Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T19:29:54.224Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The developmental trajectory of diurnal cortisol in autistic and neurotypical youth

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 July 2023

Blythe A. Corbett*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA Vanderbilt Kennedy Center, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
Trey McGonigle
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA
Rachael A. Muscatello
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA
Jinyuan Liu
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA
Simon Vandekar
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA
*
Corresponding author: Blythe A. Corbett; Email: blythe.corbett@vumc.org
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Increasing age and puberty affect the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis maturation, which is likely associated with an increase in environmental demands (e.g., social) and vulnerability for the onset of psychiatric conditions (e.g., depression). There is limited research as to whether such patterns are consonant in youth with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a condition marked by social challenges, dysregulation of the HPA axis, and higher rates of depression setting the stage for enhanced vulnerability during this developmental period.

The current study interrogated diurnal cortisol by examining (1) cortisol expression longitudinally over the pubertal transition between autistic and neurotypical youth, (2) the trajectory of diurnal cortisol and the unique contributions of age vs. puberty, and (3) potential sex differences. As hypothesized, results indicate autistic compared to typically developing youth demonstrate a shallower diurnal slope and elevated evening cortisol. These differences were in the context of higher cortisol and flatter rhythms based on age and pubertal development. Also, sex-based differences emerged such that females in both groups had higher cortisol, flatter slopes, and higher evening cortisol than males. The results show that despite the trait-like stability of diurnal cortisol, HPA maturation is impacted by age, puberty, sex, as well as an ASD diagnosis.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

The dynamic adolescent period is marked by significant physiological changes in the regulation and responsivity of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis resulting in elevations in diurnal basal cortisol levels (Barra et al., Reference Barra, Silva, Rodrigues, Santos and Colosimo2015) and higher cortisol in response to perceived stressors (Gunnar et al., Reference Gunnar, Wewerka, Frenn, Long and Griggs2009). Cortisol is a glucocorticoid secreted by the adrenal cortices and the final hormonal output of the HPA axis in humans. Cortisol exhibits a characteristic diurnal rhythm with peak concentrations in the morning and nadir concentrations in the evening. Specifically, cortisol rises immediately after waking followed by an increase 30–60 min post-waking, then a sharp decline with a relative plateau in the afternoon and the lowest levels in the evening. The cortisol awakening response (CAR) refers to the sharp increase of 38–75% of blood levels of cortisol (Pruessner et al., Reference Pruessner, Gaab, Hellhammer, Lintz, Schommer and Kirschbaum1997; Wust et al., Reference Wust, Federenko, Hellhammer and Kirschbaum2000). The CAR is present in the majority of adults but less consistent in adolescents and shows significant intra- and inter-variability (Almeida et al., Reference Almeida, Piazza and Stawski2009; Platje et al., Reference Platje, Vermeiren, Branje, Doreleijers, Meeus, Koot, Frijns, van Lier and Jansen2013; Ross et al., Reference Ross, Murphy, Adam, Chen and Miller2014). The rise of cortisol in the morning is thought to be related to the act of waking (Fries et al., Reference Fries, Dettenborn and Kirschbaum2009; Wilhelm et al., Reference Wilhelm, Born, Kudielka, Schlotz and Wust2007); thereby, preparing the person for enhanced physiological arousal to meet the demands of the day. While cortisol levels fluctuate in a circadian rhythm (Knutsson et al., Reference Knutsson, Dahlgren, Marcus, Rosberg, Bronnegard, Stierna and Albertsson-Wikland1997), there is a trait-like stability explaining an estimated 72% of the variance that is distinct from response to state-based events or stressors, and this trait-like component is largely stable over time (Shirtcliff et al., Reference Shirtcliff, Allison, Armstrong, Slattery, Kalin and Essex2012).

Adolescence refers to the developmental transition shaped by psychological and social experiences measured by age (Spear, Reference Spear2000; Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2005) and generally defined as early (10–14 years), middle (15–17 years), and late adolescence/young adulthood (17–24). Puberty explicitly refers to biological maturation initiated via activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–gonadal (HPG) axis resulting in significant changes in cognitive, emotional, and physiological development (e.g., Chrousos et al., Reference Chrousos, Torpy and Gold1998; Spear, Reference Spear2000; Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2005).

Increasing age and puberty affect HPA axis maturation reflected by a general increase in basal levels of cortisol (Gunnar et al., Reference Gunnar, Wewerka, Frenn, Long and Griggs2009) and a flatter diurnal slope (e.g., Shirtcliff et al., Reference Shirtcliff, Allison, Armstrong, Slattery, Kalin and Essex2012). The neurobiological change during early adolescence is likely associated with an increase in environmental demands (e.g., social; Blakemore & Mills, Reference Blakemore and Mills2014; Crone & Dahl, Reference Crone and Dahl2012) and vulnerability for the onset of psychiatric conditions (e.g., depression; Breslau et al., Reference Breslau, Gilman, Stein, Ruder, Gmelin and Miller2017; Gunnar et al., Reference Gunnar, Wewerka, Frenn, Long and Griggs2009; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005). In fact, evening cortisol has been associated with higher rates of depression in adolescents 13–18 years (Van den Bergh & Van Calster, Reference Van den Bergh and Van Calster2009).

Furthermore, like many aspects of maturation, the magnitude and shape of the slope is different for males and females. Sex-based comparisons reveal that across adolescence, females evidence a stronger circadian rhythm as well as higher basal cortisol (Rosmalen et al., Reference Rosmalen, Oldehinkel, Ormel, de Winter, Buitelaar and Verhulst2005; Shirtcliff et al., Reference Shirtcliff, Allison, Armstrong, Slattery, Kalin and Essex2012), which are primarily driven by age rather than earlier entry into puberty in females (Shirtcliff et al., Reference Shirtcliff, Allison, Armstrong, Slattery, Kalin and Essex2012). It has been speculated that such differences in HPA profiles may be related to differential emergence of various mental and physical health conditions between females and males (Zahn-Waxler et al., Reference Zahn-Waxler, Shirtcliff and Marceau2008).

In addition to developmental factors, Dallman et al. (Reference Dallman, la Fleur, Pecoraro, Gomez, Houshyar and Akana2004) highlight that HPA axis regulation is influenced by metabolic factors such as body mass index (BMI) (e.g., Dallman et al., Reference Dallman, la Fleur, Pecoraro, Gomez, Houshyar and Akana2004; Rosmalen et al., Reference Rosmalen, Oldehinkel, Ormel, de Winter, Buitelaar and Verhulst2005; Shirtcliff et al., Reference Shirtcliff, Allison, Armstrong, Slattery, Kalin and Essex2012). Also, certain medications have been shown to impact HPA axis activity (see Granger et al., Reference Granger, Hibel, Fortunato and Kapelewski2009). As a result, BMI and medication factors must be considered when examining diurnal cortisol measurement and analysis.

