Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T21:30:23.524Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What is the cell hydration status of healthy children in the USA? Preliminary data on urine osmolality and water intake

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 January 2012

Jodi D. Stookey*
Affiliation:
Children's Hospital Oakland Research Institute, 5700 Martin Luther King Jr. Way, Oakland, CA 94609, USA
Bernie Brass
Affiliation:
Children's Hospital Oakland Research Institute, 5700 Martin Luther King Jr. Way, Oakland, CA 94609, USA
Ava Holliday
Affiliation:
Children's Hospital Oakland Research Institute, 5700 Martin Luther King Jr. Way, Oakland, CA 94609, USA
Allen Arieff
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Email jstookey@chori.org
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

Hyperosmotic stress on cells limits many aspects of cell function, metabolism and health. International data suggest that schoolchildren may be at risk of hyperosmotic stress on cells because of suboptimal water intake. The present study explored the cell hydration status of two samples of children in the USA.

Design

Cross-sectional study describing the urine osmolality (an index of hyperosmotic cell shrinkage) and water intake of convenience samples from Los Angeles (LA) and New York City (NYC).

Setting

Each participant collected a urine sample at an outpatient clinic on the way to school on a weekday morning in spring 2009. Each was instructed to wake, eat, drink and do as usual before school, and complete a dietary record form describing the type and amounts of all foods and beverages consumed after waking, before giving the sample.

Subjects

The children (9–11 years) in LA (n 337) and NYC (n 211) considered themselves healthy enough to go to school on the day they gave the urine sample.

Results

Elevated urine osmolality (>800 mmol/kg) was observed in 63 % and 66 % of participants in LA and NYC, respectively. In multivariable-adjusted logistic regression models, elevated urine osmolality was associated with not reporting intake of drinking water in the morning (LA: OR = 2·1, 95 % CI 1·2, 3·5; NYC: OR = 1·8, 95 % CI 1·0, 3·5). Although over 90 % of both samples had breakfast before giving the urine sample, 75 % did not drink water.

Conclusions

Research is warranted to confirm these results and pursue their potential health implications.

Type
Nutrition and health
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2012

Cell hydration is integral to endocrine, immune, neural, cardiovascular, gastrointestinal, muscle and skeletal function. Changes in cell volume are required, for example, to mediate hormone release and response(Reference Lang, Busch and Ritter1), produce the electrical signals for neuron activation(Reference Andrew and MacVicar2Reference Krause, deKloet and Flak5) and regulate gene transcription and metabolic pathways(Reference Yancey, Clark and Hand6, Reference Haussinger7). When solute concentrations outside the cell are elevated, they create an osmotic force that draws water out of cells, limiting cell swelling and the processes that depend on cell volume change(Reference Andrew and MacVicar2, Reference Hernanz-Schulman, Vanholder and Waterloos8Reference Schliess and Haussinger16).

Cell dehydration is acutely associated with a wide variety of adverse outcomes in healthy adults and children, including insulin resistance(Reference Berneis, Ninnis and Haussinger10), an exaggerated cortisol(Reference Judelson, Maresh and Yamamoto13) and immune(Reference McKenzie, Greenleaf and Looft-Wilson17) response to exercise, decreased sympathetic nervous activity(Reference Brown, Barberini and Dulloo11), impaired thermoregulation(Reference Cheung and McLellan18) and impaired cognitive and physical performance(Reference Ainslie, Campbell and Frayn19, Reference Sawka20). In patients, cell dehydration is also associated with a wide variety of adverse outcomes, including diabetic ketoacidosis(Reference Silnik21), neurological complications(Reference Trachtman22), renal nephropathy(Reference Vercellino, Bezante and Balbi23), susceptibility to infection(Reference Pier24, Reference Kuroda, Harada and Tsutsumi25), increased risk of red cell lysis in sickle cell anaemia(Reference Platt26, Reference Tripette, Loko and Samb27), nutrient malabsorption(Reference El-Mougi, Hendawi and Koura28, Reference Hahn, Kim and Garner29) and mortality(Reference Arora30, Reference Alshayeb, Showkat and Babar31).

Although much is known about the effects of cell dehydration in acute or extreme conditions (e.g. marathons) and critical illness, little is known about the longer-term public health impact of osmotic stress on cells, more generally. Data from the Duke EPESE (Duke site of the Established Populations for Epidemiologic Studies of the Elderly) study indicate that cell dehydration is associated with increased risk of incident diabetes(Reference Stookey, Pieper and Cohen32), frailty over 4 years and mortality over 8 years(Reference Stookey, Purser and Pieper33) in community-dwelling older adults. The DONALD (Dortmund Nutritional and Anthropometric Longitudinally Designed) study in Germany has collected longitudinal data over 15 years on hydration status in free-living children(Reference Kroke, Manz and Kersting34). Population-based, longitudinal studies are needed to determine if/how cell dehydration translates into disease risk in representative samples of non-acutely ill, free-living individuals.

In vivo, cell hydration status can be studied using urine osmolality. Cell shrinkage stimulates release of anti-diuretic hormone (ADH), which signals the kidney to concentrate urine. Above a threshold osmolality of approximately 280 mmol/kg, ADH is secreted in direct proportion to the osmotic force drawing water out of cells. As ADH levels increase, urine osmolality increases linearly until it reaches a maximum of approximately 1200 mmol/kg in children and younger adults(Reference Star35, Reference Manz and Wentz36). Values over 800 mmol/kg are considered elevated(Reference Edelmann, Barnett and Stark37Reference Philip, Chaimovitz and Singer42); values over 1000 mmol/kg are considered maximally concentrated. In healthy individuals, urine osmolality normally increases following overnight water restriction and decreases over the course of the day with water intake at meals(Reference Van Hoeck, Bael and Lax43). Elevated urine osmolality at a point in time reflects cell shrinkage 2 to 4 h prior to the sample collection(Reference Nose, Mack and Shi44, Reference Merson, Maughan and Shirreffs45). Elevated urine osmolality from a standardized 24 h urine collection, begun after the bladder is emptied, reflects sustained cell shrinkage over the 24 h collection period. Urine osmolality is less invasive and expensive and/or more sensitive to mild cell dehydration than other indicators, such as plasma osmolality, plasma ADH, urine specific gravity and urine colour.

