Andrés2017; Simonoff et al., Reference Simonoff, Pickles, Charman, Chandler, Loucas and Baird2008). As well as showing links with other autistic features, sensory differences have increasingly been found to be associated with anxiety disorders (Amos et al., Reference Amos, Byrne, Chouinard and Godber2019; Carpenter et al., Reference Carpenter, Baranek, Copeland, Compton, Zucker, Dawson and Egger2019; Green et al., Reference Green, Ben-Sasson, Soto and Carter2012; South & Rodgers, Reference South and Rodgers2017). This relationship between sensory differences and anxiety in autisim is well documented and has also been shown to exist in other developmental disorders such as ADHD, as well as in neurotypical children (Carpenter et al., Reference Carpenter, Baranek, Copeland, Compton, Zucker, Dawson and Egger2019; Lane et al., Reference Lane, Reynolds and Dumenci2010a, Reference Lane, Reynolds and Thacker2010b; MacLennan et al., Reference MacLennan, Rossow and Tavassoli2021; Neil et al., Reference Neil, Olsson and Pellicano2016; Wigham et al., Reference Wigham, Rodgers, South, McConachie and Freeston2015). Although the directionality of the association between sensory differences and anxiety requires further elucidation and is likely to be complex in nature, two longitudinal studies in groups with and without autism found a unidirectional relationship between sensory hyper-reactivity and anxiety (Carpenter et al., Reference Carpenter, Baranek, Copeland, Compton, Zucker, Dawson and Egger2019; Green & Ben-Sasson, Reference Green and Ben-Sasson2010). Thus, sensory hyper-reactivity appears to be an early and important risk factor in the development of anxiety.

In efforts to define the mechanisms underlying the link between sensory differences and anxiety, several studies have highlighted the important role of intolerance of uncertainty (IU). IU is a multidimensional construct that is broadly defined as having a negative perception and/or reaction to situations or events that are deemed uncertain or unpredictable (Dugas et al., Reference Dugas, Freeston and Ladouceur1997). IU is known to be a dispositional risk factor for the development and maintenance of multiple anxiety disorders, including generalized anxiety disorder, social anxiety, OCD, and panic disorder (Mahoney & McEvoy, Reference Mahoney and McEvoy2012; Carleton et al., Reference Carleton, Fetzner, Hackl and McEvoy2013). However, the role of IU in autism and sensory processing is less understood. Though IU is an independent construct, it shares similarities with some of the core features of autism, such as a desire for sameness and rigidity in routine (Joyce et al., Reference Joyce, Honey, Leekam, Barrett and Rodgers2017). Thus, many studies have sought to understand the relationship between IU and clinical features of autism (Vasa et al., Reference Vasa, Kreiser, Keefer, Singh and Mostofsky2018), as well as its role in anxiety in autism (Boulter & Freeston, Reference Boulter and Freeston2014; Hodgson et al., Reference Hodgson, Freeston, Honey and Rodgers2017; Jenkinson et al., Reference Jenkinson, Milne and Thompson2020; Keefer et al., Reference Keefer, Kreiser, Singh, Blakeley-Smith, Duncan, Johnson, Klinger, Meyer, Reaven and Vasa2017). Importantly, IU has been shown to be implicated in the relationship between sensory differences and anxiety, in both pediatric and adult autistic samples (Glod et al., Reference Glod, Riby and Rodgers2019; Hwang et al., Reference Hwang, Arnold, Srasuebkul and Trollor2020; MacLennan et al., Reference MacLennan, Rossow and Tavassoli2021; Neil et al., Reference Neil, Olsson and Pellicano2016; Wigham et al., Reference Wigham, Rodgers, South, McConachie and Freeston2015).

Thus far, studies investigating sensory reactivity, IU, and anxiety have predominantly assessed sensory differences using questionnaire-based measures (e.g., parent/caregiver- or self-report). While informative, questionnaires often do not distinguish between different levels of the sensory processing pathway, including low-level perceptual sensitivity (i.e., how well an individual can detect or discriminate sensory stimuli) and sensory reactivity (i.e., how an individual feels or reacts towards sensory stimuli). Increasingly, evidence from prior studies in the visual and auditory domains has shown that objective measures of perceptual sensitivity (i.e., threshold data) and self-reported measures of sensory reactivity do not always correlate, thus highlighting that perceptual sensitivity and sensory reactivity are distinct constructs (Kuiper et al., Reference Kuiper, Verhoeven and Geurts2019; Sapey-Triomphe et al., Reference Sapey-Triomphe, Dierckx, Vettori, van Overwalle and Wagemans2023; Schulz & Stevenson, Reference Schulz and Stevenson2022). To our knowledge, only one study has assessed the associations of perceptual sensitivity and self-reported sensory reactivity with IU and anxiety (Sapey-Triomphe et al., Reference Sapey-Triomphe, Dierckx, Vettori, van Overwalle and Wagemans2023). Using a comprehensive set of assessments in a sample of autistic and neurotypical adults, Sapey-Triomphe et al. (Reference Sapey-Triomphe, Dierckx, Vettori, van Overwalle and Wagemans2023) showed that higher self-reported sensory reactivity, and to a lesser extent, visual perceptual sensitivity, were associated with more IU and anxiety, particularly in the autism group. However, thresholds for detection (visual perceptual sensitivity) and affective responses to visual stimuli did not correlate in either autistic or neurotypical adults. Those findings suggest that perceptual sensitivity and sensory reactivity have important, but separate, contributions to IU and anxiety. Thus, it remains unclear as to how the contributions of sensory differences to IU and anxiety are driven by alterations at the basic perceptual level and/or at the affective/behavioral level. Understanding the mechanisms for how different levels of sensory processing impact anxiety could help to guide specific interventions if appropriate and may lessen the development of additional co-occurring challenges.

Unlike questionnaire-based measures, psychophysical approaches can offer insight into the biological processes that underlie sensory processing, owing to their ability to probe lower-level perceptual function, and therefore link to neural responding patterns (Read, Reference Read2015). Since different perceptual processes such as detection and discrimination involve distinct mechanisms, their unique associations to behavioral measures allow a better understanding of the specific biological processes that govern those relationships. Studies using psychophysics have revealed altered sensory processing in autism across multiple sensory domains (e.g., tactile, auditory, and visual), as well as different sub-domains (e.g., detection and discrimination; Bonnel et al., Reference Bonnel, McAdams, Smith, Berthiaume, Bertone, Ciocca, Burack and Mottron2010; Dwyer et al., Reference Dwyer, Takarae, Zadeh, Rivera and Saron2022; He et al., Reference He, Wodka, Tommerdahl, Edden, Mikkelsen, Mostofsky and Puts2021b; Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Hudry, Ludlow and Hill2008; Puts et al., Reference Puts, Wodka, Tommerdahl, Mostofsky and Edden2014, Reference Puts, Wodka, Harris, Crocetti, Tommerdahl, Mostofsky and Edden2017; Tavassoli et al., Reference Tavassoli, Latham, Bach, Dakin and Baron-Cohen2011). Recent findings within the tactile domain have shown that differences in perceptual ability were related to several core traits of autism, such as social and communication differences (Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Woynaroski, Wallace and Cascio2019; He et al., Reference He, Wodka, Tommerdahl, Edden, Mikkelsen, Mostofsky and Puts2021b; Ide et al., Reference Ide, Yaguchi, Sano, Fukatsu and Wada2019). Furthermore, psychophysical studies have shown that low-level perceptual sensitivity is associated with differences in cortical excitation/inhibition balance, enabling inferences to the potential neurophysiological substrates of these differences (He et al., Reference He, Oeltzschner, Mikkelsen, Deronda, Harris, Crocetti, Wodka, Mostofsky, Edden and Puts2021; Puts et al., Reference Puts, Edden, John Evans, McGlone and McGonigle2011; Read, Reference Read2015). Thus, psychophysical approaches are a useful objective measure that can complement questionnaire-based assessments of sensory differences and their contributions to anxiety and IU.

In the current study, we aimed to investigate the associations between low-level tactile perception, sensory reactivity, anxiety, and IU, in a cohort of 8–12-year-old children with an autism diagnosis. We focused on low-level perception within the tactile domain as differences in touch processing are among the most widely observed in autism, and because alterations in tactile processing are known to contribute to other core features of autism, such as social and communicative problems (Cascio et al., Reference Cascio, Lorenzi and Baranek2016; Foss-Feig et al., Reference Foss-Feig, Heacock and Cascio2012; Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Hepburn and Wehner2003). As autism and ADHD commonly co-occur, and sensory differences are likely to be exacerbated in autistic individuals with co-occurring ADHD, we included participants with both an ASD only or ASD and ADHD diagnosis. First, we used parent- and child-report questionnaires to understand the relationships previously observed in the literature. We used the Sensory Processing Measure (SPM; Palmer et al., Reference Palmer, Ecker, Miller Kuhaneck, Henry and Glennon2007) to assess sensory reactivity, the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED; Birmaher et al., Reference Birmaher, Khetarpal, Brent, Cully, Balach, Kaufman and Neer1997) to assess anxiety, and the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale for Children (IUSC; Comer et al., Reference Comer, Roy, Furr, Gotimer, Beidas, Dugas and Kendall2009), to assess IU. We then used a well-validated battery of performance-based psychophysical tasks to assess whether individuals’ tactile detection and discrimination thresholds were associated with questionnaire measures of sensory reactivity, anxiety, and IU. We hypothesized that the relationship between sensory differences and anxiety would be mediated by intolerance of uncertainty, as previously shown in the literature. We further hypothesized that both tactile perceptual alterations and sensory reactivity would be associated with increased IU and anxiety.

Methods

Participants

Data from 38 children aged 8-12 years, were included in the analyses. Of the 38 children, 27 also had a clinical diagnosis of ADHD (11 had a diagnosis of autism only). Though the sample was predominantly male (32 male; 6 female), there were no apparent differences between male and female participants in age, IQ, or any of the study measures (Supplementary Table 1). Informed consent was provided by the caregiver for each child. Studies from which these data were collected received ethical approval from an Institutional Review Board. Relevant descriptive variables including age, IQ, and sex, are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of participant demographics and study variables

IUSC = intolerance of uncertainty scale for children; SPM = sensory processing measure; SCARED = screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders; SFD = simultaneous frequency discrimination; SD = standard deviation. Sensory reactivity, intolerance of uncertainty, and anxiety correspond to SPM Total Sensory Systems T-scores, IUSC (child-report) total scores, and SCARED (parent-report) total scores, respectively.

a Data from one participant was missing as they did not complete the SFD task.

Participants were principally recruited through local schools and additionally through community advertisements, advocacy organizations, and pediatricians. Screening was done via telephone interview with the child’s parent or caregiver. Children were excluded if they had a history of intellectual disability, seizures, brain injury, known causes of ASD (e.g., Fragile X), or other neurological disorders (e.g., Tourette’s syndrome), or were taking psychotropic medications other than stimulants. Children taking stimulants temporarily ceased their stimulant medication on the day before, and on, the day of testing. To check for possible deviations in attention or focus that could result from ceasing stimulant medication, we visually examined participants’ response tracking (evaluating correct/incorrect responses across the duration of trials) for each of the vibrotactile tasks. Response tracking and reaction times for all participants appeared normal.

Children were excluded if they had a history of, or met criteria for, major depressive disorder, conduct disorder, bipolar disorder, mania, adjustment disorder, or schizophrenia, as assessed by the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents Fourth Edition (DICA; Reich, Reference Reich2000) and/or the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (K-SADS; Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Rao, Flynn, Moreci, Williamson and Ryan1997). Children with a co-occurring diagnosis of anxiety disorders, obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), or oppositional defiance disorder were not excluded. Intellectual ability was assessed using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale of Children Fourth edition (WISC; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2003). Children with full-scale IQ scores below 80 (based on the lower band for clinical average scores [80-119]) were excluded unless there was an index discrepancy of equal or greater than 12, in which case one of either the Verbal Comprehension or Perceptual Reasoning Index scores had to exceed 80 and the lower of the two had to exceed 65.

Children included in the study met the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth (DSM-IV) and/or Fifth (DSM-5) edition criteria for ASD (American Psychiatric Association, 2000, 2013). An ASD diagnosis was confirmed using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic or 2 (ADOS; Lord et al., Reference Lord, Risi, Lambrecht, Cook, Leventhal, DiLavore, Pickles and Rutter2000, Reference Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, Gotham, Bishop and Risi2012) and Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Lord et al., Reference Lord, Rutter and le Couteur1994). ASD children were also included if they met additional DSM-IV or DSM-5 criteria for ADHD. ADHD diagnosis was confirmed through the Conners or Conners 3 Parent and Teacher Rating Scales-Revised: Long Form (ADHD-specific broad behavior rating scales and the ADHD Rating Scale-IV, home and school versions [ADHD-RS or DuPaul scale]; Conners et al., Reference Conners, Sitarenios, Parker and Epstein1998a; Reference Conners, Sitarenios, Parker and Epstein1998b Conners, Reference Conners2008). Diagnostic information was reviewed and verified by an experienced child neurologist.

Measures

Screen for child anxiety-related emotional disorders

The Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED; Birmaher et al., Reference Birmaher, Khetarpal, Brent, Cully, Balach, Kaufman and Neer1997) was used to measure child anxiety symptoms. The SCARED is a 41-item dual-informant scale with identical parent and child versions that ask about various DSM anxiety symptoms. A total score of ≥ 25 may indicate the presence of an anxiety disorder (Birmaher et al., Reference Birmaher, Khetarpal, Brent, Cully, Balach, Kaufman and Neer1997). The SCARED has been found to have good internal consistency in autistic children (Carruthers et al., Reference Carruthers, Kent, Hollocks and Simonoff2020; Stern et al., Reference Stern, Gadgil, Blakeley-Smith, Reaven and Hepburn2014). Total parent-reported SCARED scores were reported here.

Intolerance of uncertainty scale children

The Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale for Children (IUSC) is a 27-item questionnaire that assesses children’s tendency to react negatively to uncertain situations or events (Comer et al., Reference Comer, Roy, Furr, Gotimer, Beidas, Dugas and Kendall2009). The IUSC includes a child-report form and parent-report form, which were adapted from the original Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale for adults (Buhr & Dugas, Reference Buhr and Dugas2002). Higher scores indicate higher IU (Comer et al., Reference Comer, Roy, Furr, Gotimer, Beidas, Dugas and Kendall2009). Total IUSC scores from the child report were used in this study owing to its stronger convergent validity with measures of anxiety in typically developing children compared with the parent-report version (Comer et al., Reference Comer, Roy, Furr, Gotimer, Beidas, Dugas and Kendall2009).

Sensory processing measure

The Sensory Processing Measure (SPM) is a norm-referenced parent/caregiver-report measure that assesses sensory processing, praxis, and social participation (Palmer et al., Reference Palmer, Ecker, Miller Kuhaneck, Henry and Glennon2007). Higher scores indicate more difficulties with the associated scales. The SPM has shown excellent reliability and validity, including convergent validity with the more commonly adopted Sensory Profile (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Morrison and Stagnitti2010). T-scores for the Total Sensory Systems Scale of the SPM (TOT; combining scores from the vision, hearing, touch, body awareness and balance, and motion scales) were used in the analyses. Analyses using TOT raw scores (unadjusted for age or sex), allowing age to be included as a confounder, yielded similar results (Supplementary Table 2; Supplementary Figure 1; Supplementary Table 5).

Psychophysical assessment of tactile perception

Children’s tactile perceptual thresholds were assessed using a Cortical Metrics Braingauge stimulator (Fig. 1a) using a vibrotactile battery of tasks originally developed by Puts et al. (Reference Puts, Edden, Wodka, Mostofsky and Tommerdahl2013). All participants underwent psychophysical assessment. Children were asked to place their left hand on the stimulator, which delivered vibrotactile stimuli to the participants’ left index and middle fingers, with an amplitude of 0–350 µm and frequency in the flutter range of 0-50 Hz. Participants responded to each protocol using a computer mouse in their right hand. Vibrotactile stimulus parameters (i.e., amplitude and frequency) were controlled, and data collected and saved, by running a cortical metrics script on a computer. To ensure participants understood the protocols correctly, they had to pass three consecutive practice trials before starting each of the tasks. A stepwise adaptive tracking strategy was used for all conditions (detailed below and in the Supplementary Methods for each protocol). Participants were allowed to take breaks as needed between the protocols, except for protocols within the same domain. Data were visualized and analyzed using a custom-made software package in the programing language R (available at: https://github.com/HeJasonL/BATD).

Figure 1. a. Illustration of a cortical metrics braingauge vibrotactile stimulator. Children were asked to place their left hand on the stimulator and their right hand on a computer mouse. Two 5 mm cylindrical probes delivered sinusoidal pulses to the index and middle fingers. Participants responded with their right index and middle finger. Stimuli amplitude and frequency were between 0–350 μm and 0–50 Hz, respectively. b. Visual schematic of the simultaneous frequency discrimination task. Using a two-alternative forced choice design, both fingers are stimulated, one of which receives a standard stimulus of constant frequency (30Hz), and the other receives a comparison stimulus where the frequency can vary (initial frequency was 40 Hz). Children were asked to indicate on which finger they felt the higher stimulus. Following a staircase approach, the comparison stimulus decreased in frequency after a correct response and increased in frequency after an incorrect response. Details of the other vibrotactile tasks can be found in the supplementary methods.

Data from a total of five tasks (simultaneous frequency discrimination, static detection, dynamic detection, amplitude discrimination, and temporal order judgment) in the vibrotactile battery were included in the correlational analyses. As meaningful correlations were found only between the simultaneous frequency discrimination (SFD) task and our questionnaire measures, only the SFD task (Fig. 1b) is described here in detail. Brief descriptions for the remaining tasks are given below. Further details of each task are reported in the Supporting Information.