Autism spectrum disorder

The integrity of the HPA axis has been explored in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a neurodevelopmental disorder defined by impairments in reciprocal social communication and restricted and repetitive interests and behaviors (APA, 2013). Notably, autistic children have heightened sensory sensitivity (e.g., APA, 2013; Tomchek & Dunn, Reference Tomchek and Dunn2007) and poor adaptability to change (Walker et al., Reference Walker, Walder and Reynolds2001) which can lead to increased stress and arousal. ASD occurs more often in males than females ranging from a 4:1 male-to-female ratio (Maenner et al., Reference Maenner, Warren, Williams, Amoakohene, Bakian, Bilder, Durkin, Fitzgerald, Furnier, Hughes, Ladd-Acosta, McArthur, Pas, Salinas, Vehorn, Williams, Esler, Grzybowski, Hall-Lande and Shaw2023) to 2:1 or 3:1 with such discrepancies ostensibly due to under-diagnosis (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Leventhal, Koh, Fombonne, Laska, Lim, Cheon, Kim, Kim, Lee, Song and Grinker2011; Loomes et al., Reference Loomes, Hull and Mandy2017) and a unique female phenotype (Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Vandekar, Muscatello and Tanguturi2020; Kreiser & White, Reference Kreiser and White2014; Mandy et al., Reference Mandy, Chilvers, Chowdhury, Salter, Seigal and Skuse2012; Uljarevic et al., Reference Uljarević, Cooper, Bebbington, Glasson, Maybery, Varcin, Alvares, Wray, Leekam and Whitehouse2020). Currently, there is debate regarding the use of terminology and whether person-first language in which the individual (e.g., adolescent) is referenced before the condition (e.g., autism) or whether identity-first language (e.g., autistic adolescent) should be used. When formally referencing the diagnostic or group category, the authors have opted to use the term ASD; otherwise, the term autistic will be used.

Elevation in cortisol during the pubertal transition is adaptive and helps prepare youth for the increased demands and novel challenges characteristic of adolescence. However, adolescence is postulated to be a time of increased susceptibility in autistic youth (Picci & Scherf, Reference Picci and Scherf2015), in part due to enhanced physiological arousal (Muscatello & Corbett, Reference Muscatello and Corbett2018) and poor adaption to change (Walker et al., Reference Walker, Walder and Reynolds2001) including developmental transitions (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Adams and Bishop2017; Taylor & Seltzer, Reference Taylor and Seltzer2010). Investigations examining the integrity of the HPA axis in autistic children have shown more variable and dysregulated rhythms (Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Mendoza, Abdullah, Wegelin and Levine2006; Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Mendoza, Wegelin, Carmean and Levine2008; Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Schupp, Levine and Mendoza2009; Hoshino et al., Reference Hoshino, Yokoyama, Watanabe, Murata, Kaneko and Kumashiro1987; Tomarken et al., Reference Tomarken, Han and Corbett2015). Also, elevated evening cortisol has been consistently found in autistic children and adolescents compared to TD peers (Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Schupp, Levine and Mendoza2009; Tordjman et al., Reference Tordjman, Anderson, Kermarrec, Bonnot, Geoffray, Brailly-Tabard, Chaouch, Colliot, Trabado, Bronsard, Coulon, Botbol, Charbuy, Camus and Touitou2014), which contributes to a blunted slope in at least a subgroup of children (Tomarken et al., Reference Tomarken, Han and Corbett2015). However, previous research on the CAR does not distinguish autistic children from their TD peers (Corbett & Schupp, Reference Corbett and Schupp2014; Taylor & Corbett, Reference Taylor and Corbett2014; Zinke et al., Reference Zinke, Fries, Kliegel, Kirschbaum and Dettenborn2010). Although differences have been reported in one study of autistic adolescents (Brosnan et al., Reference Brosnan, Turner-Cobb, Munro-Naan and Jessop2009), the CAR was reported to be associated with self-report of depression in autistic females (Sharpley et al., Reference Sharpley, Bitsika, Andronicos and Agnew2016).

While not the focus of the current study, it is important to note that enhanced social stress as measured by cortisol has been frequently reported (Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Schupp, Levine and Mendoza2009, Reference Corbett, Schupp and Lanni2012; Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Schupp, Simon, Ryan and Mendoza2010) with increased cortisol responsivity with age (Schupp et al., Reference Schupp, Simon and Corbett2013) and pubertal development (Muscatello & Corbett, Reference Muscatello and Corbett2018). Amidst the differences demonstrated between autistic females and males such as pubertal timing (Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Vandekar, Muscatello and Tanguturi2020), internalizing symptoms (Hedley et al., Reference Hedley, Uljarevic, Foley, Richdale and Trollor2018; May et al., Reference May, Cornish and Rinehart2014; Schwartzman et al., Reference Schwartzman, Williams and Corbett2022), and social communication (Wood-Downie et al., Reference Wood-Downie, Wong, Kovshoff, Cortese and Hadwin2021), previous research interrogating the HPA axis has been limited by relatively few or absent female participants (Corbett & Schupp, Reference Corbett and Schupp2014; Edmiston et al., Reference Edmiston, Blain and Corbett2017).

To date, research examining the diurnal regulation of cortisol has shown atypical patterns postulated to be related to heightened sensitivity to change, events of the day and sensory sensitivity (e.g., Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Schupp, Levine and Mendoza2009). What is less clear is the extent to which such predisposition is further impacted by physiological and environmental changes brought on by puberty and adolescence, respectively.

The purpose of the current study was to extend previous research in diurnal cortisol (i.e., slope, CAR, evening) by (1) studying cortisol expression longitudinally over the pubertal transition between autistic and neurotypical youth, (2) determine the trajectory of diurnal cortisol (collected over 3 consecutive weekdays) and the unique contributions of age vs. puberty, and (3) examine potential sex differences in female and male youth. The following hypotheses (Hyp) are advanced: Hyp 1. Circadian Rhythmicity (slope, CAR and evening): it is hypothesized that autistic youth compared to TD youth will have a shallower slope, higher evening cortisol and no group differences based on the CAR; Hyp 2. Impact of Age and Puberty: it is hypothesized that as the youth develop, they will have higher cortisol and flatter circadian rhythms based on age (Hyp 2.1) and pubertal development (Hyp 2.2); and Hyp 3. Sex-Based Differences: It is hypothesized that females in both groups (ASD/TD) will have higher cortisol, flatter slopes, and higher evening cortisol than males. Additionally, females with higher evening cortisol will have more depression symptoms.

Method

The research was carried out in accordance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki). The Vanderbilt Institutional Review Board approved the study. Informed written consent and assent was obtained from all parents and study participants, respectively, prior to inclusion in the study.

Participants

Data were collected as part of a longitudinal study on pubertal development and stress (Corbett, Reference Corbett2017). The current study includes data from the first three assessment years: Year-1 (Y1) enrollment when the children were between 10-years-0-months to 13-years-11-months of age, Year-2 (Y2) 1 year after the participant’s initial visit, and Year-3 (Y3) 1 year after Y2. Diagnostic procedures were completed in Y1. The physical exam (Tanner staging), physiological measures (e.g., cortisol), and psychological testing and questionnaires (e.g., CBCL) were completed annually.

In Y1, the sample included 245 total youth, with 239 participants that completed the physical exam described below. The ASD group consisted of 140 participants (median age 11.2) including 36 females and 104 males. The TD group consisted of 105 participants (median age 11.7) including 46 females and 59 males. One autistic male was missing a measurement for G stage, and one TD female was missing measurement for PH stage.

For Year 1, the racial and ethnic characterization of the sample was comprised of 7.8% Black, 83.3% White, and 8.6% multiracial. Participants were recruited from a broad community sample in the southern United States covering a 200-mile radius that targeted medical and health-related services, clinics, research registries, regional disability organizations, schools, and social media platforms. Inclusion required an IQ score ≥ 70 due to task demands in the source longitudinal study. Children were excluded if taking medications that alter the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal (HPA) axis (e.g., corticosteroids; see Granger et al., Reference Granger, Hibel, Fortunato and Kapelewski2009) or HPG axis (e.g., growth hormone), or medical condition known to impact pubertal development (e.g., Cushing’s Disease). Demographic information for each group is presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics stratified by diagnosis

N is the number of non-missing value. 1Kruskal-Wallis. 2Pearson. 3Wilcoxon.

*p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01, ***p ≤ 0.001.

In Y2 there were 174 participants, the ASD group had a median age of 12.5 years, and the TD group had a median age of 12.7 years. The overall attrition rate was 26.89%, which was comparable to other longitudinal studies after the initial enrollment (Negriff et al., Reference Negriff, Blankson and Trickett2015). At Y3, there were 163 participants, with a median age of 13.3 years for the ASD group and 13.8 years for the TD group. At Y2 and Y3, some participants were unable to complete the full physical examination due to restrictions on in-person lab visits resulting from the COVID-19 pandemic (Y2 N = 43; Y3 N = 59).

Diagnostic procedures and assessment measures

The diagnosis of ASD was based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual-5 (APA, 2013) and confirmed by an established diagnosis by a psychologist, psychiatrist, or behavioral pediatrician with ASD expertise, current clinical judgment by a study team member, and corroborated by the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-2; Lord et al., Reference Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, Risi, Gotham and Bishop2012).

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Second Edition (ADOS-2; Lord et al., Reference Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, Risi, Gotham and Bishop2012) is a semi-structured interactive play and interview-based instrument used to support the diagnosis of ASD. The ADOS Module III was administered by research-reliable personnel.

Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; Rutter et al., Reference Rutter, Bailey and Lord2003) is a screening questionnaire to assess for symptoms of ASD. A score of 15 is suggestive of a diagnosis of ASD. Due to lower sensitivity and specificity (Barnard-Brak et al., Reference Barnard-Brak, Brewer, Chesnut, Richman and Schaeffer2016), TD children with a score ≥ 10 were excluded from the study.

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, Second Edition (WASI-II, Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2011) a measure of cognitive ability, was used to obtain an estimate of the participant’s intellectual functioning. Inclusion for the study required an IQ ≥ 70.

The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001) is a broad-based parent report form used to provide children’s competencies and behavioral/emotional problems from 6 to 18 years of age. The CBCL affective (Depression) domain was used in regression data analysis. The psychometric properties of the CBCL are good. Specifically, the test retest intraclass correlation for the DSM-Oriented scales is r = 0.88 and the internal consistency alpha ranges from 0.72 to 0.91.

Physical exam

The standardized physical exam was completed in a clinical exam room during each annual visit to reliably identify pubertal development and assign Tanner stage (Marshall & Tanner, Reference Marshall and Tanner1969, Reference Marshall and Tanner1970). The exam ascertained two measures with 5 stages for Male External Genitalia (G1–G5 for males) and Female Breast (B1–B5 for females) (G/B stage) and Pubic hair (P1–P5 for both sexes) (PH stage). The exam consisted of visual inspection and categorization of pubertal and genital maturation. To be consistent with the original Tanner staging and to maximize participation, palpation of breasts or measurement of testes was not conducted.

The exam was conducted by fully trained, licensed study physicians. A male physician conducted most of the exams; however, a female physician provided same-sex exams as requested. Previous research has shown that the sex of the physician is not as important as the participant’s comfort level and the competence of the physician (Dorn et al., Reference Dorn, Dahl, Woodward and Biro2006). To this end, the physicians first established rapport, explained the rationale for the exam and addressed any questions or concerns. Next, the participant’s height and weight were measured. Subsequently, the adolescent was requested to loosen clothing to fully expose breast and lower genital region, rather than fully disrobing. This approach aided in the level of comfort for the participants. A companion (e.g., parent) was offered to accompany the participant in the exam room.

Physician inter-rater reliability was established between study physicians on 10 randomly selected participants. Cohen’s Kappa (κ) was calculated between study physicians to assess the degree to which raters were able to identify Tanner stages for G/B and PH markers. Inter-rater reliability for markers ranged from κ = 0.62 to 0.75 (all p < 0.001; substantial agreement). Absolute agreement was 0.75. Kappa was also calculated to assess the extent to which physicians were able to reliably and independently identify when participants had initiated pubertal maturation (Stage 2) for each marker. Kappa ranged from 0.62 to 1.00 (good to very good). In cases of disagreement, physician ratings were never greater than one stage difference.

Cortisol sampling procedure

Diurnal cortisol saliva samples were collected at home four times a day: In the morning following natural waking (Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Evans, Hucklebridge and Clow2001; Kudielka & Kirschbaum, Reference Kudielka and Kirschbaum2003; Wilhelm et al., Reference Wilhelm, Born, Kudielka, Schlotz and Wust2007), 30-min Post Waking, Afternoon, and Evening, over 3 consecutive weekdays (non-holidays) immediately prior to the in-lab visit using established procedures (e.g., passive drool, postponed if sick; Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Mendoza, Abdullah, Wegelin and Levine2006, Reference Corbett, Mendoza, Wegelin, Carmean and Levine2008). Families and participants were rigorously trained on collection procedures, received instructional materials and DVD demonstrations, and were prompted with sampling reminders in the days prior to and on scheduled sampling dates. Furthermore, families completed daily diaries to track sample times, in addition to recording the collection time on sample labels. Diaries also included prompts for recording time to bed, time woken, total sleep, and any important notes about the day (e.g., early release from school, atypical schedule). For two children who had difficulty with the passive drool method because of oral motor or sensory aversion issues, a cotton roll and syringe procedure was used for each of their samples (Tordjman et al., Reference Tordjman, Anderson, Kermarrec, Bonnot, Geoffray, Brailly-Tabard, Chaouch, Colliot, Trabado, Bronsard, Coulon, Botbol, Charbuy, Camus and Touitou2014). Sensitivity analyses were conducted excluding these two children and results were not meaningfully different, and we proceed with the full dataset. Participants were instructed to not eat or drink 1-hr prior to salivary collection and to refrain from brushing teeth in the morning until after Post Waking. Per the protocol, participants passively drooled into a test tube using a straw collecting approximately 1 mL of saliva. Participants were instructed to collect home salivary samples in the 3 weekdays prior to the lab visit. Samples were refrigerated in the home until returning to the lab at which time they were placed in a −80°C freezer. In an effort to account for hormonal changes throughout the menstrual cycle, female participants that had begun menstruating were scheduled during the luteal phase, based on date of last menses, as previous research has shown women in the luteal phase to have comparable cortisol levels to men (Kirschbaum et al., Reference Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Gaab, Schommer and Hellhammer1999).

Cortisol assay

Cortisol assays were performed using a Coat-A-Count® radioimmunoassay kit (Siemens Medical Solutions Diagnostics, Los Angeles, CA) modified to accommodate lower levels of cortisol in human saliva. Samples stored at –80°C, were thawed and centrifuged at 3460 r.p.m. for 15 min to separate the aqueous component from mucins and other suspended particles. The coated tube from the kit was substituted with a glass tube into which 100 ul of saliva, 100 ul of cortisol antibody (courtesy of Wendell Nicholson, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN), and 100 ul of 125I-cortisol were mixed. After incubation at 4°C for 24 hr 100 ul of normal rat serum in 0.1% PO4/EDTA buffer (1:50) and precipitating reagent (PR81) were added. The mixture was centrifuged at 3460 r.p.m. for 30 min, decanted, and counted [for details see Corbett et al. (Reference Corbett, Swain, Newsom, Wang, Song and Edgerton2013)]. Serial dilution of samples indicated a linearity of 0.99. The intra-assay coefficients of variation were as follows Y1 CV = 2.06%; Y2 CV = 2.80%; Y3 CV = 1.90%. The total intra-assay (across all 3 years) = 2.22%. The inter-assay CV was 9.52%.