Although population-representative data on urine osmolality are rare(Reference Manz and Wentz36), available data suggest that cell dehydration may be prevalent in healthy, free-living children at school. Studies from Israel report that a majority of healthy Jewish children have a random urine osmolality over 800 mmol/kg on arrival at school(Reference Bar-David, Urkin and Kozminsky40) and/or at noon-time at school(Reference Bar-David, Urkin and Kozminsky40, Reference Bar-David, Urkin and Landau41, Reference Bar David, Landau and Bar David46). The elevated urine osmolality is attributed to insufficient water intake, because neighbouring Bedouin-Arab children, who live under the same arid conditions but have different drinking habits, do not have an elevated mean urine osmolality (<800 mmol/kg)(Reference Bar-David, Urkin and Kozminsky40, Reference Bar David, Landau and Bar David46). Healthy children at school may be at risk of low water intake and cell dehydration because of limited access to drinking water and/or a phenomenon known as ‘voluntary dehydration’(Reference Bar-Or47, Reference D'Anci, Constant and Rosenberg48).

Data from other countries suggest that elevated urine osmolality may be prevalent in healthy children throughout the day. In Germany, the mean 24 h urine osmolality of a representative sample of children, aged 3–18 years, was 801 mmol/kg(Reference Manz and Wentz36). In Sweden, the mean 18 h urine osmolality of a convenience sample of twenty adolescents (12–17 years) was 841 mmol/kg(Reference Manz and Wentz36). In Germany, elevated urine osmolality is associated with lower total water intake and less water from beverages(Reference Stahl, Kroke and Bolzenius49).

The current prevalence of elevated urine osmolality in children in the USA is unknown. According to nationally representative data, however, US children have suboptimal water intake relative to recommendations(Reference Kant and Graubard50), and most US school districts do not have wellness policies that ensure the availability of drinking water(51). In Los Angeles, access to drinking water is limited in some schools(Reference Patel, Bogart and Uyeda52).

The present study describes the urine osmolality of healthy children in two separate convenience samples from different regions of the USA, Los Angeles (LA) and New York City (NYC). To facilitate comparison of the results with the data from Israel, the study evaluated random urine osmolality on a weekday morning at a time of day when the participants were normally at school. To pursue whether elevated urine osmolality might be attributable to low water intake, the study tested for an association between elevated urine osmolality and lower total water intake, under temperate spring conditions, controlling for other hydration determinants. To determine if elevated urine osmolality might, furthermore, be attributable to drinking habits, the source of water intake (drinking water, water from other beverages or water from food) was also evaluated.

Methods

Participants and study protocol

The present cross-sectional study recruited healthy boys and girls, aged 9–11 years, living in LA and NYC in spring 2009. In each city, 50 000 households were randomly selected from age-targeted mailing lists by a professional mailing company (Metro Mailing, Sacramento, CA, USA) to receive a postcard invitation to participate in a study of ‘normal kidney function in healthy children’. Each postcard had a unique study id and invited only one eligible child to participate. Area of residence, age and general health were the only inclusion/exclusion criteria applied. Consistent with an expected response rate of 1 % for postcard mailings (per the mailing company), 548 families in LA and 335 families in NYC responded to the mailing.

Respondents were mailed study materials, including child assent and parental consent forms, and instructions to choose a weekday morning when the child felt healthy enough to go to school to participate in the study. The child was instructed to wake as usual on the selected morning, eat, drink and do whatever is normal for him/her before 08.30 hours, then go to a local Quest Diagnostics Patient Service Center clinic to void a clean catch sample in a cup between 08.30 and 09.30 hours and complete a questionnaire. The study materials did not disclose the study purpose to study cell hydration or urine osmolality. Respondents were offered a $US 50 incentive for participation. Between 1 May and 15 June 2009, 337 children in LA and 211 in NYC completed the urine collection and study questionnaire. The maximum ambient temperatures in LA and NYC did not exceed 22°C during the study period. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Children's Hospital & Research Center Oakland where the study was coordinated. All study staff were trained in the protection of human research subjects. All study participants returned signed parental consent and child assent forms. All data records were collected and analysed in Oakland, CA, USA.

Urine osmolality

Urine osmolality was determined by freezing point depression osmometer at central Quest Diagnostics Laboratories (West Hills, CA, USA and Teterboro, NJ, USA). Consistent with the studies from Israel, elevated urine osmolality was defined as over 800 mmol/kg(Reference Bar-David, Urkin and Kozminsky40, Reference Philip, Chaimovitz and Singer42, Reference Bar David, Landau and Bar David46).

