Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T06:13:33.266Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Dualism and the ‘difficult patient’: why integrating neuroscience matters

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 September 2020

Andrew M. Novick*
Affiliation:
MD, PhD, is Assistant Professor of Psychiatry in the Department of Psychiatry at the University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus, Aurora, Colorado, USA.
David A. Ross
Affiliation:
MD, PhD, is Associate Program Director for Adult Psychiatry Training in the Department of Psychiatry at Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, Connecticutt, USA, and co-chair of the National Neuroscience Curriculum Initiative (NNCI).
*
Correspondence Andrew M. Novick. Email: andrew.m.novick@cuanschutz.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Patients with psychiatric illness present a unique challenge to clinicians: in contrast to the traditional medical model, in which patients are conceptualised as being stricken by a disease, patients with certain psychiatric illnesses may seem complicit in the illness. Questions of free will, choice and the role of the physician can cause clinicians to feel helpless, disinterested or even resentful. These tensions are a lasting legacy of centuries of mind–body dualism. Over the past several decades, modern tools have finally allowed us to break down this false dichotomy. Integrating a modern neuroscience perspective into practice allows clinicians to conceptualise individuals with psychiatric illness in a way that promotes empathy and enhances patient care. Specifically, a strong grasp of neuroscience prevents clinicians from falling into the trap in which behavioural aspects of a patient's presentation are perceived as being separate from the disease process. We demonstrate the value of incorporating neuroscience into a biopsychosocial formulation through the example of a ‘difficult patient’.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2020

LEARNING OBJECTIVES

After reading this article you will be able to:

  • appreciate historical factors that have contributed to dualism in psychiatry

  • appreciate the value of an integrative perspective

  • describe how psychological and social factors integrate with a neuroscience perspective in substance use disorders.

Hippocrates is commonly considered the father of modern medicine. His work defined the foundations of the profession: diseases are a natural process, rather than a divine affliction; the role of the physician is to identify the biological cause and to facilitate a corresponding treatment. Across the centuries, an increasing number of medical successes – from antibiotics to life-saving surgeries to modern precision medicine – have all reinforced this approach.

This same model remains foundational to training: from the first day of medical school, students are taught basic science with the promise that they will ultimately apply this knowledge to diagnose and treat their patients. Armed with empathy and optimism, they arrive on the wards, ready to leverage years of training to pursue their goal from the outset – to help suffering patients.

But something happens. A disturbing research finding is that physicians may actually experience a decrease in empathy throughout medical training (Hojat Reference Hojat, Vergare and Maxwell2009). Although the reasons for this are undoubtedly multifactorial, one mechanism may be through their interactions with a subset of complex – or ‘difficult’ – patients.

Take, for instance, Mr A, a 39-year-old man with a history of intravenous heroin use, admitted to the internal medicine service for the treatment of infective endocarditis. Mr A is well known to the residents and attending physicians (trainee doctors and consultants) on the service owing to his frequent admissions, demands for pain medication, verbal outbursts and tendency to leave against medical advice prior to completing his course of intravenous antibiotics. Physicians describe Mr A's complaints of radiating neck pain, attributed to an old motor vehicle accident, as ‘lacking an identifiable organic source’. His uncooperative attitude is frequently documented as ‘manipulative’ or ‘indicative of a pathological temperament’. Not surprisingly, the medical staff feel frustrated with their inability to help Mr A, to the point where they often question the amount of resources that have been dedicated to someone who continues to make such self-destructive choices. Behind closed doors, they may disparagingly mutter that ‘this is all in his head’.

The third-year medical student rotating on the service may feel stuck. On one hand, Mr A's infectious and immunobiological problems will align well with the student's training. But on the other, the patient's affective and behavioural dysregulation may be baffling. Because these problems are ‘lacking an identifiable organic source’ they do not readily fit within the basic framework of the medical model – and, accordingly, they may strand the student outside the traditional role of doctor, leaving them feeling helpless and frustrated.

Haunted by Descartes’ ghost

To understand why something like a substance use disorder and its sequelae are so easily relegated to ‘lacking an identifiable organic source’, it is helpful to look back to 17th-century philosophy and, specifically, to the work of René Descartes. Descartes promoted the idea that mind and body were separate entities and that mental activity arose from a substance distinct from the body (Descartes Reference Descartes and Cottingham1641). This idea is now referred to as ‘Cartesian dualism’ (in contrast to more modern perspectives on the problem (Maung Reference Maung2019)). Although Descartes had his contemporary detractors, his broad influence as it applied to medicine would go largely unchallenged for the next 250 years.