Aims, hypotheses and definitions

1. Circadian Rhythmicity: The aim was to examine diurnal cortisol longitudinally (slope, CAR, and evening) between the groups (ASD & TD). Slope was comprised of 4 samples: Waking, Post Waking, Afternoon, and Evening for 3 consecutive days. For analyses, each of these cortisol time periods was modeled across the 3 sampling days across 3 years with random effects for hypothesis 1, 2, and 3.1, but evening values were averaged for hypothesis 3.2. The CAR was calculated by subtracting the 30-min sample from immediate morning sample across the three days. Hyp1.1: It was hypothesized that autistic youth compared to TD youth will have a more shallow slope, higher evening cortisol and no group differences based on the CAR. Hyp 1.2: It was hypothesized that autistic youth will have elevated evening cortisol.

2. Developmental Trajectory: The aim was to examine diurnal slope in consideration of age and pubertal development separately and together. Puberty was examined by physical exam alone (without age in the model) as a marker of development. Age and Puberty were also examined together to evaluate the possibility of combinatory effect on cortisol levels. Hyp 2.1: It was hypothesized that as the youth age, they will have higher cortisol and flatter circadian rhythms. Hyp 2.2: It was hypothesized that cortisol levels will be higher as the groups advance through puberty. Moreover, the slopes will become flatter. Hyp 2.3: It was hypothesized that the effect of puberty will be less or nonsignificant when controlling for age.

3. Sex-Based Differences: The aim was to examine diurnal slope and evening cortisol over development based on sex. Sex was defined as biological sex assigned at birth to compare sex (males/females) while controlling for key factors (BMI and medication use). Hyp 3.1: It was hypothesized that females in both groups (ASD/TD) will have higher cortisol, flatter slopes, and higher evening cortisol than males. Depression associations: The CBCL Affective Problems index was used to determine if sex differences exist by looking at associations between sex (females) and depression. Since CBCL scores were only collected yearly, evening cortisol was taken to be the average of the three evening samples for this analysis. Hyp 3.2: It was hypothesized that females with higher evening cortisol will have increased depression symptoms.

Statistical analyses

Characteristics of participants were stratified by diagnosis and summarized in Table 1, using proportions and frequencies for categorical measures, and the 1st, 2nd (median), and 3rd quartiles for numerical measures. Cortisol values were positively skewed; therefore, values were log10 transformed prior to statistical analyses. Additionally for categorical variables, Pearson’s χ2 tests of homogeneity were conducted to identify whether the frequency distributions differed between the two groups (TD vs. ASD). Finally, Wilcoxon rank-sum tests were conducted for continuous variables to test whether the probability distributions differed by diagnosis. The null hypothesis in each test purported that there was no difference in distributions between the two groups.

To test Aim 1, we fit a linear mixed effects model on log10 transformed cortisol values. We modeled diurnal cortisol levels including diagnosis by time period (period) and age by period interactions, controlling for sex, body mass index (BMI), and whether the patient was on medication. To account for the correlation structure, random intercepts for subject, year, and day (ordered as nested) were included in the model. For this and all subsequent models, we used heteroskedasticity consistent standard errors and performed tests using type 2 sum of squares analysis of variance. To test whether the slope differs between groups at the 3 period transitions of interest, – CAR (Post Waking – Waking), Afternoon – Post Waking, & Evening – Post Waking, – we calculated 2-way pairwise contrasts between periods and diagnosis (ASD-TD) while controlling for the period by age interaction. In all models, when performing multiple tests across timepoints we used the Bonferroni correction to adjust for the three multiple comparisons. To test whether the ASD group has a higher evening cortisol level than the TD group (Hyp 1.2), we conducted a linear contrast of the evening cortisol levels between TD and ASD youth.

To investigate hypothesis 2.1, we first modeled and tested log10 diurnal cortisol levels including a diagnosis by period by age interaction, while controlling for sex, body mass index (BMI), and whether the participant was on medication. After, we considered a model without the three-way interaction and including only the diagnosis by period and age by period interaction (as in Hyp 1). For hypothesis 2.2, we considered the above models with a pubertal development effect in lieu of age. In all these models, random intercepts for subject, year, and day (nested as ordered) were included to account for the correlation structure in the data. We considered nonlinear effects for age and puberty using natural cubic splines but retained linear effects only after comparing model fit with AIC. To test if age increases cortisol levels, we tested the main effect of age using the hypothesis 2.1 model excluding the age–period interaction with type II ANOVA. To test whether age has a flattening effect on circadian cortisol responses throughout the day, we conducted period contrasts of the age trends. These testing procedures were mirrored for hypothesis 2.2, replacing age with pubertal stage. To test if the effect of puberty exists after controlling for age (Hyp 2.3), we fit a model including both the age by period and pubertal stage by period interactions as well as a model with an age by period by pubertal stage interaction.

Additionally, we considered a separate random effects structure adding a random intercept for period nested within subject. These models also allowed the day and period random effects variability to vary between diagnosis groups. Ultimately, the random effects variance did not significantly differ between the groups (Table S1), and the simpler random effects structure was retained for all analyses.

To test hypothesis 3.1, we expanded the linear mixed effects model from hypothesis 2.1 to include a sex by period interaction term. To test whether females have higher cortisol, we tested the main effect of sex in the linear mixed effects model fit in hypothesis 2.1 (no sex-period interaction). To test whether females have shallower slopes in regard to cortisol changes throughout the day, we conducted sex contrasts of the slope between each time period contrast. We tested whether females had higher evening cortisol levels than males using a linear contrast of the evening cortisol levels by sex. Next, to address hypothesis 3.2, using only data on the 69 females, we considered a mixed effects model on affective depression scores including evening mean cortisol (across year) as the variable of interest while controlling for BMI and existence of medication. This second model only included random intercepts for subject because depressive scores were measured on a year-to-year basis. All analyses were performed using R 4.2.1.

Results

Hyp 1.1: To investigate differences in mean cortisol and diurnal cortisol response between groups we fit a linear mixed effects model and tested components of the diagnosis by period interaction. There was not sufficient evidence to detect differences between the groups for the CAR Post Waking – Waking response (corrected p = 0.3406; Table 2). There was evidence of a difference between the groups in terms of the Afternoon – Post Waking (corrected p = 0.0001; Table 2). The contrast estimate (0.1113) shows that the ASD group has a shallower cortisol slope over this period (Figure 1). The expected baseline cortisol change from Post Waking to Afternoon is an approximate 77.50% decrease in cortisol levels. Over this transition, for ASD, the expected decrease in cortisol levels is 29.21% less extreme than TD. The coefficient table, random effects estimates, and ANOVA tables are given in the Supplementary material (Table S2, S3).

Figure 1. Mean diurnal cortisol levels over the course of the day. Note. The figures depict raw untransformed or log-transformed diurnal cortisol levels, which unlike the model, do not adjust for covariates nor the within subject correlation.

Table 2. Diagnosis contrasts for aim 1

We also saw a significant difference between the groups in terms of the Evening – Post Waking (corrected p < 0.0001; Table 2). The contrast estimate (0.1803) shows that the ASD group has a shallower cortisol slope over this period transition. The expected baseline cortisol change from Post Waking to Evening is an approximate 93.81% decrease in cortisol levels. For autistic youth, the expected decrease in cortisol levels is 51.48% less extreme than TD youth.

Hyp 1.2: Within the same model, we tested the difference in evening cortisol levels between TD and ASD. This contrast shows that autistic youth have a significantly higher cortisol level in the Evening (p = 0.0117) when compared to TD youth (Table 2). On average in the evening, youth with ASD have 25.01% higher cortisol levels when compared to TD youth.