Water intake

Study participants recorded the type and quantity of all foods and/or beverages consumed in the morning before the urine collection on a diet record form with free spaces for reporting each item's time of consumption, name, description (e.g. low-fat v. whole milk, brand name, whole wheat v. white bread, margarine v. butter) and amount consumed. Only morning intake was recorded because urine osmolality reflects cell hydration since the previous void (2–4 h)(Reference Nose, Mack and Shi44, Reference Merson, Maughan and Shirreffs45). The diet records were entered into Nutrition Diet Systems (NDS-R) software (1998–2008; Nutrition Coordinating Center, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA) by two trained research staff to estimate intake of drinking water, water from other beverages, water from food and total dietary water (the sum of drinking water, water from other beverages and water from food). Metabolic water was excluded from the total water intake calculation to focus on water intake behaviour or ‘drinking habits’. One researcher was trained to use the NDS-R software in Minnesota by the Nutrition Coordinating Center. Drinking water was defined as tap, spring, mineral or unsweetened sparkling water. Other beverages were defined as any beverage other than drinking water, following the food group codes in the NDS-R software. Smoothies and milkshakes were considered beverages. Milk added to cereal was considered food.

If details about food or beverage type or portion size (such as the type or amount of milk added to cereal) were missing, systematic assumptions were applied, as there was no follow-up contact with families. To address potential reporting errors in the individual water intake estimates, dichotomous indicator variables were created to group participants.

Study participants were grouped with respect to the total amount of water reported as ‘higher’ v. ‘lower’ total water intake, defined using an arbitrary cut-off of 500 ml, equivalent to about 20 % of the Adequate Intake of water for children 9–13 years(53).

Participants were also grouped with respect to the reported source of water intake: drinking water, water from other beverages and water from food. Study participants who reported any drinking water, with or without food or other beverages, were identified as ‘water drinkers’. Participants who reported any volume of any beverage other than drinking water, with or without food, were grouped in an ‘other beverage’ group. Participants who reported eating food, but had no beverage, were grouped in a ‘food only’ group. A ‘no food or beverage’ group reported no intake at all on the morning of the urine sample.

Potential confounding factors

The study questionnaire collected information about factors other than water intake that influence hydration status. Study participants self-reported their age, sex, body weight, height, general health and ‘whether they noticed any skin changes, especially pimples’ in the past 2 months. Recent skin changes are a validated index of pubertal hormones(Reference Bond, Clements and Bertalli54Reference Shirtcliff, Dahl and Pollak56) that modify ADH release(Reference Ikeda, Tanaka and Oki57, Reference Stachenfeld58). The participants self-reported how many minutes they moved enough to sweat (like fast walking) or moved so fast that they got out of breath (like running) on the previous day. In sixth graders, self-reports of previous-day physical activity correlate with measurements made by accelerometer(Reference Weston, Petosa and Pate59, Reference Welk, Dzewaltowski and Hill60). The participants recorded their use of medications and the time they went to sleep on the day before the urine collection. They recorded what time they woke on the morning of the urine collection. The hours of sleep were calculated to index the length of overnight fluid restriction. The NDS-R software generated estimates of the morning total energy intake and potential renal solute load (mmol = 5·7 × (g of protein) + (mg of Na/23) + (mg of K/39) + 0·55 × (mg of P/31))(Reference Friis-Hansen61). The latter is an index of ‘osmotically effective’ solute that cannot freely cross cell membranes and can, therefore, draw water out of cells by osmosis. Missing values for any of these factors were set to the sample mean value.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were done using the STATA statistical software package version 9·2 (2006; StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA). To describe the prevalence of elevated urine osmolality and its relationship with water intake in the sample from each locale, data from the LA and NYC samples were analysed separately. All logistic and linear regression models adjusted for all of the covariates (age, sex, general health, body weight, height, recent skin changes, use of medications, physical activity level, sleep, and the morning total energy intake and potential renal solute load).

Sample characteristics associated with elevated urine osmolality

To identify variables that might confound relationships between elevated urine osmolality and water intake, multivariable logistic regression models tested for associations between each covariate listed above and the relative odds of elevated urine osmolality.

Reported water intake by level of urine osmolality

Linear regression models were used to describe how the raw (continuous) water intake variables related to urine osmolality before they were collapsed to group indicator variables. The models tested for associations between urine osmolality and total water, drinking water, water from other beverages and water from food, expressed in ml units.

Relative odds of elevated urine osmolality by level of total water intake and source of water intake

Logistic regression models tested for greater odds of elevated urine osmolality associated with a lower v. higher level of total water intake. Separate logistic regression models estimated the relative odds of elevated urine osmolality associated with different sources of water intake. The group that reported drinking water (the reference category) was compared with the groups that reported beverages other than drinking water, food only or no food or beverage. Additional logistic regression models also compared the group that reported drinking water with all of the other groups combined.

Mean urine osmolality by level of total water intake and source of water intake

It was not possible to estimate the relative odds of elevated urine osmolality associated with source of water, stratified by the level of total water. Only one out of thirteen participants from LA who reported over 500 ml total water, without drinking water, had a urine osmolality below 800 mmol/kg; only five out of twenty-five in NYC. Linear regression models therefore estimated the mean urine osmolality associated with drinking water v. not drinking water, stratified by the level of total water intake.

Results

Sample characteristics

Table 1 describes the samples from LA and NYC. The mean body weight and height were 39 (sd 10) kg and 144 (sd 9) cm in LA, and 39 (sd 12) kg and 143 (sd 11) cm in NYC.

Table 1 Characteristics of the study samples: healthy children aged 9–11 years in Los Angeles (LA) and New York City (NYC), spring 2009

Over 90 % of both samples reported eating or drinking something in the hours before giving the urine sample (see Table 2). The mean reported total energy intake was 1348 (sd 816) kJ (322 (sd 195) kcal) for the sample from LA and 1319 (sd 808) kJ (315 (sd 193) kcal) for the sample from NYC. The mean osmotic load was 98 (sd 69) mmol for the sample from LA and 97 (sd 67) mmol for the sample from NYC. Total water intake ranged from zero to 1000 ml for the sample from LA, with 88 % of intakes below 500 ml and a median intake of 260 ml. For the sample from NYC, total water intake ranged from zero to 990 ml, with 85 % of intakes below 500 ml and a median intake of 270 ml. Approximately 75 % of both samples did not report drinking water. The majority reported a beverage other than water or food with no beverage.

Table 2 Relative odds of elevated urine osmolality by reported level of total water intake and source of water intake: healthy children aged 9–11 years in Los Angeles (LA) and New York City (NYC), spring 2009

†The odds ratio was adjusted for age, sex, general health, body weight, height, recent skin changes, use of medications, physical activity level, sleep in the 24 h before the urine sample, and the total energy intake and potential renal solute load on the morning of the urine sample.

‡Lower total water intake, <500 ml; higher total water intake, ≥500 ml.

§Reference category in the logistic regression model.

∥Study participants were grouped by the source of water intake. The ‘drinking water’ group reported intake of plain tap water, mineral, spring or unsweetened sparkling water, with or without intake of other beverages or food. The ‘other beverages’ group reported intake of beverages other than water. Specifically, they reported non-fat, 1 %, 2 % and whole milks; almond-, strawberry-, chocolate- and coffee-flavoured milks; smoothies; milkshakes; soya milk; orange, apple, cranberry, grape, pineapple, pomegranate, strawberry–banana and tomato juices; lemonade; sweetened teas; sports drinks; seltzer; and soda. The ‘food only’ group reported food, but no beverages. The ‘no food or beverages’ group did not eat or drink before the urine collection.

Prevalence of elevated urine osmolality

Figure 1 describes the distribution of urine osmolality in both samples. Urine osmolality ranged from 99 to 1372 mmol/kg for participants from LA, with a mean of 827 (sd 250) mmol/kg. It ranged from 186 to 1259 mmol/kg, with a mean of 853 (sd 219) mmol/kg, for participants from NYC. The prevalence of elevated urine osmolality (over 800 mmol/kg) was 63 % in the sample from LA and 66 % in the sample from NYC. Maximally concentrated urine (over 1000 mmol/kg) was observed for 25 % of participants from LA and 27 % of participants from NYC.

Fig. 1 Distribution of random morning urine osmolality of healthy children aged 9–11 years in (a) Los Angeles (n 337) and (b) New York City (n 211), spring 2009

Sample characteristics associated with elevated urine osmolality

In the sample from LA, elevated urine osmolality was significantly more likely among boys (adjusted OR = 2·0, 95 % CI 1·2, 3·3, P = 0·005) and less likely among those reporting recent skin changes (adjusted OR = 0·6, 95 % CI 0·4, 1·0). Similar trends for male gender and skin changes in the NYC sample were not statistically significant. The other covariates listed above were not significantly associated with elevated urine osmolality in either sample.

Reported water intake by level of urine osmolality

Figure 2 illustrates the bivariate relationships between urine osmolality and reported water intake. Urine osmolality was inversely associated with total water intake in the sample from LA, but not in the sample from NYC. Urine osmolality was inversely associated with drinking water in both samples. Urine osmolality was not significantly associated with water from other beverages or water from food in either sample.

Fig. 2 Mean morning water intake by water source (, drinking water; , water from other beverages; , water from food only) and level of urine osmolality in healthy children aged 9–11 years in (a) Los Angeles (n 337) and (b) New York City (n 211), spring 2009. Drinking water was defined as tap, spring, mineral or unsweetened sparkling water. Other beverages and food were defined following the Nutrition Data Systems (NDS-R) food group codes. Linear regression models were used to test for associations between urine osmolality and water intake, adjusting for age, sex, general health, body weight, height, recent skin changes, use of medications, physical activity level, sleep in the 24 h before the urine sample, and the total energy intake and potential renal solute load on the morning of the urine sample. aSignificant linear trend in drinking water (P < 0·05) after adjusting for covariates; bsignificant linear trend in total water intake (P < 0·05) after adjusting for covariates

Relative odds of elevated urine osmolality by level of total water intake

Table 2 describes the relative odds of elevated urine osmolality associated with a lower v. higher level of total water intake. Elevated urine osmolality was significantly more likely in participants who reported a lower level of total water intake in the sample from LA, but not in the sample from NYC.

Relative odds of elevated urine osmolality by source of water intake

Table 2 describes the relative odds of elevated urine osmolality associated with the source of water. In both samples, after adjusting for all of the covariates listed above, participants who reported a beverage other than drinking water were two times more likely to have elevated urine osmolality than those who reported drinking water. In both samples, participants who did not report any drinking water (i.e. participants who reported another beverage, food only or no food or beverage, combined) were significantly more likely to have elevated urine osmolality than those who reported drinking water (adjusted OR = 2·2, 95 % CI 1·3, 3·7, P = 0·003 in the LA sample and adjusted OR = 2·0, 95 % CI 1·0, 3·8, P = 0·045 in the NYC sample).

Mean urine osmolality by level of total water intake and source of water intake

Not reporting drinking water was associated with higher urine osmolality, independent of a higher level of total water intake, in both samples (see Fig. 3). Participants who reported a total water intake of more than 500 ml, but no drinking water, had a mean urine osmolality above 800 mmol/kg. Participants who reported more than 500 ml total water intake, but included drinking water, had a mean urine osmolality below 800 mmol/kg.

