Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-d8cs5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T01:37:27.683Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A generalized reward processing deficit pathway to negative symptoms across diagnostic boundaries

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2025

Michael J. Spilka
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
Zachary B. Millman
Affiliation:
Psychotic Disorders Division, McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA, USA Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
James A. Waltz
Affiliation:
Maryland Psychiatric Research Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Maryland School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
Elaine F. Walker
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
Jason A. Levin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
Albert R. Powers
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
Philip R. Corlett
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
Jason Schiffman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Science, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA
James M. Gold
Affiliation:
Maryland Psychiatric Research Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Maryland School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
Steven M. Silverstein
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry, Neuroscience and Ophthalmology, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY, USA
Lauren M. Ellman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
Vijay A. Mittal
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA
Scott W. Woods
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
Richard Zinbarg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA
Gregory P. Strauss*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Gregory P. Strauss; Email: gstrauss@uga.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Negative symptoms are a key feature of several psychiatric disorders. Difficulty identifying common neurobiological mechanisms that cut across diagnostic boundaries might result from equifinality (i.e., multiple mechanistic pathways to the same clinical profile), both within and across disorders. This study used a data-driven approach to identify unique subgroups of participants with distinct reward processing profiles to determine which profiles predicted negative symptoms.

Methods

Participants were a transdiagnostic sample of youth from a multisite study of psychosis risk, including 110 individuals at clinical high-risk for psychosis (CHR; meeting psychosis-risk syndrome criteria), 88 help-seeking participants who failed to meet CHR criteria and/or who presented with other psychiatric diagnoses, and a reference group of 66 healthy controls. Participants completed clinical interviews and behavioral tasks assessing four reward processing constructs indexed by the RDoC Positive Valence Systems: hedonic reactivity, reinforcement learning, value representation, and effort–cost computation.

Results

k-means cluster analysis of clinical participants identified three subgroups with distinct reward processing profiles, primarily characterized by: a value representation deficit (54%), a generalized reward processing deficit (17%), and a hedonic reactivity deficit (29%). Clusters did not differ in rates of clinical group membership or psychiatric diagnoses. Elevated negative symptoms were only present in the generalized deficit cluster, which also displayed greater functional impairment and higher psychosis conversion probability scores.

Conclusions

Contrary to the equifinality hypothesis, results suggested one global reward processing deficit pathway to negative symptoms independent of diagnostic classification. Assessment of reward processing profiles may have utility for individualized clinical prediction and treatment.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Negative symptoms, though historically conceptualized within psychotic disorders, are now recognized to have a transdiagnostic presentation within a wide range of psychiatric illnesses, including mood, anxiety, and trauma-related syndromes (Strauss & Cohen, Reference Strauss and Cohen2017). Transdiagnostic studies of the neurobehavioral mechanisms thought to give rise to these phenomena have the potential to reveal treatment targets across diagnostic categories. Such approaches may be especially valuable in the early and high-risk stages of psychopathology, when neurodevelopment is still underway, pathogenic mechanisms are presumably forming, and secondary prevention of adverse clinical outcomes is possible (McGorry et al., Reference Mcgorry, Keshavan, Goldstone, Amminger, Allott, Berk and Hickie2014). Growing evidence indicates that youth at clinical high risk (CHR) for psychosis present with abnormalities in laboratory measures of reward processing, and these abnormalities are often associated with negative symptom severity (Bartolomeo, Chapman, Raugh, & Strauss, Reference Bartolomeo, Chapman, Raugh and Strauss2021; Strauss, Bartolomeo, & Luther, Reference Strauss, Bartolomeo and Luther2023). However, the extent to which reward-related behavioral alterations in youth at CHR diverge or overlap with those seen among help-seeking peers not at CHR for psychosis remains unknown, representing a key limitation in our understanding of negative symptom etiology (Millman, Gold, Mittal, & Schiffman, Reference Millman, Gold, Mittal and Schiffman2019; Strauss & Cohen, Reference Strauss and Cohen2017).

The National Institute of Mental Health’s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) provides a useful framework for studying negative symptoms across diagnostic categories, where the Positive Valence Systems domain includes dissociable aspects of reward processing that contribute to motivated behavior (Cuthbert, Reference Cuthbert2014). For example, individuals with schizophrenia, depression, and bipolar disorder often show qualitatively similar alterations in reward valuation, including reduced willingness to exert effort for rewards (both cognitive and physical; Culbreth, Moran, & Barch, Reference Culbreth, Moran and Barch2018; Hershenberg et al., Reference Hershenberg, Satterthwaite, Daldal, Katchmar, Moore, Kable and Wolf2016; Zou et al., Reference Zou, Ni, Wang, Yu, Lui, Zhou and Chan2020), and greater discounting of the value of delayed versus immediate rewards (Amlung et al., Reference Amlung, Marsden, Holshausen, Morris, Patel, Vedelago and Mccabe2019). However, accumulating evidence within this framework has also highlighted heterogeneity in reward processing dysfunction across diagnostic boundaries (e.g., hedonic reactivity is altered in major depressive disorder and spared in schizophrenia; explicit reinforcement learning is relatively impaired in schizophrenia and spared in major depression; implicit reinforcement learning is relatively impaired in depression and spared in schizophrenia; Barch et al., Reference Barch, Carter, Gold, Johnson, Kring, Macdonald and Strauss2017; Bylsma, Morris, & Rottenberg, Reference Bylsma, Morris and Rottenberg2008; Cohen & Minor, Reference Cohen and Minor2010; Gold et al., Reference Gold, Waltz, Matveeva, Kasanova, Strauss, Herbener and Frank2012; Whitmer, Frank, & Gotlib, Reference Whitmer, Frank and Gotlib2012). Anxiety, trauma, substance use, and obsessive-compulsive disorders are also associated with overlapping and divergent patterns of reward-processing behavior (Amlung et al., Reference Amlung, Marsden, Holshausen, Morris, Patel, Vedelago and Mccabe2019; Bishop & Gagne, Reference Bishop and Gagne2018; Kanen, Ersche, Fineberg, Robbins, & Cardinal, Reference Kanen, Ersche, Fineberg, Robbins and Cardinal2019; Weaver et al., Reference Weaver, Kroska, Ross, Sartin-Tarm, Sellnow, Schaumberg and Cisler2020). The noted heterogeneity in reward processing dysfunction across disorders suggests that there may be equifinality (i.e., multiple mechanistic pathways to a given clinical outcome) in negative symptoms despite their transdiagnostic presentation (Nusslock & Alloy, Reference Nusslock and Alloy2017; Strauss & Cohen, Reference Strauss and Cohen2017; Whitton, Treadway, & Pizzagalli, Reference Whitton, Treadway and Pizzagalli2015).

Among youth at CHR, evidence to date suggests that the profile of reward-related abnormalities may overlap with both psychotic (e.g., altered reward learning, reduced effort expenditure) and mood (e.g., diminished hedonic reactivity, reduced effort expenditure) disorders (Gruber, Strauss, Dombrecht, & Mittal, Reference Gruber, Strauss, Dombrecht and Mittal2018; Millman et al., Reference Millman, Gallagher, Demro, Schiffman, Reeves, Gold and Redman2020; Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Nunez, Ahmed, Barchard, Granholm, Kirkpatrick and Allen2018; Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Bartolomeo and Luther2023) while also possessing distinct areas of intact performance (e.g., intact delay discounting; Bartolomeo et al., Reference Bartolomeo, Chapman, Raugh and Strauss2021). The emerging picture that youth at CHR present with reward-processing dysfunction that shows both overlap and divergence with other psychiatric syndromes suggests the possibility that equifinality might also occur within clinical syndromes and diagnostic categories. Therefore, clinically based comparisons of multiple diagnostic groups may be limited in their ability to capture the full range of mechanistic heterogeneity underlying negative symptoms.