Hyp 2.1: To investigate the effect of age on the whole diurnal cortisol trajectory, we tested the main effect of age without the age–period interaction, which showed cortisol levels increase 5.78% with each year aged (Table 3). To investigate the effect of age on the shape of the diurnal cortisol trajectory we tested interaction of age with specific components of the trajectory. There was not sufficient evidence for a difference in age trends for the CAR (p = 1.000; Table 3). However, we did find evidence for a difference in age trends for the Afternoon – Post Waking change (corrected p < 0.0001) indicating that age has a flattening effect on the cortisol slope over this period (Table 3; Figure 2a). Specifically, for each year aged, there is a 9.14% decrease in the change of cortisol levels between Post Waking and Afternoon measurements. We also find a flattening effect in the Evening – Post Waking change (corrected p = 0.0004; Table 3). Specifically, the expected decrease in cortisol levels over this period transition is 12.65% less extreme for each year aged.

Figure 2. (a) Observed log cortisol vs. age with smoothing (loess) curve for all 4 periods. (b) Observed log cortisol vs. pubertal stage with a smoothing (loess) curve for all 4 periods.

Table 3. Age effect and age trend contrasts by period for Hyp 2.1

Note. Age effect is from the model with no age:period interaction.

Hyp 2.2: To investigate the effect of puberty on the whole diurnal cortisol trajectory, we tested the main effect for pubertal stage shown to have significant positive effect on cortisol levels (Table 4). We found that for each stage advanced through puberty, the expected cortisol level would rise by 5.52%.

Table 4. Pubertal stage effect & pubertal stage trend contrasts by period for Hyp 2.2

Note. Pubertal stage effect is from the model with no puberty stage: period interaction and no age effect.

To investigate the effect of puberty on the shape of the diurnal cortisol trajectory, we tested interaction of pubertal stage with specific components of the trajectory. There was not sufficient evidence for a difference in pubertal stage trends for the CAR (Post Waking – Waking) and Evening – Post Waking responses (Table 4). However, there was evidence for a difference (corrected p = 0.0149) in puberty stage trends for the Afternoon – Post Waking transition which indicated that puberty stage has a flattening effect on the cortisol slope over this transition (Table 4). For each pubertal stage advanced, the expected decrease in cortisol levels over this transition is 6.93% less extreme (Figure 2b). The coefficient table, random effects estimates, and ANOVA tables are given in the Supplementary material (Table S4, S5).

Hyp 2.3: After including the age by period interaction in the model, the interaction between pubertal stage and period became less significant (Table S7). Further, none of its levels are individually significant in the model after the age–period interaction inclusion (Table S6). Additionally, the three-way interaction between age, puberty, and period was not significant.

Hyp 3.1. Testing the main effect for sex showed females have 22.10% higher cortisol levels (Table 5; Figure 3). There was no statistically significant difference between the sexes for the CAR, Afternoon – Post Waking, and Evening – Post Waking responses (Table 5). Testing the sex difference in evening cortisol levels showed that females have significantly (p < 0.0001) higher cortisol than males on average, with females expected to have 50.19% higher cortisol levels in the evening compared to males. The coefficient table, random effects estimates, and ANOVA tables are given in the Supplementary material (Table S8, S9).

Figure 3. Mean diurnal cortisol levels over the course of the day stratified by sex. Note. The figures depict raw untransformed or log-transformed diurnal cortisol levels, which unlike the model, do not adjust for covariates nor the within subject correlation.

Table 5. Sex effect and sex contrasts for Hyp 3.1

Note. Sex effect is from the Hyp 2.1 model.

Hyp 3.2: We did not find sufficient evidence for an association between Evening mean cortisol and depressive symptoms in females (B = 0.234, 95% CI = −0.486, 0.953, p = 0.519; Table S10).

Discussion

The purpose of the study was to extend previous research in diurnal cortisol rhythmicity by (1) studying cortisol expression longitudinally over the pubertal transition between autistic and neurotypical youth, (2) determine the trajectory of diurnal cortisol and the unique contributions of age vs. puberty, and (3) examine potential sex differences in female and male youth. As predicted, autistic youth compared to TD youth demonstrated a shallower slope reflected by significant differences between the Post-waking and Afternoon and the Post Waking and Evening cortisol values. Amidst the expected 77.50% and 93.81% decrease in slope, between Post Waking and Afternoon and Evening cortisol, autistic youth showed a reduced slope of 29.21% and 51.48% in the afternoon and evening, respectively. The findings of a reduced slope replicate previous research in diurnal cortisol in ASD (Tomarken et al., Reference Tomarken, Han and Corbett2015) yet with a much larger sample of children and adolescents broadly ranging between 10 and 16 years of age. The shallow slope is also consistent with research directly comparing a blended sample of autistic and TD children vs. adolescents aged 7–17 years showing a blunted diurnal slope attributed to group differences in morning and evening cortisol (Muscatello & Corbett, Reference Muscatello and Corbett2018).

The direct period contrasts revealed higher evening cortisol in autistic youth compared to TD youth, a finding that has been reported across several cross-sectional studies (Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Mendoza, Abdullah, Wegelin and Levine2006, Reference Corbett, Mendoza, Wegelin, Carmean and Levine2008, Reference Corbett, Schupp, Levine and Mendoza2009; Muscatello & Corbett, Reference Muscatello and Corbett2018; Tomarken et al., Reference Tomarken, Han and Corbett2015). In the current prospective study, evening cortisol in autistic youth was 25.01% higher when compared to TD youth. In ASD, elevated evening cortisol has been associated with cumulative daily stress throughout the day (Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Schupp, Levine and Mendoza2009), which may reflect persistent heightened arousal contributing to a hyperactive HPA axis (Richdale & Prior, Reference Richdale and Prior1992). Autistic children often have poor response to change (Walker et al., Reference Walker, Walder and Reynolds2001), a pattern that has been explicitly linked to evening cortisol (Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Schupp, Levine and Mendoza2009). Thus, cumulative stress as a result of changing environmental demands contribute to increased arousal late in the day reflected in elevated cortisol in autistic youth. Higher arousal levels have been linked with sleep problems in autistic children (Richdale et al., Reference Richdale, Baker, Short and Gradisar2014). However, due to the lack of difference in CAR in the current sample, sleep dysregulation appears a less likely factor related to the elevated cortisol in the evening. In non-ASD samples, elevated HPA activity and atypical sleep patterns have been characterized as risk markers for the onset of depression in high-risk adolescents (Rao et al., Reference Rao, Hammen and Poland2009). In youth, elevated evening cortisol has been associated with depression and linked to a variety of physical and mental health conditions (Lupien et al., Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009), which warrant greater study.

Taken together, it is apparent that autistic youth evidence differences in the diurnal slope and cortisol levels in the evening. The results are consonant with previous research and strongly point to greater responsivity to external (e.g., poor adaptation to change) and internal (e.g., increased sensory sensitivity) factors in autistic youth resulting in cumulative stress manifest as elevations in cortisol.