Fig. 3 Mean urine osmolality by level of total water intake among participants who reported no drinking water () v. those who reported drinking water (): healthy children aged 9–11 years in (a) Los Angeles (n 337) and (b) New York City (n 211), spring 2009. Lower v. higher level of total water intake was defined using a cut-off of 500 ml. Participants who reported drinking water reported any volume of plain tap, spring, mineral or unsweetened sparkling drinking water, with or without other beverages or food. aSignificantly different (P < 0·05) from the group that reported higher total water intake, including drinking water, adjusting for age, sex, general health, body weight, height, recent skin changes, use of medications, physical activity level, sleep in the 24 h before the urine sample, and the total energy intake and potential renal solute load on the morning of the urine sample. Urine osmolality values over 800 mmol/kg are considered elevated(Reference Bar-David, Urkin and Kozminsky40, Reference Philip, Chaimovitz and Singer42, Reference Bar David, Landau and Bar David46)

Discussion

The present study evaluated the urine osmolality of convenience samples of healthy children from two regions of the USA, in the mid-morning on a weekday under mild spring conditions, to explore whether hyperosmotic stress on cells might be a prevalent exposure in these samples. The study was motivated by recent studies in Israel(Reference Bar-David, Urkin and Kozminsky40, Reference Bar-David, Urkin and Landau41, Reference Bar David, Landau and Bar David46) suggesting a high prevalence of elevated urine osmolality in healthy children due to diet.

Elevated urine osmolality was prevalent in both study samples. Approximately 60 % of both samples had a urine osmolality above 800 mmol/kg. The observed prevalence was similar to that reported for healthy children of the same age in Israel, at the same time of day, but under hot summer conditions (63 % of children)(Reference Bar-David, Urkin and Kozminsky40).

The reports from Israel attribute the elevated urine osmolality to poor drinking habits and/or involuntary dehydration, although water intake was not empirically assessed in those studies(Reference Bar-David, Urkin and Kozminsky40, Reference Bar David, Landau and Bar David46). In the present study, urine osmolality was evaluated together with dietary intake and other hydration determinants. Consistent with Bar-David et al.'s hypothesis, elevated urine osmolality was significantly associated with not drinking water.

Approximately 75 % of the LA and NYC samples did not drink water before giving the urine sample. Given that a group mean provides an unbiased estimate of the ‘usual’ intake of the group, even if the group mean is calculated from only one diet record per individual(Reference Guenther, Kott and Carriquiry62), this result suggests that, on any given weekday, 75 % of these samples do not drink water before leaving the house.

The water intake data for the present study pertain to the window of time immediately prior to the urine sample, allowing temporality to be established, despite the cross-sectional design. The timing of these variables is important, because water intake and urine osmolality may be endogenously related, as cell shrinkage triggers both urine concentration and thirst.

In both the LA and NYC samples, elevated urine osmolality was specifically associated with not drinking water in the morning. More than 90 % of both samples reported water intake from food or beverages before giving the urine sample. In both samples, even when the total amount of water was above 500 ml, water from other beverages was associated with elevated, not dilute, urine osmolality.

Acknowledging that the observed associations might be confounded by error from the diet record or covariates, it is nevertheless biologically plausible that beverages other than drinking water do not dilute urine like drinking water. Beverages other than drinking water are many-fold higher in osmolality than drinking water. Drinking water has an osmolality below the threshold for ADH release (about 3 mmol/kg), whereas many other beverages and food have an osmolality above this threshold (e.g. milk: 300 mmol/kg, orange juice: 600 mmol/kg, cranberry juice: 1200 mmol/kg)(Reference Mettler, Rusch and Colombani63). The levels of Na, carbohydrate and amino acids in milk and juice can shrink cells and trigger ADH release(Reference Merson, Maughan and Shirreffs45, Reference Arieff, Guisado and Lazarowitz64, Reference Schmitt, Bresson and Beressi65). While drinking water decreases urine osmolality in controlled experiments, hyperosmotic beverages and food increase urine osmolality(Reference Seifert, Harmon and DeClercq66Reference Shirreffs, Watson and Maughan68). An inverse association between urine osmolality and total water intake would not be expected, if total water intake primarily includes water from hyperosmotic sources.

Water from hyperosmotic sources is retained in the extracellular space unless or until changes in intracellular solute concentrations create a gradient that allows water redistribution into cells(Reference Nose, Mack and Shi44). Hyperosmotic solutions have delayed absorption compared with drinking water(Reference Vist and Maughan69). Considering that water instantaneously follows osmotic gradients, a solution with a given osmotic load would not be expected to have the same effect as plain drinking water consumed separately and before an equivalent amount of solute. Timing may explain why drinking water was associated with lower odds of elevated urine osmolality in the present study, despite control for the morning osmotic load. An aggregate measure of osmotic load is not sensitive to the timing of solute intake relative to water intake.

The typical breakfast for children aged 8 to 10 years in Israel (a cup of chocolate milk with a few biscuits or a cup of milk with a handful of cornflakes(Reference Vaisman, Voet and Akivis70)) is not unlike the breakfasts reported by the LA and NYC samples. It is possible that the elevated urine osmolality in Israel reflects a habit of not drinking water.

In the Israeli sample, children who arrived at school with elevated urine osmolality at 08.00 hours still had elevated urine osmolality at noon-time(Reference Bar-David, Urkin and Kozminsky40). In the present study, it is not possible to know if the study participants eventually diluted their urine over the course of the day. Urine was collected at only one time point. Considering that access to drinking water may be limited in schools in the USA(Reference Patel, Bogart and Uyeda52) and that US children consume other beverages instead of drinking water(Reference Kant and Graubard50), it is conceivable that they did not dilute their urine. In Pittsburgh, fifty-eight out of 100 healthy children who collected 24 h urine samples (i.e. approximately 60 % of the sample) had 24 h urine Na concentrations that exceeded the plasma Na concentration(Reference Moritz71).

Even if the elevated urine osmolality eventually normalized with water intake later in the day, the results nevertheless suggest that 60 % of these samples experience hyperosmotic stress on cells during the morning hours. Although a single urine sample may not reflect the ‘usual’ day-to-day status of an individual child, the aggregate data represent the ‘usual’ prevalence of elevated urine osmolality at that time of day in the LA and NYC samples.

The consequences of a ‘usual’ delayed urine dilution or greater time spent activating urine-concentrating mechanisms merit consideration, given the many effects of hyperosmotic stress on cells. Adaptive responses to cell dehydration are known to be metabolically expensive(Reference Alfieri and Petronini72, Reference O'Neill73). In Israel, elevated urine osmolality was associated with reduced cognitive performance at school(Reference Bar-David, Urkin and Kozminsky40). If drinking water proves to be an important risk factor for cell hydration in schoolchildren, then drinking water campaigns and interventions may impact a wide array of health outcomes in children.

Limitations

The present cross-sectional study was limited by convenience sampling and self-reported measures. The results may not be generalizable to all US children. The participants may differ in unknown ways from children who did not participate. The associations in the study may be confounded by measurement error and/or incomplete control for confounding variables.

Conclusions

The present study reports a high prevalence of elevated urine osmolality in convenience samples of healthy free-living children from LA and NYC at a time of day when they might normally be at school. Elevated urine osmolality was associated with not drinking water in both samples. Further work is warranted to confirm these results and consider the public health significance of hyperosmotic stress on cells for children in the USA.

Acknowledgements

The study was funded by an unrestricted grant from Nestec, Switzerland. J.D.S. has received unrestricted grants for other research projects from the sponsor. The other authors have no financial relationships with the sponsor. J.D.S was responsible for the project and involved in every stage from study design to manuscript preparation. B.B. and A.H. collected data and contributed to the manuscript preparation. A.A. contributed expertise to the study design and manuscript preparation.

References

1.Lang, F, Busch, GL, Ritter, Met al. (1998) Functional significance of cell volume regulatory mechanisms. Physiol Rev 78, 247306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2.Andrew, RD & MacVicar, BA (1994) Imaging cell volume changes and neuronal excitation in the hippocampal slice. Neuroscience 62, 371383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Eilers, H & Schumacher, MA (2005) Mechanosensitivity of primary afferent nociceptors in the pain pathway. In Mechanosensitivity in Tissues and Cells, pp. 357–370 [A Kamkin and I Kiseleva, editors]. Moscow: Academia; available at http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK7514/Google Scholar
4.Le Bihan, D (2007) The ‘wet mind’: water and functional neuroimaging. Phys Med Biol 52, R57R90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Krause, EG, deKloet, AD, Flak, JNet al. (2011) Hydration state controls stress responsiveness and social behavior. J Neurosci 31, 54705476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Yancey, PH, Clark, ME, Hand, SCet al. (1982) Living with water stress: evolution of osmolyte systems. Science 217, 12141222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7.Haussinger, D (2008) Osmosensing and osmosignaling in the liver. Wien Med Wochenschr 158, 549552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Hernanz-Schulman, M, Vanholder, R, Waterloos, MAet al. (2000) Effect of radiographic contrast agents on leukocyte metabolic response. Pediatr Radiol 30, 361368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9.Lancaster, MG & Allison, F (1966) Studies on the pathogenesis of acute inflammation. VII. The influence of osmolality upon the phagocytic and clumping activity by human leukocytes. Am J Pathol 49, 11851200.Google ScholarPubMed
10.Berneis, K, Ninnis, R, Haussinger, Det al. (1999) Effects of hyper- and hypoosmolality on whole body protein and glucose kinetics in humans. Am J Physiol 276, E188E195.Google ScholarPubMed
11.Brown, CM, Barberini, L, Dulloo, AGet al. (2005) Cardiovascular responses to water drinking: does osmolality play a role? Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 289, R1687R1692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.Capra, NF & Ro, JY (2004) Human and animal experimental models of acute and chronic muscle pain: intramuscular algesic injection. Pain 110, 37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13.Judelson, DA, Maresh, CM, Yamamoto, LMet al. (2008) Effect of hydration state on resistance exercise-induced endocrine markers of anabolism, catabolism, and metabolism. J Appl Physiol 105, 816824.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.Lyall, V, Heck, GL, DeSimone, JAet al. (1999) Effects of osmolarity on taste receptor cell size and function. Am J Physiol 277, C800C813.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15.Pasantes-Morales, H & Tuz, K (2006) Volume changes in neurons: hyperexcitability and neuronal death. Contrib Nephrol 152, 221240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16.Schliess, F & Haussinger, D (2003) Cell volume and insulin signaling. Int Rev Cytol 225, 187228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17.McKenzie, MA, Greenleaf, JE, Looft-Wilson, Ret al. (1999) Leucocytosis, thrombocytosis, and plasma osmolality during rest and exercise: an hypothesis. J Physiol Pharmacol 50, 259273.Google ScholarPubMed
18.Cheung, SS & McLellan, TM (1998) Heat acclimation, aerobic fitness, and hydration effects on tolerance during uncompensable heat stress. J Appl Physiol 84, 17311739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19.Ainslie, PN, Campbell, IT, Frayn, KNet al. (2002) Energy balance, metabolism, hydration, and performance during strenuous hill walking: the effect of age. J Appl Physiol 93, 714723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20.Sawka, MN (1992) Physiological consequences of hypohydration: exercise performance and thermoregulation. Med Sci Sports Exerc 24, 657670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21.Silnik, M (1998) Practical management of diabetic ketoacidosis in childhood and adolescence. Paediatr Suppl 425, 6366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
22.Trachtman, H (1992) Cell volume regulation: a review of cerebral adaptive mechanisms and implications for clinical treatment of osmolal disturbances. Pediatr Nephrol 6, 104112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23.Vercellino, M, Bezante, GP & Balbi, M (2009) Contrast medium induced nephropathy: new insights into prevention and risk management. Cardiovasc Hematol Agents Med Chem 7, 166180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24.Pier, GB (2002) CFTR mutations and host susceptibility to Pseudomonas aeruginosa lung infection. Curr Opin Microbiol 5, 8186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25.Kuroda, T, Harada, T, Tsutsumi, Het al. (1997) Hypernatremic suppression of neutrophils. Burns 23, 338340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26.Platt, OS (1982) Exercise-induced hemolysis in sickle cell anemia: shear sensitivity and erythrocyte dehydration. Blood 59, 10551060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.Tripette, J, Loko, G, Samb, Aet al. (2010) Effects of hydration and dehydration on blood rheology in sickle cell trait carriers during exercise. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol 299, H908H914.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28.El-Mougi, M, Hendawi, A, Koura, Het al. (1996) Efficacy of standard glucose-based and reduced osmolarity maltodextrin-based oral rehydration solutions: effect of sugar malabsorption. Bull World Health Organ 74, 471477.Google ScholarPubMed
29.Hahn, S, Kim, Y & Garner, P (2001) Reduced osmolarity oral rehydration solution for treating dehydration due to diarrhoea in children: systematic review. BMJ 323, 8185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30.Arora, SK (2011) Hypernatremic disorders in the intensive care unit. J Intensive Care Med (Epublication ahead of print version).Google ScholarPubMed
31.Alshayeb, HM, Showkat, A, Babar, Fet al. (2011) Severe hypernatremia correction rate and mortality in hospitalized patients. Am J Med Sci 341, 356360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32.Stookey, JD, Pieper, CF & Cohen, HJ (2004) Hypertonic hyperglycemia progresses to diabetes faster than normotonic hyperglycemia. Eur J Epidemiol 19, 935944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33.Stookey, JD, Purser, JL, Pieper, CFet al. (2004) Plasma hypertonicity: another marker of frailty? J Am Geriatr Soc 52, 13131320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34.Kroke, A, Manz, F, Kersting, Met al. (2004) The DONALD Study. History, current status and future perspectives. Eur J Nutr 43, 4554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35.Star, RA (1990) Southwestern internal medicine conference: Hyperosmolar states. Am J Med Sci 300, 402412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
36.Manz, F & Wentz, A (2003) 24-h hydration status: parameters, epidemiology and recommendations. Eur J Clin Nutr 57, Suppl. 2, S10S18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37.JrEdelmann, CM, Barnett, HL, Stark, Het al. (1967) A standardized test of renal concentrating capacity in children. Am J Dis Child 114, 639644.Google Scholar
38.Manz, F, Wentz, A & Sichert-Hellert, W (2002) The most essential nutrient: defining the adequate intake of water. J Pediatr 141, 587592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39.Uttley, WS, Paxton, J & Thistlethwaite, D (1972) Urinary concentrating ability and growth failure in urinary tract disorders. Arch Dis Child 47, 436441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40.Bar-David, Y, Urkin, J & Kozminsky, E (2005) The effect of voluntary dehydration on cognitive functions of elementary school children. Acta Paediatr 94, 16671673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41.Bar-David, Y, Urkin, J, Landau, Det al. (2009) Voluntary dehydration among elementary school children residing in a hot arid environment. J Hum Nutr Diet 22, 455460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
42.Philip, M, Chaimovitz, C, Singer, Aet al. (1993) Urine osmolality in nursery school children in a hot climate. Isr J Med Sci 29, 104106.Google Scholar
43.Van Hoeck, K, Bael, A, Lax, Het al. (2007) Circadian variation of voided volume in normal school-age children. Eur J Pediatr 166, 579584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44.Nose, H, Mack, GW, Shi, XRet al. (1988) Role of osmolality and plasma volume during rehydration in humans. J Appl Physiol 65, 325331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45.Merson, SJ, Maughan, RJ & Shirreffs, SM (2008) Rehydration with drinks differing in sodium concentration and recovery from moderate exercise-induced hypohydration in man. Eur J Appl Physiol 103, 585594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46.Bar David, Y, Landau, D, Bar David, Zet al. (1998) Urine osmolality among elementary school children living in a hot climate: implications for dehydration. Ambul Child Health 4, 393397.Google Scholar
47.Bar-Or, O (2001) Nutritional considerations for the child athlete. Can J Appl Physiol 26, Suppl., S186S191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48.D'Anci, KE, Constant, F & Rosenberg, IH (2006) Hydration and cognitive function in children. Nutr Rev 64, 457464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
49.Stahl, A, Kroke, A, Bolzenius, Ket al. (2007) Relation between hydration status in children and their dietary profile – results from the DONALD study. Eur J Clin Nutr 61, 13861392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50.Kant, AK & Graubard, BI (2010) Contributors of water intake in US children and adolescents: associations with dietary and meal characteristics – National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2005–2006. Am J Clin Nutr 92, 887896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51. Chriqui JF, Schneider L, Chaloupka FJ et al. (2009) Local Wellness Policies: Assessing School District Strategies for Improving Children's Health. http://www.rwjf.org/files/research/20090728bridgingthegapfull.pdf (accessed October 2011).Google Scholar
52.Patel, AI, Bogart, LM, Uyeda, KEet al. (2010) Perceptions about availability and adequacy of drinking water in a large California school district. Prev Chronic Dis 7, A39.Google Scholar
53.Institute of Medicine (2004) Dietary Reference Intakes for Water, Potassium, Sodium, Chloride, and Sulfate. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.Google Scholar
54.Bond, L, Clements, J, Bertalli, Net al. (2006) A comparison of self-reported puberty using the Pubertal Development Scale and the Sexual Maturation Scale in a school-based epidemiologic survey. J Adolesc 29, 709720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
55.Petersen, AC, Crockett, L, Richards, Met al. (1988) A self-report measure of pubertal status: reliability, validity and initial norms. J Youth Adolesc 17, 117133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
56.Shirtcliff, EA, Dahl, RE & Pollak, SD (2009) Pubertal development: correspondence between hormonal and physical development. Child Dev 80, 327337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
57.Ikeda, Y, Tanaka, I, Oki, Yet al. (1993) Testosterone normalizes plasma vasopressin response to osmotic stimuli in men with hypogonadism. Endocr J 40, 387392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
58.Stachenfeld, NS (2008) Sex hormone effects on body fluid regulation. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 36, 152159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
59.Weston, AT, Petosa, R & Pate, RR (1997) Validation of an instrument for measurement of physical activity in youth. Med Sci Sports Exerc 29, 138143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
60.Welk, GJ, Dzewaltowski, DA & Hill, JL (2004) Comparison of the computerized ACTIVITYGRAM instrument and the previous day physical activity recall for assessing physical activity in children. Res Q Exerc Sport 75, 370380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
61.Friis-Hansen, B (1982) Water – the major nutrient. Acta Paediatr Scand Suppl 299, 1116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
62.Guenther, PM, Kott, PS & Carriquiry, AL (1997) Development of an approach for estimating usual nutrient intake distributions at the population level. J Nutr 127, 11061112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
63.Mettler, S, Rusch, C & Colombani, PC (2006) Osmolality and pH of sport and other drinks available in Switzerland. Schweizerische Zeitschrift fur Sportmedizin und Sporttraumatologie 54, 9295.Google Scholar
64.Arieff, AI, Guisado, R & Lazarowitz, VC (1977) Pathophysiology of Hyperosmolar States. Disturbances in Body Fluid Osmolality. Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society.Google Scholar
65.Schmitt, F, Bresson, JL, Beressi, Net al. (2003) Influence of plasma amino acid level on vasopressin secretion. Diab Metab 29, 352361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
66.Seifert, J, Harmon, J & DeClercq, P (2006) Protein added to a sports drink improves fluid retention. Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab 16, 420429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
67.Shirreffs, SM, Aragon-Vargas, LF, Keil, Met al. (2007) Rehydration after exercise in the heat: a comparison of 4 commonly used drinks. Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab 17, 244258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
68.Shirreffs, SM, Watson, P & Maughan, RJ (2007) Milk as an effective post-exercise rehydration drink. Br J Nutr 98, 173180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
69.Vist, GE & Maughan, RJ (1994) Gastric emptying of ingested solutions in man: effect of beverage glucose concentration. Med Sci Sports Exerc 26, 12691273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
70.Vaisman, N, Voet, H, Akivis, Aet al. (1996) Effect of breakfast timing on the cognitive functions of elementary school students. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 150, 10891092.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
71.Moritz, ML (2008) Urine sodium composition in ambulatory healthy children: hypotonic or isotonic? Pediatr Nephrol 23, 955957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
72.Alfieri, RR & Petronini, PG (2007) Hyperosmotic stress response: comparison with other cellular stresses. Pflugers Arch 454, 173185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
73.O'Neill, WC (1999) Physiological significance of volume-regulatory transporters. Am J Physiol 276, C995C1011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Characteristics of the study samples: healthy children aged 9–11 years in Los Angeles (LA) and New York City (NYC), spring 2009

Figure 1

Table 2 Relative odds of elevated urine osmolality by reported level of total water intake and source of water intake: healthy children aged 9–11 years in Los Angeles (LA) and New York City (NYC), spring 2009

Figure 2

Fig. 1 Distribution of random morning urine osmolality of healthy children aged 9–11 years in (a) Los Angeles (n 337) and (b) New York City (n 211), spring 2009

Figure 3

Fig. 2 Mean morning water intake by water source (, drinking water; , water from other beverages; , water from food only) and level of urine osmolality in healthy children aged 9–11 years in (a) Los Angeles (n 337) and (b) New York City (n 211), spring 2009. Drinking water was defined as tap, spring, mineral or unsweetened sparkling water. Other beverages and food were defined following the Nutrition Data Systems (NDS-R) food group codes. Linear regression models were used to test for associations between urine osmolality and water intake, adjusting for age, sex, general health, body weight, height, recent skin changes, use of medications, physical activity level, sleep in the 24 h before the urine sample, and the total energy intake and potential renal solute load on the morning of the urine sample. aSignificant linear trend in drinking water (P < 0·05) after adjusting for covariates; bsignificant linear trend in total water intake (P < 0·05) after adjusting for covariates

Figure 4

Fig. 3 Mean urine osmolality by level of total water intake among participants who reported no drinking water () v. those who reported drinking water (): healthy children aged 9–11 years in (a) Los Angeles (n 337) and (b) New York City (n 211), spring 2009. Lower v. higher level of total water intake was defined using a cut-off of 500 ml. Participants who reported drinking water reported any volume of plain tap, spring, mineral or unsweetened sparkling drinking water, with or without other beverages or food. aSignificantly different (P < 0·05) from the group that reported higher total water intake, including drinking water, adjusting for age, sex, general health, body weight, height, recent skin changes, use of medications, physical activity level, sleep in the 24 h before the urine sample, and the total energy intake and potential renal solute load on the morning of the urine sample. Urine osmolality values over 800 mmol/kg are considered elevated(40,42,46)