Data-driven stratification approaches based on multiple measures of reward-related behavior may hold promise in parsing mechanistic heterogeneity by identifying subgroups with more homogenous reward-processing profiles, with the potential to inform etiological models and facilitate individualized treatments (Marquand, Wolfers, Mennes, Buitelaar, & Beckmann, Reference Marquand, Wolfers, Mennes, Buitelaar and Beckmann2016; Mittal, Walker, & Strauss, Reference Mittal, Walker and Strauss2021). Findings from such approaches may be particularly generalizable when drawn from transdiagnostic samples of help-seeking youth, including youth at CHR as well as those with common nonpsychotic disorders (Millman et al., Reference Millman, Gold, Mittal and Schiffman2019). Although recent work has demonstrated the utility of similar strategies using neuroanatomical and clinical data in CHR and clinical comparison groups (Dean, Walther, Bernard, & Mittal, Reference Dean, Walther, Bernard and Mittal2018; Dwyer et al., Reference Dwyer, Buciuman, Ruef, Kambeitz, Sen Dong and Stinson2022; Gupta, Cowan, Strauss, Walker, & Mittal, Reference Gupta, Cowan, Strauss, Walker and Mittal2021; Healey et al., Reference Healey, Penn, Perkins, Woods, Keefe and Addington2018; Koutsouleris et al., Reference Koutsouleris, Dwyer, Degenhardt, Maj, Urquijo-Castro and Sanfelici2021; Millman et al., Reference Millman, Roemer, Vargas, Schiffman, Mittal and Gold2022; Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Addington, Bearden, Cadenhead, Cornblatt, Mathalon and Walker2018), little research has sought to identify behavioral profiles of reward processing in transdiagnostic high-risk samples. To our knowledge, this approach has only been applied to a sample that additionally included participants with more serious mental illness (e.g., schizophrenia). Luther and colleagues (Luther, Jarvis, Spilka, & Strauss, Reference Luther, Jarvis, Spilka and Strauss2024) used cluster analysis to identify three distinct profiles of reward-processing task performance in a combined sample of participants with CHR, schizophrenia, schizoaffective, bipolar disorder, and psychotic-like experiences. The reward-processing profiles consisted of clusters of participants with either preserved performance across reward processing domains, isolated hedonic reactivity deficits, or a more global deficit spanning across all domains. Only the global deficit cluster demonstrated elevated negative symptoms, suggesting the presence of a single transdiagnostic and transphasic pathway to negative symptoms. However, although CHR participants were equally represented across identified clusters, the inclusion of participants with a severe mental illness diagnosis in addition to a sample of youth at CHR may have influenced the observed cluster-level negative symptom severity scores and masked more subtle variation in negative symptom patterns in the CHR group. Therefore, additional research focusing on high-risk samples is needed to clarify negative symptom pathways in the at-risk phase of illness.

The goal of this study was to use a data-driven approach to identify unique subgroups of clinically diverse youth from their performance on tasks assessing four aspects of reward processing believed to contribute to negative symptoms (Strauss & Cohen, Reference Strauss and Cohen2017; Strauss, Waltz, & Gold, Reference Strauss, Waltz and Gold2014) and consistent with the RDoC Positive Valence Systems constructs: hedonic reactivity (initial response to reward), reinforcement learning (probabilistic reinforcement learning), reward valuation (delay discounting), and effort–cost computation (effort valuation). Distinct participant clusters were then compared on external validators not included in the cluster analysis to determine whether clusters displayed distinct profiles of clinical and demographic features. We hypothesized that equifinality would be present in the mechanistic pathways to negative symptoms, both across and within clinical groups, as indicated by distinct reward processing profiles among clusters that show clinically significant elevations in negative symptoms but do not differ in the proportion of CHR versus other clinical cases.

Methods and materials

Participants

Participants were 282 individuals 15–34 years of age from the Computerized Assessment of Psychosis Risk (CAPR) study conducted across several university research sites in the USA (Mittal et al., Reference Mittal, Ellman, Strauss, Walker, Corlett, Schiffman and Gold2021). Participants belonged to one of three groups: (1) 120 individuals at CHR for psychosis, who met progressive or persistent psychosis-risk syndrome criteria on the Structured Interview for Psychosis-Risk Syndromes (SIPS; McGlashan et al., Reference Mcglashan, Walsh, Woods, Addington, Cadenhead, Cannon and Walker2001); (2) 91 help-seeking clinical comparison participants (HSC) who were referred/self-referred for psychosis-risk symptoms and/or had at least one current DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) nonpsychotic disorder diagnosis but did not meet criteria for a SIPS psychosis-risk syndrome; and (3) 71 healthy control (HC) participants without any current or lifetime DSM-5 psychiatric diagnoses. CHR and HSC participants were recruited through targeted online and print advertisements, email, and through contact with mental health providers in hospitals, community mental health centers, and school settings. HC participants were recruited through online and print advertisements, and email.

See the Supplementary Materials and Mittal, Ellman, et al. (Reference Mittal, Ellman, Strauss, Walker, Corlett, Schiffman and Gold2021) for detailed participant eligibility criteria, recruitment, and testing procedures.

All participants provided written consent and/or assent for a single-site protocol approved by the Northwestern University Institutional Review Board.

Measures

A series of clinical interviews, questionnaires, cognitive tests, and computerized experimental tasks were completed online via videoconference and administered by trained research staff.

Eligibility and group membership were confirmed via the SIPS and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (SCID; First et al., Reference First, Williams, Karg and Spitzer2016). Negative symptom severity in CHR and HSC participants was assessed with the Negative Symptom Inventory – Psychosis Risk (NSI-PR; Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Walker, Pelletier-Baldelli, Carter, Ellman, Schiffman and Mittal2023), a clinician-rated scale designed to evaluate negative symptom severity in CHR populations. Additional measures are listed in the Supplementary Materials.

Experimental tasks

Participants completed an online battery of experimental tasks assessing distinct aspects of reward processing that are believed to contribute to negative symptoms (Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Waltz and Gold2014; Strauss & Cohen, Reference Strauss and Cohen2017): (1) a hedonic reactivity task requiring self-report ratings of positive valence in response to pleasant images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, Reference Lang, Bradley and Cuthbert1997); (2) a probabilistic reinforcement learning task (PRLT; Gold et al., Reference Gold, Waltz, Matveeva, Kasanova, Strauss, Herbener and Frank2012) assessing explicit learning from gains and losses; (3) a value representation task assessing the subjective value of a hypothetical future reward as a function of its magnitude and delay in delivery (delay discounting task; Kirby, Petry, & Bickel, Reference Kirby, Petry and Bickel1999); and (4) an effort–cost computation task in which participants had to choose between completing a lower effort task for a smaller reward or a higher effort task for a larger reward (effort expenditure for reward task; EEfRT; Treadway et al., Reference Treadway, Buckholtz, Cowan, Woodward, Li, Ansari and Zald2012). Task paradigms assessing these reward-processing constructs were selected based on prior evidence of performance deficits in CHR and/or schizophrenia samples (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Carter, Gold, Johnson, Kring, Macdonald and Strauss2017; Bartolomeo et al., Reference Bartolomeo, Chapman, Raugh and Strauss2021; Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Bartolomeo and Luther2023; Strauss, Ruiz, Visser, Crespo, & Dickinson, Reference Strauss, Ruiz, Visser, Crespo and Dickinson2018). For the reinforcement learning and effort–cost computation tasks, participants in each group were randomized to task versions using either points-based or monetary rewards, and performance differences between task versions were examined to rule out systematic differences (described below). See the Supplementary Materials for a description of each task and the computation of dependent variables.

Data processing and sample characteristics

Data for each task were inspected to identify and exclude corrupted data or participants with inappropriate response patterns, leading to the exclusion of 10 CHR, 3 HSC, and 5 HC participants (see Supplementary Materials). Only participants with complete data across the four tasks were retained, resulting in a final sample of 110 CHR, 88 HSC, and 66 HC participants (see Table 1 for group characteristics). The HSC group on average had significantly higher years of completed education than the CHR group, but groups otherwise had similar demographic characteristics. Current DSM-5 diagnoses were present in a majority of CHR and HSC participants; however, clinical severity was greater in the CHR than HSC group across several diagnoses and clinical measures (Table 1).

Table 1. Participant sample

Note. Values reflect mean (standard deviation) unless otherwise indicated. Statistical comparisons for demographic variables and task performance were performed across all groups. Statistical comparisons for clinical variables were performed for clinical high-risk and help-seeking control groups.

BACS = Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia; CES-D = Center for Epidemiologic Studies – Depression Scale; EEfRT = Effort Expenditure for Rewards Task; HVLT = Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; IAPS = International Affective Picture System; NSI-PR = Negative Symptom Inventory – Psychosis Risk; PSS = Perceived Stress Scale; PRLT = Probabilistic Reinforcement Learning Task; SIPS = Structured Interview for Psychosis-Risk Syndromes; STAI = State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; WRAT-4 = Wide-Range Achievement Test – 4.

a Proxy primary negative symptom score is the scaled difference score of N1 + N2 + N3 - G2 - G4 SIPS items.

b Reported values are log-transformed.

c Percentage of hard task choices for trials in the upper half of the reward magnitude range and in the upper (50%–88%) reward probability range.

d Percentage of correct choices on the most frequently rewarded gain stimulus pairing across learning trials.

Cluster analysis

In order to facilitate interpretations of task performance deficits, particularly in the context of participants completing the tasks in a remotely administered format and during the course of the COVID-19 pandemic, CHR and HSC task performance scores were standardized relative to those of the HC group prior to analysis, which served as a normative sample to evaluate performance (delay discounting signs were also reversed to facilitate interpretation). The standardized scores for CHR and HSC participants were then subjected to a cluster analysis using k-means clustering (Steinley, Reference Steinley2006), with squared Euclidean distance as the dissimilarity measure (starting configurations = 50, maximum iterations = 100). As a verification step, a similar cluster analysis was performed using the nonreferenced CHR and HSC performance scores (i.e., without referencing performance to the HC group prior to analysis).

The optimal number of clusters was determined by evaluating agreement across 24 quantitative clustering indices (NbClust package; Charrad, Ghazzali, Boiteau, & Niknafs, Reference Charrad, Ghazzali, Boiteau and Niknafs2014), with the cluster range constrained between 2 and 10. The robustness and stability of the clustering solution were evaluated through a nonparametric bootstrapping cluster analysis procedure consisting of resampling the dataset (random resampling with replacement), repeating the cluster analysis on the resampled data, computing Jaccard cluster similarity scores (representing the proportion of cases similarly clustered together in the original and resampled analysis), and repeating this procedure over 1000 iterations to evaluate the resulting overall cluster similarity scores. Cluster separation was confirmed using linear discriminant analysis. See the Supplementary Materials for additional analysis details. Similar cluster analysis approaches have successfully been used to characterize heterogeneity in CHR (Dean et al., Reference Dean, Walther, Bernard and Mittal2018; Gupta, Cowan, et al., Reference Gupta, Cowan, Strauss, Walker and Mittal2021) and schizophrenia samples (Paul et al., Reference Paul, Strauss, Woodyatt, Paul, Keene and Allen2022; Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Horan, Kirkpatrick, Fischer, Keller, Miski and Carpenter2013; Strauss & Herbener, Reference Strauss and Herbener2011).

Interpretations of the cluster solution were confirmed with a 3 cluster × 4 construct mixed ANOVA. Differences in clinical and demographic profiles among cluster subgroups were then examined using one-way ANOVA or Pearson’s chi-squared tests (see Supplementary Materials for additional statistical details). Variables related to the study hypothesis of equifinality in negative symptoms were clinical group membership (CHR versus HSC) and negative symptoms (the five individual NSI-PR symptom domains and two broad dimensions; (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Strauss, Ahmed, Wong, Chan, Lee and Chen2021; Strauss, Ahmed, Young, & Kirkpatrick, Reference Strauss, Ahmed, Young and Kirkpatrick2019), with statistical significance across negative symptom variables corrected for multiple comparisons using the false-discovery rate (FDR correction; Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995). Several exploratory variables were examined to further characterize the resulting cluster subgroups. Exploratory clinical variables were positive symptoms (SIPS positive symptom total); self-reported severity of depression (CES-D; Radloff, Reference Radloff1977), perceived stress (PSS; S. Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, Reference Cohen, Kamarck and Mermelstein1983), and trait anxiety (STAI; Bieling, Antony, & Swinson, Reference Bieling, Antony and Swinson1998); social and role functioning (GF:S and GF:R; Cornblatt et al., Reference Cornblatt, Auther, Niendam, Smith, Zinberg, Bearden and Cannon2007); NAPLS 1- and 2-year psychosis risk scores (CHR participants only; Cannon et al., Reference Cannon, Yu, Addington, Bearden, Cadenhead, Cornblatt and Kattan2016); a proxy score indexing putative primary (versus secondary; Goetz et al., Reference Goetz, Corcoran, Yale, Stanford, Kimhy, Amador and Malaspina2007; Kirkpatrick, Mucci, & Galderisi, Reference Kirkpatrick, Mucci and Galderisi2017; Kirschner, Aleman, & Kaiser, Reference Kirschner, Aleman and Kaiser2017; Tran et al., Reference Tran, Spilka, Raugh, Strauss, Bearden, Cadenhead and Addington2023) negative symptomatology (calculated as the discrepancy between negative and affective symptoms)Footnote 1; and prevalence of DSM-5 diagnoses. Exploratory demographic variables were sex at birth, race/ethnicity, age, and education. Exploratory cognitive variables were estimated premorbid functioning (Wilkinson & Robertson, Reference Wilkinson and Robertson1993), verbal learning (HVLT; Benedict, Schretlen, Groninger, & Brandt, Reference Benedict, Schretlen, Groninger and Brandt1998), and processing speed (BACS Symbol Coding; Keefe et al., Reference Keefe, Goldberg, Harvey, Gold, Poe and Coughenour2004). Statistical significance for ANOVAs of exploratory variables was p < 0.05 uncorrected, with follow-up pairwise comparisons evaluated at p < 0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons across cluster groups.

Because half the participants in each group completed versions of the effort–cost computation and reinforcement learning tasks that used points instead of a monetary reward, supplemental analyses examined the influence of task version on performance across groups and across cluster subgroups (see Supplementary Materials).

Results

Cluster analysis

A three-cluster solution was selected, as it was favored by the plurality (11/24) of examined clustering indices, with strong support relative to the other evaluated cluster solutions (i.e., the next most-favored solution was only recommended by four clustering indices) (see Table S1 and Figure S1 of the Supplementary Materials for the clustering index results and cluster plot). The cluster-wise bootstrap analysis indicated that the three clusters were valid and highly stable (i.e., Jaccard scores ≥0.85; (Hennig, Reference Hennig2007, Reference Hennig2008, Reference Hennig2020) across the bootstrapped samples: cluster 1 = 0.86; cluster 2 = 0.92; cluster 3 = 0.87. Linear discriminant analysis classified cases into clusters with 96% accuracy, further supporting the separation and robustness of the cluster solution (see Table S2 of the Supplementary Material).

For the two tasks in which participants completed either the version using a points-based reward or the version using a monetary reward, the task version did not significantly influence reinforcement learning task performance. Although there was a significant overall effect of task version on effort–cost computation task performance, this effect did not significantly vary as a function of the participant group in the full sample or as a function of cluster in the cluster analysis sample (see Supplementary Materials). Therefore, the analyses retained the full participant sample.

Visual inspection of the cluster results (Figure 1) revealed that cluster 1 represented participants with below-average value representation but above-average hedonic reactivity and reinforcement learning (Value Representation Deficit cluster; n = 106; 54%). In contrast, Cluster 2 was composed of participants with impaired performance across all reward-processing constructs, particularly reinforcement learning and value representation (Generalized Deficit cluster; n = 34; 17%). Cluster 3 contained participants with blunted hedonic reactivity but above-average value representation and reinforcement learning (Hedonic Reactivity Deficit cluster; n = 58; 29%).

Figure 1. Reward processing profiles of each cluster. Clusters were characterized by a value representation deficit (cluster 1), a generalized deficit across reward processing domains (cluster 2), and a hedonic reactivity deficit (cluster 3). Diamonds denote mean scores and boxplots indicate the median and interquartile range. The dotted line at z = 0 represents the mean value of healthy control reference group to which task scores were z-scored.

The cluster analysis was also repeated on CHR and HSC performance scores without standardizing to the HC group performance, and these results are reported in the Supplementary Material. The results between the two approaches were generally similar, both for the resulting three-cluster solution and for the pattern of cluster subgroup differences in clinical variables. However, the analysis using HC group-referenced scores led to a more stable and interpretable cluster solution, including a more clearly recommended optimal number of clusters among the evaluated cluster indices, higher cluster stability scores, and a more strongly differentiated pattern of task performance within clusters (e.g., the Generalized Deficit cluster had a larger magnitude of deficits across reward processing constructs). Therefore, the HC group-referenced cluster analysis results are reported here, and the full results of the cluster analysis using nonreferenced scores are reported in the Supplementary Material.

A 3-cluster × 4-construct ANOVA confirmed the cluster interpretation, which indicated a significant cluster-by-construct interaction, F(6585) = 50.90, p < 0.001, η p2 = 0.34, with significant cluster differences for each reward processing construct with the exception of effort–cost computation (Table 2). Follow-up comparisons indicated significantly lower value representation scores in the Value Representation Deficit and Generalized Deficit clusters compared to the Hedonic Reactivity Deficit cluster. In contrast, hedonic reactivity scores were significantly lower in the Hedonic Reactivity Deficit cluster compared to the other two clusters and significantly lower in the Generalized Deficit cluster compared to the Value Representation Deficit cluster. The Generalized Deficit cluster had lower reinforcement learning than the Hedonic Reactivity Deficit and Value Representation Deficit clusters. Effort–cost computation scores were below the normative HC group average and qualitatively lower in the Generalized Deficit cluster compared to the other two clusters; however, cluster differences were not statistically different.

Table 2. Cluster subgroup characteristics

Note: Values reflect mean (standard deviation) unless otherwise indicated. p-values with false-discover rate (FDR) correction for the seven negative symptom variables are indicated by p FDR in the test statistic column, while unadjusted p-values are reported for exploratory variables. All significant pairwise comparison p-values for both hypothesis-related and exploratory variables are corrected for multiple comparisons unless otherwise indicated by p uncorrected.

BACS = Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia; CHR = clinical high-risk participants; CES-D = Center for Epidemiologic Studies – Depression Scale; EEfRT = effort expenditure for rewards task; F w = Welch’s F-statistic; HSC = help-seeking control participants; NSI-PR = negative symptom inventory – psychosis risk; PSS = Perceived Stress Scale; SIPS = structured interview for psychosis-risk syndromes; STAI = State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; WRAT-4 = Wide-Range Achievement Test – 4; HVLT = Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; IAPS = International Affective Picture System; PRLT = probabilistic reinforcement learning task; p FDR = false-discovery rate-corrected p-value.

a Proxy primary negative symptom score is the scaled difference score of N1 + N2 + N3 - G2 - G4 SIPS items.

b Percentage of hard task choices for trials in the upper half of the reward magnitude range and in the upper (50%–88%) reward probability range.

c Percentage of correct choices on the most frequently rewarded gain stimulus pairing across learning trials.

Cluster comparisons of external variables

Clinical and demographic characteristics of each cluster subgroup and their statistical comparisons are reported in Table 2.

Variables related to testing the hypothesis of equifinality in negative symptoms

Clusters did not significantly differ in the proportion of CHR and HSC participants. Comparing clusters on negative symptoms indicated greater symptom severity in the Generalized Deficit cluster compared to the other two clusters, for both the diminished motivation and pleasure and the diminished expression symptom factors (FDR-corrected p < 0. 05) (Figure 2). Cluster differences were also found (FDR-corrected p < 0. 05) when examining the five individual negative symptom domains, with more severe asociality, blunted affect, and alogia in the Generalized Deficit cluster compared to the other two clusters (Figure 2). Cluster comparisons on negative symptoms were also performed in the CHR group only (evaluated using uncorrected p < 0.05 due to the reduced sample size), with consistent results for all negative symptom factors and domains except for alogia, which was no longer significantly different between clusters (see Table S4 of the Supplementary Material).

Figure 2. Negative symptom severity in each cluster. (a) Mean negative symptom factor scores across clusters. (b) Mean negative symptom domain scores across clusters.

* p < 0.05.

** p < 0.01.

Despite the cluster differences in negative symptom severity, there was nonetheless some indication that clinically significant negative symptoms were present across the three reward processing clusters when examining the proportion of participants with mild-or-greater (rating of ≥2) item-level symptom severity ratings, particularly for the motivation and pleasure factor and the asociality domain where proportions were above 50% (see Table S3 of the Supplementary Material), and when examining negative symptom severity in CHR participants within clusters (see Table S4 of the Supplementary Material).

Exploratory variables to characterize differences across clusters

When comparing clusters on exploratory external variables, clusters were comparable in terms of most demographic variables (with the exception of a greater proportion of females in the Hedonic Reactivity Deficit compared to the Value Representation Deficit cluster), self-reported symptom severity, severity of subthreshold psychotic symptoms, and rates of current psychiatric diagnoses. The Generalized Deficit cluster had reduced social functioning, greater proxy primary negative symptom scores (reflecting greater likelihood of primary negative symptoms; see footnote), higher psychosis conversion risk probability scores, lower estimated premorbid intellectual functioning, and reduced total verbal learning performance.

Discussion

The current study aimed to identify data-driven reward processing profiles and their associated negative symptom presentation in a transdiagnostic clinical help-seeking sample. Cluster analysis of CHR and HSC participant performance across four reward processing constructs previously associated with negative symptoms (hedonic reactivity, reinforcement learning, value representation, effort–cost computation) revealed three distinct clusters. Clusters were primarily characterized by: (1) a value representation deficit, (2) a generalized reward processing deficit, or (3) a hedonic reactivity deficit.

As hypothesized, clusters did not differ in rates of clinical group membership (CHR versus HSC) or psychiatric diagnoses, suggesting that distinct reward processing profiles are transdiagnostic and not bound by diagnostic categories. In contrast, the hypothesized presence of multiple mechanistic pathways to negative symptom presentation (i.e., equifinality) was not strongly supported. Negative symptoms were most robustly observed and at greatest severity in the Generalized Deficit cluster compared to the clusters where deficits were restricted to a specific reward processing component, suggesting that negative symptoms were associated with a single, more global reward processing deficit.

Although an equifinality perspective has been proposed to explain the transdiagnostic nature of negative symptoms across different psychiatric disorders (Nusslock & Alloy, Reference Nusslock and Alloy2017; Strauss & Cohen, Reference Strauss and Cohen2017; Whitton et al., Reference Whitton, Treadway and Pizzagalli2015), few prior studies have included multiple clinical groups, assessed more than one reward processing construct, or targeted heterogeneous CHR and HSC samples that capture more normative variation in negative symptom presentation and reward system functioning than those of traditional studies restricted to individuals with depression and/or schizophrenia. The current results suggest that heterogeneity in reward-processing alterations is transdiagnostic and exists within diagnostic categories; however, a single global reward-processing deficit profile has the strongest association with negative symptom severity. These results build on prior studies that have documented multiple reward processing alterations in CHR samples, albeit studied individually (Gruber et al., Reference Gruber, Strauss, Dombrecht and Mittal2018; Millman et al., Reference Millman, Gallagher, Demro, Schiffman, Reeves, Gold and Redman2020; Pratt, Treadway, Strauss, & Mittal, Reference Pratt, Treadway, Strauss and Mittal2024; Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Bartolomeo and Luther2023; Strauss, Ruiz, et al., Reference Strauss, Ruiz, Visser, Crespo and Dickinson2018), and help to explain prior findings of heterogeneous overlap and divergence in reward processing dysfunction relative to other psychiatric diagnoses like schizophrenia and depression. Furthermore, the current finding of distinct profiles of reward processing dysfunction but with only the Generalized Deficit cluster clearly showing elevated negative symptom severity is consistent with a recent study (Luther et al., Reference Luther, Jarvis, Spilka and Strauss2024) using a similar data-driven approach that identified a single, global reward processing deficit underlying negative symptoms in a transphasic serious mental illness sample (i.e., including participants across putative phases of psychotic illness). These findings suggest that in contrast to the equifinality model (Nusslock & Alloy, Reference Nusslock and Alloy2017; Whitton et al., Reference Whitton, Treadway and Pizzagalli2015) in which different mechanistic pathways between disorders (e.g., schizophrenia versus depression) can give rise to the common presence of negative symptoms, a single, generalized reward-processing deficit was most strongly associated with negative symptoms when examined in a transdiagnostic sample of help-seeking and CHR participants. It may be that equifinality is not as strongly supported within a sample of help-seeking and CHR youth, where symptoms may be present in milder form and where psychopathological processes might still be emerging through developmental and/or environmental factors. Nonetheless, there was some indication for the presence of mild negative symptoms across clusters, particularly asociality and the motivation and pleasure factor. Future research is needed to corroborate the presence and nature of these findings; for example, whether the symptoms observed in the other clusters reflect secondary negative symptoms, as suggested by the greater estimated primary negative symptom score in the Generalized Deficit cluster.

In contrast to the Hedonic Reactivity Deficit and Value Representation Deficit clusters, where below-average performance was only observed in a single reward-processing construct relative to the normative group, the Generalized Deficit cluster was characterized by deficits across the four reward-processing constructs. These findings suggest that a more global reward-processing deficit profile is the predominant pathway to negative symptom formation. However, it should be noted that reinforcement learning, followed by value representation, were the most prominent deficits in this cluster. Prior research in schizophrenia samples indicates that difficulties representing the expected value of rewards during decision-making contribute to reward learning deficits, particularly in participants with elevated negative symptoms (Gold et al., Reference Gold, Waltz, Matveeva, Kasanova, Strauss, Herbener and Frank2012; Gold, Waltz, Prentice, Morris, & Heerey, Reference Gold, Waltz, Prentice, Morris and Heerey2008; Hernaus et al., Reference Hernaus, Frank, Brown, Brown, Gold and Waltz2019; Hernaus, Gold, Waltz, & Frank, Reference Hernaus, Gold, Waltz and Frank2018; Strauss & Herbener, Reference Strauss and Herbener2011; Waltz, Frank, Robinson, & Gold, Reference Waltz, Frank, Robinson and Gold2007; Waltz, Frank, Wiecki, & Gold, Reference Waltz, Frank, Wiecki and Gold2011). Therefore, an alternate possibility is that a reward processing profile predominantly consisting of reinforcement learning deficits characterized by value representation abnormalities may be a particularly strong pathway toward negative symptom formation. The Value Representation Deficit cluster did not clearly display elevated negative symptoms, further suggesting that a value representation deficit may be most relevant to negative symptom formation when this deficit interacts with or contributes to other reward processing difficulties (e.g., when value representations cannot be updated efficiently to guide learning and decision-making; Gold et al., Reference Gold, Waltz, Prentice, Morris and Heerey2008). Additional research is needed to further characterize the potential additive and/or interactive effects of multiple reward processing deficits on negative symptom severity in the context of equifinality.

In addition to having the greatest severity of negative symptoms, the Generalized Deficit cluster also had reduced social functioning, greater proxy scores of putative primary (versus secondary) negative symptoms, as well as the highest psychosis conversion risk probability scores. This cluster nonetheless had comparable severity of most other symptoms and several measures of cognition, suggesting that this clinical profile was not simply a correlate of greater overall symptom load. However, this cluster demonstrated reduced estimated premorbid intellectual functioning and overall verbal learning performance (but not rate of learning or processing speed), suggesting the potential contribution of subtle cognitive deficits to the observed reward processing deficits (e.g., reinforcement learning). Taken together, these results suggest that a global reward-processing deficit profile is associated with increased functional impairment and confers greater psychosis risk. These findings are consistent with longitudinal research indicating that more elevated baseline negative symptoms predict transition to psychosis in CHR youth (Gupta et al., Reference Gupta, Strauss, Cowan, Pelletier-Baldelli, Ellman, Schiffman and Mittal2021; Piskulic et al., Reference Piskulic, Addington, Cadenhead, Cannon, Cornblatt, Heinssen and Walker2012) and further suggest it may be possible to identify a reward-processing profile associated with increased psychosis risk. Task-based assessment of reward processing profiles might therefore provide an objective and scalable measure of psychosis risk-related mechanisms to enhance the sensitivity and precision of existing risk calculators (Cannon et al., Reference Cannon, Yu, Addington, Bearden, Cadenhead, Cornblatt and Kattan2016; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Xu, Tang, Li, Tang, Cui and Group2019). Planned longitudinal analysis of the ongoing CAPR study will address this aim.

Another finding was the greater proportion of females in the Hedonic Reactivity Deficit cluster compared to the Value Representation Deficit cluster. Given that there was some indication for clinically significant negative symptoms present across the three reward processing clusters, and that the Hedonic Reactivity Deficit cluster had the largest deficit in hedonic reactivity compared to the other two clusters, it is possible that a hedonic reactivity deficit pathway to negative symptoms may be more prominent in females than males. However, it should be noted that there were no significant differences in the proportion of females between the Hedonic Reactivity Deficit and Generalized Deficit clusters. Hedonic reactivity deficits are also reported in individuals with depressive disorders (Bylsma et al., Reference Bylsma, Morris and Rottenberg2008), where reward responsiveness is associated with symptoms of anhedonia (Pizzagalli, Iosifescu, Hallett, Ratner, & Fava, Reference Pizzagalli, Iosifescu, Hallett, Ratner and Fava2008), and where there is a greater prevalence of depression in women compared to men (Salk, Hyde, & Abramson, Reference Salk, Hyde and Abramson2017). However, there were no cluster differences in anhedonia severity or rates of depressive disorders. Therefore, further research is needed to examine the potential presence of sex-related differences in hedonic reactivity in the context of negative symptoms transdiagnostically.

An outstanding question concerns the neurobiological correlates of the identified performance-based reward-processing profiles. Although prior research has documented alterations in frontal–striatal systems during reward processing in psychosis (Barch & Dowd, Reference Barch and Dowd2010; Chase, Loriemi, Wensing, Eickhoff, & Nickl-Jockschat, Reference Chase, Loriemi, Wensing, Eickhoff and Nickl-Jockschat2018; Kesby, Murray, & Knolle, Reference Kesby, Murray and Knolle2023; Radua et al., Reference Radua, Schmidt, Borgwardt, Heinz, Schlagenhauf, Mcguire and Fusar-Poli2015), depression (Keren et al., Reference Keren, O’callaghan, Vidal-Ribas, Buzzell, Brotman, Leibenluft and Wolke2018), and in CHR youth (Howes, Hird, Adams, Corlett, & McGuire, Reference Howes, Hird, Adams, Corlett and Mcguire2020; Kesby et al., Reference Kesby, Murray and Knolle2023; Radua et al., Reference Radua, Schmidt, Borgwardt, Heinz, Schlagenhauf, Mcguire and Fusar-Poli2015), future neuroimaging research that leverages heterogeneity in symptom presentation independent of clinical diagnosis and focuses on cross-task performance patterns will help to address this question. Similarly, examining whether performance-based reward processing profiles map onto subgroups identified in prior biotype-based stratification studies (Clementz et al., Reference Clementz, Trotti, Pearlson, Keshavan, Gershon, Keedy and Tamminga2020; Dinga et al., Reference Dinga, Schmaal, Penninx, Van Tol, Veltman, Van Velzen and Marquand2019; Drysdale et al., Reference Drysdale, Grosenick, Downar, Dunlop, Mansouri, Meng and Liston2017; Dwyer et al., Reference Dwyer, Buciuman, Ruef, Kambeitz, Sen Dong and Stinson2022; Planchuelo-Gómez et al., Reference Planchuelo-Gómez, Lubeiro, Núñez-Novo, Gomez-Pilar, De Luis-García, Del Valle and Molina2020) may help clarify neurobiological underpinnings of distinct reward processing profiles with the potential to aid in negative symptom treatment innovation.

Although clusters were characterized by patterns of reward processing deficits relative to the HC group serving as the reference sample, there were also areas of greater reward processing performance relative to the HC group. Specifically, the Value Representation Deficit cluster had above-average hedonic reactivity and reinforcement learning, while the Hedonic Reactivity Deficit cluster had greater value representation and reinforcement learning. The above-average reinforcement learning in these clusters is consistent with the interpretation that the reinforcement learning deficit unique to the Generalized Deficit cluster is an important feature of the elevated negative symptom presentation in this cluster. Additionally, the opposite profiles of hedonic reactivity and value representation in the Hedonic Reactivity Deficit and Value Representation Deficit clusters suggest a reciprocal relationship: participants with elevated in-the-moment hedonic reactivity were more prone to discounting the value of a delayed reward (due to difficulty maintaining a representation of the value of the delayed reward), while participants with blunted hedonic reactivity were more likely to display elevated value representation (due to being less likely to discount the value of the delayed reward). However, given that deficits in both hedonic reactivity and value representation were found in the Generalized Deficit cluster, the relationship between these two constructs may depend on the extent to which there is co-occurring dysfunction in other aspects of reward processing (e.g., in reinforcement learning and effort–cost computation). Given that the observed reward processing profiles were reproduced when the cluster analysis was performed using the unreferenced task scores (i.e., when task scores were scaled within the clinical sample rather than scaled relative to the HC group; see the Supplementary Material), the pattern of elevations and deficits in reward processing performance across clusters was not attributable to the HC group’s reward processing task performance. However, the Generalized Deficit cluster was the cluster that most clearly displayed elevated negative symptoms and had significant findings on other examined clinical variables (e.g., social functioning, psychosis risk conversion probability); therefore, the clinical significance of the higher performance scores relative to the HC group in the Hedonic Reactivity Deficit and Value Representation Deficit clusters may be limited.

Similarly, in the full sample, none of the help-seeking participant groups (i.e., CHR or HSC) displayed significantly reduced task performance compared to the HC group, and the CHR group had comparable performance to HSC peers (Table 1). Prior studies have documented reward processing abnormalities in CHR samples relative to community control groups; however, findings have been mixed across studies and constructs examined (Ermakova et al., Reference Ermakova, Knolle, Justicia, Bullmore, Jones, Robbins and Murray2018; Gruber et al., Reference Gruber, Strauss, Dombrecht and Mittal2018; Karcher, Hua, & Kerns, Reference Karcher, Hua and Kerns2019; Millman et al., Reference Millman, Gallagher, Demro, Schiffman, Reeves, Gold and Redman2020; Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Bartolomeo and Luther2023; Strauss, Ruiz, et al., Reference Strauss, Ruiz, Visser, Crespo and Dickinson2018). Furthermore, recent meta-analytic work on neuropsychological deficits in CHR samples has indicated that any excess impairment in CHR groups relative to HSCs is predominantly attributed to CHR participants who transition to psychosis, with comparable deficits in CHR participants without transition to psychosis (Millman et al., Reference Millman, Roemer, Vargas, Schiffman, Mittal and Gold2022). Similar to the meta-analytic findings regarding neuropsychological profiles, and consistent with the current cluster analysis results of more global reward processing deficits and reduced neuropsychological functioning in the cluster of participants with the greatest psychosis conversion risk probability scores, the heterogeneous reward processing profiles in youth at CHR might not be fully represented when analyzed at the group level. These findings highlight the importance of accounting for clinical heterogeneity in the study of psychosis risk, such as by using data-driven stratification approaches like the cluster analysis in the current study.

The study has several limitations. First, although the observed cluster solution was found to be stable and robust when evaluated against bootstrapped samples, the participant sample size was small for data-driven analysis and results warrant validation in external samples. Second, the study was cross-sectional, preventing firm conclusions about the causal contributions of the observed reward processing profiles to the development of negative symptoms. Planned longitudinal follow-up assessment of CAPR study participants will help clarify the stability of reward processing profiles and determine whether these represent mechanistic pathways to symptom development or common consequences of shared etiological factors. Third, negative symptom severity was generally mild across clinical groups and additional research is needed to determine whether the identified transdiagnostic reward processing profiles generalize to samples with more severe negative symptoms. However, negative symptom severity in the current sample was comparable to that of previously published CHR samples (e.g., Chang et al., Reference Chang, Strauss, Ahmed, Wong, Chan, Lee and Chen2021; Gupta, Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Bartolomeo and Luther2023), Furthermore, mild negative symptom severity scores are typically observed even in schizophrenia samples when not specifically selecting for participants with severe negative symptoms or deficit syndrome presentation (e.g., Strauss, Nunez, et al., Reference Strauss, Nunez, Ahmed, Barchard, Granholm, Kirkpatrick and Allen2018 Supplement). Fourth, the assessment of reward profiles was limited to four component processes; future studies using an expanded array of RDoC reward-processing subconstructs (e.g., reward anticipation) will provide a more comprehensive characterization of profiles associated with negative symptoms. Fifth, multiple processes contribute to the probabilistic reinforcement learning construct, including hedonic reactivity, value representation, and other sub-processes (e.g., prediction error signaling), which will require further analysis to uncover the origins of this deficit (Gold et al., Reference Gold, Waltz, Matveeva, Kasanova, Strauss, Herbener and Frank2012).

In conclusion, this study indicates that distinct data-driven reward processing profiles can be identified transdiagnostically but that negative symptoms are most strongly linked to a single profile of global reward processing deficits. These findings support the view that reward processing dysfunction is heterogeneous both between and within psychiatric disorders but do not support a transdiagnostic equifinality model of negative symptoms, where both shared and distinct pathways to negative symptoms can be present across and within psychiatric disorders. Future studies are needed to determine whether computerized assessment of reward-processing constructs can assist in the development of individualized prediction and treatment of negative symptoms.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172400326X.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the participants who took part in the study, and the research staff who conducted participant recruitment and data collection.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health: R01MH120088 (Mittal), R01MH120089 (Corlett, Woods), R01MH120090 (Gold), R01MH120091 (Ellman), R01MH120092 (Strauss), R01MH115031 (Waltz), and T32MH016259 (Millman).

Competing interest

Michael J. Spilka is employed at Cambridge Cognition Ltd and retains stock options. Zachary B. Millman has received speaker honoraria from Mount Auburn Hospital. Philip R. Corlett is a cofounder, board member, and holds equity in Tetricus Labs, a precision psychiatry company. They did not fund this work. James M. Gold receives royalty payments from Vera Sci for the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia. Scott W. Woods reports that he has received speaking fees from the American Psychiatric Association and from Medscape Features. He has been granted US patent no. 8492418 B2 for a method of treating prodromal schizophrenia with glycine agonists. He is a consultant to and is a partner and owns stock in NW PharmaTech. Gregory P. Strauss is one of the original developers of the Brief Negative Symptom Scale (BNSS) and receives royalties and consultation fees from Medavante-ProPhase LLC in connection with commercial use of the BNSS and other professional activities; these fees are donated to the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation. Dr. Strauss has received honoraria and travel support from Medavante-ProPhase LLC for training pharmaceutical company raters on the BNSS. Dr. Strauss has consulted for and/or been on the speaker bureau for Minerva Neurosciences, Acadia, Lundbeck, Sunovion, Boehringer Ingelheim, and Otsuka pharmaceutical companies. All other authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

1 Proxy scores for primary negative symptoms (versus negative symptoms secondary to other factors such as co-occurring depression) were computed from SIPS ratings, modeled after the approaches of Kirkpatrick, Mucci, & Galderisi, Reference Kirkpatrick, Mucci and Galderisi2017 and Goetz et al., Reference Goetz, Corcoran, Yale, Stanford, Kimhy, Amador and Malaspina2007 for the identification of deficit syndrome in schizophrenia based on the difference in severity of core negative symptoms versus mood and anxiety symptoms. Proxy scores were calculated from the sum of social anhedonia, avolition, and emotional expression SIPS ratings (N1 + N2 + N3) subtracted by the sum of dysphoric mood and stress intolerance SIPS ratings (G2 + G4), with the obtained difference score scaled across the combined sample of CHR and HSC participants. Similar approaches have been used to characterize negative symptom presentation in CHR samples (Tran et al., Reference Tran, Spilka, Raugh, Strauss, Bearden, Cadenhead and Addington2023).

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (Vol. 5): American psychiatric association Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Amlung, M., Marsden, E., Holshausen, K., Morris, V., Patel, H., Vedelago, L., … Mccabe, R. E. (2019). Delay discounting as a transdiagnostic process in psychiatric disorders: A meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(11), 11761186. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., Gold, J. M., Johnson, S. L., Kring, A. M., Macdonald, A. W. Iii, … Strauss, M. E. (2017). Explicit and implicit reinforcement learning across the psychosis spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(5), 694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D. M., & Dowd, E. C. (2010). Goal representations and motivational drive in schizophrenia: The role of prefrontal-striatal interactions. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(5), 919934. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbq068CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartolomeo, L. A., Chapman, H. C., Raugh, I. M., & Strauss, G. P. (2021). Delay discounting in youth at clinical high-risk for psychosis and adults with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 51(11), 18981905. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720000677CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedict, R. H., Schretlen, D., Groninger, L., & Brandt, J. (1998). Hopkins verbal learning test–revised: Normative data and analysis of inter-form and test-retest reliability. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12(1), 4355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bieling, P. J., Antony, M. M., & Swinson, R. P. (1998). The state-trait anxiety inventory, trait version: Structure and content re-examined. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(7–8), 777788. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00023-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, S. J., & Gagne, C. (2018). Anxiety, depression, and decision making: A computational perspective. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 41, 371388. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-080317-062007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bylsma, L. M., Morris, B. H., & Rottenberg, J. (2008). A meta-analysis of emotional reactivity in major depressive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(4), 676691. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, T. D., Yu, C., Addington, J., Bearden, C. E., Cadenhead, K. S., Cornblatt, B. A., … Kattan, M. W. (2016). An individualized risk calculator for research in prodromal psychosis. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(10), 980988. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15070890CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, W. C., Strauss, G. P., Ahmed, A. O., Wong, S. C. Y., Chan, J. K. N., Lee, E. H. M., … Chen, E. Y. H. (2021). The latent structure of negative symptoms in individuals with attenuated psychosis syndrome and early psychosis: support for the 5 consensus domains. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 47(2), 386394. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbaa129CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charrad, M., Ghazzali, N., Boiteau, V., & Niknafs, A. (2014). NbClust: An R package for determining the relevant number of clusters in a data set. Journal of Statistical Software, 61, 136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chase, H. W., Loriemi, P., Wensing, T., Eickhoff, S. B., & Nickl-Jockschat, T. (2018). Meta-analytic evidence for altered mesolimbic responses to reward in schizophrenia. Human Brain Mapping, 39(7), 29172928. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24049CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clementz, B. A., Trotti, R. L., Pearlson, G. D., Keshavan, M. S., Gershon, E. S., Keedy, S. K., … Tamminga, C. A. (2020). Testing psychosis phenotypes from bipolar-schizophrenia network for intermediate phenotypes for clinical application: biotype characteristics and targets. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 5(8), 808818. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.03.011Google Scholar
Cohen, A. S., & Minor, K. S. (2010). Emotional experience in patients with schizophrenia revisited: meta-analysis of laboratory studies. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(1), 143150. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbn061CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385396. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6668417CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornblatt, B. A., Auther, A. M., Niendam, T., Smith, C. W., Zinberg, J., Bearden, C. E., & Cannon, T. D. (2007). Preliminary findings for two new measures of social and role functioning in the prodromal phase of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(3), 688702. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbm029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Culbreth, A. J., Moran, E. K., & Barch, D. M. (2018). Effort-cost decision-making in psychosis and depression: Could a similar behavioral deficit arise from disparate psychological and neural mechanisms? Psychological Medicine, 48(6), 889904. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717002525CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuthbert, B. N. (2014). The RDoC framework: Facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry, 13(1), 2835. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20087CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dean, D. J., Walther, S., Bernard, J. A., & Mittal, V. A. (2018). Motor clusters reveal differences in risk for psychosis, cognitive functioning, and thalamocortical connectivity: evidence for vulnerability subtypes. Clinical Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 6(5), 721734. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702618773759CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinga, R., Schmaal, L., Penninx, B., Van Tol, M. J., Veltman, D. J., Van Velzen, L., …. Marquand, A. F. (2019). Evaluating the evidence for biotypes of depression: Methodological replication and extension of. NeuroImage: Clinical, 22, 101796. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101796CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drysdale, A. T., Grosenick, L., Downar, J., Dunlop, K., Mansouri, F., Meng, Y., … Liston, C. (2017). Resting-state connectivity biomarkers define neurophysiological subtypes of depression. Nature Medicine, 23(1), 2838. https://doi.org/10.1038/nm.4246CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dwyer, D. B., Buciuman, M. O., Ruef, A., Kambeitz, J., Sen Dong, M., Stinson, C., … PRONIA Consortium (2022). Clinical, brain, and multilevel clustering in early psychosis and affective stages. JAMA Psychiatry, 79(7), 677689. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2022.1163CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ermakova, A. O., Knolle, F., Justicia, A., Bullmore, E. T., Jones, P. B., Robbins, T. W., … Murray, G. K. (2018). Abnormal reward prediction-error signalling in antipsychotic naive individuals with first-episode psychosis or clinical risk for psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology, 43(8), 16911699. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-018-0056-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M. B., Williams, J.B., Karg, R.S., Spitzer, R.L. (2016). SCID-5-CV: Structured clinical interview for DSM-5 disorders: Clinician version. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing.Google Scholar
Goetz, R. R., Corcoran, C., Yale, S., Stanford, A. D., Kimhy, D., Amador, X., & Malaspina, D. (2007). Validity of a ‘proxy’ for the deficit syndrome derived from the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS). Schizophrenia Research, 93(1–3), 169177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.02.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, J. M., Waltz, J. A., Matveeva, T. M., Kasanova, Z., Strauss, G. P., Herbener, E. S., … Frank, M. J. (2012). Negative symptoms and the failure to represent the expected reward value of actions: Behavioral and computational modeling evidence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(2), 129138. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1269CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, J. M., Waltz, J. A., Prentice, K. J., Morris, S. E., & Heerey, E. A. (2008). Reward processing in schizophrenia: A deficit in the representation of value. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(5), 835847. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbn068CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruber, J., Strauss, G. P., Dombrecht, L., & Mittal, V. A. (2018). Neuroleptic-free youth at ultrahigh risk for psychosis evidence diminished emotion reactivity that is predicted by depression and anxiety. Schizophrenia Research, 193, 428434. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.08.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gupta, T., Cowan, H. R., Strauss, G. P., Walker, E. F., & Mittal, V. A. (2021). Deconstructing negative symptoms in individuals at clinical high-risk for psychosis: Evidence for volitional and diminished emotionality subgroups that predict clinical presentation and functional outcome. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 47(1), 5463. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbaa084CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gupta, T., Strauss, G. P., Cowan, H. R., Pelletier-Baldelli, A., Ellman, L. M., Schiffman, J., & Mittal, V. A. (2021). Secondary sources of negative symptoms in those meeting criteria for a clinical high-risk syndrome. Biological Psychiatry Global Open Science, 1(3), 210218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsgos.2021.05.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Healey, K. M., Penn, D. L., Perkins, D., Woods, S. W., Keefe, R. S. E., & Addington, J. (2018). Latent profile analysis and conversion to psychosis: Characterizing subgroups to enhance risk prediction. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44(2), 286296. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx080CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hennig, C. (2007). Cluster-wise assessment of cluster stability. Computational Statistics & Data Analysis, 52(1), 258271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hennig, C. (2008). Dissolution point and isolation robustness: Robustness criteria for general cluster analysis methods. Journal of Multivariate Analysis, 99(6), 11541176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hennig, C. (2020). Fpc: Flexible Procedures for Clustering. R package version 2.2–5. doi: 10.32614/CRAN.package.fpcCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hernaus, D., Frank, M. J., Brown, E. C., Brown, J. K., Gold, J. M., & Waltz, J. A. (2019). Impaired expected value computations in schizophrenia are associated with a reduced ability to integrate reward probability and magnitude of recent outcomes. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 4(3), 280290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.11.011Google ScholarPubMed
Hernaus, D., Gold, J. M., Waltz, J. A., & Frank, M. J. (2018). Impaired expected value computations coupled with overreliance on stimulus-response learning in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(11), 916926. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.03.014Google ScholarPubMed
Hershenberg, R., Satterthwaite, T. D., Daldal, A., Katchmar, N., Moore, T. M., Kable, J. W., & Wolf, D. H. (2016). Diminished effort on a progressive ratio task in both unipolar and bipolar depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 196, 97100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howes, O. D., Hird, E. J., Adams, R. A., Corlett, P. R., & Mcguire, P. (2020). Aberrant salience, information processing, and dopaminergic signaling in people at clinical high risk for psychosis. Biological Psychiatry, 88(4), 304314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.03.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanen, J. W., Ersche, K. D., Fineberg, N. A., Robbins, T. W., & Cardinal, R. N. (2019). Computational modelling reveals contrasting effects on reinforcement learning and cognitive flexibility in stimulant use disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: remediating effects of dopaminergic D2/3 receptor agents. Psychopharmacology, 236(8), 23372358. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-019-05325-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karcher, N. R., Hua, J. P. Y., & Kerns, J. G. (2019). Probabilistic category learning and striatal functional activation in psychosis risk. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 45(2), 396404. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sby033CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keefe, R. S., Goldberg, T. E., Harvey, P. D., Gold, J. M., Poe, M. P., & Coughenour, L. (2004). The brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia: Reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophrenia Research, 68(2–3), 283297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keren, H., O’callaghan, G., Vidal-Ribas, P., Buzzell, G. A., Brotman, M. A., Leibenluft, E., … Wolke, S. (2018). Reward processing in depression: a conceptual and meta-analytic review across fMRI and EEG studies. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(11), 11111120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kesby, J. P., Murray, G. K., & Knolle, F. (2023). Neural circuitry of salience and reward processing in psychosis. Biological Psychiatry Global Open Science, 3(1), 3346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsgos.2021.12.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., & Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(1), 7887. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.128.1.78CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirkpatrick, B., Mucci, A., & Galderisi, S. (2017). Primary, enduring negative symptoms: An update on research. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43(4), 730736. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx064CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirschner, M., Aleman, A., & Kaiser, S. (2017). Secondary negative symptoms – A review of mechanisms, assessment and treatment. Schizophrenia Research, 186, 2938. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.05.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koutsouleris, N., Dwyer, D. B., Degenhardt, F., Maj, C., Urquijo-Castro, M. F., Sanfelici, R., … PRONIA Consortium (2021). Multimodal machine learning workflows for prediction of psychosis in patients with clinical high-risk syndromes and recent-onset depression. JAMA Psychiatry, 78(2), 195209. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.3604CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, 1(39–58), 3.Google Scholar
Luther, L., Jarvis, S. A., Spilka, M. J., & Strauss, G. P. (2024). Global reward processing deficits predict negative symptoms transdiagnostically and transphasically in a severe mental illness-spectrum sample. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 274(7), 17291740. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-023-01714-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marquand, A. F., Wolfers, T., Mennes, M., Buitelaar, J., & Beckmann, C. F. (2016). Beyond lumping and splitting: A review of computational approaches for stratifying psychiatric disorders. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 1(5), 433447. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.04.002Google ScholarPubMed
Mcglashan, T. H., Walsh, B. C., Woods, S. W., Addington, J., Cadenhead, K., Cannon, T., & Walker, E. (2001). Structured interview for psychosis-risk syndromes. New Haven, CT: Yale School of Medicine.Google Scholar
Mcgorry, P., Keshavan, M., Goldstone, S., Amminger, P., Allott, K., Berk, M., …. Hickie, I. (2014). Biomarkers and clinical staging in psychiatry. World Psychiatry, 13(3), 211223. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20144CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Millman, Z. B., Gallagher, K., Demro, C., Schiffman, J., Reeves, G. M., Gold, J. M., …. Redman, S. (2020). Evidence of reward system dysfunction in youth at clinical high-risk for psychosis from two event-related fMRI paradigms. Schizophrenia Research, 226, 111119. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.03.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Millman, Z. B., Gold, J. M., Mittal, V. A., & Schiffman, J. (2019). The critical need for help-seeking controls in clinical high-risk research. Clinical Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 7(6), 11711189. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702619855660CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Millman, Z. B., Roemer, C., Vargas, T., Schiffman, J., Mittal, V. A., & Gold, J. M. (2022). Neuropsychological performance among individuals at clinical high-risk for psychosis versus putatively low-risk peers with other psychopathology: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 48(5), 9991010. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbac031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mittal, V. A., Ellman, L. M., Strauss, G. P., Walker, E. F., Corlett, P. R., Schiffman, J., …. Gold, J. M. (2021). Computerized assessment of psychosis risk. Journal of Psychiatry and Brain Science, 6(3). https://doi.org/10.20900/jpbs.20210011Google ScholarPubMed
Mittal, V. A., Walker, E. F., & Strauss, G. P. (2021). The COVID-19 pandemic introduces diagnostic and treatment planning complexity for individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 47(6), 15181523. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbab083CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nusslock, R., & Alloy, L. B. (2017). Reward processing and mood-related symptoms: An RDoC and translational neuroscience perspective. Journal of Affective Disorders, 216, 316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.02.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, N. B., Strauss, G. P., Woodyatt, J. J., Paul, M. G., Keene, J. R., & Allen, D. N. (2022). Cluster analysis of negative symptoms identifies distinct negative symptom subgroups. Schizophrenia Research, 246, 207215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2022.06.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piskulic, D., Addington, J., Cadenhead, K. S., Cannon, T. D., Cornblatt, B. A., Heinssen, R., … Walker, E. F. (2012). Negative symptoms in individuals at clinical high risk of psychosis. Psychiatry Research, 196(2–3), 220224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pizzagalli, D. A., Iosifescu, D., Hallett, L. A., Ratner, K. G., & Fava, M. (2008). Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: evidence from a probabilistic reward task. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43(1), 7687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Planchuelo-Gómez, Á., Lubeiro, A., Núñez-Novo, P., Gomez-Pilar, J., De Luis-García, R., Del Valle, P., … Molina, V. (2020). Identificacion of MRI-based psychosis subtypes: Replication and refinement. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 100, 109907.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pratt, D. N., Treadway, M. T., Strauss, G. P., & Mittal, V. A. (2024). Diminished differentiation of rewards in individuals at clinical high-risk for psychosis. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-024-01794-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radua, J., Schmidt, A., Borgwardt, S., Heinz, A., Schlagenhauf, F., Mcguire, P., & Fusar-Poli, P. (2015). Ventral striatal activation during reward processing in psychosis: a neurofunctional meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(12), 12431251. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2196CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, A. T., Addington, J., Bearden, C. E., Cadenhead, K. S., Cornblatt, B. A., Mathalon, D. H., … Walker, E. F. (2018). Latent class cluster analysis of symptom ratings identifies distinct subgroups within the clinical high risk for psychosis syndrome. Schizophrenia Research, 197, 522530. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.12.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salk, R. H., Hyde, J. S., & Abramson, L. Y. (2017). Gender differences in depression in representative national samples: Meta-analyses of diagnoses and symptoms. Psychological Bulletin, 143(8), 783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinley, D. (2006). K-means clustering: a half-century synthesis. The British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 59(Pt 1), 134. https://doi.org/10.1348/000711005X48266CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, G. P., Ahmed, A. O., Young, J. W., & Kirkpatrick, B. (2019). Reconsidering the Latent Structure of Negative Symptoms in Schizophrenia: A Review of Evidence Supporting the 5 Consensus Domains. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 45(4), 725729. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sby169CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, G. P., Bartolomeo, L. A., & Luther, L. (2023). Reduced willingness to expend effort for rewards is associated with risk for conversion and negative symptom severity in youth at clinical high-risk for psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 53(3), 714721. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172100204XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, G. P., & Cohen, A. S. (2017). A transdiagnostic review of negative symptom phenomenology and etiology. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43(4), 712719. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx066CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, G. P., & Herbener, E. S. (2011). Patterns of emotional experience in schizophrenia: differences in emotional response to visual stimuli are associated with clinical presentation and functional outcome. Schizophrenia Research, 128(1–3), 117123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2011.01.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, G. P., Horan, W. P., Kirkpatrick, B., Fischer, B. A., Keller, W. R., Miski, P., … Carpenter, W. T. Jr. (2013). Deconstructing negative symptoms of schizophrenia: Avolition-apathy and diminished expression clusters predict clinical presentation and functional outcome. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47(6), 783790. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.01.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, G. P., Nunez, A., Ahmed, A. O., Barchard, K. A., Granholm, E., Kirkpatrick, B., … Allen, D. N. (2018). The Latent Structure of Negative Symptoms in Schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry, 75(12), 12711279. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.2475CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, G. P., Ruiz, I., Visser, K. H., Crespo, L. P., & Dickinson, E. K. (2018). Diminished Hedonic response in neuroleptic-free youth at ultra high-risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition, 12, 17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2017.12.001Google ScholarPubMed
Strauss, G. P., Walker, E. F., Pelletier-Baldelli, A., Carter, N. T., Ellman, L. M., Schiffman, J., … Mittal, V. A. (2023). Development and validation of the negative symptom inventory-psychosis risk. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 49(5), 12051216. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbad038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, G. P., Waltz, J. A., & Gold, J. M. (2014). A review of reward processing and motivational impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(Suppl_2), S107S116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tran, T., Spilka, M. J., Raugh, I. M., Strauss, G. P., Bearden, C. E., Cadenhead, K. S., … Addington, J. M. (2023). Negative Symptom Trajectories in Individuals at Clinical High Risk for Psychosis: Differences Based on Deficit Syndrome, Persistence, and Transition Status. Schizophr Bull Open, 4(1), sgad014. https://doi.org/10.1093/schizbullopen/sgad014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treadway, M. T., Buckholtz, J. W., Cowan, R. L., Woodward, N. D., Li, R., Ansari, M. S., … Zald, D. H. (2012). Dopaminergic mechanisms of individual differences in human effort-based decision-making. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(18), 61706176. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6459-11.2012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waltz, J. A., Frank, M. J., Robinson, B. M., & Gold, J. M. (2007). Selective reinforcement learning deficits in schizophrenia support predictions from computational models of striatal-cortical dysfunction. Biological Psychiatry, 62(7), 756764. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waltz, J. A., Frank, M. J., Wiecki, T. V., & Gold, J. M. (2011). Altered probabilistic learning and response biases in schizophrenia: behavioral evidence and neurocomputational modeling. Neuropsychology, 25(1), 8697. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020882CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weaver, S. S., Kroska, E. B., Ross, M. C., Sartin-Tarm, A., Sellnow, K. A., Schaumberg, K., … Cisler, J. M. (2020). Sacrificing reward to avoid threat: Characterizing PTSD in the context of a trauma-related approach-avoidance conflict task. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(5), 457468. doi:10.1037/abn0000528CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitmer, A. J., Frank, M. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2012). Sensitivity to reward and punishment in major depressive disorder: effects of rumination and of single versus multiple experiences. Cognition and Emotion, 26(8), 14751485. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2012.682973CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitton, A. E., Treadway, M. T., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2015). Reward processing dysfunction in major depression, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 28(1), 712. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000122CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkinson, G. S., & Robertson, G. J. (1993). Wide range achievement test 4. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology.Google Scholar
Zhang, T., Xu, L., Tang, Y., Li, H., Tang, X., Cui, H., … Group, S. S. (2019). Prediction of psychosis in prodrome: development and validation of a simple, personalized risk calculator. Psychological Medicine, 49(12), 19901998. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718002738CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zou, Y. M., Ni, K., Wang, Y. Y., Yu, E. Q., Lui, S. S. Y., Zhou, F. C., … Chan, R. C. K. (2020). Effort-cost computation in a transdiagnostic psychiatric sample: Differences among patients with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depressive disorder. Psychological Journal, 9(2), 210222. https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.316Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant sample

Figure 1

Figure 1. Reward processing profiles of each cluster. Clusters were characterized by a value representation deficit (cluster 1), a generalized deficit across reward processing domains (cluster 2), and a hedonic reactivity deficit (cluster 3). Diamonds denote mean scores and boxplots indicate the median and interquartile range. The dotted line at z = 0 represents the mean value of healthy control reference group to which task scores were z-scored.

Figure 2

Table 2. Cluster subgroup characteristics

Figure 3

Figure 2. Negative symptom severity in each cluster. (a) Mean negative symptom factor scores across clusters. (b) Mean negative symptom domain scores across clusters.* p < 0.05.** p < 0.01.

Supplementary material: File

Spilka et al. supplementary material

Spilka et al. supplementary material
Download Spilka et al. supplementary material(File)
File 256.7 KB