Subsequently, we examined the developmental trajectory of the diurnal slope in consideration of age and pubertal development separately and together. The results supported the notion that age has a flattening effect on the diurnal cortisol slope from Post Waking to Afternoon as well as Post Waking to Evening. When examining age trends across diurnal periods, the slope from Post waking to Afternoon decreased with advancing age (see Figure 2a). Muscatello and Corbett (Reference Muscatello and Corbett2018) reported that in addition to group differences, older autistic adolescents showed a flatter slope due to higher evening cortisol and lower morning values. It appears that in addition to the adaptive rise in basal cortisol and flatter diurnal slope (Gunnar et al., Reference Gunnar, Wewerka, Frenn, Long and Griggs2009; Shirtcliff et al., Reference Shirtcliff, Allison, Armstrong, Slattery, Kalin and Essex2012), autistic youth evidence even higher basal cortisol during adolescence. In other words, both diagnostic and developmental effects contribute to higher cortisol in autistic youth.

Similar to age effects, significant difference in pubertal stage trends was observed for the Afternoon – Post Waking diurnal period resulting in a flattening of the slope. When pubertal trends were examined across diurnal periods, the slope from Post waking to Afternoon grew more shallow with advancing pubertal development (see Figure 2b). These results confirm hypotheses and are consistent with the limited cross-sectional research in ASD (e.g., Muscatello & Corbett, Reference Muscatello and Corbett2018).

As predicted, there were no differences between autistic and TD youth based on the CAR. The finding is consistent with the extant literature (Corbett & Schupp, Reference Corbett and Schupp2014; Taylor & Corbett, Reference Taylor and Corbett2014; Zinke et al., Reference Zinke, Fries, Kliegel, Kirschbaum and Dettenborn2010) with the exception of one study of adolescents with ASD (Brosnan et al., Reference Brosnan, Turner-Cobb, Munro-Naan and Jessop2009).

Since many previous studies in autism have been conducted with male youth only or very few females, the current study made a significant contribution by including a relatively large group of autistic females. Sex-based differences were explored for diurnal slope and evening cortisol over development predicting that females in both groups would have higher cortisol, flatter slopes, and higher evening cortisol than males. The hypothesis was partially confirmed showing that females have higher evening cortisol levels than males such that females have a 50.19% higher cortisol level when compared to males. Studies in typically developing youth have revealed higher basal cortisol and stronger circadian rhythm in females driven primarily by age rather than earlier pubertal development (Rosmalen et al., Reference Rosmalen, Oldehinkel, Ormel, de Winter, Buitelaar and Verhulst2005; Shirtcliff et al., Reference Shirtcliff, Allison, Armstrong, Slattery, Kalin and Essex2012). Moreover, it has been surmised that sex-based differences in HPA profiles may be related to differential emergence of various mental and physical health conditions between males and females (Zahn-Waxler et al., Reference Zahn-Waxler, Shirtcliff and Marceau2008).

Based on results showing elevations in cortisol and associations with depression in neurotypical (e.g., Van den Bergh & Van Calster, Reference Van den Bergh and Van Calster2009) and autistic female (e.g., Sharpley et al., Reference Sharpley, Bitsika, Andronicos and Agnew2016) adolescents, these associations were explored. Surprisingly, evening cortisol did not predict symptoms of depression in autistic females in the current study. Thus, despite being at higher risk for depression (Schwartzman et al., Reference Schwartzman, Williams and Corbett2022), elevated evening cortisol (Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Mendoza, Abdullah, Wegelin and Levine2006, Reference Corbett, Mendoza, Wegelin, Carmean and Levine2008; Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Schupp, Levine and Mendoza2009; Muscatello & Corbett, Reference Muscatello and Corbett2018; Tomarken et al., Reference Tomarken, Han and Corbett2015), and advanced puberty (Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Vandekar, Muscatello and Tanguturi2020), there was no observed association between cortisol level and symptoms of depression. It is unknown if a connection would exist in autistic females with a confirmed diagnosis of depression. A recent meta-analysis reported that elevated morning and nocturnal cortisol may be risk factors for onset of major depressive disorder (MDD) in adolescence or young adulthood (Zajkowska et al., Reference Zajkowska, Gullett, Walsh, Zonca, Pedersen, Souza, Kieling, Fisher, Kohrt and Mondelli2022) such that hyperactivity of the HPA precedes and contributes to the onset of depression during adolescence. The current study did not characterize the sample with regard to confirming the co-occurrence of MDD, rather a symptom profile was used. Nevertheless, symptoms of depression are more prevalent in ASD during early adolescence especially in females (Schwartzman et al., Reference Schwartzman, Williams and Corbett2022). Thus, it remains a cautionary tale and expanded research is needed to explore potential links between elevated cortisol as a risk factor for developing depression in autistic youth especially females.

Strengths, limitations, and future directions

Although previous research has examined the integrity of the HPA axis in ASD, the research has been largely cross-sectional, with small-to-moderate sample sizes, a lack of female participants, and use of a questionnaire to determine pubertal status (e.g., Corbett et al., Reference Corbett, Mendoza, Abdullah, Wegelin and Levine2006, Reference Corbett, Mendoza, Wegelin, Carmean and Levine2008; Edmiston et al., Reference Edmiston, Blain and Corbett2017; Muscatello & Corbett, Reference Muscatello and Corbett2018; Tordjman et al., Reference Tordjman, Anderson, Kermarrec, Bonnot, Geoffray, Brailly-Tabard, Chaouch, Colliot, Trabado, Bronsard, Coulon, Botbol, Charbuy, Camus and Touitou2014). The current study addresses these gaps by utilizing a 3-year prospective longitudinal design, inclusion of a large, well-characterized sample including autistic females, and use of gold-standard pubertal Tanner staging. Even so, limitations remain to include a lack of participants with intellectual disability and less representation of minoritized individuals. Further, during Y2 and Y3, some participants were unable to complete the full physical examination due to restrictions on in-person lab visits resulting from the COVID-19 pandemic (Y2 N = 43; Y3 N = 59), which only affected models with pubertal stage as a variable. Additionally, to measure symptoms of depression, only a subdomain from a broad parent-report measure was used rather than a focused psychiatric interview. Future studies are needed to be more inclusive in terms of race, ethnicity, SES, and diagnostic severity. Furthermore, to more rigorously measure co-occurring conditions (e.g., depression), a standardized psychiatric interview may be optimal to capture the nuance of mental health risk profiles. Lastly, while menstruating females were scheduled during the luteal phase of their cycle, future research with larger samples of females will benefit from rigorous menstrual cycle tracking (e.g., Backward and Forward counting method). Similarly, tracking and controlling for other environmental factors that may influence the HPA axis, such as seasonality, will be important.

In summary, the current study extended research in ASD during the pivotal adolescent and pubertal transition and revealed that despite the trait-like stability of diurnal cortisol, HPA maturation is impacted by age, puberty, sex as well as an ASD diagnosis. Questions remain as to the long-term medical sequelae and mental health outcomes in autistic youth from dysregulated HPA axis. Further, an enhanced understanding of moderating effects on the differences in diurnal slope and basal levels will inform developmental risk during adolescence and the pubertal transition to adulthood.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000810

Funding statement

This study was funded by the NIMH (MH111599 PI: Corbett) with core support from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (U54 HD083211, PI: Neul) and the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (CTSA UL1 TR000445). None of the funding sources were involved in the study design, collection, analysis and interpretation of the data, writing of the report, or the decision to submit the article for publication.

Competing interests

None.