Simultaneous frequency discrimination

During the simultaneous frequency discrimination (SFD) task (Fig. 1b), vibrotactile stimuli were delivered simultaneously to both fingers (duration: 500 ms; amplitude: 200 µm). On one finger, a standard vibration (frequency = 30 Hz) was delivered, while a comparative vibration (starting frequency = 40 Hz) was delivered to the other. The order of the standard and comparative stimuli was pseudorandomized between each finger, in which the order was never the same in five or more consecutive trials. Using a two-alternative forced choice design, participants were asked to indicate which finger they thought had received the higher frequency stimulation, as soon as they felt it. The protocol included 20 trials, which were inter-spaced with a period of 5 seconds. In the first 10 trials, the comparative stimulus frequency was decreased for each correct response that the participant gave and increased for each incorrect response (one–up–one-down tracking paradigm). For the remaining trials, the comparative stimulus frequency was decreased for every two correct answers and increased for one incorrect answer (two–up–one-down tracking paradigm). SFD threshold for each participant was determined by taking the frequency mean of the last five trials, by which point the threshold (i.e., the minimum difference in frequency the participant can perceive) was assumed to have been reached. Higher thresholds are indicative of lower tactile perceptual sensitivity.

Other vibrotactile tasks

For the static and dynamic detection tasks, vibrotactile stimuli were delivered to a single fingertip, alternating between each finger in a pseudorandomized order, at either a constant (static) or gradually increasing (dynamic) amplitude. Participants were asked to indicate on which finger they felt the stimulus, as soon as they felt it. For the amplitude discrimination task, stimuli were delivered simultaneously to both fingertips, of which one differed in amplitude. Participants were asked to indicate on which finger they felt the stronger stimulus. In the temporal order judgment task, participants received stimuli to both fingertips at differing time intervals. Participants were asked to indicate on which finger they felt the first stimulus. Further detail of the vibrotactile tasks can be found in the Supporting Information.

Statistical analyses

Data were analyzed using R (version 4.0.3). All data and code for the analyses and figures can be found on the Open Science Framework (OSF; https://osf.io/uw8vb/).

Differences in age, IQ, SFD thresholds, SPM, SCARED, and IUSC scores between participants with a single diagnosis of autism (ASD only) and participants with a dual diagnosis of autism and ADHD (ASD + ADHD), and between male and female participants (Supplementary Table 1), were assessed using a series of Wilcoxon Rank Sum tests. Significance level was set at p ≤ .008 following Bonferroni’s correction for multiple comparisons (.05/6 tests).

Pearson correlation analyses were run between the parent-reported Total Sensory Systems (TOT) T-scores of the SPM (termed from now on as ‘sensory reactivity’), parent-reported total SCARED scores, child-reported IUSC total scores, the vibrotactile thresholds, and age. Cook’s Distance with a Di cutoff of 0.2 (below which values are not considered to have major influence) was used to identify potential outliers. Results did not significantly differ between Pearson correlations with and without outliers, therefore data analyses are shown here without the outliers removed. Results of all Pearson correlation analyses, including those without outliers as identified by Cook’s Distance, can be found in Supplementary Table 2 and Supplementary Table 3. Levene’s tests showed homoscedasticity between all variables. Inspection of histogram and Q-Q plots showed that distributions for IUSC scores and SFD thresholds appeared normal, however, the distributions for SCARED and SPM scores were skewed. Therefore, we also investigated relationships using Spearman rank correlations (Supplementary Table 3). As the results between Spearman and Pearson correlations were similar, only the Pearson correlations are shown here. Significance levels were set at p ≤ .05, except for analyses involving the vibrotactile thresholds, which were adjusted to p ≤ .006 after Bonferroni’s correction (.05/9 tests).

Mediation analyses using the non-parametric percentile bootstrap method were run using the mediation package in R for Causal Mediation Analysis (Tingley et al., Reference Tingley, Yamamoto, Hirose, Keele and Imai2014). Evidence of mediation is indicated by a significant indirect effect (the association between the predictor and outcome variable runs through the mediator). Full mediation exists when the indirect effect is significant, but the direct effect (the effect of the predictor on the outcome variable, controlling for the mediator) is not. If both indirect and direct effects are significant, the result is partial mediation. Unstandardized indirect effects were computed for each of the 10,000 bootstrapped samples, and the 95% confidence interval was computed by determining the indirect effects at the 2.5th and 97.5th percentiles. Interaction effects between predictor and mediator were not examined as we did not have a sufficiently large sample size. To check for the influence of outliers, we also compared the mediation results from the percentile bootstrap method with those of a bootstrap method using data cleaning via multivariate winsorization and coefficient estimation via maximum likelihood estimation of the covariance matrix (Zu & Yuan, Reference Zu and Yuan2010). Results from the multivariate winsorization bootstrap method and the percentile bootstrap method yielded almost identical results (Supplementary Table 6). As an assumption of causal mediation analysis is the correct causal ordering of independent, mediator, and dependent variables, we also tested the significance of indirect effects for different path combinations, which are shown in Supplementary Tables 4 and 5.

Results

Group differences between autism-only participants and those with co-occurring ADHD

To test for differences between participants with a single autism (ASD only) diagnosis and those with co-occurring ADHD (ASD + ADHD), a series of Wilcoxon rank sum tests were run. There were no significant differences between the two groups for age, SFD thresholds, anxiety scores or intolerance of uncertainty (IU) scores (Table 1). However, IQ scores were lower, and SPM scores higher, in the ASD + ADHD group compared with the ASD-only group (p = .005 and .003, respectively), concordant with the literature (Mattard-Labrecque erg et al., Reference Mattard-Labrecque erg, ben Amor and Couture erg2013; Sanz-Cervera et al., Reference Sanz-Cervera, Pastor-Cerezuela, González-Sala, Tárraga-Mínguez and Fernández-Andrés2017). IQ scores for the ASD + ADHD group were still within clinical average range.

Correlational analyses

Pearson’s correlation analyses showed that parent-reported sensory reactivity was positively correlated with parent-reported anxiety (Fig. 2a; R = .37; p = .023) and child-reported intolerance of uncertainty (Fig. 2b; R = .40; p = .013). Intolerance of uncertainty (IU) was also positively correlated with anxiety (Fig. 2c; R = .48; p = .002). When investigating the associations between tactile perceptual thresholds determined using vibrotactile psychophysics, we found that only simultaneous frequency discrimination (SFD) threshold significantly correlated with parent-reported sensory reactivity (Fig. 2d; R = .41; p = .011), although after Bonferroni’s correction (p = .006), this significance was lost. In addition, contrary to our hypothesis, no significant associations were found between SFD threshold and anxiety or IU (Fig. 2e, f; R = .21; p = .22 and R = .14; p = .40, respectively). There were no significant correlations between age and the study variables or between the raw scores of the SPM that were unadjusted for age and sex (Supplementary Table 2). Child-reported SCARED total scores were also investigated, however, despite a significant correlation between child-reported SCARED and IUSC scores (R = .45; p = .005; Supplementary Table 2), there were no significant correlations observed between child-reported SCARED total scores and SPM scores or SFD thresholds. Results of all correlational analyses can be found in Supplementary Table 2.

Figure 2. Pearson’s correlations between sensory reactivity, IU, anxiety and SFD threshold. a-c. There were moderate positive associations between all three measures of sensory reactivity, IU, and anxiety. d-f. There was a moderate positive association between SFD threshold and sensory reactivity. No significant associations were found between SFD threshold and anxiety or IU. IU = intolerance of uncertainty; IUSC = intolerance of uncertainty scale for children; SCARED = screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders; SFD = simultaneous frequency discrimination; SPM TOT = sensory processing measure total sensory systems.

Mediation analyses

IU mediates the relationship between sensory reactivity and anxiety

To replicate findings from previous studies in which IU has commonly been shown to mediate the relationship between sensory reactivity and anxiety, we first ran mediation analyses between these constructs. As a recent study by MacLennan et al. (Reference MacLennan, Rossow and Tavassoli2021) also found a full mediation effect between sensory hyper-reactivity and IU mediated through anxiety, in addition to a full mediation effect between sensory hyper-reactivity and anxiety through IU, we aimed to test both these models (MacLennan et al., Reference MacLennan, Rossow and Tavassoli2021). Mediation analyses revealed a significant full mediation effect of child-reported IU on the association between parent-reported sensory reactivity and parent-reported anxiety (β = 0.30; 95% CI [0.03, 0.76]; p = .02; Fig. 3a). We also found a significant, partial mediation effect of anxiety on the association between sensory reactivity and IU (β = 0.34; 95% CI [0.04, 0.74]; p = .02; Fig. 3b). No significant mediation effects were found for different mediation paths (Supplementary Table 4). These results suggest that in our sample of autistic children, the impact of sensory reactivity on anxiety appears to be fully driven by intolerance of uncertainty. As higher anxiety also appears to contribute to a higher IU, our findings suggest that IU and anxiety could be reinforcing factors of each other in the presence of augmented sensory reactivity. To test whether mediation effects were influenced by ADHD traits such as inattention, Conners and DuPaul scores were added to the mediation analyses as covariates. The indirect effects for both models remained significant (Supplementary Table 7), indicating that attentional difficulties or other ADHD traits did not influence the relationship between sensory reactivity and anxiety.

Figure 3. Visual schematic of the mediation path models between sensory reactivity (SPM total sensory systems T-scores), intolerance of uncertainty (IU; IUSC total scores), and anxiety (SCARED total scores). Dashed arrows indicate the indirect effect between the predictor and outcome that is mediated by the mediator. Solid arrows between the predictor and outcome indicate the total effect, with the direct effect in parentheses. a. Full mediation effect of IU on the relationship between sensory reactivity and anxiety. b. Partial mediation effect of anxiety on the relationship between sensory reactivity and IU. IUSC = intolerance of uncertainty scale for children; SPM = sensory processing measure; SCARED = screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders. **p ≤ .01; *p ≤ .05.

Sensory reactivity mediates the relationship between tactile perceptual sensitivity and anxiety

We next investigated two additional mediation models to assess the relationship between SFD threshold, IU, and anxiety, with sensory reactivity as the mediator. We hypothesized that SFD threshold would be a predictor of sensory reactivity, given that lower-level perception of sensory input occurs before higher-order processing. We also hypothesized that sensory reactivity is likely to precede anxiety and IU, as this has been the predominant finding in literature, and is consistent with our results from the previous two mediation models. For both mediation models, we found that parent-reported sensory reactivity significantly mediated the association between SFD threshold and parent-reported anxiety (β = 0.58; 95% CI [0.06, 1.56]; p = .02; Figure 4a), as well as between SFD threshold and child-reported IU (β = 0.80; 95% CI [0.11, 1.99]; p = .01; Figure 4b). Analyses of the reverse of those relationships yielded significant indirect effects, however, the corresponding regression coefficients were close to zero (β = 0.04; Supplementary Table 4), suggesting that those results are unlikely to be meaningful. Taken together, our results suggest that differences in tactile perceptual sensitivity impact anxiety through differences in sensory reactivity at the affective level. When Conners and DuPaul scores were added as covariates to the mediation analyses, the indirect effects for both mediation models remained significant (Supplementary Table 7), suggesting that ADHD-related traits did not contribute to the outcomes.

Figure 4. Visual schematic of the mediation path models between SFD threshold, sensory reactivity (SPM total sensory systems T-scores), intolerance of uncertainty (IU; IUSC total scores), and anxiety (SCARED total scores). Dashed arrows indicate the indirect effect between the predictor and outcome that is mediated by the mediator. Solid arrows between the predictor and outcome indicate the total effect, with the direct effect in parentheses. a. Full mediation effect of sensory reactivity on the relationship between SFD threshold and anxiety. b. Full mediation effect of sensory reactivity on the relationship between SFD threshold and IU. IUSC = intolerance of uncertainty scale for children; SPM = sensory processing measure; SCARED = screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders; SFD = simultaneous frequency discrimination. **p ≤ .01; *p ≤ .05.

Discussion

Here we assessed the role of tactile perceptual alterations on the relationship between sensory reactivity, intolerance of uncertainty (IU), and anxiety, in a sample of 8-12-year-old autistic children with and without co-occurring ADHD. Consistent with previous findings, our mediation analyses revealed that child-reported IU fully mediated an association between parent-reported sensory reactivity and parent-reported anxiety and that anxiety partially mediated an association between sensory reactivity and IU. When investigating the association between tactile perceptual sensitivity and questionnaire measures, we found that simultaneous frequency discrimination (SFD) was the only vibrotactile task for which thresholds significantly correlated with sensory reactivity. However, we found no direct correlations between SFD threshold and anxiety or IU. Interestingly, we found that sensory reactivity fully mediated an association between SFD threshold and anxiety, and between SFD threshold and IU. These results suggest a stepwise relationship whereby increased SFD threshold, in which higher thresholds indicate lower perceptual sensitivity towards frequency discrimination of tactile stimuli, contribute to increased prevalence of sensory reactivity, leading in turn to increased anxiety and IU. It is possible that the absence of a direct correlation between SFD threshold and anxiety and IU could therefore be explained by the fact that the effects of a higher SFD threshold on anxiety and IU may only occur when they are mediated by sensory reactivity at the affective level. Although these findings warrant replication in a larger, more sex-balanced cohort, our results suggest a bottom-up perceptual mechanism underlying the relationship between sensory differences and anxiety in autism.

Implications of SFD threshold in the associations between sensory reactivity, IU, and anxiety

Owing to the complexity and heterogeneity of both sensory differences and anxiety in autism, understanding how alterations in low-level neural functioning contribute to sensory differences at the affective level is crucial. In the current study, we found that individuals with higher (i.e., worse) SFD thresholds had higher sensory reactivity. The mechanisms underlying the relationship between perceptual processing and sensory reactivity have not been clearly established. However, in the context of this study, it is possible that individuals with altered SFD thresholds who are worse at discriminating stimuli could experience difficulties with interpreting or integrating sensory information, thus creating a sensory environment that is uncertain or ‘noisy’ (Pellicano & Burr, Reference Pellicano and Burr2012; van de Cruys et al., Reference van de Cruys, Evers, van der Hallen, van Eylen, Boets, de-Wit and Wagemans2014). Persistent unpredictability of sensory stimuli could therefore lead to greater negative affective responses and/or sensory avoidance. Over time, this difficulty with regulating sensory information at the perceptual and affective level could worsen one’s intolerance to perceived uncertainty of stimuli, which could further result in distress or anxiety.

Interestingly, there were no significant correlations between any of the other vibrotactile tasks and the questionnaire measures for sensory reactivity, anxiety, or IU. Though we cannot rule out that the absence of associations between other tasks and sensory reactivity could be because of insufficient power, it is possible that the specific mechanisms that underpin frequency discrimination are most accordant with those driving sensory-related anxiety and IU. Increasingly, studies have shown differences in only some perceptual sub-domains (e.g., amplitude, frequency, or temporal discrimination) but not others, suggesting that symptom presentations could have sub-domain-specific mechanisms, rather than associations to “perception” as a whole (Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Woynaroski, Wallace and Cascio2019; He et al., Reference He, Wodka, Tommerdahl, Edden, Mikkelsen, Mostofsky and Puts2021b; Ide et al., Reference Ide, Yaguchi, Sano, Fukatsu and Wada2019). Furthermore, our previous work has shown that autistic children with co-occurring ADHD had significantly more difficulty with discriminating the frequency and amplitude of simultaneously delivered vibrotactile stimuli compared with typically developing children and those with ADHD only (He et al., Reference He, Wodka, Tommerdahl, Edden, Mikkelsen, Mostofsky and Puts2021b). Thresholds for both tasks were correlated with sub-scales of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale (ADOS), but only SFD, and not amplitude discrimination, showed additional associations with some sub-scales of the Sensory Experiences Questionnaire (He et al., Reference He, Wodka, Tommerdahl, Edden, Mikkelsen, Mostofsky and Puts2021b). Those findings suggest that SFD may play a more important role in sensory reactivity at the affective level and that this may be specific to autism.

SFD requires complex encoding of temporal, intensity, and spatial information, and has previously been reported to be more difficult than other vibrotactile tasks, including simultaneous amplitude discrimination (Puts et al., Reference Puts, Edden, Wodka, Mostofsky and Tommerdahl2013). As such, SFD might encapsulate the complexity of tactile perceptual processing more so than other tasks. As the ability to discriminate sensory stimuli involves GABA-mediated lateral inhibition of cortical minicolumns (Casanova et al., Reference Casanova, Buxhoeveden and Gomez2003), SFD may also require a higher demand for GABA (Cardin et al., Reference Cardin, Carlén, Meletis, Knoblich, Zhang, Deisseroth, Tsai and Moore2009; Harris et al., Reference Harris, Arabzadeh, Fairhall, Benito and Diamond2006). In alignment with this, our previous work using magnetic resonance spectroscopy showed that higher vibrotactile SFD thresholds were associated with lower GABA and higher glutamate/glutamine concentrations in the sensorimotor region and thalamus, respectively (He et al., Reference He, Oeltzschner, Mikkelsen, Deronda, Harris, Crocetti, Wodka, Mostofsky, Edden and Puts2021a; Puts et al., Reference Puts, Edden, John Evans, McGlone and McGonigle2011). This could suggest that poorer frequency discrimination may be linked to excessive neural excitation or reduced inhibition. The concept of an excitation/inhibition (E/I) imbalance has long been proposed as a theoretical framework underlying neural processes associated with autism, including those related to sensory differences (Rubenstein & Merzenich, Reference Rubenstein and Merzenich2003). In the context of anxiety, as GABAergic signaling in the amygdala is known to play a crucial role in regulating anxiety and threat responses (Davis, Reference Davis1992), we speculate that the cortical and cognitive processes that govern poorer frequency discrimination may also be related to those contributing to anxiety.

In the current study, we used vibrotactile stimuli for our measure of perceptual sensitivity, but the extent to which the perceptual contributions found here could translate to other sensory domains (e.g., visual, auditory) remains unclear. As signals encoding sensory input are known to converge in both cortical and subcortical regions, despite distinct receptive origins, it is possible that psychophysical measures could provide insight into task-specific biological mechanisms that are domain-general (Stein & Stanford, Reference Stein and Stanford2008). However, further study comparing perceptual contributions to IU and anxiety across different sensory domains would be needed.