Freud and the psychoanalytic approach

In the 19th century, new ideas about natural selection and conservation of energy often seemed incompatible with Cartesian dualism. This inspired a new generation of researchers – from Charcot to Broca to Babinski to Kraepelin – to try to bridge the mind–body divide (Smith Reference Smith2017). One such individual was the young neurologist Sigmund Freud. With training in both neuroanatomy and neurophysiology, Freud seemed to be in a perfect position to integrate the biological with the psychological (Freud Reference Freud, Bonaparte, Freud, Bonaparte, Freud, Kris, Mosbacher and Strachey1954 reprint). But when the scientific tools of his day proved inadequate for such an undertaking, Freud's work shifted to exploring psychological models in greater depth (Northoff Reference Northoff2012). His work developing psychotherapy and psychoanalysis set in motion a split – brain from mind – that, following the advent of electroencephalography (EEG), would ultimately lead to the separation of neurology and psychiatry (Eloge Reference Eloge, Ross and Cooper2020). Ironically, for someone who set out to integrate the field, he may have inadvertently exacerbated the severe dualism that would become cemented in psychiatry for another 100 years.

Advances in psychopharmacology

Psychoanalytic thought largely dominated the field until the 1950s, when psychopharmacology took the world by storm. One might have hoped that these new tools could have been integrated into a unified psychiatry – psychodynamic theory and biology side by side – but sadly, it proved to be a wedge.

On the biological side, Schildkraut (Reference Schildkraut1965) and van Rossum (Reference van Rossum1966) put forth their hypotheses that affective and psychotic disorders were related to aberrant levels of neurotransmitters that could occur independently of a psychological or environmental aetiology. Advances in pharmacological science allowed both Schildkraut and van Rossum to extrapolate knowledge of drug mechanisms into hypotheses of disease mechanisms. If imipramine could treat depression by increasing catecholamines, and reserpine could exacerbate depression by depleting catecholamines, then it followed that depression was a disease of catecholamine deficit. Psychiatry had finally identified a set of biological targets: this ushered in a golden age of research in which drugs were not only treatments but tools to investigate the mysteries of the brain.

R.D. Laing and the challenge to the ‘medical model’

But not everyone was impressed. The British psychiatrist R.D. Laing had little interest in catecholamines. He, along with many other experts of the time, believed that psychiatric illnesses were not biologically driven processes; rather, they were emergent properties of complex psychological, social and family dynamics (this was at the same time as the ‘schizophrenogenic mother’ model) (Laing Reference Laing1960). For these reasons, he did not believe that psychiatrists should adhere to the standard medical process of eliciting a chief complaint, conducting a history and examination, testing, diagnosis and treatment. In fact, Laing ultimately coined the term ‘medical model’ in order to distinguish the work of psychiatrists from that of the rest of medicine (Laing Reference Laing1971). Looking back, this is an astonishing moment in the history of our field – perhaps the height of the division between physicians of the ‘mind’ and physicians of the ‘brain’.

The biopsychosocial model and modern neuroscience

In the wake of this fractious debate, George Engel offered a strong corrective: the biopsychosocial model (Engel Reference Engel1977). Rather than splitting and separating, the framework acknowledges and embraces the interconnectivity between a person's illness, their social situation and their behaviour. On one hand, Engel's solution was the definition of scientific elegance: both simple and applicable. But it also served as a lightning rod for criticism (Ghaemi Reference Ghaemi2009) – for being overly reductionistic or, paradoxically, for unduly emphasising the separateness of each perspective. The problem stemmed from a lack of mechanistic understanding – it's all well and good to say that the different perspectives are intertwined, but the devil is in the details – how does it actually work? Until recently, these questions were unanswerable. It was a model before its time.

Modern neuroscience is increasingly providing the crucial data to bridge this divide and to demonstrate the nuanced ways in which bio-, psycho- and social processes are all mediated through the brain. For example, a large body of literature has demonstrated the ability of psychotherapy to alter brain activity in a way that promotes improved emotion regulation (Miller Reference Miller, Ross and Novick2020). Early social experiences, such as a history of abuse and/or neglect, can blunt the brain's response to rewards in the environment (such as money or social praise), while increasing sensitivity to drugs of abuse (Oswald Reference Oswald, Wand and Kuwabara2014; Novick Reference Novick, Levandowski and Laumann2018). A burgeoning literature is illustrating that one key mechanism through which experiences influence brain function is alterations in gene expression, otherwise known as epigenetics. This was elegantly demonstrated by Michael Meaney's research group, who showed that variations in maternal care in rodents altered methylation of key genes related to the stress response and anxiety-like behaviour (Weaver Reference Weaver, Cervoni and Champagne2004). As concluded by Eric Kandel, winner of the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 2000 for describing how learning is dependent on changes in gene expression in neurons, ‘all of “nurture” is ultimately expressed as “nature”’ (Kandel Reference Kandel1998).

