Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T01:35:25.679Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Supplemented zinc does not alter mood in healthy older European adults – a randomised placebo-controlled trial: the Zenith study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 January 2011

Barbara J Stewart-Knox*
Affiliation:
Northern Ireland Centre for Food and Health, University of Ulster, Coleraine, BT521SA, Northern Ireland, UK
Gordon Rae
Affiliation:
Psychology Research Institute, University of Ulster, Coleraine, Northern Ireland, UK
Ellen EA Simpson
Affiliation:
Psychology Research Institute, University of Ulster, Coleraine, Northern Ireland, UK
Chris McConville
Affiliation:
Psychology Research Institute, University of Ulster, Coleraine, Northern Ireland, UK
Jacqueline O’Connor
Affiliation:
Northern Ireland Centre for Food and Health, University of Ulster, Coleraine, BT521SA, Northern Ireland, UK
Angela Polito
Affiliation:
National Research Institute on Food and Nutrition, Department of Nutritional Science, Rome, Roma, Italy
Maud Andriollo-Sanchez
Affiliation:
Faculté de Pharmacie, Laboratoire de Biologie de Stress Oxydant, Nutrition Vieillissement et Maladies Cardiovasculaires, University de Joseph Fourier, Grenoble, France
Charles Coudray
Affiliation:
Unité des Maladies Métaboliques et Micronutrients, INRA, Centre de Recherche en Nutrition Humaine d’Auvergne, Clermont-Ferrand/Theix, France
JJ Strain
Affiliation:
Northern Ireland Centre for Food and Health, University of Ulster, Coleraine, BT521SA, Northern Ireland, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Email b.knox@ulster.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

Older people are vulnerable to zinc deficiency, which may impact upon their mood. This randomised, placebo-controlled, double-blind intervention study aimed to investigate the effect of oral zinc gluconate supplementation (15 mg/d; 30 mg/d; and placebo) on subjective mood (affect) in older Europeans.

Subjects

Healthy volunteers (n 387) aged 55–87 years were recruited.

Setting

Volunteers in Rome (Italy; n 108) and Grenoble (France; n 91) were aged 70–87 years and those in Coleraine (Northern Ireland; n 93) and Clermont-Ferrand (France; n 95) were aged 55–70 years.

Design

Mood was measured using the Positive and Negative Affect Scale on four occasions per day over 4 d at baseline, 3 and 6 months post-intervention.

Results

Mixed ANOVA indicated that neither positive nor negative affect altered in response to zinc (15 mg/d or 30 mg/d) compared to placebo in either the 55–70 years or the ≥70 years age group.

Conclusions

These results suggest that zinc does not benefit mood in healthy older people.

Type
Research paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2011

Psychological well-being is high on the list of public health policy priorities, with the emphasis on prevention(Reference Wanless1). Zinc is an essential trace element(Reference Hambidge2) that is present in large quantities in the brain and may be important for the maintenance of psychological well-being. Zinc-containing neurons are found in the forebrain interconnecting the cerebral cortex (the cognitive or ‘thinking’ part of the brain) and the limbic system (the affective or ‘feeling’ part of the brain)(Reference Frederickson, Won Suh and Silva3). The synaptic vesicles of the hippocampus and amygdala, which are both limbic structures, contain particularly large quantities of zinc(Reference Colvin, Davis and Nipper4), implying a role for zinc in the control of emotion. Zinc-containing neurons are receptive to glutamine, an amino acid neurotransmitter that is excitatory and that exerts a neuromodulatory effect on post-synaptic glutamate receptors within the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (HPA) system(Reference Colvin, Davis and Nipper4, Reference Szewczyk, Poleszak and Wlaz5). Rodent studies have indicated that this action occurs mainly through the hippocampus synapses, which are involved in the regulation of the HPA system(Reference Matias, Saggau and Quinta-Ferreira6Reference Takeda11). Zinc may also be involved in the control of the serotonergic system(Reference Szewczyk, Poleszak and Wlaz5, Reference Cichy, Sowa-Kucma and Legutko12, Reference Barrondo and Salles13). Zinc has been shown to increase the density of 5-HT serotonin receptors in the hippocampus and frontal cortex in rats(Reference Cichy, Sowa-Kucma and Legutko12). Post-mortem evidence suggests that zinc may have a role in the nutritional methylation processes(Reference Michel, Frangou and Thiemeyer14) that are also believed to be important in the regulation of mood(Reference Kaplan, Field and Crawford15). The human central nervous system appears to be adversely affected by zinc deficiency. Lower serum zinc is associated with depression(Reference Marcellini, Giuli and Papa16Reference Maes, Vandoolaeghe and Neels20) and a marker of treatment resistance in clinical depression(Reference Maes, Vandoolaeghe and Neels20, Reference Siwek, Dudek and Paul21). Lower serum zinc is also associated with disordered behaviour(Reference Walsh, Isaacson and Rehman22Reference Hanson, Malecha and Mackenzie24). Zinc supplementation has also been shown to ameliorate depression in clinical groups(Reference Maes, Neels and Demedts25, Reference Levenson26). There is an apparent dearth of human studies of zinc and well-being in healthy (non-clinical) groups. Among the few studies of healthy individuals is a recent cross-sectional study of Korean women (n 570), which indicated that among other nutrients and trace elements, lower dietary zinc intake was associated with higher stress scores on the short version of the Psychological Well-being Index(Reference Hwang, Lee and Kim27). A recent double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of zinc (7 mg/d) in young women (n 30) found reduced scores on the anger/hostility and depression scales of the Profile of Mood States(Reference Sawada and Yokoi28). These studies imply a link between zinc and psychological well-being in health. There appear to be no existing trials of zinc and well-being or mood in men and/or older people.

