Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T23:28:58.324Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Age, Alzheimer's disease, and the big picture

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 September 2011

Mary Ganguli
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh, School of Medicine and Graduate School of Public Health, Pittsburgh, USA Email: gangulim@upmc.edu
Eric Rodriguez
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh, School of Medicine and Graduate School of Public Health, Pittsburgh, USA Email: gangulim@upmc.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

The recently published revised National Institute on Aging/Alzheimer's Association clinical diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer's disease (AD) (Albert et al., 2011; Jack et al., 2011; McKhann et al., 2011; Sperling et al., 2011) have been hailed for incorporating a number of timely and important advances. They reflect new understanding that has been gained since the previous criteria were published in 1984 (McKhann et al., 1984). They include recognition of the state of mild cognitive impairment that is present before the threshold is crossed into dementia; they recognize the potential role of biomarkers in enhancing the specificity of diagnosis; they also address emerging work in the preclinical stage of AD that could help in understanding the sequence and stages of the core pathology before symptoms emerge. Among the previously listed diagnostic features that have disappeared was the requirement that onset of dementia occur before the age of 90 years. Meanwhile, the Neurocognitive Disorders Work Group for DSM-5 (the 5th edition of the American Psychiatric Association's Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; American Psychiatric Association, 2010) is also doing away with the previous distinction between early-onset and late-onset dementia in AD, where an arbitrary division had been placed at age 65 (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). These changes are driven by the lack of biological data to support the age-based dichotomy, while recognizing the unique genetic characteristics of the relatively rare, autosomal dominantly inherited forms of AD which typically occur early. However, the disappearance of the age-based diagnostic dichotomy by no means implies that age is irrelevant to AD.

Type
Guest Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2011

The recently published revised National Institute on Aging/Alzheimer's Association clinical diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer's disease (AD) (Albert et al., Reference Albert2011; Jack et al., Reference Jack2011; McKhann et al., Reference McKhann2011; Sperling et al., Reference Sperling2011) have been hailed for incorporating a number of timely and important advances. They reflect new understanding that has been gained since the previous criteria were published in 1984 (McKhann et al., Reference McKhann1984). They include recognition of the state of mild cognitive impairment that is present before the threshold is crossed into dementia; they recognize the potential role of biomarkers in enhancing the specificity of diagnosis; they also address emerging work in the preclinical stage of AD that could help in understanding the sequence and stages of the core pathology before symptoms emerge. Among the previously listed diagnostic features that have disappeared was the requirement that onset of dementia occur before the age of 90 years. Meanwhile, the Neurocognitive Disorders Work Group for DSM-5 (the 5th edition of the American Psychiatric Association's Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; American Psychiatric Association, 2010) is also doing away with the previous distinction between early-onset and late-onset dementia in AD, where an arbitrary division had been placed at age 65 (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). These changes are driven by the lack of biological data to support the age-based dichotomy, while recognizing the unique genetic characteristics of the relatively rare, autosomal dominantly inherited forms of AD which typically occur early. However, the disappearance of the age-based diagnostic dichotomy by no means implies that age is irrelevant to AD.

Epidemiology examines the distribution of disease at the population level, and the epidemiologist's role is therefore to step back and look at the big picture. Population studies find that the incidence of AD increases exponentially with age and does not stop at age 90, although few studies have included enough individuals over that age to determine whether the incidence rate levels off or continues to rise (Jorm and Jolley, Reference Jorm and Jolley1998; Gao et al., Reference Gao, Hendrie, Hall and Hui1998). As life expectancy rises the world over (Kinsella and Wan, Reference Kinsella and Wan2009), the largest and fastest-growing proportion of people with clinical AD are in the oldest age group however that group is defined (Ferri et al., Reference Ferri2005; Alzheimer's Disease International, 2010)

Epidemiologists also seek to identify the factors which drive the observed distribution of disease, i.e. those that appear to increase or reduce the probability (“risk”) of developing disease. Since aging is associated with increased incidence of AD, it behooves us to explore age-related differences in risk factors for AD across the spectrum from early-onset to “late-late” onset AD. As population-based cohorts are observed over many years, a curious pattern emerging from longitudinal studies might support the following model.

Let us conceptualize clinical AD as falling into not two but three groups based on age at onset: the young group, with symptom onset roughly between ages 40 and 60, an intermediate group with onset in what might be termed early old age (say, 60 to 85 years), and a late old age group with clinical onset after age 85.

In the young-onset group, positive family history and identified autosomal dominant genes are the best-known primary risk factors (Kamboh, Reference Kamboh2004). Occasional, apparently sporadic cases of AD in younger persons may be found on further investigation to represent previously unrecognized genetic mutations (Bartram et al., Reference Bartram, Lill and Tanzi2010). Exposures such as head trauma may hasten the onset of symptoms (van den Heuvel et al., Reference van den Heuvel, Thornton and Vink2007) but, for the most part, early-onset AD might be considered AD in pure culture; other comorbid diseases are rarely present to the extent that they confound the clinical picture.

In the intermediate-onset group, a number of risk factors have been identified: the APOE*4 genotype (Corder et al., Reference Corder1993), cardiovascular and cerebrovascular disease, high blood pressure, diabetes mellitus, higher cholesterol and body mass index, typically observed during midlife (Craft, Reference Craft2009; Hughes and Ganguli, Reference Hughes and Ganguli2009). Vascular comorbidity is the norm rather than the exception in this intermediate group (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Arvanitakis, Bang and Bennett2007), a fact which has bedeviled development of diagnostic criteria for cognitive impairment and dementia of vascular origin (O'Brien, Reference O'Brien2006). It seems likely that the presence of vascular disease promotes the clinical expression of dementia of AD type (Dodge et al., Reference Dodge, Chang, Kamboh and Ganguli2011).

In the oldest-onset group, however, it appears that no risk factor other than increasing age is associated with developing AD (Kuller and Lopez, 2009; Reference Kuller and Lopez2011). Vascular disease is frequently present along with AD but some degree of vascular disease is almost ubiquitous in the ninth and tenth decades of life (Price et al., Reference Price1997; de Leeuw et al., Reference de Leeuw2001; Vermeer et al., Reference Vermeer, Koudstaal, Oudkerk, Hofman and Breteler2002). Further, diffuse amyloid plaques are present in the brains of many older adults who do not have other pathologic or clinical evidence of AD (Aizenstein et al., Reference Aizenstein2008; Price et al., Reference Price2009). The APOE*4 genotype, already neither a necessary nor sufficient cause at earlier ages, appears to have exhausted its predictive power impact by age 80 or so. The contribution of other postulated genetic factors to population-attributable risk, even in the larger late-onset AD population, seems small (Naj et al., Reference Naj2011).

We now possess the means to plot AD progression, including preclinical disease, along several axes (e.g. clinical stages, CSF biomarker levels, structural and functional brain changes as evident on neuroimaging) (Albert et al., Reference Albert2011; Jack et al., Reference Jack2011; McKhann et al., Reference McKhann2011; Sperling et al., Reference Sperling2011). We propose that aging itself constitutes another dimension for describing or predicting expected features of the disease, in effect distinguishing subtypes of the disease, by their pathogenesis, pathology, and pathophysiology. Thus, age, or a variety of neurologic changes for which age serves as surrogate and sole identified cause, is associated with variant forms (subspecies or perhaps distinct “species”) of what investigators often treat as a unitary disease.

So, for example, the role of deterministic genes is greatest at youngest ages and decreases with age. Brain reserve and cognitive reserve (Katzman, Reference Katzman1993; Stern, Reference Stern2002), by buffering brain function against progressive pathologic change, may play a role in determining age at “onset,” i.e. the age at which disease becomes clinically manifest. With increasing age, comorbid disease and aging-related changes such as decreased synaptic density (Masliah et al., Reference Masliah, Crews and Hansen2006) and reduced synaptic plasticity (Lister and Barnes, Reference Lister and Barnes2009) play greater roles in promoting both Alzheimer-type and non-specific pathology. Late-onset dementia, then, would be dementia whose onset has been delayed by greater reserve, the absence of strong genetic determinants, and favorable risk factor profiles. Clinical dementia emerges when protective factors have been depleted, and the burden of disease and age-related brain pathology has become heavy, with a more diverse and less AD-specific neuropathology than seen in younger persons (Stricker et al., Reference Stricker2011). In fact, growing evidence documents an age-associated weakening of the association between Alzheimer's pathology and clinical dementia: while typical Alzheimer's pathology becomes more prevalent with age in persons without dementia, members of the “old-old” set with dementia have burdens of typical pathology similar to that of their normal counterparts (Savva et al., Reference Savva, Wharton, Ince, Forster, Matthews and Brayne2009).