The Modern Enlightenment

The Age of Enlightenment was thought to have begun with Descartes. It is ironic, then, that one of his most influential ideas has pervaded medicine in a way that prevents us from understanding patients in a scientific manner. The true challenge for clinicians today is to be able to thoughtfully integrate a patient's experience beyond vital signs and crude diagnostic tests. For example, coming back to the case of Mr A it would be interesting to learn more about his family and early life experiences. Heritability of substance use disorders can be as high as 70% (Bevilacqua Reference Bevilacqua and Goldman2009). Thus, while environmental factors obviously play a role, it is crucial to keep in mind that these disorders may have a stronger genetic component than breast cancer (Möller Reference Möller, Mucci and Harris2016), coronary artery disease (Dai Reference Dai, Wiernek and Evans2016), type 1 diabetes mellitus (Cerolsaletti Reference Cerolsaletti, Hao and Greenbaum2019) and most other medical illnesses. As described above, a history of childhood adversity is associated with brain changes that can increase vulnerability to addiction. The prominent role of both genetics and early life experiences should challenge our instinctive tendency to view addiction as a choice. Careful investigation into his pain might reveal that Mr A's previous injury led to long-term plasticity in his nervous system that has resulted in chronic pain (Baller Reference Baller and Ross2017). Mr A's use of exogenous opioids may also have created a state of opioid-induced hyperalgesia via increased release of excitatory neurotransmitters (Bommersbach Reference Bommersbach, Ross and De Aquino2020). The ability of opioids to directly suppress the immune system may also contribute to his current infection (Plein Reference Plein and Rittner2018).

Approaching Mr A's case from a fully integrated biopsychosocial perspective will not necessarily cure his addiction or prevent him from leaving the hospital against medical advice. However, incorporating neuroscience into the formulation allows his clinicians to appreciate that his illness is a medical condition that is within their scope of practice. In turn, this understanding serves to invite empathy, to promote continued inquiry and discourse, and it leaves less room for helplessness and resentment. Lastly, it may allow psychiatrists to advocate for their patients by reminding their colleagues that, while some patients’ symptoms may be ‘in their head’, it is a head that houses the body's most complex organ.

Acknowledgement

This work was created as part of the authors’ affiliation with the National Neuroscience Curriculum Initiative (NNCI). The NNCI (http://www.nncionline.org) aims to create, pilot and disseminate a comprehensive set of shared resources that will help train psychiatrists and other mental health professionals to integrate a modern neuroscience perspective into every facet of their clinical work.

Author contributions

The authors contributed equally to the conception, writing and final submission of this article.

Funding

A.M.N. receives support from the Center for Women's Health Research at the University of Colorado as well as National Institutes of Health grant no. U54 AG062319 (PI Kohrt). D.A.R. is co-chair of the National Neuroscience Curriculum Initiative (NNCI), which is supported by the National Institutes of Health grants nos. R25 MH08646607S1 and R44 MH115546-01.

Declaration of interest

None.

ICMJE forms are in the supplementary material, available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bja.2020.60.

MCQs

Select the single best option for each question stem

  1. 1 Cartesian dualism refers to:

    1. a a mathematical theorem used in computational psychiatry

    2. b the idea that biological, psychological and social elements of an individual's illness are interconnected

    3. c a phrase coined by Freud to describe opposing influences on behaviour

    4. d the idea that mind and body are separate entities, made of distinct substances

    5. e a neurobiological concept that explains paradoxical responses to opioid receptor agonism based on drug dose.

  2. 2 Biological models in psychiatry that emerged in the 1960s were predicated on:

    1. a comparisons of patients who were and were not responsive to psychoanalysis

    2. b research in the neuroanatomy and neuropathology of individuals with psychiatric disorders

    3. c the observation that both insulin and electroconvulsive therapy could induce seizures

    4. d extrapolating pathophysiology of illness from the mechanisms of drugs that treated the illness

    5. e the application of Mendelian genetic principles.

  3. 3 The origin of the term ‘medical model’ can be traced to:

    1. a R.D. Laing's attempt to distinguish the methods that physicians use in psychiatry from those used in the rest of medicine

    2. b Hippocrates’ work separating the practice of medicine from theology

    3. c William Osler's The Principles and Practice of Medicine

    4. d George Engel's ‘The need for a new medical model’

    5. e Eric Kandel's research that resulted in his Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine.

  4. 4 A formative step towards a modern integrative approach to psychiatry can be attributed to:

    1. a Descartes’ ideas on the relationship between mind and body

    2. b Laing's work on the role of family and social determinants of schizophrenia

    3. c Meaney's research on epigenetics

    4. d Schildkraut's catecholamine hypothesis of affective disorder

    5. e Engel's biopsychosocial model.

  5. 5 As regards the neurobiology of substance use and chronic pain:

    1. a the development of chronic pain following injury is due to long-term potentiation in pain circuits

    2. b opioid-induced hyperalgesia develops in response to excess GABA inhibition of opioid-receptor-containing neurons

    3. c variability in the propensity to develop opioid use disorder is mainly due to access to substances and socioeconomic variables rather than genetics

    4. d early life stress results in changes to the dopamine system that decrease sensitivity to cocaine while increasing sensitivity to opioids

    5. e opioid-induced increases in immune activity can result in chronic pain and inflammation.

MCQ answers

1 d 2 d 3 a 4 e 5 a

Footnotes

A podcast linked to this article is available at https://soundcloud.com/bjpsych/bja-2020-60

References

Baller, EB, Ross, DA (2017) Your system has been hijacked: the neurobiology of chronic pain. Biological Psychiatry, 82(8): e61–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bevilacqua, L, Goldman, D (2009) Genes and addictions. Clinical Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 85: 359–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bommersbach, T, Ross, DA, De Aquino, JP (2020) Perpetual hunger: the neurobiological consequences of long-term opioid use. Biological Psychiatry, 87(1): e13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cerolsaletti, K, Hao, W, Greenbaum, CJ (2019) Genetics coming of age in type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care, 42: 189–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dai, X, Wiernek, S, Evans, JP, et al. (2016) Genetics of coronary artery disease and myocardial infarction. World Journal of Cardiology, 8: 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Descartes, R (1641) Meditationes de Prima Philosophia. Reprinted (2017) as Descartes: Meditations on First Philosophy. With Selections from the Objections and Replies (2nd edn) (trans & ed Cottingham, J). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eloge, JC, Ross, DA, Cooper, JJ (2020) Afflicted by the gods: the shared history and neurobiology of psychosis and epilepsy. Biological Psychiatry, 87(12): e35–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engel, GL (1977) The need for a new medical model: a challenge for biomedicine. Science, 196: 129–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freud, S, & Bonaparte, M, Freud, A, et al. (1954) Project for a scientific psychology. In The Origins of Psycho-Analysis: Letters to Wilhelm Fliess, Drafts and Notes: 1887–1902 (eds Bonaparte, M, Freud, A, Kris, E; trans Mosbacher, E, Strachey, J): 347445. Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghaemi, SN (2009) The rise and fall of the biopsychosocial model. British Journal of Psychiatry, 195: 34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hojat, M, Vergare, MJ, Maxwell, K, et al. (2009) The devil is in the third year: a longitudinal study of erosion of empathy in medical school. Academic Medicine, 84: 1182–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kandel, ER (1998) A new intellectual framework for psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155: 457–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laing, RD (1960) The Divided Self: An Existential Study in Sanity and Madness. Tavistock.Google Scholar
Laing, RD (1971) The Politics of the Family and Other Essays. Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Maung, HH (2019) Dualism and its place in a philosophical structure for psychiatry. Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy, 22: 5969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, CWT, Ross, DA, Novick, AM (2020) “Not Dead Yet!” – confronting the legacy of dualism in modern psychiatry. Biological Psychiatry, 87(7): e15–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Möller, S, Mucci, LA, Harris, JR, et al. (2016) The heritability of breast cancer among women in the nordic twin study of cancer. Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention, 25: 145–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northoff, G (2012) Psychoanalysis and the brain – why did Freud abandon neuroscience? Frontiers in Psychology, 3: 71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Novick, AM, Levandowski, ML, Laumann, LE, et al. (2018) The effects of early life stress on reward processing. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 101: 80103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oswald, LM, Wand, GS, Kuwabara, H, et al. (2014) History of childhood adversity is positively associated with ventral striatal dopamine responses to amphetamine. Psychopharmacology, 231: 2417–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plein, LM, Rittner, HL (2018) Opioids and the immune system – friend or foe. British Journal of Pharmacology, 175: 2717–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schildkraut, J (1965) The catecholamine hypothesis of affective disorders: A review of supporting evidence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 122: 509522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, D (2017) Freud the Philosopher [From Philosophy to Psychoanalysis: A Classic Freudian Move]. Aeon (https://aeon.co/essays/from-philosophy-to-psychoanalysis-a-classic-freudian-move).Google Scholar
van Rossum, J (1966) The significance of dopamine-receptor blockade for the mechanism of action of neuroleptic drugs. Archives Internationales De Pharmacodynamie Et De Therapie, 160: 492494.Google ScholarPubMed
Weaver, ICG, Cervoni, N, Champagne, FA, et al. (2004) Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 7: 847–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: PDF

Novick and Ross supplementary material

Novick and Ross supplementary material 1

Download Novick and Ross supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.3 MB
Supplementary material: PDF

Novick and Ross supplementary material

Novick and Ross supplementary material 2

Download Novick and Ross supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.3 MB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.