Perceptual sensitivity and hypo- vs. hyper-reactivity

Our finding of an association between higher vibrotactile thresholds (i.e., hyposensitivity) and increased parent-reported sensory reactivity is consistent with that of a previous study, in which higher auditory detection thresholds were found to associate with increased self-reported auditory reactivity (Kuiper et al., Reference Kuiper, Verhoeven and Geurts2019). However, our results contrast with work by Sapey-Triomphe et al. (Reference Sapey-Triomphe, Dierckx, Vettori, van Overwalle and Wagemans2023), who reported an association between heightened visual perceptual sensitivity and increased self-reported responsivity (Sapey-Triomphe et al., Reference Sapey-Triomphe, Dierckx, Vettori, van Overwalle and Wagemans2023). Together, those findings suggest that both hypo- and hyper-sensitivities at the perceptual level can relate to more sensory reactivity at the affective level.

In MacLennan et al. (Reference MacLennan, Rossow and Tavassoli2021)’s study, sensory hyper-reactivity, but not hypo-reactivity or sensory seeking, specifically predicted outcomes of IU and anxiety. Conversely, other studies, including one study involving autistic children with a similar age range to those in the current study (Wigham et al., Reference Wigham, Rodgers, South, McConachie and Freeston2015), have highlighted relationships between sensory reactivity, IU and anxiety involving both hypo- and hyper-reactivity (Hwang et al., Reference Hwang, Arnold, Srasuebkul and Trollor2020; Sapey-Triomphe et al., Reference Sapey-Triomphe, Dierckx, Vettori, van Overwalle and Wagemans2023; Wigham et al., Reference Wigham, Rodgers, South, McConachie and Freeston2015). As the SPM does not provide specific sub-scores for hypo- or hyper-reactivity, we could not test for associations between those phenotypes directly. However, in previous work, higher tactile perceptual thresholds were shown to positively associate with both hypo- and hyper-reactivity sub-scores (He et al., Reference He, Wodka, Tommerdahl, Edden, Mikkelsen, Mostofsky and Puts2021b). As individuals can experience both hypo-reactivity and hyper-reactivity to different stimuli across and within sensory domains, we hypothesize that higher SFD thresholds could relate to affective responses to sensory stimuli that manifest as either hypo-reactive or hyper-reactive phenotypes.

IU in the context of sensory differences and anxiety

Our findings of a full mediation effect between sensory reactivity and anxiety through IU, and a partial mediation effect between sensory reactivity and IU through anxiety are like those of MacLennan et al., (Reference MacLennan, Rossow and Tavassoli2021)’s study, who found significant mediating roles for both IU and anxiety with sensory hyper-reactivity as the predictor in preschool-aged children. While the issue of reverse causality cannot be ruled out as our data was cross-sectional, our findings provide support for the notion that sensory-related IU and anxiety could be involved in a feedback loop, both contributing to the development and maintenance of anxiety and IU. Green & BenSasson (Reference Green and Ben-Sasson2010) originally hypothesized in their ‘Primary Sensory Over-responsivity Model’ that individuals could develop anxiety through an increased aversity towards unpredictable or uncontrollable sensory stimuli within the environment (Green & Ben-Sasson, Reference Green and Ben-Sasson2010). As IU could lead to a state of hypervigilance, which could worsen one’s anxiety and ability to regulate affective and behavioral responses to sensory input (i.e., sensory reactivity), this could further reinforce intolerance towards unpredictable sensory stimuli and/or the associated environment.

Perceptual sensitivity and sensory reactivity are related, but separate, constructs

As our questionnaire measure for sensory reactivity was significantly correlated with anxiety and IU, but SFD threshold was not, our findings support the view that low-level perceptual sensitivity and sensory reactivity are related, but separate constructs, with unique contributions to IU and anxiety. These results highlight that a possible reason why some studies may fail to demonstrate direct associations between low-level physiological measures and clinical symptoms is that associations may only exist when they are mediated by an external factor. These findings further highlight the need for measures that differentiate between perceptual and affective/behavioral responses to sensory stimuli. As most clinical measures for assessing sensory processing in children are reliant on teacher-, parent-, and/or self-report, the sensory differences that are captured in the assessments are often restricted to affective or behavioral responses to sensory stimuli (note, where some studies refer to “behavioral” responses as objective measures of perceptual sensitivity, here we use the term “behavioral” to mean a subjective response or reaction to sensory stimuli [e.g., avoidance]). Few questionnaire measures include questions relating to sensory differences at the lower level (i.e., how well an individual perceives stimuli), and if present, do not provide this information as separate scores. Being able to specify the level of sensory processing being measured, whether perceptual, affective, behavioral, or neural, would improve our understanding of the mechanisms involved, and facilitate clearer comparisons between studies (He et al., Reference He, Williams, Harris, Powell, Schaaf, Tavassoli and Puts2023; Schulz & Stevenson, Reference Schulz and Stevenson2022; Ward, Reference Ward2018).

Limitations

Several limitations must be considered when interpreting the findings from our study. First, the sample size in our study was relatively small. It is therefore possible that we did not have enough power to detect certain relationships, e.g., between vibrotactile tasks other than SFD. Moreover, our sample had a larger proportion of male relative to female participants. Consequently, our findings presented here may principally apply to male autistic children. As evidence for sex differences in sensory processing of non-painful stimuli in autism have been observed at the affective/behavioral level (Osório et al., Reference Osório, Rodríguez-Herreros, Richetin, Junod, Romascano, Pittet, Chabane, Gygax and Maillard2021; Tavassoli et al., Reference Tavassoli, Hoekstra and Baron-Cohen2014), but not at the perceptual level (Asaridou et al., Reference Asaridou, Wodka, Edden, Mostofsky, Puts, He and Morgan2022), future studies, using a larger cohort with a higher representation of female participants, would be valuable to replicate the associations observed in this study and evaluate potential sex differences. A larger sample size would also allow the use of more complex modeling techniques, such as path analysis or structural equational modeling, to better define the relationships between SFD threshold, sensory differences, IU, and anxiety by combining them into a single model. However, such techniques require access to large data sets, which can be challenging in clinical populations.

Furthermore, the data that we used in the study was cross-sectional. Therefore, the stepwise associations that we demonstrate must be interpreted with caution. Ideally, mediation analyses should assess variables that are measured at separate timepoints, so that any reverse relationships can be ruled out. While we found non-significant mediation effects for different path combinations outside the paths of interest, future work would benefit from assessing the relationships between perceptual alterations, sensory reactivity, IU, and anxiety using longitudinal or temporally-ordered data.

We note that our study had a high percentage of ASD individuals with co-occurring ADHD (n = 27; 71%). While the prevalence of co-occurring ADHD in autistic individuals varies in the literature, the rate of co-existing ADHD in our sample was generally consistent with rates previously reported (40-80%; Rau et al., Reference Rau, Skapek, Tiplady, Seese, Burns, Armour and Kenworthy2020; Rong et al., Reference Rong, Yang, Jin and Wang2021; Sinzig et al., Reference Sinzig, Walter and Doepfner2004). Evidence for both distinct and overlapping sensory processing patterns has been found in autism and ADHD, though these have not been clearly defined (Bijlenga et al., Reference Bijlenga, Tjon-Ka-Jie, Schuijers and Kooij2017; Dellapiazza et al., Reference Dellapiazza, Michelon, Vernhet, Muratori, Blanc, Picot, Baghdadli, Baghdadli, Chabaux, Chatel, Cohen, Damville, Geoffray, Gicquel, Jardri, Maffre, Novo, Odoyer and Oreve2021; He et al., Reference He, Wodka, Tommerdahl, Edden, Mikkelsen, Mostofsky and Puts2021b; Little et al., Reference Little, Dean, Tomchek and Dunn2018; Scheerer et al., Reference Scheerer, Pourtousi, Yang, Ding, Stojanoski, Anagnostou, Nicolson, Kelley, Georgiades, Crosbie, Schachar, Ayub and Stevenson2022). However, some results from neurophysiological studies suggest that the two conditions may share underlying mechanisms (Itahashi et al., Reference Itahashi, Fujino, Sato, Ohta, Nakamura, Kato, Hashimoto, Martino and Aoki2020). Though studies have previously been limited by the preclusion of a dual diagnosis of autism and ADHD prior to the DSM-5, more recent studies suggest that sensory differences in those with both ASD and ADHD traits are similar, or more severe than in either condition alone (Mattard-Labrecque erg et al., Reference Mattard-Labrecque erg, ben Amor and Couture erg2013; Sanz-Cervera et al., Reference Sanz-Cervera, Pastor-Cerezuela, González-Sala, Tárraga-Mínguez and Fernández-Andrés2017). Thus, there is a critical need to understand the neural mechanisms that contribute to phenotypic sensory differences in autistic individuals with and without co-occurring ADHD. In this exploratory study, we did not have the statistical power to run separate mediation analyses for those with and without a co-occurring ADHD diagnosis, however as all mediation effects remained significant when Conners and DuPauls scores were added as covariates, the results suggest that the outcomes are unlikely to be an effect of ADHD-related traits. In addition, except for higher SPM scores in the ASD + ADHD group, there were no statistical differences in SFD thresholds, anxiety, or IU between the two groups. Thus, we believe our findings are clinically relevant to autistic individuals with and without co-occurring ADHD. Nevertheless, studies evaluating how atypical low-level sensory perception contributes to clinical symptoms of anxiety and IU in individuals with ASD, ADHD, and co-occurring ASD and ADHD, would be useful for understanding how underlying neurophysiological mechanisms might differ between them.

A final limitation is that we used parent-report measures from the SCARED assessment and not child reports. While child-reported SCARED scores were significantly correlated with child-reported IUSC scores (Supplementary Results), no significant correlations were observed between child-reported SCARED and SPM scores. As the SPM is an assessment that is completed by the parent or caregiver, it is possible that no significant correlations were found between the SPM score and child-reported SCARED score owing to child-parent report inconsistencies, a phenomenon that is often observed in psychology studies (De Los Reyes et al., Reference De Los Reyes, Augenstein, Wang, Thomas, Drabick, Burgers and Rabinowitz2015). Using a combination of both parent and child reports where appropriate, and reports within different contexts, are likely to be the most informative.

Conclusion

In conclusion, our study showed that parent-reported sensory reactivity fully mediated an association between tactile perceptual alterations and parent-reported anxiety, and between tactile perceptual alterations and child-reported IU, in autistic children with and without co-occurring ADHD. Our findings add to current literature by suggesting that bottom-up perceptual alterations in relation to frequency discrimination of vibrotactile stimuli, contribute to sensory reactivity at the affective level, which could in turn lead to increased anxiety and IU. Understanding how differences in perceptual, affective, and behavioral levels of the sensory processing pathway, as well as differences in specific subdomains, contribute to behavioral challenges that commonly co-occur in autism, such as anxiety and IU, could help to guide more specific, individualized interventions.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423001360.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge funding support from NIH/NIMH R21MH098228, R01MH106564, R01MH078160, and R00MH107719. J.L. H., E.L.W., and N.A.J.P. received salary support from the Nancy Lurie Marks Family Foundation as part of Autism Sensory Research Consortium. S.H.M. receives royalties for US Patent 10,410,041. Studies from which these data were collected received ethical approval from the Kennedy Krieger Institute and the John Hopkins School of Medicine Institutional Review Boards. We would like to thank all the participants and their families for their participation in our research. We would also like to thank all the research coordinators and assistants who helped with acquiring the data.

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

1 While we acknowledge that individual preferences are preferred in practice, we use identity-first language here following recent recommendations (Bottema-Beutel et al., 2021)

References

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890423349 Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.893619 Google Scholar
Amos, G. A., Byrne, G., Chouinard, P. A., & Godber, T. (2019). Autism traits, sensory over-responsivity, anxiety, and stress: A test of explanatory models. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(1), 98112. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3695-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asaridou, M., Wodka, E. L., Edden, R. A. E., Mostofsky, S. H., Puts, N. A. J., He, J. L., & Morgan, R. H. (2022). Could sensory differences be a sex-indifferent biomarker of autism? Early investigation comparing tactile sensitivity between autistic males and females. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-022-05787-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baranek, G. T., David, F. J., Poe, M. D., Stone, W. L., & Watson, L. R. (2006). Sensory experiences questionnaire: Discriminating sensory features in young children with autism, developmental delays, and typical development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 47(6), 591601. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01546.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bijlenga, D., Tjon-Ka-Jie, J. Y. M., Schuijers, F., & Kooij, J. J. S. (2017). Atypical sensory profiles as core features of adult ADHD, irrespective of autistic symptoms. European Psychiatry, 43, 5157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.02.481 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birmaher, B., Khetarpal, S., Brent, D., Cully, M., Balach, L., Kaufman, J., & Neer, S. M. K. (1997). The screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders (SCARED): Scale construction and psychometric characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(4), 545553. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199704000-00018 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonnel, A., McAdams, S., Smith, B., Berthiaume, C., Bertone, A., Ciocca, V., Burack, J. A., & Mottron, L. (2010). Enhanced pure-tone pitch discrimination among persons with autism but not asperger syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 48(9), 24652475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.020 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boulter, C., & Freeston, M. (2014). Intolerance of uncertainty as a framework for understanding anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(6), 13911402. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-2001-x 1391-1402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, T., Morrison, I. C., & Stagnitti, K. (2010). The convergent validity of two sensory processing scales used with school-age children: Comparing the Sensory profile and the sensory processing measure. New Zealand Journal of Occupational Therapy, 57(2), 56–65. http://hdl.handle.net/10536/DRO/DU:30032049 Google Scholar
Bryant, L. K., Woynaroski, T. G., Wallace, M. T., & Cascio, C. J. (2019). Self-reported sensory hypersensitivity moderates association between tactile psychophysical performance and autism-related traits in neurotypical adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(8), 31593172. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04043-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buhr, K., & Dugas, M. J. (2002). The intolerance of uncertainty scale: psychometric properties of the English version. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 931945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butera, C., Ring, P., Sideris, J., Jayashankar, A., Kilroy, E., Harrison, L., Cermak, S., & Aziz-Zadeh, L. (2020). Impact of sensory processing on school performance outcomes in high functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Mind, Brain, and Education, 14(3), 243254. https://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12242 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cardin, J. A., Carlén, M., Meletis, K., Knoblich, U., Zhang, F., Deisseroth, K., Tsai, L. H., & Moore, C. I. (2009). Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature, 459(7247), 663667. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carleton, R. N., Fetzner, M. G., Hackl, J. L., & McEvoy, P. (2013). Intolerance of uncertainty as a contributor to fear and avoidance symptoms of panic attacks. Cognitive behaviour therapy, 42(4), 328341. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2013.792100 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, K. L. H., Baranek, G. T., Copeland, W. E., Compton, S., Zucker, N., Dawson, G., & Egger, H. L. (2019). Sensory over-responsivity: An early risk factor for anxiety and behavioral challenges in young children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(6), 10751088. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0502-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, S., Kent, R., Hollocks, M. J., & Simonoff, E. (2020). Brief report: Testing the psychometric properties of the spence children’s anxiety scale (SCAS) and the screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders (SCARED) in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(7), 26252632. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3774-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casanova, M. F., Buxhoeveden, D., & Gomez, J. (2003). Disruption in the inhibitory architecture of the cell minicolumn: Implications for autism. Neuroscientist, 9(6), 496507. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858403253552 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cascio, C. J., Lorenzi, J., & Baranek, G. T. (2016). Self-reported pleasantness ratings and examiner-coded defensiveness in response to touch in children with ASD: Effects of stimulus material and bodily location. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(5), 15281537. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1961-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, M. H., Wei, H. T., Chen, L. C., Su, T. P., Bai, Y. M., Hsu, J. W., Huang, K. L., Chang, W. H., Chen, T. J., & Chen, Y. S. (2015). Autistic spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and psychiatric comorbidities: A nationwide study. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 10, 16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.10.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comer, J. S., Roy, A. K., Furr, J. M., Gotimer, K., Beidas, R. S., Dugas, M. J., & Kendall, P. C. (2009). The intolerance of uncertainty scale for children: A psychometric evaluation. Psychological Assessment, 21(3), 402411. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016719 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conners, C. K. (2008). Conners 3rd edition: Manual, Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Conners, C. K., Sitarenios, G., Parker, J. D., & Epstein, J. N. (1998a). Revision and restandardization of the conners teacher rating scale (CTRS-R): Factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26(4), 279291. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1022606501530 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conners, C. K., Sitarenios, G., Parker, J. D., & Epstein, J. N. (1998b). The revised conners’ parent rating scale (CPRS-R): Factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26(4), 257268. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1022602400621 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crane, L., Goddard, L., & Pring, L. (2009). Sensory processing in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 13(3), 215228. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361309103794 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, M. (1992). The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 15, 353–375. www.annualreviews.org.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, T. E., Hess, J. A., Moree, B. N., Fodstad, ` J. C., Dempsey, T., Jenkins, W. S., & Matson, J. L. (2011). Anxiety symptoms across the lifespan in people diagnosed with autistic disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 112118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.02.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Los Reyes, A., Augenstein, T. M., Wang, M., Thomas, S. A., Drabick, D. A. G., Burgers, D. E., & Rabinowitz, J. (2015). The validity of the multi-informant approach to assessing child and adolescent mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 141(4), 858900.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dellapiazza, F., Michelon, C., Vernhet, C., Muratori, F., Blanc, N., Picot, M. C., Baghdadli, A., Baghdadli, A., Chabaux, C., Chatel, C., Cohen, D., Damville, E., Geoffray, M. M., Gicquel, L., Jardri, R., Maffre, T., Novo, A., Odoyer, R., Oreve, M. J.et al. (2021). Sensory processing related to attention in children with ASD, ADHD, or typical development: Results from the ELENA cohort. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30(2), 283291. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-020-01516-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dellapiazza, F., Vernhet, C., Blanc, N., Miot, S., Schmidt, R., & Baghdadi, A. (2018). Links between sensory processing, adaptive behaviours, and attention in children with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review. Psychiatry Research, 270, 7888. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.09.023 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dugas, M. J., Freeston, M. H., & Ladouceur, R. (1997). Intolerance of uncertainty and problem orientation in worry 1. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26(6), 593606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dwyer, P., Takarae, Y., Zadeh, I., Rivera, S. M., & Saron, C. D. (2022). A multidimensional investigation of sensory processing in autism: Parent- and self-report questionnaires, psychophysical thresholds, and event-related potentials in the auditory and somatosensory modalities. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 16, 811547. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.811547 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foss-Feig, J. H., Heacock, J. L., & Cascio, C. J. (2012). Tactile responsiveness patterns and their association with core features in autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(1), 337344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.06.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glod, M., Riby, D. M., & Rodgers, J. (2019). Short report: Relationships between sensory processing, repetitive behaviors, anxiety, and intolerance of uncertainty in autism spectrum disorder and Williams syndrome. Autism Research, 12(5), 759765. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2096 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, S. A., & Ben-Sasson, A. (2010). Anxiety disorders and sensory over-responsivity in children with autism spectrum disorders: Is there a Causal relationship? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(12), 14951504. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1007-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, S. A., Ben-Sasson, A., Soto, T. W., & Carter, A. S. (2012). Anxiety and sensory over-responsivity in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: Bidirectional effects across time. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 11121119. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1361-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. A., Arabzadeh, E., Fairhall, A. L., Benito, C., & Diamond, M. E. (2006). Factors affecting frequency discrimination of vibrotactile stimuli: Implications for cortical encoding. PLoS ONE, 1(1), 100. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000100 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, J. L., Oeltzschner, G., Mikkelsen, M., Deronda, A., Harris, A. D., Crocetti, D., Wodka, E. L., Mostofsky, S. H., Edden, R. A. E., & Puts, N. A. J. (2021a). Region-specific elevations of glutamate + glutamine correlate with the sensory symptoms of autism spectrum disorders. Translational Psychiatry, 11(1), 411. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-021-01525-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, J. L., Williams, Z. J., Harris, A., Powell, H., Schaaf, R., Tavassoli, T., & Puts, N. A. J. (2023). A working taxonomy for describing the sensory differences of autism. Molecular Autism, 14(1), 15. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-022-00534-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, J. L., Wodka, E., Tommerdahl, M., Edden, R. A. E., Mikkelsen, M., Mostofsky, S. H., & Puts, N. A. J. (2021b). Disorder-specific alterations of tactile sensitivity in neurodevelopmental disorders. Communications Biology, 4(1), 97. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-020-01592-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heaton, P., Hudry, K., Ludlow, A., & Hill, E. (2008). Superior discrimination of speech pitch and its relationship to verbal ability in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25(6), 771782. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290802336277 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodgson, A. R., Freeston, M. H., Honey, E., & Rodgers, J. (2017). Facing the unknown: Intolerance of uncertainty in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 30(2), 336344. https://doi.org/10.1111/jar.12245 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howe, F. E. J., & Stagg, S. D. (2016). How sensory experiences affect adolescents with an autistic spectrum condition within the classroom. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(6), 2273–1668. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2693-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hwang, Y. I., Arnold, S., Srasuebkul, P., & Trollor, J. (2020). Understanding anxiety in adults on the autism spectrum: An investigation of its relationship with intolerance of uncertainty, sensory sensitivities and repetitive behaviours. Autism, 24(2), 411422. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361319868907 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ide, M., Yaguchi, A., Sano, M., Fukatsu, R., & Wada, M. (2019). Higher tactile temporal resolution as a basis of hypersensitivity in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(1), 4453. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3677-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Itahashi, T., Fujino, J., Sato, T., Ohta, H., Nakamura, M., Kato, N., Hashimoto, R.-I., Martino, A.di, & Aoki, Y. Y. (2020). Neural correlates of shared sensory symptoms in autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain Communications, 2(2), fcaa186, https://doi.org/10.1093/braincomms/fcaa186 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenkinson, R., Milne, E., & Thompson, A. (2020). The relationship between intolerance of uncertainty and anxiety in autism: A systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Autism, 24(8), 19331944. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361320932437 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joshi, G., Wozniak, J., Petty, C., Martelon, M. K., Fried, R., Bolfek, A., Kotte, A., Stevens, J., Furtak, S. L., Bourgeois, M., Caruso, J., Caron, A., & Biederman, J. (2012). Psychiatric comorbidity and functioning in a clinically referred population of adults with autism spectrum disorders: A comparative study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(6), 13141325. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1679-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joyce, C., Honey, E., Leekam, S. R., Barrett, S. L., & Rodgers, J. (2017). Anxiety, intolerance of uncertainty and restricted and repetitive behaviour: Insights directly from young people with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(12), 37893802. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3027-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., Williamson, D., & Ryan, N. (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), 980988. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keefer, A., Kreiser, N. L., Singh, V., Blakeley-Smith, A., Duncan, A., Johnson, C., Klinger, L., Meyer, A., Reaven, J., & Vasa, R. A. (2017). Intolerance of uncertainty predicts anxiety outcomes following CBT in youth with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(12), 39493958. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2852-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keefer, A., Kreiser, N. L., Singh, V., Blakeley-Smith Judy Reaven, A., & Vasa, R. A. (2018). Exploring relationships between negative cognitions and anxiety symptoms in youth with autism spectrum disorder. Behavior Therapy, 49(5), 730740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kojovic, N., Hadid, L.ben, Franchini, M., & Schaer, M. (2019). Sensory processing issues and their association with social difficulties in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 8(10), 1508. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm8101508 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuiper, M. W. M., Verhoeven, E. W. M., & Geurts, H. M. (2019). Stop making noise! auditory sensitivity in adults with an autism spectrum disorder diagnosis: Physiological habituation and subjective detection thresholds. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(5), 21162128. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-03890-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lai, M. C., Kassee, C., Besney, R., Bonato, S., Hull, L., Mandy, W., Szatmari, P., & Ameis, S. H. (2019). Prevalence of co-occurring mental health diagnoses in the autism population: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 6(10), 819829. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30289-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lane, A. E., Young, R. L., Baker, A. E. Z., & Angley, M. T. (2010). Sensory processing subtypes in autism: Association with adaptive behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(1), 112122. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0840-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lane, S. J., Reynolds, S., & Dumenci, L. (2010a). Sensory overresponsivity and anxiety in typically developing children and children with autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Cause or coexistence ? The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 66(5), 595603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lane, S. J., Reynolds, S., & Thacker, L. (2010b). Sensory over-responsivity and ADHD: Differentiating using electrodermal responses, cortisol, and anxiety. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 4, 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2010.00008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leekam, S. R., Nieto, C., Libby, S. J., Wing, L., & Gould, J. (2007). Describing the sensory abnormalities of children and adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(5), 894910. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0218-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leyfer, O. T., Folstein, S. E., Bacalman, S., Davis, N. O., Dinh, E., Morgan, J., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Lainhart, J. E. (2006). Comorbid psychiatric disorders in children with autism: Interview development and rates of disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(7), 849861. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0123-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, L. M., Dean, E., Tomchek, S., & Dunn, W. (2018). Sensory processing patterns in autism, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and typical development. Physical and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 38(3), 243254. https://doi.org/10.1080/01942638.2017.1390809 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H. Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Gotham, K., Bishop, S., & Risi, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule, second edition (ADOS-2) manual (Part I): Modules 1-4. Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Lord, C., Rutter, M., le Couteur, A., & Free Hospital, R. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02172145 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLennan, K., Rossow, T., & Tavassoli, T. (2021). The relationship between sensory reactivity, intolerance of uncertainty and anxiety subtypes in preschool-age autistic children. Autism, 25(8), 23052316. https://doi.org/10.1177/13623613211016110 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahoney, A. E. J., & McEvoy, P. M. (2012). A transdiagnostic examination of intolerance of uncertainty across anxiety and depressive disorders. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 41(3), 212222. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2011.622130 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattard-Labrecque erg, C., ben Amor, L., & Couture erg, M. M. (2013). Children with autism and attention difficulties: A pilot study of the association between sensory, motor, and adaptive behaviors. J Can Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 22(2), 139–146.Google Scholar
Mazurek, M. O., Vasa, R. A., Kalb, L. G., Kanne, S. M., Rosenberg, D., Keefer, A., Murray, D. S., Freedman, B., & Ann Lowery, L. (2012). Anxiety, sensory over-responsivity, and gastrointestinal problems in children with autism spectrum disorders. Abnorm Child Psychol, 41(1), 165176. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9668-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McVey, A. J., Schiltz, H. K., Haendel, A. D., Dolan, B. K., Willar, K. S., Pleiss, S. S., Karst, J. S., Carlson, M., Krueger, W., Murphy, C. C., Casnar, C. L., Yund, B., & van Hecke, A. V. (2018). Social difficulties in youth with autism with and without anxiety and ADHD symptoms. Autism Research, 11(12), 16791689. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2039 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neil, L., Olsson, N. C., & Pellicano, E. (2016). The relationship between intolerance of uncertainty, sensory sensitivities, and anxiety in autistic and typically developing children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(6), 19621973. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2721-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osório, J. M. A., Rodríguez-Herreros, B., Richetin, S., Junod, V., Romascano, D., Pittet, V., Chabane, N., Gygax, M. J., & Maillard, A. M. (2021). Sex differences in sensory processing in children with autism spectrum disorder. Austim Research, 14(11), 24122423. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2580 Google Scholar
Palmer, L. D., Ecker, C., Miller Kuhaneck, H., Henry, D. A., & Glennon, T. J. (2007). Sensory processing measure (SPM): Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Pellicano, E., & Burr, D. (2012). When the world becomes too real: A Bayesian explanation of autistic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(10), 504510. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puts, N. A. J., Edden, R. A. E., John Evans, C., McGlone, F., & McGonigle, D. J. (2011). Regionally specific human GABA concentration correlates with tactile discrimination thresholds. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(46), 1655616560. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4489-11.2011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puts, N. A. J., Edden, R. A. E., Wodka, E. L., Mostofsky, S. H., & Tommerdahl, M. (2013). A vibrotactile behavioral battery for investigating somatosensory processing in children and adults. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 218(1), 3947. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.04.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puts, N. A. J., Wodka, E. L., Harris, A. D., Crocetti, D., Tommerdahl, M., Mostofsky, S. H., & Edden, R. A. E. (2017). Reduced GABA and altered somatosensory function in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 10(4), 608619. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1691 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puts, N. A. J., Wodka, E. L., Tommerdahl, M., Mostofsky, S. H., & Edden, R. A. E. (2014). Impaired tactile processing in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurophysiology, 111(9), 18031811. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00890.2013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rau, S., Skapek, M. F., Tiplady, K., Seese, S., Burns, A., Armour, A. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2020). Identifying comorbid ADHD in autism: Attending to the inattentive presentation. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 69, 101468. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2019.101468 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Read, J. C. A. (2015). The place of human psychophysics in modern neuroscience. Neuroscience, 296, 116129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.05.036 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reich, W. (2000). Diagnostic interview for children and adolescents (DICA). Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(1), 5966. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200001000-00017 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodgers, J., & Ofield, A. (2018). Understanding, recognising and treating co-occurring anxiety in autism. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 5(1), 5864. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40474-018-0132-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S., & Wehner, E. (2003). Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(6), 631642. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JADD.0000006000.38991.a7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rong, Y., Yang, C. J., Jin, Y., & Wang, Y. (2021). Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 83, 101759. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2021.101759 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubenstein, J. L. R., & Merzenich, M. M. (2003). Model of autism: Increased ratio of excitation/ inhibition in key neural systems. Genes, Brain and Behaviour, 2, 255267. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1601-183X.2003.00037.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanz-Cervera, P., Pastor-Cerezuela, G., González-Sala, F., Tárraga-Mínguez, R., & Fernández-Andrés, M.-I. (2017). Sensory processing in children with autism spectrum disorder and/or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in the home and classroom contexts. Frontiers in Psychology | Www. Frontiersin.Org, 8, 1772. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01772 Google Scholar
Sapey-Triomphe, L. A., Dierckx, J., Vettori, S., van Overwalle, J., & Wagemans, J. (2023). A multilevel investigation of sensory sensitivity and responsivity in autistic adults. Autism Research, 16(7), 12991320. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2962 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheerer, N. E., Pourtousi, A., Yang, C., Ding, Z., Stojanoski, B., Anagnostou, E., Nicolson, R., Kelley, E., Georgiades, S., Crosbie, J., Schachar, R., Ayub, M., & Stevenson, R. A. (2022). Transdiagnostic patterns of sensory processing in autism and ADHD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-022-05798-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schulz, S. E., & Stevenson, R. A. (2020). Differentiating between sensory sensitivity and sensory reactivity in relation to restricted interests and repetitive behaviours. Autism, 24(1), 121134. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361319850402 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schulz, S. E., & Stevenson, R. A. (2022). Convergent validity of behavioural and subjective sensitivity in relation to autistic traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 52(2), 758770. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-04974-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., & Baird, G. (2008). Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: Prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(8), 921929. https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinzig, J., Walter, D., & Doepfner, M. (2004). Attention deficit/Hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder symptom or syndrome? Journal of Attention Disorders, 13(2), 117126. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054708326261 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
South, M., & Rodgers, J. (2017). Sensory, emotional and cognitive contributions to anxiety in autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 20. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00020 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, B. E., & Stanford, T. R. (2008). Multisensory integration: Current issues from the perspective of the single neuron. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(4), 255266. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2331 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, J. A., Gadgil, M. S., Blakeley-Smith, A., Reaven, J. A., & Hepburn, S. L. (2014). Psychometric properties of the SCARED in youth with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8(9), 12251234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.06.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tavassoli, T., Hoekstra, R. A., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2014). The sensory perception quotient (SPQ): Development and validation of a new sensory questionnaire for adults with and without autism. Molecular Autism, 5(1), 29. https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-5-29 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tavassoli, T., Latham, K., Bach, M., Dakin, S. C., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). Psychophysical measures of visual acuity in autism spectrum conditions. Vision Research, 51(15), 17781780. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.06.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tingley, D., Yamamoto, T., Hirose, K., Keele, L., & Imai, K. (2014). Mediation: R package for Causal mediation analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 59(5), 138. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v059.i05 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van de Cruys, S., Evers, K., van der Hallen, R., van Eylen, L., Boets, B., de-Wit, L., & Wagemans, J. (2014). Precise minds in uncertain worlds: Predictive coding in autism. Psychological Review, 121(4), 649675. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037665 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vasa, R. A., Keefer, A., McDonald, R. G., Hunsche, M. C., & Kerns, C. M. (2020). A scoping review of anxiety in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 13(12), 20382057. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2395 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vasa, R. A., Kreiser, N. L., Keefer, A., Singh, V., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2018). Relationships between autism spectrum disorder and intolerance of uncertainty. Autism Research, 11(4), 636644. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1916 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, J. (2018). Individual differences in sensory sensitivity: A synthesizing framework and evidence from normal variation and developmental conditions. Cognitive Neuroscience, 8928(3), 139157. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2018.1557131 Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for children, fourth edition (WISC-IV); San Antonia, TX: PsychCorp.Google Scholar
Wigham, S., Rodgers, J., South, M., McConachie, H., & Freeston, M. (2015). The interplay between sensory processing Abnormalities, intolerance of uncertainty, anxiety and restricted and repetitive behaviours in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(4), 943952. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2248-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zu, J., & Yuan, K. H. (2010). Local influence and robust procedures for mediation analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 45(1), 144. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273170903504695 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of participant demographics and study variables

Figure 1

Figure 1. a. Illustration of a cortical metrics braingauge vibrotactile stimulator. Children were asked to place their left hand on the stimulator and their right hand on a computer mouse. Two 5 mm cylindrical probes delivered sinusoidal pulses to the index and middle fingers. Participants responded with their right index and middle finger. Stimuli amplitude and frequency were between 0–350 μm and 0–50 Hz, respectively. b. Visual schematic of the simultaneous frequency discrimination task. Using a two-alternative forced choice design, both fingers are stimulated, one of which receives a standard stimulus of constant frequency (30Hz), and the other receives a comparison stimulus where the frequency can vary (initial frequency was 40 Hz). Children were asked to indicate on which finger they felt the higher stimulus. Following a staircase approach, the comparison stimulus decreased in frequency after a correct response and increased in frequency after an incorrect response. Details of the other vibrotactile tasks can be found in the supplementary methods.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Pearson’s correlations between sensory reactivity, IU, anxiety and SFD threshold. a-c. There were moderate positive associations between all three measures of sensory reactivity, IU, and anxiety. d-f. There was a moderate positive association between SFD threshold and sensory reactivity. No significant associations were found between SFD threshold and anxiety or IU. IU = intolerance of uncertainty; IUSC = intolerance of uncertainty scale for children; SCARED = screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders; SFD = simultaneous frequency discrimination; SPM TOT = sensory processing measure total sensory systems.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Visual schematic of the mediation path models between sensory reactivity (SPM total sensory systems T-scores), intolerance of uncertainty (IU; IUSC total scores), and anxiety (SCARED total scores). Dashed arrows indicate the indirect effect between the predictor and outcome that is mediated by the mediator. Solid arrows between the predictor and outcome indicate the total effect, with the direct effect in parentheses. a. Full mediation effect of IU on the relationship between sensory reactivity and anxiety. b. Partial mediation effect of anxiety on the relationship between sensory reactivity and IU. IUSC = intolerance of uncertainty scale for children; SPM = sensory processing measure; SCARED = screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders. **p ≤ .01; *p ≤ .05.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Visual schematic of the mediation path models between SFD threshold, sensory reactivity (SPM total sensory systems T-scores), intolerance of uncertainty (IU; IUSC total scores), and anxiety (SCARED total scores). Dashed arrows indicate the indirect effect between the predictor and outcome that is mediated by the mediator. Solid arrows between the predictor and outcome indicate the total effect, with the direct effect in parentheses. a. Full mediation effect of sensory reactivity on the relationship between SFD threshold and anxiety. b. Full mediation effect of sensory reactivity on the relationship between SFD threshold and IU. IUSC = intolerance of uncertainty scale for children; SPM = sensory processing measure; SCARED = screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders; SFD = simultaneous frequency discrimination. **p ≤ .01; *p ≤ .05.

Supplementary material: File

Powell et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 641.5 KB