The current situation has recently been summed up as supporting “three major hypotheses related to dementia: amyloid deposition and secondary synaptic loss, as a unique disease; vascular injury; and ‘aging’” (Kuller and Lopez, Reference Kuller and Lopez2011). But how do we prevent “aging”? And what might be the implications for potential prevention and treatment strategies for AD?

In the young-onset group, those at genetic risk can be identified for prevention or early intervention. Unless the appropriate gene therapy can be devised, the common therapeutic goal would be to strike early at the key disease-promoting pathway – for example, interfering with the production and aggregation of insoluble beta amyloid protein (Rafii and Aisen, Reference Rafii and Aisen2009; Golde et al., 2009) and possibly tau phosphorylation (Schneider and Mandelkow, Reference Schneider and Mandelkow2008) in the brain.

In the intermediate-onset group, the goals might include similar efforts at prevention as in young onset patients. But, especially when preclinical disease is suspected based on the detection of amyloid biomarkers, therapies may also be designed to undo already existing disease, for example, by promoting clearance and excretion of already deposited amyloid (Rafii and Aisen; Reference Rafii and Aisen2009; Mawuenyega et al., Reference Mawuenyega2010) and tau (Schneider and Mandelkow, Reference Schneider and Mandelkow2008) proteins. In this group there might also be benefits to controlling inflammation and vascular risk (Craft, Reference Craft2009), so that even if insoluble amyloid and hyperphosphorylated tau proteins have begun to accumulate, clinical expression of the dementia can be delayed or prevented.

The oldest-onset group presents a different and more formidable challenge, because their only risk factor is age, or, stated otherwise, they have yet to demonstrate a “preventable determinant” (Kuller and Lopez, 2009). In this group, the much-sought agents which interrupt the amyloid pathway may slow Alzheimer-type pathogenesis without having a proportionate impact on incident dementia. Given the complex etiology of dementia in the very old, extrapolating from “cleaner” pathophysiologic models derived from the study of younger patients may not yield insights likely to produce effective therapies for the very old.

The final, somewhat ironic, twist in the plot is that the youngest group is very small while the oldest group is the largest and fastest growing (Kinsella and Wan, Reference Kinsella and Wan2009). Thus, in terms of the potential market for pharmaceutical treatment of AD, the largest group of customers may be the one in which treatment targeting amyloid pathways may prove the least likely to be effective. Meanwhile, the early-onset population, in which disease-specific, even pathway-specific, interventions seem the most promising, is so small that true anti-AD agents may be virtual “orphan drugs.” Like most syndromes of the elderly, dementia of AD type in the oldest-old may be a disorder of multifactorial causation, with causes including multiple and perhaps ineluctable aging processes. In that case, prospects for meaningfully effective intervention may be least likely in the very population which largely drives the dire predictions regarding the looming societal costs of health care for AD (Alzheimer's Disease International, 2010).

The aesthetically less than satisfying reality may be that prevention and treatment of AD in the very old will involve searching out therapeutic footholds in a variety of contributory disorders. The objective may be to delay onset and attenuate clinical severity rather than to attack a central, even singular, cause. Perhaps the giant against whom we struggle is less like Goliath, to be felled by a single strategically placed pebble from a slingshot, and more like Gulliver, to be wounded and contained by a myriad Lilliputian arrows and shackles.

References

Aizenstein, H. J. et al. (2008). Frequent amyloid deposition without significant cognitive impairment among the elderly. Archives of Neurology, 65, 15091517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albert, M. S. et al. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer's Association workshop on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 7, 270279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alzheimer's Disease International (2010). World Alzheimer Report 2010. Available at: http://www.alz.co.uk/research/world-report; last accessed 12 August 2011.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn, text revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Assocation.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2010). DSM 5 Development: Neurocognitive Disorders. Available at http://www.dsm5.org/proposedrevision/Pages/NeurocognitiveDisorders.aspx; last accessed 12 August 2011.Google Scholar
Bartram, L., Lill, C. M., and Tanzi, R. E. (2010). The genetics of Alzheimer disease: back to the future. Neuron, 68, 270281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corder, E. H. et al. (1993). Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer's disease in late onset families. Science, 261, 921923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craft, S. (2009). The role of metabolic disorders in Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia: two roads converged. Archives of Neurology, 66, 300305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Leeuw, F-E et al. (2001). Prevalence of cerebral white matter lesions in elderly people: a population-based magnetic resonance imaging study. The Rotterdam Scan Study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 70, 914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodge, H. H., Chang, C-C. H., Kamboh, M. I. and Ganguli, M. (2011). Risk of Alzheimer disease incidence attributable to vascular disease in the population. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 7, 356360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferri, C. P. et al. (2005). Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study. Lancet, 366, 21122117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gao, S., Hendrie, H. C., Hall, K. S., and Hui, S. (1998). The relationships between age, sex, and the incidence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease: a meta-analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 809815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golde, T. E., Schneider, L. S., and Koo, E. H. (2011). Anti-Aβ therapeutics in Alzheimer's disease: the need for a paradigm shift. Neuron, 69, 203213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, T. F. and Ganguli, M. (2009). Modifiable mid-life risk factors for late-life cognitive impairment and dementia. Current Psychiatry Reviews, 5, 7392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jack, C. R. et al. (2011). Introduction to the recommendations from the National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer's Association workshop on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 7, 257262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jorm, A. F., and Jolley, D. (1998). The incidence of dementia: a meta-analysis. Neurology, 51, 728733CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamboh, M. I. (2004). Molecular genetics of late-onset Alzheimer's disease. Annals of Human Genetics, 68, 381404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katzman, R. (1993). Education and the prevalence of dementia in Alzheimer's disease. Neurology, 43, 1320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinsella, K. and Wan, H. (2009). An Aging World: 2008. US Census Bureau, International Population Reports, P95/09–1, Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office. Available at http://www.census.gov/prod/2009pubs/p95-09-1.pdf; last accessed 12 August 2011.Google Scholar
Kuller, L. H. and Lopez, O. L. (2010). Commentary on “Developing a national strategy to prevent dementia: Leon Thal Symposium 2009.” Is dementia among older individuals 75+ a unique disease? Alzheimer's & Dementia, 6,142144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuller, L. H. and Lopez, O. L. (2011). Dementia and Alzheimer's disease: a new direction. The 2010 Jay L. Foster Memorial Lecture. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 7, 540555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lister, J. P. and Barnes, C. A. (2009). Neurobiological changes in the hippocampus during normative aging. Archives of Neurology, 66, 829833.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masliah, E., Crews, L, and Hansen, L. (2006). Synaptic remodeling during aging and in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 9 (Suppl.), 9199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mawuenyega, K. G. et al. (2010). Decreased clearance of CNS β-amyloid in Alzheimer's disease. Science, 330, 1774.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKhann, G. M. et al. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of the Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's disease. Neurology, 34, 939944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKhann, G. M. et al. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer's Association workshop on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 7, 263269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Naj, A. C. et al. (2011). Common variants at MS4A4, CD2AP, CD33 and EPHA1 are associated with late-onset Alzheimer's disease. Nature Genetics, 43, 436441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Brien, J. T. (2006). Vascular cognitive impairment. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14, 724733.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, J. L. et al. (2009). Neuropathology of nondemented aging: presumptive evidence for preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 30, 10261036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, T. R. et al. (1997). Silent brain infarction on magnetic resonance imaging and neurological abnormalities in community-dwelling older adults: the Cardiovascular Study Collaborative Research Group. Stroke; 28, 11581164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rafii, M. S. and Aisen, P. S. (2009). Recent developments in Alzheimer's disease therapeutics. BMC Medicine, 7, 7. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-7-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savva, G. M., Wharton, S. B., Ince, P. G., Forster, G., Matthews, F. E, and Brayne, C. for the MRCCFA Study (2009). Age, neuropathology and dementia. New England Journal of Medicine, 360, 23022309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, A. and Mandelkow, E. (2008). Tau-based treatment strategies in neurodegenerative diseases. Neurotherapeutics, 5, 443457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, J. A., Arvanitakis, Z., Bang, W. and Bennett, D. A. (2007). Mixed brain pathologies account for most dementia cases in community-dwelling older persons. Neurology, 69, 21972204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperling, R. A. et al. (2011). Towards defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer's Association workshop on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 7, 280292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stricker, N. H. et al. (2011). Distinct profiles of brain and cognitive changes in the very old with Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 77, 713721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Heuvel, C., Thornton, E. and Vink, R. (2007). Traumatic brain injury and Alzheimer's disease: a review. Progress in Brain Research, 161, 303316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vermeer, S. E., Koudstaal, P. J., Oudkerk, M., Hofman, A. and Breteler, M. B. (2002). Prevalence and risk factors of silent brain infarcts in the population-based Rotterdam Scan Study. Stroke, 33, 2125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed