Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T04:38:56.731Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cicchetti’s organizational-developmental perspective of Down syndrome: Contributions to the emergence of developmental psychopathology and the study of persons with neurodevelopmental conditions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 October 2024

Jacob A. Burack*
Affiliation:
Department of Educational & Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
Natalie Russo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY, USA
David W. Evans
Affiliation:
Program in Neuroscience, Bucknell University, Lewisburg, PA, USA
Anna-Francesca Boatswain-Jacques
Affiliation:
Department of Educational & Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
Gabriela Rey
Affiliation:
Department of Educational & Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
Grace Iarocci
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
Robert M. Hodapp
Affiliation:
Department of Special Education, Vanderbilt University Peabody College, Nashville, TN, USA
*
Corresponding author: J. A. Burack; Email: jake.burack@mcgill.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Dante Cicchetti’s earliest work, his studies of social-emotional development in infants and children with Down syndrome, set the stage for the emergence of the larger field of developmental psychopathology. By applying basic developmental principles, methodologies, and questions to the study of persons with Down syndrome, Dante took on the challenge of searching for patterns in atypical development. In doing so, he extended traditional developmental theory and introduced a more “liberal” approach that both continues to guide developmentally based research with persons with neurodevelopmental conditions (NDCs), including Down syndrome. We highlight five themes from Dante’s work: (1) appreciating the importance of developmental level; (2) prioritizing the organization of development; (3) examining whether developmental factors work similarly in those with known genetic conditions; (4) rethinking narratives about ways of being; and (5) examining the influence of multiple levels of the environment on the individual’s functioning. We highlight ways that these essential lessons anticipated present-day research with persons with a variety of NDCs, including Down syndrome, other genetic syndromes associated with intellectual disability, and autism. We conclude with visions to the future for research with these populations as well as for the field of developmental psychopathology more generally.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

In honoring and extending Dante Cicchetti’s legacy in the field of developmental psychopathology, we focus here on his earliest line of research – the study of socio-emotional development in children with Down syndrome. This work began while Dante was an undergraduate with Felisicima Serafica at the University of Pittsburgh, and continued while he was a graduate student with Alan Sroufe at the University of Minnesota, and then as a faculty member at Harvard University. In their focus on children with Down syndrome, these studies differ considerably from much of Dante’s subsequent work with children and youth at risk for deleterious outcomes due to environmental experience (e.g., maltreatment) or to the complex interplay of environmental and genetic factors (e.g., depression). However, Dante’s work on Down syndrome was foundational in his establishing developmental psychopathology as a formal scholarly discipline.

The historical context of cicchetti’s research on Down syndrome

Dante’s studies of persons with Down syndrome came at a time of considerable fractionation in the relevant scholarly fields. The study of persons with intellectual disability, including those with Down syndrome, was largely constrained to the disciplines of special education and psychiatry as developmental psychologists still primarily focused on typically developing persons. Against this backdrop, a developmental approach to intellectual disability was only beginning to emerge (e.g., Zigler, Reference Zigler1967, Reference Zigler1969), as was the notion of a field of developmental psychopathology (e.g., Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1974), both of which Dante would go on to transform.

Based on world views from special education and psychiatry, studies of intellectual disability at the time were dominated by perspectives of deficit, pathology, and segregation. In the United States, the study of intellectual disability was largely dominated by so-called defect or difference theorists, researchers who sought to identify one or more key deficits that could be cited as the source of the lowered IQ scores that define intellectual disability. These researchers typically studied persons with intellectual disability as a single entity, despite its many different causes or etiologies (for discussions and critiques, see Burack et al., Reference Burack, Hodapp and Zigler1988; Burack, Reference Burack, Hodapp, Burack and Zigler1990; Burack & Zigler, Reference Burack and Zigler1990; Dykens et al., Reference Dykens, Hodapp and Finucane2000). Examining performance on various cognitive or behavioral tasks, they compared groups of persons with and without intellectual disability of the same chronological age; and the inevitable finding of worse performance by the participants with intellectual disability was then interpreted as reflecting a primary deficit (for presentations of several of these deficit theories, see Zigler & Balla, Reference Zigler and Balla1982).

The situation was somewhat different in the United Kingdom, where a few large-scale longitudinal studies on the development of children with Down syndrome were led by scholars from a range of backgrounds, including Janet Carr, a clinical psychologist, Cliff Cunningham, a former teacher, and Ann Gath, a child psychiatrist. In the first and longest-standing example, Carr studied 54 infants born between December 1963 and November 1964 within three boroughs of London and the county of Surrey. Capitalizing on medical advances that lengthened the life span of persons with Down syndrome well into their adult years, Carr extended her initial plan to study the developmental trajectories of these infants only during their first 4 years of life. Ultimately, she re-examined these individuals at 11 years, 21 years, and then more regularly until they were 50 years old. Publishing primarily in journals on intellectual disability, Carr (Reference Carr1970a, Reference Carr1970b; Carr & Collins, Reference Carr and Collins2014, Reference Carr and Collins2018; Murphy & Carr, Reference Murphy and Carr2020) provided extraordinary amounts of nuanced information about the biological, cognitive, and social development of her participants; their relationships with family members and others; their involvement in various aspects of society; and the effects of aging and dementia. In a research program about the early development of approximately 160 children with Down syndrome from a university research center in Manchester, Cunningham and colleagues (e.g., Berger & Cunningham, Reference Berger and Cunningham1981, Reference Berger and Cunningham1983; Glenn et al., Reference Glenn, Cunningham and Joyce1981) published findings on early vocal behaviors and interactions, social smiling, and eye contact between mothers and infants. These studies were among the first on Down syndrome to appear in mainstream developmental journals such as Developmental Psychology and Child Development. Based on a cohort of children with Down syndrome in Oxford, Gath and Gumley (Reference Gath and Gumley1984, Reference Gath and Gumley1986a, Reference Gath and Gumley1986b, Reference Gath and Gumley1987) published a series of papers that appeared primarily in psychiatry journals on the impact of having a child with Down syndrome on families, with a focus on the development of siblings.

Although also providing considerable knowledge about infants and children with Down syndrome, Dante’s studies were more experimental and were based in big-picture questions about universal developmental theory. For this reason, Dante’s work most converged with that of Ed Zigler (e.g., Reference Zigler1967, Reference Zigler1969), who had a few years earlier proposed a developmental approach to persons with intellectual disability that he contrasted with the various defect theories (for recent reviews, see Burack et al., Reference Burack, Evans, Napoleon, Weva, Russo and Iarocci2021a, Reference Burack, Evans, Russo, Napoleon, Goldman and Iarocci2021b; Hodapp, Reference Hodapp2021). In an early attempt to base the study of intellectual disability in traditional developmental theory, Zigler emphasized that persons with intellectual disability, regardless of intelligence level, traverse developmental milestones in the same order (i.e., the “similar sequence hypothesis”), albeit at slower rates and with lower asymptotes (for discussions, see Hodapp, Reference Hodapp, Hodapp, Burack and Zigler1990; Hodapp & Zigler, Reference Hodapp, Zigler, Cicchetti and Beeghly1990; Weisz & Zigler, Reference Weisz and Zigler1979; Zigler & Hodapp, Reference Zigler and Hodapp1986; Weisz et al., Reference Weisz, Yeates, Zigler, Zigler and Balla1982). He also hypothesized that, when matched on developmental level (mental age rather than chronological age), groups with and without intellectual disability should function at the same cognitive or linguistic levels (the “similar structure” hypothesis; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Weisz and Bromfield1986; Weisz & Yeates, Reference Weisz and Yeates1981). Also concerned with the “whole child” and the consequences of the frequent failures that this group experiences, Zigler (Reference Zigler1970a) argued that personality and motivational factors needed to be considered in understanding both task and real-world performance of persons with intellectual disability.

Zigler’s developmental approach represented an early attempt to base the study of intellectual disability in traditional developmental theory but was limited in its sole focus on the subgroup of children with so-called familial, cultural-familial, or sociocultural-familial intellectual disability (Burack & Zigler, Reference Burack and Zigler1990). This group is comprised of children for whom no clear organic cause could be identified for their delays in development. Following Gottesman (Reference Gottesman1963), who noted that IQ is partially determined by the operation of many inherited genes working together (i.e., “polygenic inheritance”), Zigler (Reference Zigler1970b) emphasized that these children were simply those who fell along the very lowest part of the normal distribution of IQ; their intellectual disability was not due to any specific organic etiology per se. The remaining children with intellectual disability were described by Zigler (Reference Zigler1967) as having “organic” intellectual disability. This group was comprised of children with Down syndrome or with any of the other approximately 1,100 genetic disorders (Batshaw et al., Reference Batshaw, Roizen and Pellegrino2019), associated with intellectual disability. Taking a conservative approach to development, Zigler (Reference Zigler1967) asserted, “The general developmental approach is applicable only to the familial [child with intellectual disabilities], and this approach does not speak to the issue of differences discovered between normal children and organic [children with intellectual disability]” (p. 269).