Europe and much of the industrialised Western world has a growing ageing population(Reference Wanless1), hence, the need to maintain psychological well-being and quality of life in this vulnerable age group. The elderly are particularly at risk of zinc deficiency, the likelihood of which increases with age(Reference Seiler29Reference Maes, de Vos and Wauters33) as a result of a range of physiological, social, psychological and economic factors(Reference Reichies, Felsenberg and Gessner34) that may be associated with the decreased consumption of zinc-rich foods(Reference Drewnowski and Shulz35Reference Webb, Schofield and Lazerus38). Dysregulation of the HPA system is associated with the increased risk of CVD, diabetes, cancer, inflammatory conditions and neurodegenerative disorders(Reference Cole and Dendukuri39). HPA system activity has been shown previously to be related to positive affect (PA) in the elderly(Reference Simpson, McConville and Rae40). Maintaining a higher PA in old age, therefore, is potentially important for general health and well-being as well as disease prevention(Reference Ostir, Ottenbacher and Markides41) in otherwise healthy older people. Assuming that zinc exerts a neuromodulatory effect through the HPA system, optimising zinc status in older people, therefore, is likely to have far-reaching benefits to psychological health. Yet, there do not appear to be any previous studies that have considered zinc and affect in healthy older individuals. Existing evidence for benefits of zinc to psychological well-being is mainly derived from post hoc investigation using psychiatric in-patients. Trials in healthy populations are necessary to further our understanding of how dietary constituents such as zinc impact upon health and well-being and to provide evidence to support or refute potential functional claims for zinc-rich food products. Trials in healthy individuals are especially important, given the widespread and increasing use of self-prescribed nutrient supplements by the older public(Reference Goh, Vitry and Semple42). The Zenith Study aimed to assess the effects of zinc supplementation in the normal healthy older population. It was necessary, therefore, to intervene with zinc at dosages that would be available for purchase over the counter without prescription. This randomised, placebo-controlled, double-blind intervention study reports the effect upon PA and negative affect (NA) of zinc (15mg/d and 30 mg/d) supplements administered orally over 6 months compared to placebo in healthy 55–70-year-olds recruited to the Zenith Study in Coleraine and Clermont-Ferrand and ≥70-year-olds in Grenoble and Rome. This research meets an imperative for controlled supplementation trials investigating the effect of zinc on psychological well-being in the older population. It is hypothesised that zinc supplementation will alter subjective affective state compared to placebo.

Methods

The Zenith Study was a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled intervention trial of zinc in older adults, which was conducted in four European centres. Ethics approval for the study was obtained from recognised research centres in each country. Informed, written consent was obtained from each participant.

Sampling

Volunteers were outreached in different ways including through local television and radio broadcast, posters and through leaflets distributed in supermarkets, clubs that free-living elderly people frequently visit, health centres, and family doctors. Prospective volunteers then initiated contact with the research group who then arranged an appointment for the preliminary screening. The volunteers were screened first by clinical examination, anthropometry, smoking and alcohol consumption, cognitive impairment, depression and biochemistry profiles. Those selected for inclusion in the study were apparently healthy, not morbidly obese and with a BMI of between 20 kg/m2 and 30 kg/m2(43). Only those who had no evidence of dementia, as indicated by a score >23 in the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) test(Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh44), were included. Volunteers were screened for depression using the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS)(Reference Yesavage, Brink and Rose45). The GDS has been validated as a screening tool for depression against the DSM-IV criteria according to which only those scoring ≥6 can be considered not clinically depressed(Reference Almeida and Almeida46). Those with a score of ≤5 were included. Participants were also excluded if they had a positive serology for HIV or hepatitis C, if they smoked more than ten cigarettes per day; consumed alcohol >30 g/d for men and >20 g/d for women; had unusual dietary habits (vegetarianism and veganism); used a mineral supplement in the 3 months preceding the study; used more than three drugs daily if aged 55–70 years; used more than four drugs daily if over 70 years; used drugs including antidepressants, laxatives and hormone replacement therapy; or had a pathological disease (cancer, diabetes, insufficient renal or hepatic performance, malabsorption and inflammatory chronic pathologies). Complete details about screening and recruitment processes have been described previously by Polito et al.(Reference Polito, Meunier and Andriollo-Sanchez47).

Serum zinc

Unfortunately, there is no single accurate measure of zinc status(Reference Gibson48, Reference Miller, Krebs and Hambridge49). For the purpose of the present study, serum zinc was taken as a putative indicator of current zinc status(Reference Anriollo-Sanchez, Hininger-Favier and Meunier50). Fasting blood samples were obtained for the zinc assay in the morning. Blood samples for the zinc assay were collected via venepuncture by qualified the phlebotomists, using trace element-free Vacutainer® tubes (Becton, Dickinson and company Ltd, Oxford, UK). The blood sample was taken in the morning after a 12 h fast by the participants. Immediately after drawing, the blood samples were kept on ice, and then centrifuged at 1000g for 15 min at 4°C. Serum zinc was immediately isolated, aliquoted and stored at −80°C. Serum zinc concentrations at baseline were determined at INRA Clermont-Ferrand, France, by flame atomic absorption spectrometry using a Perkin-Elmer 560 instrument (Perkin Elmer, Cambridge, UK)(Reference Anriollo-Sanchez, Hininger-Favier and Meunier50). Internal quality control was checked using Seronorm® trace element serum (Sero®, Billingstad, Norway).

Dietary zinc

Dietary zinc intake was assessed at baseline by means of a semi-structured standardised 4 d food diary over two weekdays and two weekend days consecutively, which included estimates of portion size, the brand name of the product and/or the recipe if cooked from fresh. The information in the food diaries was analysed using the NetWISP version 3·0 (Tinuviel Software Anglesey, UK) database. A full account of the dietary assessment procedure has been provided previously by Polito et al.(Reference Polito, Intorre and Andriollo-Sanchez51).

Positive and Negative Affect Scale

The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) is a well-validated twenty-item self-reported psychometric measure of subjective mood developed by Watson et al.(Reference Watson, Clarke and Tellegen52). Watson and Tellegen(Reference Watson and Tellegen53) proposed that there are two distinct dimensions of mood state, PA and NA, which together account for over two-thirds of the variance in mood. PA is associated with feelings of alertness, enthusiasm and happiness and NA with displeasure and dissatisfaction(Reference Watson and Clark54). PANAS scores have been shown to correlate with salivary cortisol levels(Reference Simpson, McConville and Rae40) and whole blood serotonin levels in healthy individuals(Reference Williams, Stewart-Knox and Helander55, Reference Duffy, Stewart-Knox and McConville56). The scales have been found to have high internal consistency, with Cronbach’s α ranging from 0·84 to 0·87 for the NA scale and 0·84 to 0·90 for the PA scale(Reference Watson and Walker57). Repeated measures of affect were taken during the week before the centre visit, including one further measure taken on the morning of testing. The participants were asked to complete four self-reported PANAS questionnaires daily (on rising, after lunch, after dinner, and before going to bed) for four consecutive days at baseline, 3 and 6 months. The scale took approximately 2 min to complete. The items were: interested; distressed; excited; upset; strong; guilty; scared; hostile; enthusiastic; proud; irritable; alert; ashamed; inspired; nervous; determined; attentive; jittery; active; and afraid. Responses to the PANAS were recorded on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from ‘not at all’ = 1 to ‘extremely’ = 5. A score for each scale was obtained by summing item scores.

Intervention

Eligible volunteers were randomly assigned to three groups to receive either 15 mg/d or 30 mg/d zinc gluconate or placebo orally. Zinc capsules were issued in dated pillboxes at baseline and at 3 and 6 months into the supplementation phase. The participants were instructed to swallow the capsules with breakfast. The degree of compliance was estimated by collecting the number of returned capsules at 3 and 6 months. Compliance was tested and was found to be >98 % for all groups.

Data analysis

The mood data were analysed using a mixed ANOVA. Separate analyses were computed for the 55–70-year-olds (n 188; from Coleraine and Clermont-Ferrand) and for the ≥70-year-olds (n 199; from Grenoble and Rome). There were three between-group factors (sex, centre and treatment) and one repeated measure (baseline, 3 and 6 months post-intervention) included in each ANOVA. Mean PA and NA were treated as separate dependent variables, which also required separate ANOVAs. In the absence of relevant previous studies in this area, a conservative value of f 2 = 0·025 for effect sizes was adopted. For the mood scale it was assumed that the correlations among each of the repeated measures were 0·42 (taken from the PANAS manual). Power calculations were performed using GPOWER general power analysis computer program (G*Power, Bonn, Germany)(Reference Erdfelder, Faul and Buchner58). With α = 0·05 and assuming 180 participants within each age group (55–70 and ≥70 years), the power values for the between-groups and within-groups and interaction effects were 0·36, 0·99 and 0·81, respectively. As the interaction effects (particularly the treatment–time interaction) were of prime concern in the present study, the sample sizes for both the younger (n 188) and the older (n 199) age groups were regarded as satisfactory. Significance was set at P < 0·05. The Statistical Package for Social Sciences statistical software package version 11·5 (SPSS UK Ltd., Feltham, UK) was used for statistical analysis.

Results

Approximately 10–15 % of those who initiated contact were included in the study. Equal proportions of healthy male and female older volunteers were recruited from four European centres. Coleraine, Northern Ireland (n 93) and Clermont-Ferrand, France (n 95) recruited participants aged 55–70 years, while Rome, Italy (n 108) and Grenoble, France (n 91) recruited participants who were >70 years of age. In total, 387 participants successfully completed the trial (Table 1). A full description of the sample characteristics has been provided elsewhere by Simpson et al.(Reference Simpson, O’Connor and Livingstone59).

Table 1 Sociodemographic variables for each region, given as percentages

Total participants=387: Northern Ireland = 93; Clermont-Ferrand = 95; Rome = 108; and Grenoble = 91.

Baseline measures

Neither sex nor age differed between the treatment groups. The younger (55–70 years) age group comprised a higher proportion (84 %) of professional occupations than the older (≥70 years) age group (68 %). A higher proportion of those in the younger age group (96 %) than the older age group (35 %) were educated to the tertiary level. Neither social class nor educational level differed between the treatment groups. There were no apparent differences by age group or treatment group in dietary zinc intake at baseline. There were no differences in MMSE or (GDS) scores by age group or treatment condition(Reference Maylor, Simpson and Secker60).

Serum zinc

Mean serum zinc was within the normal range (11–18 μmol/l)(Reference McConville, Simpson and Rae61) for the placebo (mean = 13·20 (sd 1·69) μmol/l), 15 mg (mean = 13·28 (sd 1·84) μmol/l) and 30 mg (mean = 13·13 (sd 1·63) μmol/l) supplemented groups at baseline and remained so throughout the intervention period. Serum zinc increased over time (F(4197) = 11·021, P = 0·000) in both the 15 mg (mean = 13·99 (sd 2·47) μmol/l; P = 0·018) and 30 mg (mean = 15·03 (sd 3·17) μmol/l; P = 0·000) supplemented groups compared to placebo (mean = 13·05 (sd 1·66) μmol/l) suggesting compliance with the intervention. Serum zinc concentrations in response to zinc (30 mg) were higher among those recruited in Rome (mean = 16·26 (sd 3·41) μmol/l) than Grenoble (mean = 13·64 (sd 2·21) μmol/l) post-intervention (F(4197) = 3·526, P = 0·008).

Affect (the Positive and Negative Affect Scale) and zinc

There were no treatment (15 mg/d zinc; 30 mg/d zinc; or placebo) × time (baseline; 3 months; or 6 months post-intervention) or interaction effects, indicating that the treatment had no differential effects on affect (PANAS) over time. For NA among the 55–70-year-olds, the interaction effect was F(4340) = 1·56, P = 0·185 (Table 1) and for the ≥70-year-olds, F(4372) = 0·466, P = 0·761 (Table 2). Interaction effects for PA among the 55–70-year-olds were F(4340) = 0·37, P = 0·833 (Table 2) and among the ≥70-year-olds were F(4372) = 1·32, P = 0·261 (Table 3). None of the triple interaction effects involving treatment and time were significant.

Table 2 PANAS scores for each treatment group (55–70-year-olds/Coleraine and Clermont-Ferrand) over time (n 188)

PANAS, Positive and Negative Affect Score.

Table 3 PANAS scores for each treatment group (≥70-year-olds/Rome and Grenoble) over time (n 199)

PANAS, Positive and Negative Affect Score.

Centre and affect (the Positive and Negative Affect Scale)

There was a significant treatment × centre interaction effect for NA among the ≥70-year age group (F(1186) = 3·73, P = 0·025). For the placebo, 15 mg/d and 30 mg/d treatment groups, the means were 11·45, 13·43 and 11·94, respectively, for Rome and 13·06, 12·83 and 14·38, respectively, for Grenoble. For Grenoble, the trend for NA was U-shaped showing a decrease at 3 months into the intervention and returning to baseline at 6 months. For Rome, the trend for NA was an inverted U-shape such that NA was increased at 3 months into the intervention and returning to baseline at 6 months. There were no significant interactions between treatment and centre for PA in either the 55–70-year or ≥70-year age group.

Gender and affect (the Positive and Negative Affect Scale)

With the exception of a significant gender × centre interaction effect for NA among the 55–70-year-olds (F(1170) = 4·71, P = 0·031), there were no significant main effects or interaction effects for gender. The significant gender × centre interaction effect was such that male volunteers reported higher NA than female volunteers in the Coleraine sample (mean = 12·00 and 11·39, respectively), whereas the reverse was true in the Clermont-Ferrand sample (mean = 11·48 and 12·14).

Discussion

Studies of clinically depressed(Reference Bodnor and Wisner18Reference Hanson, Malecha and Mackenzie24) and healthy(Reference Hwang, Lee and Kim27, Reference Sawada and Yokoi28) groups have suggested a link between zinc status and psychological well-being. Given evidence to suggest that for a multiplicity of reasons, ageing can be associated with zinc depletion(Reference Seiler, Itin and Stahelin31), it was hypothesised that supplemented zinc would enhance mood in older people. It was, therefore, surprising to find no alteration in PA or NA in response to zinc (15 mg/d or 30 mg/d) compared to placebo in either the younger (55–70 years) or the older (≥70 years) age group, who had lower PA than the younger age group at baseline(Reference McConville, Simpson and Rae61). Both NA and PA were unaffected by zinc supplementation in either age group. In the older age group (≥70 years), NA increased among those in Rome and decreased among those in Grenoble 3 months into the intervention and returned to baseline in both groups at 6 months. This trend is difficult to explain. Although serum zinc was higher among those in Rome than Grenoble at 3 months, serum zinc is not an accurate indicator of zinc status and could have been altered in response to a range of factors unrelated to affect(Reference Miller, Krebs and Hambridge49). Increased serum zinc concentrations post-treatment, however, suggest good compliance with the intervention in both groups. The PANAS is a well-validated subjective (self-reported) psychometric measure of affect(Reference Watson, Clarke and Tellegen52), which has been widely used and previously used in other studies involving elderly adults(Reference Hill, Van Boxtel and Ponds62). The PANAS has also been shown to correlate with the biochemical markers of affect(Reference Simpson, McConville and Rae40, Reference Williams, Stewart-Knox and Helander55, Reference Duffy, Stewart-Knox and McConville56). This correlation indicates that although the scales measure subjective affect, the data generated can be considered sensitive to corresponding changes in neurochemical activity. The PANAS can, therefore, be assumed to have good concurrent validity for the measurement of affect. Although the PA data collected for the present study agree with those of the established norms, the NA scores appear to be less than those suggested by established norms for older people(Reference Watson, Clarke and Tellegen52). The relatively low NA reported by our sample at baseline may explain the lack of change in NA following zinc supplementation. The apparent lack of change in affect in response to zinc could also be because the participants were zinc replete(Reference Anriollo-Sanchez, Hininger-Favier and Meunier50). Screening procedures were stringent and care was taken to recruit only physically and psychologically healthy older people to the Zenith Study. Health and well-being tend to be confounded with social factors. It is possible that in selecting only healthy individuals, those from socio-economically deprived segments of society have been excluded. That our sample was biased towards the higher social classes and those who had spent a relatively longer period in education (Table 1) may also explain the apparently good psychological well-being among our sample and lack of response to the intervention. The participants, by virtue of having volunteered, may not be entirely representative of the general population, potentially limiting the degree to which the findings can be extrapolated to the general older population. Those who have spent longer periods of time in education and those in more privileged social classes tend to have lower mortality and morbidity(Reference Wanless1) and may, therefore, also have a greater chance of being recruited to such studies.

Conclusion

The present study seems to be the first randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled intervention trial investigating the impact of supplemented zinc upon affect in healthy older adults. Intervention outcomes were assessed using the PANAS, which is a well-validated subjective (self-reported) psychometric measure of affect. The study was also strong in that it used a relatively large sample comprising late middle-aged and elderly individuals from both northern and southern European populations with adequate power with which to uphold the null hypothesis and within which sex and cultural effects were controlled. These data suggest that zinc supplementation at considered 15 mg/d or 30 mg/d does not alter mood in healthy elderly European adults.

Acknowledgements

The project was funded by the European Commission ‘Quality of Life and Management of Living Resources’ Fifth Framework Programme, Contract no. QLK1-CT-2001-00168. The authors have no conflicts of interest. B.J.S.-K. contributed to the design of the study and drafted the paper; G.R. undertook the statistical analysis; E.E.A.S. undertook data collection and database development; J.M.O. coordinated research at University of Ulster; C.Mc.C. contributed to the mood protocol; A.P. supervised the data collection at INRAN; M.A.-S. supervised the data collection at Grenoble; C.C. supervised the data collection at INRA and coordinated Zenith; and J.J.S. contributed to the nutritional protocol and advised on the zinc intervention.

References

1.Wanless, D (2004) Securing Good Health for the Whole Population: Final Report. London: HM Treasury; available at http://www.hmtreasury.gov.uk/consultations and legislation/wanless/consultwanless04final.cfmGoogle Scholar
2.Hambidge, M (2000) Human zinc deficiency. J Nutr 130, 5S Suppl., 1344S1349S.Google Scholar
3.Frederickson, CJ, Won Suh, S, Silva, D et al. (2000) Importance of zinc in the central nervous system: the zinc-containing neuron. J Nutr 130, 5S Suppl., 1471S1483S.Google Scholar
4.Colvin, RA, Davis, N, Nipper, RW et al. (2000) Zinc transport in the brain: routes of zinc influx and efflux in neurons. J Nutr 130, 5S Suppl., 1484S1487S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Szewczyk, B, Poleszak, E, Wlaz, P et al. (2009) The involvement of the serotonergic system in the antidepressant effect of zinc in the forced swim test. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 33, 323329.Google Scholar
6.Matias, CM, Saggau, P & Quinta-Ferreira, ME (2010) Blockade of presynaptic K ATP channels reduces the zinc-mediated posttetanic depression at hippocampal mossy fiber synapses. Brain Res 1320, 2227.Google Scholar
7.Watanabe, M, Tamano, H, Kikuchi, T et al. (2010) Susceptibility to stress in young rats after 2-week zinc deprivation. Neurochem Int 56, 410416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Sowa-Kucma, M, Legutko, B, Szewczyk, B et al. (2008) Antidepressant-like activity of zinc: further behavioral and molecular evidence. J Neural Transm 115, 16211628.Google Scholar
9.Takeda, A & Tamado, H (2009) Insight into zinc signaling from dietary zinc deficiency. Brain Res Rev 62, 3344.Google Scholar
10.Tamano, H, Kan, F, Kawamura, M et al. (2009) Behavior in the forced swim test and neurochemical changes in the hippocampus in young rats after 2-week zinc deprivation. Neurochem Int 55, 536541.Google Scholar
11.Takeda, A (2009) Behavior in the forced swim test and neurochemical changes in the hippocampus in young rats after 2-week zinc deprivation. Neurochem Int 55, 536541.Google Scholar
12.Cichy, A, Sowa-Kucma, M, Legutko, B et al. (2009) Zinc-induced adaptive changes in NMDA/glutamatergic and serontonergic receptors. Pharmacol Rep 61, 11841191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
13.Barrondo, S & Salles, J (2009) Allosteric modulation of 5-HT1A receptors by zinc: binding sites. Neuropharmacology 56, 455462.Google Scholar
14.Michel, TM, Frangou, S, Thiemeyer, D et al. (2007) Evidence for oxidative stress in the frontal cortex in patients with recurrent depressive disorder – a post-mortem study. Psychiatry Res 151, 145150.Google Scholar
15.Kaplan, BJ, Field, CJ, Crawford, SG et al. (2007) Vitamins, minerals and mood. Psychol Bull 133, 747760.Google Scholar
16.Marcellini, F, Giuli, C, Papa, R et al. (2006) Zinc status, psychological and nutritional assessment in old people recruited in five European studies: Zincage study. Biogerontology 7, 339345.Google Scholar
17.Siwek, MS, Wrobel, A, Dudek, D et al. (2005) The role of zinc in the pathogenesis and treatment of affective disorders. Psychiatr Pol 39, 899909.Google Scholar
18.Bodnor, LM & Wisner, KL (2005) Nutrition and depression: implications for improving mental health among childbearing aged women. Biol Psychiatry 58, 679685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
19.Maes, M, de Vos, N, Demedts, P et al. (1999) Lower serum zinc in major depression in relation to changes in serum acute phase proteins. J Affect Disord 56, 189194.Google Scholar
20.Maes, M, Vandoolaeghe, E, Neels, H et al. (1997) Lower serum zinc in major depression is a sensitive marker of treatment resistance and of the immune/inflammatory response in the illness. Soc Biol Psychiatry 42, 349358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21.Siwek, M, Dudek, D, Paul, IA et al. (2009) Zinc supplementation augments efficacy of imipramine in treatment resistant patients. J Affect Disord 118, 187195.Google Scholar
22.Walsh, WJ, Isaacson, HR, Rehman, F et al. (1997) Elevated blood copper/zinc ratios in assaultive young males. Physiol Behav 62, 327329.Google Scholar
23.Little, KY, Castellanos, X, Humphries, LL et al. (1989) Altered zinc metabolism in mood disorder patients. Biol Psychiatry 26, 646648.Google Scholar
24.Hanson, CR, Malecha, M, Mackenzie, TB et al. (1982) Copper and zinc deficiencies in association with depression and neurological findings. Biol Psychiatry 18, 395401.Google Scholar
25.Maes, M, Neels, H, Demedts, P et al. (1997) Lower serum zinc in major depression is a sensitive marker. Biol Psychiatry 42, 349353.Google Scholar
26.Levenson, CW (2006) Zinc: the new antidepressant? Nutr Rev 64, 3942.Google Scholar
27.Hwang, JY, Lee, SE, Kim, SH et al. (2010) Psychological distress is associated with inadequate dietary intake in Vietnamese marriage immigrant women in Korea. J Am Diet Assoc 110, 779785.Google Scholar
28.Sawada, T & Yokoi, K (2010) Effect of zinc supplementation on mood states in young women: a pilot study. Eur J Clin Nutr 64, 331333.Google Scholar
29.Seiler, WO (1999) Nutritional status in ill elderly subjects. Z Gerontol Geriatr 32, 711.Google Scholar
30.Haase, H, Mocchegiani, E & Rink, L (2006) Correlation between zinc status and immune function in the elderly. Biogerontology 7, 421428.Google Scholar
31.Seiler, WO, Itin, P & Stahelin, HB (2002) Zinc deficiency, a problem of old age frequently not recognised. Ernahrungsschau 49, 260.Google Scholar
32.Rybka, J, Kedziora-Kornatowska, K, Kedziora, J et al. (2009) Immunosenescence and late life depression. Cent Eur J Immunol 34, 271275.Google Scholar
33.Maes, M, de Vos, N, Wauters, A et al. (1999) Inflammatory markers in younger vs elderly normal volunteers. J Psychiatr Res 33, 397405.Google Scholar
34.Reichies, FM, Felsenberg, D, Gessner, R et al. (1998) Age and dementia effect on neuropsychological test performance in very old age-influence of risk factors for dementia. J Neural Transm 54, 6976.Google Scholar
35.Drewnowski, A & Shulz, JM (2001) Impact of aging on eating behaviour, food choices, nutrition and health status. J Nutr Health Aging 5, 7579.Google Scholar
36.CidRuzafa, J, Caulfield, LE, Barron, Y et al. (1999) Nutrient intakes and adequacy among an older population on the eastern shore of Maryland. J Am Diet Assoc 99, 546571.Google Scholar
37.Kant, AK & Schatzkin, A (1999) Relation of age and self reported medical condition status with dietary nutrient intake in the US population. J Am Coll Nutr 18, 6976.Google Scholar
38.Webb, KL, Schofield, WN, Lazerus, R et al. (1999) Prevalence and socio-demographic predictors of dietary goal attainment in an older population. Aust NZ J Public Health 23, 578584.Google Scholar
39.Cole, MG & Dendukuri, N (2003) Risk factors for depression among elderly community subjects: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 160, 11471156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40.Simpson, EEA, McConville, C, Rae, G et al. (2008) Salivary cortisol, stress and mood in healthy older adults: the Zenith Study. Biol Psychol 78, 19.Google Scholar
41.Ostir, GV, Ottenbacher, KJ & Markides, KS (2004) Onset of frailty in older adults and the protective role of positive affect. Psychol Aging 19, 402408.Google Scholar
42.Goh, LY, Vitry, AI, Semple, SJ et al. (2009) Self-medication with over-the-counter drugs and complementary medications in South Australia’s elderly population. BMC Comple Alt Med 9, 42.Google Scholar
43.World Health Organization (1995) Physical Status: The Use and Interpretation of Anthropometry. Report of a WHO Expert Committee. WHO Technical Report Series no. 854. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
44.Folstein, MF, Folstein, E & McHugh, PR (1975) Mini Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
45.Yesavage, JA, Brink, TL, Rose, TL et al. (1983) Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res 17, 3749.Google Scholar
46.Almeida, OP & Almeida, SA (1999) Short versions of the geriatric depression scale: a study of their validity for the diagnosis of a major depressive episode according to ICD-10 and DSM IV. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 14, 858865.Google Scholar
47.Polito, A, Meunier, N, Andriollo-Sanchez, M et al. (2005) Screening and recruitment procedure of late-middle aged and older subjects. Eur J Clin Nutr 59, S8S12.Google Scholar
48.Gibson, RS (2005) Principles of Nutritional Assessment, 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
49.Miller, LV, Krebs, NF & Hambridge, MK (2000) Development of a compartmental model of human zinc metabolism: identifiability and multiple studies analyses. Am J Physiol Regul Intergr Comp Physiol 279, R11671R11684.Google Scholar
50.Anriollo-Sanchez, M, Hininger-Favier, I, Meunier, N et al. (2005) Zinc intake and status in middle-aged and older European Subjects: the Zenith study. Eur J Clin Nutr 59, S31S36.Google Scholar
51.Polito, A, Intorre, F, Andriollo-Sanchez, M et al. (2005) Estimation of intake and status of vitamin A, vitamin E and folate in older European adults: the Zenith study. Eur J Clin Nutr 59, S42S47.Google Scholar
52.Watson, D, Clarke, LA & Tellegen, A (1988) Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scale. J Pers Soc Psychol 47, 10631070.Google Scholar
53.Watson, D & Tellegen, A (1985) Towards a consensual structure of mood. Psychol Bull 98, 219235.Google Scholar
54.Watson, D & Clark, LA (1992) On traits and temperament: general and specific factors of emotional experience and their relation to the five factor model. J Pers 60, 441476.Google Scholar
55.Williams, E, Stewart-Knox, B, Helander, A et al. (2006) Associations between whole-blood serotonin and subjective mood in healthy male volunteers. Biol Psychol 71, 171174.Google Scholar
56.Duffy, M, Stewart-Knox, B, McConville, C et al. (2006) The relationship between whole-blood serotonin and subjective mood in apparently healthy postmenopausal women. Biol Psychol 73, 165168.Google Scholar
57.Watson, D & Walker, LM (1996) The long term stability and predictive validity of trait measures of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol 70, 567577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
58.Erdfelder, E, Faul, F & Buchner, A (1996) GPOWER: a general power analysis program. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 28, 111.Google Scholar
59.Simpson, EEA, O’Connor, JM, Livingstone, MBE et al. (2005) Health and lifestyle characteristics of older European adults: the Zenith study. Eur J Clin Nutr 59, S13S21.Google Scholar
60.Maylor, EA, Simpson, EEA, Secker, DL et al. (2006) Effects of zinc supplementation on cognitive function in healthy middle-aged and older adults: the Zenith study. Br J Nutr 96, 752760.Google Scholar
61.McConville, C, Simpson, EEA, Rae, G et al. (2005) Positive and negative mood in the elderly: the Zenith study. Eur J Clin Nutr 59, S22S25.Google Scholar
62.Hill, RD, Van Boxtel, MPJ, Ponds, R et al. (2005) Positive affect and its relationship to free recall memory performance in a sample of older Dutch adults from the Maastricht aging study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 20, 429435.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Sociodemographic variables for each region, given as percentages

Figure 1

Table 2 PANAS scores for each treatment group (55–70-year-olds/Coleraine and Clermont-Ferrand) over time (n 188)

Figure 2

Table 3 PANAS scores for each treatment group (≥70-year-olds/Rome and Grenoble) over time (n 199)