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles: An integrated system of multi-informant assessment. ASEBA.Google Scholar
Almeida, D. M., Piazza, J. R., & Stawski, R. S. (2009). Interindividual differences and intraindividual variability in the cortisol awakening response: An examination of age and gender. Psychological Aging, 24(4), 819827. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017910 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
APA (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Barnard-Brak, L., Brewer, A., Chesnut, S., Richman, D., & Schaeffer, A. M. (2016). The sensitivity and specificity of the social communication questionnaire for autism spectrum with respect to age. Autism Research, 9(8), 838845. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1584 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barra, C. B., Silva, I. N., Rodrigues, T. M., Santos, J. L., & Colosimo, E. A. (2015). Morning serum Basal cortisol levels are affected by age and pubertal maturation in school-aged children and adolescents. Hormone Research in Paediatrics, 83(1), 5561. https://doi.org/10.1159/000369801 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S. J., & Mills, K. L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology, 65(1), 187207. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, J., Gilman, S. E., Stein, B. D., Ruder, T., Gmelin, T., & Miller, E. (2017). Sex differences in recent first-onset depression in an epidemiological sample of adolescents. Translational Psychiatry, 7(5), e1139. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2017.105 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brosnan, M., Turner-Cobb, J., Munro-Naan, Z., & Jessop, D. (2009). Absence of a normal cortisol awakening response (CAR) in adolescent males with Asperger syndrome (AS). Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(7), 10951100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chrousos, G. P., Torpy, D. J., & Gold, P. W. (1998). Interactions between the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and the female reproductive system: Clinical implications. Annals of Internal Medicine, 129(3), 229240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbett, B. A. (2017). Examining stress and arousal across pubertal development in ASD. National Institute of Mental Health.Google Scholar
Corbett, B. A., Mendoza, S., Abdullah, M., Wegelin, J. A., & Levine, S. (2006). Cortisol circadian rhythms and response to stress in children with autism. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31(1), 5968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbett, B. A., Mendoza, S., Wegelin, J. A., Carmean, V., & Levine, S. (2008). Variable cortisol circadian rhythms in children with autism and anticipatory stress. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 33(3), 227234.Google ScholarPubMed
Corbett, B. A., & Schupp, C. W. (2014). The Cortisol Awakening Response (CAR) in male children with autism spectrum disorder. Hormones and Behavior, 65(4), 345350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.01.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbett, B. A., Schupp, C. W., & Lanni, K. E. (2012). Comparing biobehavioral profiles across two social stress paradigms in children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Molecular Autism, 3(1), 13. https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-3-13 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbett, B. A., Schupp, C. W., Levine, S., & Mendoza, S. (2009). Comparing cortisol, stress, and sensory sensitivity in children with autism. Autism Research, 2(1), 3949. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.64 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbett, B. A., Schupp, C. W., Simon, D., Ryan, N., & Mendoza, S. (2010). Elevated cortisol during play is associated with age and social engagement in children with autism. Molecular Autism, 1(1), 13. https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-1-13 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbett, B. A., Swain, D. M., Newsom, C., Wang, L., Song, Y., & Edgerton, D. (2013). Biobehavioral profiles of arousal and social motivation in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(8), 924934. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12184 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbett, B. A., Vandekar, S., Muscatello, R. A., & Tanguturi, Y. (2020). Pubertal timing during early adolescence: Advanced pubertal onset in females with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 13(12), 22022215. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2406 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crone, E. A., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social-affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(9), 636650. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3313 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dallman, M. F., la Fleur, S. E., Pecoraro, N. C., Gomez, F., Houshyar, H., & Akana, S. F. (2004). Minireview: Glucocorticoids--food intake, abdominal obesity, and wealthy nations in 2004. Endocrinology, 145(6), 26332638. https://doi.org/10.1210/en.2004-0037 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dorn, L. D., Dahl, R. E., Woodward, H. R., & Biro, F. (2006). Defining the boundaries of early adolescence: A user’s guide to assessing pubertal status and pubertal timing in research with adolescents. Applied Developmental Science, 10(1), 3056. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532480xads1001_3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edmiston, E. K., Blain, S. D., & Corbett, B. A. (2017). Salivary cortisol and behavioral response to social evaluative threat in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 10(2), 346358. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1660 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, S., Evans, P., Hucklebridge, F., & Clow, A. (2001). Association between time of awakening and diurnal cortisol secretory activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26(6), 613622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fries, E., Dettenborn, L., & Kirschbaum, C. (2009). The cortisol awakening response (CAR): Facts and future directions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72(1), 6773. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.03.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granger, D. A., Hibel, L. C., Fortunato, C. K., & Kapelewski, C. H. (2009). Medication effects on salivary cortisol: Tactics and strategy to minimize impact in behavioral and developmental science. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(10), 14371448. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.06.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., Wewerka, S., Frenn, K., Long, J. D., & Griggs, C. (2009). Developmental changes in hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal activity over the transition to adolescence: Normative changes and associations with puberty. Development and Psychopathology, 21(1), 6985. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409000054 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedley, D., Uljarevic, M., Foley, K. R., Richdale, A., & Trollor, J. (2018). Risk and protective factors underlying depression and suicidal ideation in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 35(7), 648657. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22759 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoshino, Y., Yokoyama, F., Watanabe, M., Murata, S., Kaneko, M., & Kumashiro, H. (1987). The diurnal variation and response to dexamethasone suppression test of saliva cortisol level in autistic children. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 41(2), 227235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593602. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, Y. S., Leventhal, B. L., Koh, Y. J., Fombonne, E., Laska, E., Lim, E. C., Cheon, K. A., Kim, S. J., Kim, Y. K., Lee, H. K., Song, D. H., & Grinker, R. R. (2011). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders in a total population sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(9), 904912. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10101532 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B. M., Gaab, J., Schommer, N. C., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1999). Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral contraceptives on the activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61(2), 154162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knutsson, U., Dahlgren, J., Marcus, C., Rosberg, S., Bronnegard, M., Stierna, P., & Albertsson-Wikland, K. (1997). Circadian cortisol rhythms in healthy boys and girls: Relationship with age, growth, body composition, and pubertal development. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 82(2), 536540. https://doi.org/10.1210/jcem.82.2.3769 Google ScholarPubMed
Kreiser, N. L., & White, S. W. (2014). ASD in females: Are we overstating the gender difference in diagnosis? Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 17(1), 6784. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-013-0148-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudielka, B. M., & Kirschbaum, C. (2003). Awakening cortisol responses are influenced by health status and awakening time but not by menstrual cycle phase. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28(1), 3547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loomes, R., Hull, L., & Mandy, W. P. L. (2017). What is the male-to-female ratio in autism spectrum disorder? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(6), 466474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-2) (2nd ed.). Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 434445. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2639 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maenner, M. J., Warren, Z., Williams, A. R., Amoakohene, E., Bakian, A. V., Bilder, D. A., Durkin, M. S., Fitzgerald, R. T., Furnier, S. M., Hughes, M. M., Ladd-Acosta, C. M., McArthur, D., Pas, E. T., Salinas, A., Vehorn, A., Williams, S., Esler, A., Grzybowski, A., Hall-Lande, J., …Shaw, K. A. (2023). Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2020. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 72(2), 114. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.ss7202a1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mandy, W., Chilvers, R., Chowdhury, U., Salter, G., Seigal, A., & Skuse, D. (2012). Sex differences in autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from a large sample of children and adolescents. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(7), 13041313. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1356-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, W. A., & Tanner, J. M. (1969). Variations in pattern of puberal development in girls. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 44(235), 291303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, W. A., & Tanner, J. M. (1970). Variations in the pattern of pubertal changes in boys. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 45(239), 1323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
May, T., Cornish, K., & Rinehart, N. (2014). Does gender matter? A one year follow-up of autistic, attention and anxiety symptoms in high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(5), 10771086. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1964-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muscatello, R. A., & Corbett, B. A. (2018). Comparing the effects of age, pubertal development, and symptom profile on cortisol rhythm in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 11(1), 110120. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1879 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Negriff, S., Blankson, A. N., & Trickett, P. K. (2015). Pubertal timing and tempo: Associations with childhood maltreatment. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 25(2), 201213. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12128 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Picci, G., & Scherf, K. S. (2015). A two-hit model of autism: Adolescence as the second hit. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(3), 349371. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614540646 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Platje, E., Vermeiren, R. R., Branje, S. J., Doreleijers, T. A., Meeus, W. H., Koot, H. M., Frijns, T., van Lier, P. A., & Jansen, L. M. (2013). Long-term stability of the cortisol awakening response over adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(2), 271280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.06.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, J. C., Gaab, J., Hellhammer, D. H., Lintz, D., Schommer, N., & Kirschbaum, C. (1997). Increasing correlations between personality traits and cortisol stress responses obtained by data aggregation. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 22(8), 615625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rao, U., Hammen, C. L., & Poland, R. E. (2009). Risk markers for depression in adolescents: Sleep and HPA measures. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34(8), 19361945. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.27 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richdale, A. L., Baker, E., Short, M., & Gradisar, M. (2014). The role of insomnia, pre-sleep arousal and psychopathology symptoms in daytime impairment in adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Sleep Medicine, 15(9), 10821088. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2014.05.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richdale, A. L., & Prior, M. R. (1992). Urinary cortisol circadian rhythm in a group of high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22(3), 433447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosmalen, J. G., Oldehinkel, A. J., Ormel, J., de Winter, A. F., Buitelaar, J. K., & Verhulst, F. C. (2005). Determinants of salivary cortisol levels in 10-12 year old children; a population-based study of individual differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30(5), 483495. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.12.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, K. M., Murphy, M. L. M., Adam, E. K., Chen, E., & Miller, G. E. (2014). How stable are diurnal cortisol activity indices in healthy individuals? Evidence from three multi-wave studies. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 39, 184193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.09.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). The Social Communication Questionnaire. Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Schupp, C. W., Simon, D., & Corbett, B. A. (2013). Cortisol responsivity differences in children with autism spectrum disorders during free and cooperative play. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(10), 24052417. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1790-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartzman, J. M., Williams, Z. J., & Corbett, B. A. (2022). Diagnostic- and sex-based differences in depression symptoms in autistic and neurotypical early adolescents. Autism, 26(1), 256269. https://doi.org/10.1177/13623613211025895 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharpley, C. F., Bitsika, V., Andronicos, N. M., & Agnew, L. L. (2016). Further evidence of HPA-axis dysregulation and its correlation with depression in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Data from girls. Physiology and Behavior, 167, 110117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.09.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shirtcliff, E. A., Allison, A. L., Armstrong, J. M., Slattery, M. J., Kalin, N. H., & Essex, M. J. (2012). Longitudinal stability and developmental properties of salivary cortisol levels and circadian rhythms from childhood to adolescence. Developmental Psychobiology, 54(5), 493502. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20607 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(4), 417463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 6974. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, J. L., Adams, R. E., & Bishop, S. L. (2017). Social participation and its relation to internalizing symptoms among youth with autism spectrum disorder as they transition from high school. Autism Research, 10(4), 663672. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1709 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, J. L., & Corbett, B. A. (2014). A review of rhythm and responsiveness of cortisol in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 49, 207228. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.07.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, J. L., & Seltzer, M. M. (2010). Changes in the autism behavioral phenotype during the transition to adulthood. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(12), 14311446. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1005-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomarken, A. J., Han, G. T., & Corbett, B. A. (2015). Temporal patterns, heterogeneity, and stability of diurnal cortisol rhythms in children with autism spectrum disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 62, 217226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomchek, S. D., & Dunn, W. (2007). Sensory processing in children with and without autism: A comparative study using the short sensory profile. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 61(2), 190200. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.61.2.190 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tordjman, S., Anderson, G. M., Kermarrec, S., Bonnot, O., Geoffray, M. M., Brailly-Tabard, S., Chaouch, A., Colliot, I., Trabado, S., Bronsard, G., Coulon, N., Botbol, M., Charbuy, H., Camus, F., & Touitou, Y. (2014). Altered circadian patterns of salivary cortisol in low-functioning children and adolescents with autism. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 50, 227245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.08.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uljarević, M., Cooper, M. N., Bebbington, K., Glasson, E. J., Maybery, M. T., Varcin, K., Alvares, G. A., Wray, J., Leekam, S. R., & Whitehouse, A. J. O. (2020). Deconstructing the repetitive behaviour phenotype in autism spectrum disorder through a large population-based analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(9), 10301042. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13203 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van den Bergh, B. R., & Van Calster, B. (2009). Diurnal cortisol profiles and evening cortisol in post-pubertal adolescents scoring high on the Children’s Depression Inventory. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(5), 791794. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.12.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, E. F., Walder, D. J., & Reynolds, F. (2001). Developmental changes in cortisol secretion in normal and at-risk youth. Development and Psychopathology, 13(3), 721732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2011). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence II (2nd ed.). PsychCorp.Google Scholar
Wilhelm, I., Born, J., Kudielka, B. M., Schlotz, W., & Wust, S. (2007). Is the cortisol awakening rise a response to awakening? Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32(4), 358366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.01.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood-Downie, H., Wong, B., Kovshoff, H., Cortese, S., & Hadwin, J. A. (2021). Research review: A systematic review and meta-analysis of sex/gender differences in social interaction and communication in autistic and nonautistic children and adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 62(8), 922936. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13337 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wust, S., Federenko, I., Hellhammer, D. H., & Kirschbaum, C. (2000). Genetic factors, perceived chronic stress, and the free cortisol response to awakening. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 25(7), 707720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahn-Waxler, C., Shirtcliff, E. A., & Marceau, K. (2008). Disorders of childhood and adolescence: Gender and psychopathology. Annual Reviews of Clinical Psychology, 4(1), 275303. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091358 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zajkowska, Z., Gullett, N., Walsh, A., Zonca, V., Pedersen, G. A., Souza, L., Kieling, C., Fisher, H. L., Kohrt, B. A., & Mondelli, V. (2022). Cortisol and development of depression in adolescence and young adulthood - a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 136, 105625. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105625 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zinke, K., Fries, E., Kliegel, M., Kirschbaum, C., & Dettenborn, L. (2010). Children with high-functioning autism show a normal cortisol awakening response (CAR). Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(10), 15781582. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.03.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics stratified by diagnosis

Figure 1

Figure 1. Mean diurnal cortisol levels over the course of the day. Note. The figures depict raw untransformed or log-transformed diurnal cortisol levels, which unlike the model, do not adjust for covariates nor the within subject correlation.

Figure 2

Table 2. Diagnosis contrasts for aim 1

Figure 3

Figure 2. (a) Observed log cortisol vs. age with smoothing (loess) curve for all 4 periods. (b) Observed log cortisol vs. pubertal stage with a smoothing (loess) curve for all 4 periods.

Figure 4

Table 3. Age effect and age trend contrasts by period for Hyp 2.1

Figure 5

Table 4. Pubertal stage effect & pubertal stage trend contrasts by period for Hyp 2.2

Figure 6

Figure 3. Mean diurnal cortisol levels over the course of the day stratified by sex. Note. The figures depict raw untransformed or log-transformed diurnal cortisol levels, which unlike the model, do not adjust for covariates nor the within subject correlation.

Figure 7

Table 5. Sex effect and sex contrasts for Hyp 3.1

Supplementary material: File

Corbett et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S10

Download Corbett et al. supplementary material(File)
File 37.8 KB