In contrast, in a series of articles and book chapters, Dante emphasized that developmental principles should extend even to the organic group, with his focus on children with Down syndrome. In this regard, Dante’s application of developmental theory to the study of children with Down syndrome also contrasted with the Zeitgeist of developmental theorists at the time. Traditional developmental theory, such as articulated by Piaget, had historically been framed almost solely within neurotypical Western frameworks, with only minimal reference to populations with potentially divergent developmental pathways, including those with intellectual disability or from non-Western backgrounds. By the 1970s, however, developmental theory was increasingly being deconstructed by those who emphasized its limitations in accounting for deviations from typical patterns, such as those associated with individual and cultural differences (for a discussion, see Bronfenbrenner et al., Reference Bronfenbrenner, Kessel, Kessen and White1986). These researchers questioned whether mainstream developmental theory should be maintained in light of the vast differences across persons that are seen in the real world across persons (Kessen, Reference Kessen and Sternberg1984).

Given Zigler’s (Reference Zigler1967) more conservative developmental approach to intellectual disability and the de-construction of the very idea of development, Dante’s commitment to asking big developmental questions about persons with Down syndrome represented a bold theoretical stance. By applying basic developmental principles, methodologies, and questions to the study of persons with Down syndrome, Dante took on the challenge of searching for developmental patterns in atypical development, thereby extending, rather than rejecting, traditional developmental theory.

Dante’s scholarship on children with Down syndrome

As early as the mid-1970’s, almost a decade before his special issue inauguration of the field of developmental psychopathology (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984), Dante infused developmental tenets and principles in his research with children with Down syndrome. Fostering the premise that human development follows a universally organized and integrated process, Dante and colleagues highlighted the coherence of development (Motti et al., Reference Motti, Cicchetti and Sroufe1983), that is, the process rather than the pace of development (Cicchetti & Sroufe, Reference Cicchetti, Sroufe, Lewis and Rosenblum1978). In Dante’s early work on the level and quality of play in children with Down Syndrome, individual differences in play were strongly predicted by indexes of affective expressiveness and Bayley Developmental Quotient scores. Moreover, when matched by mental age, children with Down syndrome had the same level of play as typically developing children (Motti et al., Reference Motti, Cicchetti and Sroufe1983).

The issue of developmental level and appropriate comparison strategies was also central to Cicchetti and Sroufe’s (Reference Cicchetti and Sroufe1976) consideration of tensions between developmental level (as determined by mental age) and chronological age in the study of the development of laughter among infants with Down Syndrome. They noted that, although potentially delayed, the development of these children proceeds in a similar sequence to non-intellectually disabled infants. In essence, cognitive ability, not chronological age, drove the development of laughter in 4–8-month-old infants with Down syndrome, with development occurring in the same order across 30 developmentally ordered items.

The complexity of applying the developmental approach was further evident in Butterworth and Cicchetti’s (Reference Butterworth and Cicchetti1978) findings that attachment behaviors were largely similar to those of typically developing children in terms of sequences and behaviors, although with a few exceptions. The ways in which these exceptions were interpreted by Butterworth and Cicchetti also suggests a deep respect for differences in the experiences of those with Down syndrome (e.g., different interpretation of what it means to be alone); a deep-seated desire to integrate across levels of measurement that included genes, neurotransmitters, cognition, and behavior; and a continued focus on the general universality of developmental sequences and structures. Butterworth and Cicchetti highlighted the reciprocal relationship between typical and atypical development by focusing on how the study of the visual calibration of posture over early development among those with Down syndrome can inform our understanding of the development of these same behaviors in typically developing children.

Dante identified similar patterns of developmental coherence across domains of functioning. For example, Beeghly and Cicchetti (Reference Beeghly and Cicchetti1997) found both that infants with Down Syndrome and typically developing infants followed the same general sequence of internal state lexicon language skills and a strong association among age, general cognitive functioning, and expressive language ability. In a study of self-recognition, Mans et al. (Reference Mans, Cicchetti and Sroufe1978) found that children with Down syndrome and typically developing children showed self-recognition at the same time when matched on cognitive age rather than chronological age. They argued that “In Down Syndrome, the emergence of self-recognition appears to be an orderly and meaningful process. Lower intelligence, per se, does not imply a different developmental sequence.” (p. 1249). Although Thompson et al. (Reference Thompson, Cicchetti, Lamb and Malkin1985) found differences in the quality of separation distress among children with Down syndrome versus children without Down syndrome in the strange situation attachment task, they also identified a “consistent organization underlying this aspect of socioemotional responsiveness” for both groups (p.828).

These early observations are all grounded in traditional developmental frameworks (e.g., Piaget, Reference Piaget1970), including those of the study of intellectual disability (Hodapp et al., Reference Hodapp, Burack, Zigler, Hodapp, Burack and Zigler1990), and an example of how evidence from an atypical population tells us about the necessity or inevitability of developmental processes. Despite the considerable evidence of universality, Serafica and Cicchetti (Reference Serafica and Cicchetti1976) warned against studying individuals with intellectual disability as a single group, highlighting that clear patterns of development can only be found by studying etiologically homogeneous groupings such as persons with Down syndrome. Within the context of this type of homogeneous population, Dante and colleagues highlighted the importance of contextualizing development to incorporate the diverse processes and environments that the child is experiencing.

The originality of the contribution of Dante’s broadening of the developmental approach is perhaps best illustrated in Cicchetti and Pogge-Hesse’s (Reference Cicchetti, Pogge-Hesse, Zigler and Balla1982) chapter in Zigler and Balla’s (Reference Zigler and Balla1982) volume, Mental retardation: The developmental-difference controversy. In that chapter, the only one on development in persons with an organic form of intellectual disability (i.e., not familial intellectual disability), Cicchetti and Pogge-Hesse invoked a more “liberal,” expansive view of development (for a later iteration, see Cicchetti & Ganiban, Reference Cicchetti, Ganiban, Hodapp, Burack and Zigler1990). Premised on Werner’s (Reference Werner1948, Reference Werner and Harris1957) orthogenetic principle, they envisioned a developmental approach that guided research about Down syndrome as well as other conditions associated with intellectual disability. Cicchetti and Pogge-Hesse explicitly extended beyond discussions of the “typical” or “usual” sequences and structures of development that had been applied to persons with familial intellectual disability (Weisz et al., Reference Weisz, Yeates, Zigler, Zigler and Balla1982; Zigler & Balla, Reference Zigler and Balla1982). Instead, in their critiques of defect/difference theorists, Cicchetti and Pogge-Hesse’s (Reference Cicchetti, Pogge-Hesse, Zigler and Balla1982) took on more complex developmental questions, such as those about the organization of development and mutual influences between typical and atypical development.

Using persons with Down syndrome as a proxy for all etiological-specific groups of persons with organic forms of intellectual disability (e.g., persons with Williams syndrome or fragile syndrome), Cicchetti and Pogge-Hesse (Reference Cicchetti, Pogge-Hesse, Zigler and Balla1982) were setting the stage for a field of developmental psychopathology that Dante would soon articulate. As Dante would later note (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984), the basic principles of developmental psychopathology specify that: (1) scholarship about persons with pathology and differences needed to be fused with and informed by developmental theory; and relatedly (2) that typical and atypical development were mutually informative.

Based on these developmental principles and Dante’s early studies of the development of infants with Down syndrome, Cicchetti and Pogge-Hesse (Reference Cicchetti, Pogge-Hesse, Zigler and Balla1982) argued that the defect approach, and de facto any similar pathologizing approach, “overlooks the possibility that the behavior and development of retarded children is organized, adaptive, and integrated just as is the case for nonretarded children and infants. We know that they are retarded; the important and challenging research questions concern the developmental process” (Cicchetti & Pogge-Hesse, Reference Cicchetti, Pogge-Hesse, Zigler and Balla1982, p. 279). They further noted that “Organically retarded persons are not only ‘different’ from nonretarded persons… They are organized in their own right… Thus, a contribution will be made to a general and integrated structural-organismic theory of development that tries to account for development in all human beings across all behavioral domains” (Cicchetti & Pogge-Hesse, Reference Cicchetti, Pogge-Hesse, Zigler and Balla1982, p. 313). In doing so, Dante highlighted the extent to which the understanding of persons with Down syndrome, like those of any population, is inextricably linked to and informative about the study of the general population. Thus “children with Down syndrome are, indeed, a particularly interesting population to study from a developmental view” (Cicchetti & Pogge-Hesse, Reference Cicchetti, Pogge-Hesse, Zigler and Balla1982, p. 293) as they (1) provide rare insight into the sequelae of an extra autosomal chromosome; and (2) allow for a better understanding of true convergences and discontinuities due to the slower cognitive development. These quotes are all precursors to the essential point, or dictum, of Dante’s introductory article (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984) to the special issue of Child Development (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984) introducing the field of developmental psychopathology, that, “you can learn more about the normal functioning of an organism by studying its pathology, and, likewise, more about its pathology by studying its normal condition” (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984, p. 1).

The relevance of this dictum was evident in the first two publications on intellectual disability that appeared in this journal, the flagship publication of the field of developmental psychopathology. In the first paper, Hodapp and Burack (Reference Hodapp and Burack1990) addressed half of the dictum, “What mental retardation teaches us about typical development: The examples of sequences, rates, and cross-domain relations.” They portrayed intellectual disability research as an “experiment of nature” that can be used to both replicate findings from the study of typical development and provide evidence that extends developmental theory. In the second paper, comparing developmental patterns of language, affect, and visuo-spatial skills between children with Down syndrome and those with William Syndrome, Reilly et al. (Reference Reilly, Klima and Bellugi1990) extended Dante’s dictum by comparing two “atypical” populations. That is, just as typical and atypical development can be mutually informative, so too can the development of two different atypical populations.

Over the past few decades, both comparative frameworks have been central to cutting-edge research in persons with intellectual disability. For example, Landau and colleagues have utilized findings from persons with Williams syndrome to provide compelling cases for a better general understanding of various aspects of development including the organization of spatial representation (Landau, Reference Landau and Burack2012), spatial language (Landau & Zukowski, Reference Landau and Zukowski2003) and spatial cognition (Landau & Hoffman, Reference Landau and Hoffman2005); and the disassociations between intuitive physics and psychology (Kamps et al., Reference Kamps, Julian, Battaglia, Landau, Kanwisher and Dilks2017) and of vision for perception and vision for action (Dilks et al., Reference Dilks, Hoffman and Landau2008). Similarly, Mervis (Reference Mervis, Burack, Hodapp, Iarocci and Zigler2012) highlighted the extent to which evidence from children with Williams syndrome provides a strong case for the interdependence of language and cognition throughout development. Cross-etiology comparisons have grown to be essential to the study of the development across many areas of functioning, including attention (Cornish & Wilding, Reference Cornish and Wilding2010), language (Hofmann & Müller, Reference Hofmann and Müller2021), visual processing (Ly & Hodapp, Reference Ly and Hodapp2005), and maladaptive behavior (Dykens & Kasari, Reference Dykens and Kasari1997).

Dante’s questions as precursors of scholarship about persons with neurodevelopmental conditions

Just as Dante’s early work on Down syndrome provided a guiding framework for the emergence and growth of developmental psychopathology, it largely anticipated contemporary research among persons with NDCs including Down syndrome, other genetic syndromes, and autism. In discussing advances from our own and others’ research, we again invoke Dante’s seminal conceptualizations, and essential quotes, from his work with children with Down syndrome.

Conceptualization 1: appreciate the importance of developmental level

“…the important and challenging research questions concern the developmental process..”

For the study of group comparisons, Dante extended the idea of mental age matching, or considering developmental level, in research to include individuals whose intellectual disability is due to an organic etiology. This lesson remains critical to understanding unique facets and universals in development and has clinical implications as well. One example involves children with fetal alcohol effects (FAEs) who are often described as inattentive – more than 90% of children with FAE are diagnosed with ADHD and many of those children are placed on medications.

To assess attentional difficulties and contrast clinical measures of attention, Lane et al. (Reference Lane, Stewart, Fernandes, Russo, Enns and Burack2014) compared children with FAE to a group of children whose development was typical and did not include exposure to alcohol. Although the group of children with FAE had cognitive abilities in the average range, they were in the low average range and as a result, Lane et al. (Reference Lane, Stewart, Fernandes, Russo, Enns and Burack2014) compared the groups on the basis of developmental level rather than chronological age to account for these IQ differences. On the Conners (Reference Conners1997), a parent report of inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity, the children with FAE were rated as being in the clinically significant range. In a clinical setting, this would be a strong indicator that a diagnosis of ADHD combined presentation might be appropriate (if this occurred across contexts, was reported by one or more people, and impaired the child’s functioning). In contrast, on the Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch) (Manly et al., Reference Manly, Robertson, Anderson and Nimmo-Smith1998), the children with FAE performed in a manner that was largely similar to the typically developing children when the groups were compared on the basis of developmental level or mental age. Together, these findings highlight the critical importance of the metric by which comparisons are made for those with lower cognitive abilities and the relatively high potential for empirical findings to have real clinical impacts. In one scenario, when attention (in this case parent reported behaviors indicative of attention problems) was constructed relative to expectations based on chronological age, children with FAE would likely be diagnosed with a chronic psychiatric condition, for which medications are the front-line treatment. Conversely, in another, when attention abilities (in this case measured on the basis of a standardized test) were understood relative to children’s developmental level, they would be expected to score within the average range. The metric of comparison, chronological or developmental age, is critical and requires care and consideration in both the choice of matching measure and the interpretation of research findings (Burack et al., Reference Burack, Iarocci, Flanagan and Bowler2004; Russo et al., Reference Russo, Kaplan-Kahn, Wilson, Criss and Burack2021).

Despite the self-evident significance of appropriate developmental matching, neuroscience researchers of intellectual disability have not embraced this approach as extensively as could be hoped (Burack et al., Reference Burack, Russo, Kovshoff, Palma Fernandes, Ringo, Landry and Iarocci2016a). This field is often compromised by studies in which comparisons between individuals with and without intellectual disability that are based on chronological, rather than mental age, lead to spurious interpretations of group differences. As lower IQ is pathognomonic to genetic syndromes associated with intellectual disability, chronological age-based comparisons tell us nothing about the specific syndromes being examined (Burack et al., Reference Burack, Friedman, Lessage and Brodeur2023a). Rather, they tell us only that those with an intellectual disability are performing less well than would be expected for their age, the very definition of intellectual disability.

Conceptualization 2: prioritize the organization of development, even among children with different genetic conditions

Organically retarded persons are not only ‘different’ from nonretarded persons… They are organized in their own right…”

Dante’s studies on the development of infants and infants with Down syndrome within an organizational perspective were paradigms for the extensive study of “behavioral phenotypes” of different conditions associated with intellectual disability (e.g., Cornish & Wilding, Reference Cornish and Wilding2010; Dykens et al., Reference Dykens, Hodapp and Finucane2000). In this framework, we document ways in which specific etiological groups show their own particular, etiology-related cognitive-linguistic-adaptive organization or patterns of strengths or weaknesses across domain. Throughout this work, caveats have been provided that (a) not every child with a particular syndrome would show that syndrome’s “characteristic” strengths and weaknesses; and (b) not every etiological group would differ from every other group on all behaviors (Hodapp, Reference Hodapp2021).

As etiology-specific research progressed, researchers began to pay greater attention to development “into” the fully formed behavioral phenotypes. Across multiple studies by different research groups, etiology-specific performance of persons with Down syndrome was evident, including in executive functions (Hodapp & Fidler, Reference Hodapp, Fidler, Glidden, Abbeduto, McIntyre and Tassé2021), which relate to the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum (Edgin, Reference Edgin2013; Nadel, Reference Nadel2003), and for which lower-than-MA performance could be seen on neuropsychological tasks that involve working memory, planning, shifting, and inhibitory control. In addition to closer ties to their neuropsychological functioning, researchers are now exploring how infants and toddlers with Down syndrome progress in executive functions over the infancy period. For example, Schworer et al. (Reference Schworer, Fidler, Kaur, Needham, Prince and Daunhauer2022) compared infants with Down syndrome to typically developing, MA-matched infants (mean age = 8.62 months), on a set of executive function “precursor tasks” (i.e., attending to an object, shifting attention, and planning to perform an object like reaching). As expected by the behavioral profile, the infants with Down syndrome performed worse than the MA-matches on these precursor tasks. When a subset of the infants with Down syndrome were then examined 6 months later on an actual executive function task (the “A not B” object permanence test), performance levels on precursor tests at Time 1 were related to actual executive function tasks six months later at Time 2.

These findings have both conceptual and clinical implications. Theoretically, they illustrate the construct of “developmental cascades,” another developmental phenomenon discussed by Dante. As described by Masten and Cicchetti (Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010), “Developmental cascades refer to the cumulative consequences for development of the many interactions and transactions occurring in developing systems that result in spreading effects across levels, among domains at the same level, and across different systems or generations” (Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010, p. 491). In this preliminary work, cascades are evident among infants with Down syndrome, with the “spreading effects” evident across time (from T1 to T2) and from precursor to actual executive function tasks (Malachowski & Needham, Reference Malachowski and Needham2023).

These findings also lead to intervention implications. In what they refer to as a “syndrome-informed micro-intervention,” Fidler et al. (Reference Fidler, Schworer, Needham, Prince, Patel, Will and Daunhauer2021) launched a brief caregiver-facilitated intervention concerning goal-directed infant reaching, among the earliest examples of executive functions. Infants with Down syndrome were randomly assigned to intervention and control groups, with mothers in each group interacting with their infants around infant toys for 5–10 minutes each day. Each toy was presented to the infant at eye level and shaken, tapped, or moved to make the toy optimally salient. Infants would then reach out toward the desired toy. The only difference was that the intervention infants wore “Sticky Mittens,” or Velcro mittens that latched on to the infant toys making the toy easier for the infant to grasp. At posttreatment in which no infants had the use of the Sticky Mittens, the infants in the treatment group showed more goal-directed reaches to the desired object and contacted the object more quickly (shorter mean latency), with large effect sizes. Fidler et al., also showed that the optimal window for the intervention involved infants with Down syndrome who were from 5 to 10 months of age; younger and older infants showed less improvement. Although preliminary, etiology-based interventions seem both feasible and effective.

Conceptualization 3: examine whether developmental factors work similarly in those with known genetic conditions

“…the behavior and development of retarded children is organized, adaptive, and integrated—just as is the case for nonretarded children and infants…” (Cicchetti & Pogge-Hesse, Reference Cicchetti, Pogge-Hesse, Zigler and Balla1982)

In the developmental literature on neurotypical persons, the development of IQ is thought to be linked to polygenic inheritance. In this framework, parents who are tall typically have tall children but can have one or more short children, just as two parents with above-average intelligence can have a child of average or even below average intelligence. The relative deficit in height or in intelligence can occur not because of any specific genetic mutations, but because both height and cognitive ability are complex, largely heritable, traits stemming from polygenic effects. In most cases, however, the height or intelligence of parents is correlated with height-intelligence of their biological offspring.

Although the IQs of persons with the familial form of intellectual disability are also thought to conform to the same processes of inheritance (e.g., Zigler, Reference Zigler1967, Reference Zigler1969), known genetic causes of intellectual disability, such as Down syndrome, are often viewed as immutable conditions that are impervious to social and familial influences. They are commonly thought to be defined solely by the genetics of the condition and devoid of genetic influences that are not associated with the syndrome. However, we now know that this is not the case. This was exemplified in research with persons with Turner syndrome (X,0), a whole chromosomal aneuploidy on the sex chromosomes, resulting in a deletion of a second X (or Y) chromosome. Although Turner Syndrome shows wide phenotypic variability, one of the common physical phenotypes is short stature. However, 50 years ago, Brook et al. (Reference Brook, Mürset, Zachmann and Prader1974) observed that the adult height of females with Turner syndrome, while some 3 standard deviations below the mean height of both parents, highly correlated to bi-parental mean height (r = .84). Several other researchers have reported similar findings. In each case, despite a shift from bi-parental mean height of between 2 and 4 standard deviations, the parent–offspring correlations are preserved, and remain similar to what one would expect to find in the general population (r’s ranging from .42 to .84; see Lemli & Smith, Reference Lemli and Smith1963; Brook et al., Reference Brook, Mürset, Zachmann and Prader1974; Reference Brook, Gasser, Werder, Prader and Vanderschueren-Lodewykx1977; Holl et al., Reference Holl, Kunze, Etzrodt, Teller and Heinze1994; Massa & Vanderschueren-Lodeweyckx, Reference Massa and Vanderschueren-Lodeweyckx1991; Rochiccioli et al., Reference Rochiccioli, David, Malpuech, Colle, Limal, Battin, Mariani, Sultan, Nivelon and Simonin1994). This is also the case for Prader–Willi syndrome (15q 11–13) – although offspring are between 1 and 2 standard deviations shorter than their parents, the parent–offspring correlation remains what one would expect under typical circumstances (r = ∼ .50). Persons with Klinefelter syndrome (47, XXY) are often above average in height owing to this aneuploidy, but here again, while they are some 2 standard deviations taller than the mean height of their parents, the parent–offspring correlation is preserved (r = .64; Brook et al., Reference Brook, Gasser, Werder, Prader and Vanderschueren-Lodewykx1977).

These types of connections between parental and offspring characteristics occur as well in other traits, such as cognitive or social ability. One example involves 16p11.2 deletion, a micro-deletion syndrome comprising some 26 genes, resulting in wide phenotypic variability (Stefansson et al., Reference Stefansson, Meyer-Lindenberg, Steinberg, Magnusdottir, Morgen, Arnarsdottir, Bjornsdottir, Walters, Jonsdottir, Doyle, Tost, Grimm, Kristjansdottir, Snorrason, Davidsdottir, Gudmundsson, Jonsson, Stefansdottir, Helgadottir, Haraldsson, Jonsdottir, Thygesen, Schwarz, Didriksen, Stensbøl, Brammer, Kapur, Halldorsson, Hreidarsson, Saemundsen, Sigurdsson and Stefansson2014; Moreno De Luca et al., Reference Moreno De Luca, Evans, Boomer, Hanson, Bernier, Goin-Kochel, Myers, Challman, Moreno De Luca, Spiro, Chung, Martin and Ledbetter2014). In this case, some 25% of offspring with the deletion are diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) and approximately 25% with intellectual disability (Hanson et al., Reference Hanson, Bernier, Porche, Goin-Kochel, Snyder, Snow-Gallagher, Wallace, Campe, Zhang, Chen, Moreno De Luca, Orr, Boomer, Evans, Martin, Ledbetter, Spiro and Chung2014; Moreno De Luca et al., Reference Moreno De Luca, Evans, Boomer, Hanson, Bernier, Goin-Kochel, Myers, Challman, Moreno De Luca, Spiro, Chung, Martin and Ledbetter2014). However, although most persons with this deletion do not reach clinical thresholds for ASD or intellectual disability, within-family correlations are preserved. Thus, when compared to first degree relatives (parents and non-carrier siblings), the persons with the16p11.2 deletion demonstrated a shift of −1.7 standard deviations in full-scale IQ, even as the intraclass correlation was significant (r = .42). In terms of social development as well, scores on the Social Responsiveness Scale (a dimensional scale of autistic traits) showed connections among the persons with the deletion, parents, and the non-carrier sibling. Relative to bi-parental mean and sibling scores, the persons with the deletion were shifted more than 2 standard deviations (they were more symptomatic), even as the intraclass correlation was again significant (r = .52). This shift-but-spared pattern of correlations was also observed for fine motor ability (Moreno-De-Luca et al., Reference Moreno-De-Luca, Evans, Boomer, Hanson, Bernier, Goin-Kochel, Myers, Challman, Moreno-De-Luca, Slane, Hare, Chung, Spiro, aucett, Martin and Ledbetter2015).

The connections to familial genetics can be further extended to real-world adaptation, Evans and Uljarević (Reference Evans and Uljarević2018) examined whether the IQs of persons with DS were correlated with parental education level. Parental education is a particularly informative variable as it reflects both some margin of cognitive ability and the broader sociodemographic and cultural-familial milieu. Evans and Uljarević used a combined cross-sectional (two age groups: 4–12 years; 12–21 years) and longitudinal (over a 2-year period) design. As predicted given previous findings, bi-parental education levels were correlated with the IQs of the offspring with Down syndrome. The findings indicated that these associations were particularly strong for verbal IQ in the older age group and at Time 2; the weakest in the younger cohort at Time 1. However, overall, the patterns were fairly consistent for full-scale, verbal and non-verbal IQ, with median r values of approximately .50.

From these studies, and despite the clear impact of various genetic anomalies on various domains of functioning, persons with Down syndrome are still impacted by a variety of familial factors. We cannot argue definitively whether these factors involve polygenic effects, or whether some impact of environment mediates the developmental outcomes of children with such conditions. What is clear, however, is that similar factors are likely at play in determining outcomes of children with NDCs as with neurotypical children. The story is complicated, as the mutability of functional domains may vary depending on the degree of heritability of a given trait; some heritable traits may be more genetically complex, and some may be more influenced by environmental factors. Still, despite the presence of a known genetic condition, the development of children with and without NDCs are similarly impacted by multiple, and likely interacting, genetic and environmental factors.

Conceptualization 4: rethink narratives about ways of being

“we should study not “does × have y” but rather “in what way does × use y ”and does a particular mental structure interact with other mental structures in the same way in one group as in another?” (Cicchetti and Pogge-Hesse, Reference Cicchetti, Pogge-Hesse, Zigler and Balla1982).

Dante’s invoking of Werner’s (Reference Werner and Harris1957) orthogenetic principle that development is adaptive, organized, hierarchical, and universal implies that an organism, and the behaviors it generates at any given time point, reflect the cumulative endpoint of an organized, systematic developmental process. This type of perspective is the essence of the opposition of developmental theorists to deficit models of intellectual disability and is central to the rethinking of narratives about autistic persons who historically have been studied with the emphasis on what they cannot do or accomplish (for a discussion, please see Bottema-Beutel et al., Reference Bottema-Beutel, Kapp, Lester, Sasson and Hand2021). Over the past four decades, prominent deficit models include those related to theory of mind (Baron-Cohen et al., Reference Baron-Cohen, Leslie and Frith1985), weak central coherence (Frith & Happé, Reference Frith and Happé1994), and various aspects of attention (Burack, Reference Burack1994; Klin et al., Reference Klin, Jones, Schultz, Volkmar and Cohen2002). However, these narratives have been revised considerably in relation to: (1) evidence that autistic persons process, and therefore, act on the world in different ways than do neurotypical persons; and (2) the advent of the neurodiversity movement emanating from the voices of both the autistic and research communities (for a review, see Pellicano & den Houting, Reference Pellicano and den Houting2022). Although the term neurodiversity was coined by Judy Singer in 1990 to underscore the value of different minds, many precursors of the notion can be seen in Dante’s writings. The titles of his (more than 700) published papers do not directly mention the term, however, the fundamental tenets of developmental psychopathology are consistent with a deep respect for, and valuing of, divergence.

Within the scholarly literature, the current narratives about autistic persons are increasingly focused on their unique abilities, cognitive styles, perceptual biases, and motivation in interacting with the people and world around them (Burack et al., Reference Burack, Russo, Kovshoff, Palma Fernandes, Ringo, Landry and Iarocci2016; Happé and Frith, Reference Happé and Frith2020). This type of narrative has been presented several times over the past few decades despite the more common lay, clinical, medical, and scholarly emphases on pathology. For example, Schultz and colleagues (Grelotti et al., Reference Grelotti, Gauthier and Schultz2002; Schultz et al., Reference Schultz, Gauthier, Klin, Fulbright, Anderson, Volkmar, Skudlarski, Lacadie, Cohen and Gore2000) argued that the fusiform gyrus and related brain areas are used effectively by autistic people, but in different ways and to process different objects than might be seen in the neurotypical population. In developing their enhanced perceptual functioning model, Mottron and Burack (Reference Mottron, Burack, Burack, Charman, Yirmiya and Zelazo2001; Mottron et al., Reference Mottron, Dawson, Soulières, Hubert and Burack2006) highlighted that rather than being a deficit, the unique way that autistic people process information in the world may lead to certain advantages on specific types of tasks. This type of evidence even led to the reformulating of primary deficit theories, most notably the weak central coherence theory of autism (Happé & Frith, Reference Happé and Frith2006).

In borrowing from Cicchetti and Pogge-Hesse (Reference Cicchetti, Pogge-Hesse, Zigler and Balla1982), Burack et al. (Reference Burack, Russo, Kovshoff, Palma Fernandes, Ringo, Landry and Iarocci2016) sought to recast the literature on attention in autism within the framework of “how, rather than on how well” autistic people attend within their environments. They reviewed several examples in which the performance of autistic persons on tasks of attention and perception could be, and in some cases were, interpreted as evidence of deficit may be reframed as an alternative, and often more adaptive or efficient, style of processing and way of being. In this reshaped narrative, the consideration of context and development are essential in understanding how biases, styles, and ways of being of autistic people affect their attention in the real world. Burack et al. highlighted that cases of competition among attentional demands, preferences, biases, and perspectives might lead autistic persons to filter, orient, or selectively attend to some features over others. Although what is being attended to in any particular context may appear atypical to the outside observer, one should not infer that any particular attentional skill is impaired or better or worse than for any other person. In one extension of that approach, Brodeur et al. (Reference Brodeur, Stewart, Dawkins and Burack2018) and Burack and Brodeur (Reference Burack, Brodeur and Volkmar2020) argued that the attentional style of autistic persons is particularly utilitarian, a way of being that can lead to greater efficiency and in turn any number of benefits, including those related to education or employment.

In trying to disentangle the neural mechanisms that may underlie some of these unique ways in how autistic people attend, Russo et al. (Reference Russo, Mottron, Burack and Jemel2012) and Kaplan-Kahn et al. (Reference Kaplan-Kahn, Park and Russo2021) leveraged the exquisite temporal resolution of Event-Related Potentials or ERPs. ERPs reflect the activity of synchronized postsynaptic potentials that can be measured at the scalp via electrodes in response to a particular event. One of the most commonly measured ERP components is the N400 effect, which is a negative deflection that occurs around 400ms after a stimulus that is incongruent with its semantic context. The most classic example is provided by Kutas and Hillyard (Reference Kutas and Hillyard1980), the first to document this effect, who presented participants with sentences that either did (e.g., he spread the bread with butter) or did not (e.g., he spread the bread with socks) end as would be expected. Compared to congruent sentences, incongruent sentences yielded this negativity around 400ms.

The N400 effect, which was not found among autistic persons in several studies (for a review, see Russo et al., Reference Russo, Mottron, Burack and Jemel2012), was often cited in relation to the rather delayed language development commonly seen among autistic persons. However, Russo et al. (Reference Russo, Mottron, Burack and Jemel2012) noted that the behavioral performance of autistic youth on N400 tasks (when it was measured) did not differ from that of IQ and/or age matched youth, leaving open the question of how autistic people actually solved this semantic problem. To answer this question, they developed a simple task where they asked participants to press one button if the sound and image of an animal (e.g., a barking dog) matched and a different button if the sound and picture did not match (e.g., a barking cat). Rather than looking for the presence or absence of an N400, Russo et al. (Reference Russo, Mottron, Burack and Jemel2012) examined modulations comparing congruent and incongruent trial types in early, perceptual timeframes, based on the premise (e.g., Mottron & Burack, Reference Mottron, Burack, Burack, Charman, Yirmiya and Zelazo2001) that autism is associated with an increased reliance on perceptual processes. They replicated the findings of others as (a) the behavioral performance of the autistic and the nonautistic participants was the same and (b) the nonautistic, but not the IQ and age matched autistic, group demonstrated an N400. However, they also found that autistic youth differentiated congruent and incongruent trials by 100ms post stimulus, a full 300ms before their nonautistic peers. These findings are consistent with the emphasis that it’s not “how well” but rather how one uses what they have to process the world that matters and is scientifically informative.

In addition to highlighting the unique ways underlying how autistic individuals process their environments, Kaplan-Kahn et al. (Reference Kaplan-Kahn, Park and Russo2021) assessed whether perception interacted with semantic processes in nonautistic individuals. They asked college students without autism to complete the same animal congruence task described above while they measured ERPs. At the end of the task, the participants completed the Autism Quotient, a self-report questionnaire of autistic traits. The brain’s differentiation between barking dogs and barking cats at 100ms mediated the relationship between attention to detail as measured by the AQ and the N400 effect suggesting a similar interaction between cognitive/neural processes in autistic and nonautistic persons.

Conceptualization 5: examine the influence of multiple levels of the environment on the individual’s functioning

a contribution will be made to a general and integrated structural-organismic theory of development that tries to account for development in all human beings across all behavioral domains” (Cicchetti & Pogge-Hesse, Reference Cicchetti, Pogge-Hesse, Zigler and Balla1982, p. 313).

In his early work, Dante highlighted the central role of relationships, particularly parent–child relationships, in shaping social and emotional development. In particular, caregiver attachment was considered central to the development of social competence in children. However, Dante emphasized the need to consider social competence, the ability to effectively navigate social situations, build positive relationships, and engage in age normative social behaviors (Dodge, Reference Dodge, Schneider, Rubin and Ledingham1985; Rose-Krasnor, Reference Rose-Krasnor1997), within diverse social environments. This includes an understanding of how cultural contexts influence socialization practices and shape social interactions in the moment as well as over the span of a child’s development. In this framework, the assessment of social competence involves examining multiple dimensions and identifying key skills such as empathy, cooperation, conflict resolution, and the ability to establish and maintain connections with others.

Social competence is context-dependent, as individuals demonstrate different levels of competence across different situations. They must skillfully coordinate multiple processes (e.g., cognition, emotion) and integrate contextual factors (e.g., social norms, setting) to adequately meet the social demands of particular situations (e.g., peer-to-peer or parent–child interaction) (Crnic, Reference Crnic, Cicchetti and Beeghly1990; Iarocci et al., Reference Iarocci, Yager and Elfers2007, Reference Iarocci, Yager, Rombough and McLaughlin2008). However, how well an individual navigates different social contexts and the specific relational challenges they may encounter is also dependent on their social partner. An understanding of the relational aspects of social interactions, particularly within the context of culturally diverse social partners, can help us extend Dante’s valuable work on identifying both risk and protective factors and the potential for resilience in the social development of children with NDCs.

Autism may offer a critical window into the dynamic and relational nature of social interactions and adaptation. In one example of how cultural or, in the case of autism, subcultural differences may influence social interactions is the ‘double empathy problem’ introduced by the autistic researcher, Damian Milton (Reference Milton2012). Milton highlights a mutual struggle to understand and connect between autistic and nonautistic individuals. That is, difficulties in communication and understanding are reciprocal, as both autistic and nonautistic individuals may experience challenges in comprehending each other’s communication styles and social cues. The concept draws parallels with the idea of cultural differences in communication. Just as individuals from different cultures may experience challenges in understanding each other’s communication styles, autistic individuals, who may be considered a subculture in mainstream society, may face similar challenges due to differences in their cognitive and perceptual styles.

Recognizing the relational nature of social relationships (and the potential challenges) between autistic persons and their social partners fosters a better understanding and inclusivity. It suggests that efforts to improve communication should be bidirectional, with an emphasis on mutual accommodation and appreciation for diverse communication styles. Thus, social competence is closely tied to social awareness and sensitivity, as an individual’s awareness of social cues and their ability to adapt to the emotions and needs of others are critical to effective and inclusive social behavior. The social acceptance and inclusion of people with NDCs rely on a thorough understanding of how both neurodivergent and neurotypical individuals behave in social relationships. For example, autistic individuals’ social abilities may not be the only factors influencing their social success or lack thereof. Rather, the social perceptions and behaviors of nonautistic persons contribute to autistic individuals’ social difficulties.

Scheerer et al. (Reference Scheerer, Boucher, Sasson and Iarocci2022) attempted to improve nonautistic teenagers’ views of autistic people by teaching them about autism in a way to which they can more easily relate. They then assessed both whether the quality and quantity of contact with autistic people predicted these perceptions and whether their views of autistic people depend on how they view their own social skills. Scheerer et al., found that nonautistic high school students showed negative attitudes toward autistic people, but that these views could be improved by the informative presentation about autism. They also found that the youth who reported higher quality interactions with autistic people had less negatively biased attitudes toward them, but those who self-reported higher social competence rated autistic people the most negatively. One possible reason is that they are the most likely to value ‘normative’ social skills and be more judgmental about those who socialize in a different way. These findings helps us better understand how the social perceptions of neurotypical youth toward autistic youth may contribute to their social challenges.

Knowledge about the attitudes and perceptions of neurotypical individuals toward neurodivergent individuals can contribute to efforts aimed at reducing stigma and discrimination. This was highlighted by the finding that the social perceptions in the neurotypical youth were malleable with a deeper understanding of autistic people. By addressing misconceptions and fostering empathy, we can create social attitudes and behaviors that are mutually accepting and supportive. To date we have focused almost exclusively on neurodivergent people and their social “disabilities” without much consideration of neurotypical influences on neurodivergent people’s social behavior/outcomes. The research on the social (dis)abilities of neurodivergent people would benefit from Dante’s idea of understanding typical and atypical development within a wholistic framework. This approach views individuals of all neurotypes as valuable in their own right, contributing to the social diversity essential for human resilience.

Dante’s influence on the future of developmental psychopathology and the study of persons with neurodevelopmental conditions

Begun while he was an undergraduate and emphasized during the early part of his career, Dante Cicchetti’s contributions to developmental approaches to the study of Down syndrome and intellectual disability more generally is almost immeasurable (for a brief discussion, see Burack et al., Reference Burack, Lai, Stewart, Landry, Burack, Edgin and Abbeduto2023b). Dante infused the field with a rigorous scientific methodology encapsulated in theoretical sophistication (for a discussion, see Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Ganiban, Cicchett, Cicchetti and Beeghly1990). Dante allowed us to ask questions that we had never before considered and integrate ideas that we had never before connected as he synthesized the study of persons with Down syndrome across disparate disciplines and ways of thinking. Even as a graduate student, Dante’s vision and call for action of a developmental framework for understanding of children with Down syndrome, and de facto any NDC, was evident in the title of his dissertation, Affective development in Down’s syndrome infants: An organizational perspective (Cicchetti & Sroufe, Reference Cicchetti and Sroufe1976). This thread would continue through all his work in the field. And, although his scholarship in this area largely culminated in the publication of the seminal Cicchetti and Beeghly (Reference Cicchetti and Beeghly1990b) edited volume, “Down syndrome: A developmental perspective,” his primary ideas remained: (1) research needs to be etiology specific in order to best assess development; (2) development needs to be studied across domains simultaneously to reflect its complexity and organization in real-world context; (3) developmental matching needs to be fine-tuned to account for unique patterns of development; (4) developmental research needs to be guided by questions that tap into group and individual ways of being, styles, and biases; and (5) developmental changes need to be considered in relation to the influences of multiple levels of the environment. As Dante has taught, these essential developmental lessons will allow us to appreciate how all children develop within their environments, empowering us to unravel the transactional nature of development – the presence of changes or lack thereof, and the influences of diverse processes within and outside of the developing child (Cicchetti & Beeghly, Reference Cicchetti, Beeghly, Cicchetti and Beeghly1990a).

Such advances also foreshadow perspectives that are becoming increasingly prominent as time goes on. Dante’s ideas about the reciprocal value of typical and atypical development align well with more humane outlooks that underlie the current neurodiversity perspective. Proponents highlight that all people experience and interact with the world around them in multiple ways; there is no one “correct” way of thinking, learning, or behaving, and differences are not viewed as deficits. Social acceptance and inclusion of people with developmental disabilities rely on a thorough understanding of how both neurodivergent and neurotypical individuals behave in social relationships.

Even as the studies described in this paper highlight Dante’s influence, his impact will resonate for decades to come, and his contributions remind us of the rich landscape of questions still to be asked. For example, we still have much to learn about the development of individuals with any one or more of the 1,100 genetic conditions associated with intellectual disability, and even more to learn about how this development impacts and is impacted by the connections and interactions of various “levels” of genes-brain-behavior (Dykens, Reference Dykens, Glidden, Abbeduto, McIntyre and Tassé2021). We also need to remind ourselves and advocate to others about the fundamental impact of methodological choices such as how and to whom we make comparisons between those with and without NDCs. When framed within a developmental context, rigorous research can ultimately be translated into meaningful interventions and policy initiatives that allow us to make substantive impact in the lives of persons with NDCs and their families (Abbeduto et al., Reference Abbeduto, Edgin, Burack, Burack, Edgin and Abbeduto2023; Spiker, Reference Spiker, Cicchetti and Beeghly1990).

As we conclude we note that we have only begun to elaborate on the many ideas that Dante has provided us. In the years to come, the depth of Dante’s lessons will continue to guide the field, to impact research, education, psychology, psychiatry, and policy. Dante’s legacy will allow us, his contemporaries, and those that follow, to ask the big questions and to center his larger view of development, in both how we frame questions and in how we interpret our findings and share them with the broader community. The ultimate goal, of course, is to provide the most essential information to inform our collaborations with persons with NDCs and their family members as we work together to promote their well-being and successes as well the betterment of a more inclusive and empowering society.

Funding statement

JAB’s work on this paper was supported by funding from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada.

Competing interests

None.

References

Abbeduto, L. J., Edgin, J., & Burack, J. A. (2023). Future directions of research with people with down syndrome. In Burack, J. A., Edgin, J., & Abbeduto, L. J. (Ed.), Oxford handbook of down syndrome and development. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M. (1974). Developmental psychopathology. Ronald Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 3746. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batshaw, Mark L., Roizen, Nancy J., & Pellegrino, Louis (2019). Children with disabilities (8th). Brookes Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Beeghly, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Talking about self and other: Emergence of an internal state lexicon in young children with down syndrome. Development and Psychopathology, 9(4), 729748. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579497001417 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, J., & Cunningham, C. C. (1981). The development of eye contact between mothers and normal versus down’s syndrome infants. Developmental Psychology, 17(5), 679689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, J., & Cunningham, C. C. (1983). Development of early vocal behaviors and interactions in down’s syndrome and nonhandicapped infant-mother pairs. Developmental Psychology, 19(3), 332–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bottema-Beutel, K., Kapp, S. K., Lester, J. N., Sasson, N. J., & Hand, B. N. (2021). Avoiding ableist language: Suggestions for autism researchers. Autism in Adulthood, 3(1), 1829. https://doi.org/10.1089/aut.2020.0014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brodeur, D. A., Stewart, J., Dawkins, T., & Burack, J. A. (2018). Utilitarian attention by children with autism spectrum disorder on a filtering task. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(12), 40194027. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3619-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U., Kessel, F., Kessen, W., & White, S. (1986). Toward a critical social history of developmental psychology: A propaedeutic discussion. American Psychologist, 41(11), 12181230, https://doi-org.proxy3.library.mcgill.ca/10.1037/0003-066X.41.11.1218 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brook, C. G., Gasser, T., Werder, E. A., Prader, A., & Vanderschueren-Lodewykx, M. A. (1977). Height correlations between parents and mature offspring in normal subjects and in subjects with Turner’s and Klinefelter’s and other syndromes. Annals of Human Biology, 4(1), 1722. https://doi.org/10.1080/03014467700001911 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brook, C. G., Mürset, G., Zachmann, M., & Prader, A. (1974). Growth in children with 45,XO Turner’s syndrome. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 49(10), 789795. https://doi.org/10.1136/adc.49.10.789 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burack, J. A. (1990). Differentiating mental retardation: The two-group approach and beyond. In Hodapp, R. M., Burack, J. A., & Zigler, E. (Ed.), Issues in the developmental approach to mental retardation (pp. 2748). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511582325.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burack, J. A. (1994). Selective attention deficits in persons with autism: Preliminary evidence of an inefficient attentional lens. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(3), 535543. https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843x.103.3.535 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burack, J. A., & Brodeur, D. (2020). Utilitarian attention among persons with autism spectrum disorder. Encyclopedia of autism spectrum disorders. In Volkmar, F. R. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of autism spectrum disorder. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-6435-8_102313-1 Google Scholar
Burack, J. A., Evans, D. W., Napoleon, J. S., Weva, V. K., Russo, N., & Iarocci, G. (2021a). The science of humanity and the humanity of science: Perspectives on Ed Zigler’s contributions to developmental psychopathology and the study of all children. Development and Psychopathology, 33(2), 441452. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420002011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burack, J. A., Evans, D. W., Russo, N., Napoleon, J. S., Goldman, K. J., & Iarocci, G. (2021b). Developmental perspectives on the study of persons with intellectual disability. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 17(1), 339363. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-090532 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burack, J. A., Friedman, S., Lessage, M., & Brodeur, D. (2023a). Re-visiting the “Mysterious myth of attention deficit”: A systematic review of the recent evidence. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 67(3), 271288. https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12994 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burack, J. A., Hodapp, R. M., & Zigler, E. (1988). Issues in the classification of mental retardation: Differentiating among organic etiologies. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 29(6), 765779. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1988.tb00752.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burack, J. A., Iarocci, G., Flanagan, T. D., & Bowler, D. M. (2004). On mosaics and melting pots: Conceptual considerations of comparison and matching strategies. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(1), 6573. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JADD.0000018076.90715.00 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burack, J. A., Lai, J., Stewart, J., & Landry, O. (2023b). Developmental traditions in the study of persons with down syndrome. In Burack, J. A., Edgin, J., & Abbeduto, L. J. (Ed.), Oxford handbook of down syndrome and development. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Burack, J. A., Russo, N., Kovshoff, H., Palma Fernandes, T., Ringo, J., Landry, O., & Iarocci, G. (2016). How I attend—not how well do I attend: Rethinking developmental frameworks of attention and cognition in autism spectrum disorder and typical development. Journal of Cognition and Development, 17(4), 553567. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2016.1197226 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burack, J. A., & Zigler, E. (1990). Intentional and incidental memory in organically mentally retarded, familial retarded, and nonretarded children. American Journal On Mental Retardation, 94, 532540.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G., & Cicchetti, D. (1978). Visual calibration of posture in normal and motor retarded down’s syndrome infants. Perception, 7(5), 513525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, J. (1970a). Mental and motor development in young mongol children. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 14(3), 205220. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.1970.tb01116.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carr, J. (1970b). Mongolism: Telling the parents. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 12(2), 213221. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.1970.tb01892.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, J., & Collins, S. (2014). Ageing and dementia in a longitudinal study of a cohort with down syndrome. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 27(6), 555563. https://doi.org/10.1111/jar.12093 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carr, J., & Collins, S. (2018). 50 years with down syndrome: A longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 31(5), 743750. https://doi.org/10.1111/jar.12438 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55(1), 17. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129830 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Beeghly, M. (1990a). An organizational approach to the study of down syndrome: Contributions to an integrative theory of development. In Cicchetti, D., & Beeghly, M. (Ed.), Children with down syndrome: A developmental perspective (pp. 2962). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Beeghly, M. (1990b). Children with down syndrome: A developmental perspective. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Ganiban, J. (1990). The organization and coherence of developmental processes in infants and children with down syndrome. In Hodapp, R. M., Burack, J. A., & Zigler, E. (Ed.), Issues in the developmental approach to mental retardation (pp. 169225). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Pogge-Hesse, P. (1982). Possible contributions of the study of organically retarted persons to the developmental theory. In Zigler, E., & Balla, D. (Ed.), Mental retardation: The developmental-difference controversy (pp. 277318). L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1976). The relationship between affective and cognitive development in down’s syndrome infants. Child Development, 47(4), 920929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). An organizational view of affect: Illustration from the study of down’s syndrome infants. In Lewis, M., & Rosenblum, L. A. (Ed.), The development of affect (pp. 309350). Springer US, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-2616-8_13 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conners, C. K. (1997). The Conners’ rating scales – revised. Multi-Health Systems, Inc.Google Scholar
Cornish, K., & Wilding, J. (2010). Attention, genes, and developmental disorders. Oxford University Press, https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195179941.001.0001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crnic, K. A. (1990). Families of children with down syndrome: Ecological contexts and characteristics. In Cicchetti, D., & Beeghly, M. (Ed.), Children with down syndrome: A developmental perspective (pp. 399423). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dilks, D. D., Hoffman, J. E., & Landau, B. (2008). Vision for perception and vision for action: Normal and unusual development. Developmental Science, 11(4), 474486. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00693.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A. (1985). Facets of social interaction and the assessment of social competence in children. In Schneider, B. H., Rubin, K. H., & Ledingham, J. E. (Ed.), Children’s peer relations: Issues in assessment and intervention (pp. 322). Springer, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-6325-5_1 Google Scholar
Dykens, E. M. (2021). Behavioral phenotypes and genomic testing: Scientific and societal implications. In Glidden, L. M. Abbeduto, L. McIntyre, L. L. & Tassé, M. J. (Eds.), APA handbook of intellectual and developmental disabilities: Foundations (pp. 89122). American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dykens, E. M., Hodapp, R. M., & Finucane, B. M. (2000). Genetics and mental retardation syndromes: A new look at behavior and interventions. Paul H Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Dykens, E. M., & Kasari, C. (1997). Maladaptive behavior in children with prader-willi syndrome, down syndrome, and nonspecific mental retardation. American Journal On Mental Retardation, 102(3), 228237. https://doi.org/10.1352/0895-8017(1997)102 2.0.CO;2>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edgin, J. O. (2013). Cognition in down syndrome: A developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4(3), 307317. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1221 Google ScholarPubMed
Evans, D. W., & Uljarević, M. (2018). Parental education accounts for variability in the IQs of probands with down syndrome: A longitudinal study. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A, 176(1), 2933. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.38519 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fidler, D. J., Schworer, E. K., Needham, A., Prince, M. A., Patel, L., Will, E. A., & Daunhauer, L. A. (2021). Feasibility of a syndrome-informed micro-intervention for infants with down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 65(4), 320339. https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12814 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition, 50(1-3), 115132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gath, A., & Gumley, D. (1984). Down’s syndrome and the family: Follow-up of children first seen in infancy. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 26(4), 500508. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.1984.tb04478.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gath, A., & Gumley, D. (1986a). Behaviour problems in retarded children with special reference to down’s syndrome. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 149(2), 156161. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.149.2.156 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gath, A., & Gumley, D. (1986b). Family background of children with down’s syndrome and of children with a similar degree of mental retardation. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 149(2), 161171. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.149.2.161 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gath, A., & Gumley, D. (1987). Retarded children and their siblings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28(5), 715730. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1987.tb01554.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenn, S. M., Cunningham, C. C., & Joyce, P. F. (1981). A study of auditory preferences in nonhandicapped infants and infants with down’s syndrome. Child Development, 52(4), 13031307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottesman, I. I. (1963). Heritability of personality: A demonstration. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 77(9), 121. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093852 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grelotti, D. J., Gauthier, I., & Schultz, R. T. (2002). Social interest and the development of cortical face specialization: What autism teaches us about face processing. Developmental Psychobiology, 40(3), 213225. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.10028 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanson, E., Bernier, R., Porche, K., Goin-Kochel, R. P., Snyder, L. G., Snow-Gallagher, A., Wallace, A. S., Campe, K., Zhang, Y., Chen, Q., Moreno De Luca, A., Orr, P. T., Boomer, K. B., Evans, D. W., Martin, C. L., Ledbetter, D. H., Spiro, J., & Chung, W. (2014). The cognitive and behavioral phenotype of the 16p11.2 deletion. Biological Psychiatry, 77(9), 785793. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.04.021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 525. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2020). Annual research review: Looking back to look forward-changes in the concept of autism and implications for future research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(3), 218232. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13176 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodapp, R. M. (1990). On road or many? Issues in the similar-sequence hypothesis. In Hodapp, R. M., Burack, J. A., & Zigler, E. (Eds.), Issues in the developmental approach to mental retardation (pp. 4970). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodapp, R., & Burack, J. (1990). What mental retardation teaches us about typical development: The examples of sequences, rates, and cross-domain relations. Development and Psychopathology, 2(3), 213225. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400000730 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodapp, R. M. (2021). Ed Zigler’s developmental approach to intellectual disabilities: Past, present, and future contributions. Development and Psychopathology, 33(2), 453465. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420002084 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodapp, R. M., Burack, J. A., & Zigler, E. F. (1990). The developmental perspective in the field of mental retardation. In Hodapp, R. M., Burack, J. A., & Zigler, E. (Eds.), Issues in the developmental approach to mental retardation (pp. 326). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodapp, R. M., & Fidler, D. J. (2021). Down syndrome. In Glidden, L. M., Abbeduto, L., McIntyre, L. L., & Tassé, M. J. (Ed.), APA handbook of intellectual and developmental disabilities: Foundations (pp. 123150). American Psychological Association, https://doi.org/10.1037/0000194-006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodapp, R. M., & Zigler, E. (1990). Applying the developmental perspective to individuals with down syndrome. In Cicchetti, D., & Beeghly, M. (Ed.), Children with down syndrome: A developmental perspective (pp. 128). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hofmann, V., & Müller, C. M. (2021). Language skills and social contact among students with intellectual disabilities in special needs schools. Learning, Culture and Social Interaction, 30, 100534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holl, R. W., Kunze, D., Etzrodt, H., Teller, W., & Heinze, E. (1994). Turner syndrome: Final height, glucose tolerance, bone density and psychosocial status in 25 adult patients. European Journal of Pediatrics, 153(1), 1116. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02000780 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iarocci, G., Yager, J., & Elfers, T. (2007). What gene-environment interactions can tell us about social competence in typical and atypical populations. Brain and Cognition, 65(1), 112127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2007.01.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iarocci, G., Yager, J., Rombough, A., & McLaughlin, J. (2008). The development of social competence among persons with down syndrome: From survival to social inclusion. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 35, 87119. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0074-7750(07)35003-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamps, F. S., Julian, J. B., Battaglia, P., Landau, B., Kanwisher, N., & Dilks, D. D. (2017). Dissociating intuitive physics from intuitive psychology: Evidence from williams syndrome. Cognition, 168, 146153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.027 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan-Kahn, E. A., Park, A., & Russo, N. (2021). Pathways of perceptual primacy: ERP evidence for relationships between autism traits and enhanced perceptual functioning. Neuropsychologia, 163, 108065. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.108065 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessen, W. (1984). Introduction: The end of the age of development. In Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), Mechanisms of cognitive development (pp. 117). Freeman.Google Scholar
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809816. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203205. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7350657 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landau, B. (2012). The organization and development of spatial representation: Insights from williams syndrome. In Burack, J. (Ed.), The oxford handbook of intellectual disability and development (pp. 6188). Oxford.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landau, B., & Hoffman, J. E. (2005). Parallels between spatial cognition and spatial language: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(2), 163185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.05.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landau, B., & Zukowski, A. (2003). Objects, motions, and paths: Spatial language in children with Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23(1-2), 105137. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2003.9651889 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lane, K. A., Stewart, J., Fernandes, T., Russo, N., Enns, J. T., & Burack, J. A. (2014). Complexities in understanding attentional functioning among children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00119 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemli, L., & Smith, D. W. (1963). The XO syndrome: A study of the differentiated phenotype in 25 patients. The Journal of Pediatrics, 63(4), 577588. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3476(63)80368-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ly, T. M., & Hodapp, R. M. (2005). Children with Prader-Willi syndrome vs. Williams syndrome: Indirect effects on parents during a jigsaw puzzle task. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 49(Pt 12), 929939. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00782.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malachowski, L. G., & Needham, A. W. (2023). Infants exploring objects: A cascades perspective. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 64, 3968. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.acdb.2022.11.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Anderson, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1998). The test of everyday attention for children (TEA-Ch). Thames Valley Test Company.Google Scholar
Mans, L., Cicchetti, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Mirror reactions of down’s syndrome infants and toddlers: Cognitive underpinnings of self-recognition. Child Development, 49(4), 12471250. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128771 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Massa, G. G., & Vanderschueren-Lodeweyckx, M. (1991). Age and height at diagnosis in Turner syndrome: Influence of parental height. Pediatrics, 88(6), 11481152. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.88.6.1148 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 491495. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000222 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mervis, C. B. (2012). Language development in Williams syndrome. In Burack, J. A. Hodapp, R. M. Iarocci, G. & Zigler, E. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of intellectual disability and development (pp. 395401). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Milton, D. E. M. (2012). On the ontological status of autism: The double empathy problem. Disability & Society, 27(6), 883887. https://doi.org/10.1080/09687599.2012.710008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreno De Luca, A.*, Evans, D. W.*, Boomer, K., Hanson, E., Bernier, R., Goin-Kochel, R., Myers, S. M., Challman, T. D., Moreno De Luca, D., Spiro, J., Chung, W., Martin, C. L., & Ledbetter, D. H. (2014). Parental cognitive, behavioral and motor profiles impact the neurodevelopmental profile of individuals with de novo mutations. JAMA Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2147 [*Co-First authors].Google Scholar
Moreno-De-Luca, A., Evans, D. W., Boomer, K. B., Hanson, E., Bernier, R., Goin-Kochel, R. P., Myers, S. M., Challman, T. D., Moreno-De-Luca, D., Slane, M. M., Hare, A. E., Chung, W. K., Spiro, J. E., aucett, W. A., Martin, C. L., & Ledbetter, D. H. (2015). The role of parental cognitive, behavioral, and motor profiles in clinical variability in individuals with chromosome 16p11.2 deletions. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(2), 119. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2147 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Motti, F., Cicchetti, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1983). From infant affect expression to symbolic play: The coherence of development in down syndrome children. Child Development, 54(5), 11681175. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129672 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mottron, L., & Burack, J. A. (2001). Enhanced perceptual functioning in the development of autism. In Burack, J. A., Charman, T., Yirmiya, N., & Zelazo, P. R. (Ed.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 119134). Routledge.Google Scholar
Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 2743. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0040-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, G., & Carr, S. (2020). Dr Janet Carr: Obituary. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities. https://doi.org/10.1111/jar.12766 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nadel, L. (2003). Down’s syndrome: A genetic disorder in biobehavioral perspective. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 2(3), 156166. https://doi.org/10.1034/j.1601-183X.2003.00026.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pellicano, E., & den Houting, J. (2022). Annual research review: Shifting from normal scienceto neurodiversity in autism science. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 63(4), 381396. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13534 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970). Genetic epistemology. Columbia University Press, https://doi.org/10.7312/piag91272 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reilly, J., Klima, E., & Bellugi, U. (1990). Once more with feeling: Affect and language in atypical populations. Development and Psychopathology, 2(4), 367391. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400005782 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochiccioli, P., David, M., Malpuech, G., Colle, M., Limal, J. M., Battin, J., Mariani, R., Sultan, C., Nivelon, J. L., & Simonin, G. (1994). Study of final height in Turner’s syndrome: Ethnic and genetic influences. Acta Paediatrica, 83(3), 305308. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.1994.tb18099.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose-Krasnor, L. (1997). The nature of social competence: A theoretical review. Social Development, 6(1), 111135. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.1997.tb00097.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russo, N., Kaplan-Kahn, E. A., Wilson, J., Criss, A., & Burack, J. A. (2021). Choices, challenges, and constraints: A pragmatic examination of the limits of mental age matching in empirical research. Development and Psychopathology, 33(2), 727738. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420001480 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russo, N., Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., & Jemel, B. (2012). Parameters of semantic multisensory integration depend on timing and modality order among people on the autism spectrum: Evidence from event-related potentials. Neuropsychologia, 50(9), 21312141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheerer, N. E., Boucher, T. Q., Sasson, N. J., & Iarocci, G. (2022). Effects of an educational presentation about autism on high school students’ perceptions of autistic adults. Autism in Adulthood: Challenges and Management, 4(3), 203213. https://doi.org/10.1089/aut.2021.0046 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R. K., Anderson, A. W., Volkmar, F., Skudlarski, P., Lacadie, C., Cohen, D. J., & Gore, J. C. (2000). Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(4), 331340. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.57.4.331 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schworer, E. K., Fidler, D. J., Kaur, M., Needham, A. W., Prince, M. A., & Daunhauer, L. A. (2022). Infant precursors of executive function in down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 66(1-2), 108120. https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12824 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serafica, F. C., & Cicchetti, D. (1976). Down’s syndrome children in a strange situation: Attachment and exploration behaviors. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 22(2), 137150.Google Scholar
Spiker, D. (1990). Early intervention from a developmental perspective. In Cicchetti, D., & Beeghly, M. (Ed.), Children with down syndrome: A developmental perspective (pp. 424448). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stefansson, H., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Steinberg, S., Magnusdottir, B., Morgen, K., Arnarsdottir, S., Bjornsdottir, G., Walters, G. B., Jonsdottir, G. A., Doyle, O. M., Tost, H., Grimm, O., Kristjansdottir, S., Snorrason, H., Davidsdottir, S. R., Gudmundsson, L. J., Jonsson, G. F., Stefansdottir, B., Helgadottir, I., Haraldsson, M., Jonsdottir, B., Thygesen, J. H., Schwarz, A. J., Didriksen, M., Stensbøl, T. B., Brammer, M., Kapur, S., Halldorsson, J. G., Hreidarsson, S., Saemundsen, E., Sigurdsson, E., & Stefansson, K. (2014). CNVs conferring risk of autism or schizophrenia affect cognition in controls. Nature, 505(7483), 361366. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12818 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, R. A., Cicchetti, D., Lamb, M. E., & Malkin, C. (1985). Emotional responses of down syndrome and normal infants in the strange situation: The organization of affective behavior in infants. Developmental Psychology, 21(5), 828841. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.21.5.828 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, S., Ganiban, J., & Cicchett, D. (1990). Attention, memory, and perception in infants with down syndrome: A review and commentary. In Cicchetti, D., & Beeghly, M. (Ed.), Children with down syndrome: A developmental perspective (pp. 147179). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, B., Weisz, J. R., & Bromfield, R. (1986). Performance of retarded and nonretarded persons on information-processing tasks: Further tests of the similar structure hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 100(2), 157175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisz, J. R., & Yeates, K. O. (1981). Cognitive development in retarded and nonretarded persons: Piagetian tests of the similar structure hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 90(1), 153178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisz, J. R., Yeates, K. O., & Zigler, E. (1982). Piagetian evidence and the developmental-difference controversy. In Zigler, E., & Balla, D. (Ed.), Mental retardation: The developmental-difference controversy (pp. 213276). L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Weisz, J. R., & Zigler, E. (1979). Cognitive development in retarded and nonretarded persons: Piagetian tests of the similar sequence hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 86(4), 831851. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.4.831 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. Follett Pub. Co.Google Scholar
Werner, H. (1957). The concept of development from a comparative and organismic point of view. In Harris, D. B. (Ed.), The concept of development (pp. 125148). University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Zigler, E. (1967). Familial mental retardation: A continuing dilemma. Science, 155(3760), 292298. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.155.3760.292 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zigler, E. (1969). Developmental versus difference theories of mental retardation and the problem of motivation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 73(4), 536556.Google ScholarPubMed
Zigler, E. (1970a). Social class and the socialization process. Review of Educational Research, 40(1), 87110. https://doi.org/10.2307/1169578 Google Scholar
Zigler, E. (1970b). The environmental mystique: Training the intellect versus development of the child. Childhood Education, 46(8), 402412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zigler, E., & Balla, D. (1982). Mental retardation: The Developmental-difference Controversy (1st ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203781067.Google Scholar
Zigler, E., & Hodapp, R. M. (1986). Understanding mental retardation. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar