Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T08:03:49.050Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cross-cultural differences in the epidemiology of unexplained fatigue syndromes in primary care

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Petros Skapinakis*
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry, University of Bristol, UK and University of Ioannina School of Medicine, Greece
Glyn Lewis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Bristol, UK
Venetsanos Mavreas
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Ioannina School of Medicine, Greece
*
Petros Skapinakis, Department of Psychiatry, University of Bristol, Cotham House, Cotham Hill, Bristol BS6 6JL, UK. e-mail: p.skapinakis@bristol.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Unexplained fatigue has been extensively studied but most of the samples used were from Western countries.

Aims

To present international data on the prevalence of unexplained fatigue and fatigue as a presenting complaint in primary care.

Method

Secondary analysis of the World Health Organization study of psychological problems in general health care. A total of 5438 primary care attenders from 14 countries were assessed with the Composite International Diagnostic Interview.

Results

The prevalence of unexplained fatigue of 1-month duration differed across centres, with a range between 2.26 (95% CI 1.17–4.33) and 15.05 (95% CI 10.85–20.49). Subjects from more-developed countries were more likely to report unexplained fatigue but less likely to present with fatigue to physicians compared with subjects from less developed countries.

Conclusions

In less-developed countries fatigue might be an indicator of unmet psychiatric need, but in more-developed countries it is probably a symbol of psychosocial distress.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2003 

Various studies have shown that unexplained fatigue syndromes are common conditions in both the community (Reference Walker, Katon and JemelkaWalker et al, 1993; Reference Steele, Dobbins and FukudaSteele et al, 1998; Reference Skapinakis, Lewis and MeltzerSkapinakis et al, 2000) and primary care (Reference Bates, Schmitt and BuchwaldBates et al, 1993; Reference McDonald, David and PelosiMcDonald et al, 1993; Reference Hickie, Hooker and Hadzi-PavlovicHickie et al, 1996; Reference Wessely, Chalder and HirschWessely et al, 1997). Fatigue is also common as a presenting complaint to primary care physicians (Reference Cathebras, Robbins and KirmayerCathebras et al, 1992; Reference Fuhrer and WesselyFuhrer & Wessely, 1995). The majority of the studies, however, were carried out in well-developed countries. Data from less-developed countries are relatively sparse. The present paper aims to report the cross-cultural differences in the prevalence of fatigue syndromes, using data from the World Health Organization (WHO) international study of psychological problems in general health care carried out in 14 countries (Reference Sartorius, Ustun and Costa e SilvaSartorius et al, 1993). We also examine the hypothesis that individuals from well-developed countries will be more likely to report fatigue compared with subjects from less-developed countries.

METHODS

General description of the data-set

The WHO collaborative study of psychological problems in general health care was an international prospective study carried out in 15 centres from 14 countries that examined the prevalence, 1-year outcome and public health implications of common mental disorders in primary care (Reference Sartorius, Ustun and Costa e SilvaSartorius et al, 1993). Details on the methods of the study are given elsewhere (Reference Von Korff, Üstün, Üstün and SartoriusVon Korff & Üstün, 1995). Briefly, the study used a two-phase design in which 26 969 primary care attenders aged between 18 and 64 years were approached in each participating centre and asked to complete the 12-item general health questionnaire (GHQ—12; Reference Goldberg and WilliamsGoldberg & Williams, 1988). A total of 25 916 subjects agreed to participate (96% response rate, range across centres from 91% to 100%). Patients were selected for the second phase assessment using a stratified random sampling procedure according to site-specific GHQ—12 thresholds (100% of the subjects scoring above the 80th percentile, 35% of those scoring between the 60th and 80th percentile and 10% of those scoring below the 60th percentile). The second phase assessment included the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI; Reference Wittchen, Robins and CottlerWittchen et al, 1991) modified for use in primary care and involved 5438 out of 8698 eligible subjects (62% response rate, range from 43% to 99%). Data collection took place between May 1991 and April 1992.

In a previous paper we reported the overall prevalence of unexplained fatigue syndromes and their association with psychiatric disorders (Reference Skapinakis, Lewis and MavreasSkapinakis et al, 2003), whereas in this paper we focus on the differences between centres.

Measures

The CIDI is a fully structured interview developed for use in cross-cultural psychiatric epidemiology studies (Reference Wittchen, Robins and CottlerWittchen et al, 1991). The modified version used in the present study includes only the sections that assess mental symptoms common in primary care, notably the sections on somatisation, anxiety, depression and hypochondriasis, as well as a new section on neurasthenia. The primary care version rates both current (1-month) and life-time symptomatology. Non-English-speaking participating centres translated and back-translated the interview. Training and procedures for assuring reliability are described elsewhere (Reference Von Korff, Üstün, Üstün and SartoriusVon Korff & Üstün, 1995). The interviewer—observer reliability coefficient for the primary care version of CIDI was found to be 0.92 overall, ranging between 0.81 and 1.00 for individual sections.

Measurement of fatigue: substantial unexplained fatigue

Fatigue was assessed using the neurasthenia section of the primary care version of the CIDI. Three screening questions were put to all subjects: Q1 ‘In the past month have you felt tired all the time?’; Q2 ‘Do you get easily tired while performing everyday tasks?’; and Q3 ‘Does even minimal physical effort cause exhaustion?’ Then the interviewer asked a specific sequence of questions to determine the clinical importance and possible cause of the symptom. Fatigue was considered ‘medically explained’ if a doctor had given the patient a definite diagnosis or if there had been any abnormalities reported on examination or further investigation. Subjects with medically unexplained fatigue were also asked a fourth question that assessed the severity of fatigue: Q4 ‘Is it difficult to recover from these periods of fatigue or exhaustion when you rest?’

Subjects with medically unexplained fatigue (at least one positive answer to questions Q1-Q3) who answered positively to question Q4 were classified as cases of substantial unexplained fatigue. In comparison with the ICD—10 definition of neurasthenia (World Health Organization, 1998), our definition of unexplained fatigue differs in that: it does not include the multiple somatic symptoms criterion (Criterion B in ICD—10); it refers to 1-month duration instead of 3 (Criterion D in ICD—10); and, it does not exclude other comorbid psychiatric disorders (Criterion E in ICD—10). Therefore, it is a much broader definition compared with ICD—10 neurasthenia.

Fatigue as the main reason for consultation

The above definition of fatigue is independent of whether the subjects complained of fatigue to their primary care physician. For comparison, we also report the prevalence of fatigue as a presenting complaint. Subjects were asked to report the three main reasons for their consultation, choosing from a list of symptoms. Those who reported weakness or lethargy (the only items in the list related to fatigue) as one of their main reasons for consultation were considered as having fatigue as a presenting complaint.

Measurement of morbidity

Psychiatric morbidity was assessed with the CIDI. Diagnostic algorithms were developed to give diagnoses according to the ICD—10 criteria. For the purposes of the present study, subjects were classified as cases of psychiatric morbidity if they had any of the following current ICD—10 disorders: depressive disorders (including dysthymia); generalised anxiety disorder; agoraphobia; panic disorder; somatisation disorder; and hypochondriasis.

Chronic physical morbidity was assessed by asking patients whether they were suffering from a list of common chronic medical conditions. Patients were classified as cases of chronic physical morbidity if they were suffering from at least one chronic physical disorder.

Classification of primary care centres

Centres were entered into the analysis as dummy variables. However, for the economic development hypothesis, centres were classified into three categories according to the gross national income (GNI) per head in 2000 as follows: high-income countries, with more than US$10 000 GNI per head (Athens, Berlin, Groningen, Mainz, Manchester, Nagasaki, Paris, Seattle, Verona); middle-income countries, with less than US$10 000 but more than US$1000 (Ankara, Rio de Janeiro, Santiago); and low-income countries, with less than US$1000 (Bangalore, Ibadan, Shanghai). Data for the GNI per head were derived from the World Bank databases available on the internet (World Bank, 2000).

Analysis

All data analyses were conducted using Stata version 6.0 (Stata Corporation, 1999). The weighted prevalence with 95% confidence intervals of fatigue syndromes was estimated using the SVYPROP command. This command allows for sampling weights and is suitable for the analysis of the two-phase design of the study (Reference Dunn, Pickles and TansellaDunn et al, 1999). The association of GNI per head with fatigue was analysed by means of a series of logistic regression models (separately for each fatigue syndrome) using the SVYLOGIT command in Stata. We used fatigue case status (Yes—No) as the dependent variable, and the classification of centres according to GNI as the independent variable, adjusting for socio-demographic variables, psychiatric morbidity and physical morbidity. Odds ratios (with 95% CIs) of fatigue were calculated for each category of the GNI variable. Psychiatric and physical morbidity were entered into the models as binary variables. In all the analyses we used sampling weights.

RESULTS

Description of the sample

The sample at the baseline assessment (n=5438) was predominantly female (62%), 58% of the participants were older than 35 years, 62.1% were married, 57.4% had basic education and 58.7% were employed. As expected, socio-demographic characteristics differed significantly between centres, reflecting the diverse cultures that are represented in this data-set.

Prevalence of fatigue syndromes

Table 1 shows the prevalence of substantial unexplained fatigue and fatigue as a presenting complaint across centres.

Table 1 Prevalence of substantial unexplained fatigue and fatigue as a presenting complaint in international primary care (n=5438)

Unweighted number of cases/Total number interviewed Weighted prevalence % (95% CI)
Centre1 (Response rate)2 Substantial unexplained fatigue3 Fatigue as a presenting complaint4
Manchester (71%) 115/428 15.05 (10.85-20.49) 2/428 0.19 (0-0.76)
Santiago (46%) 61/274 12.86 (8.41-19.18) 3/274 2.17 (0.47-9.52)
Berlin (43%) 70/400 12.53 (8.77-17.6) 19/400 3.13 (1.81-5.36)
Groningen (69%) 78/340 11.75 (8.48-16.05) 26/340 6.90 (3.71-12.50)
Paris (62%) 69/405 10.89 (7.74-15.11) 59/405 10.35 (7.35-14.38)
Mainz (37%) 56/400 9.91 (7.27-13.38) 4/400 1.01 (0.31-3.22)
Nagasaki (74%) 37/336 8.05 (5.08-12.54) 62/336 10.87 (7.74-15.06)
Athens (43%) 22/196 8.00 (4.29-14.45) 4/196 3.79 (1.14-11.85)
Ankara (97%) 47/400 7.99 (5.11-12.28) 46/400 10.64 (6.94-15.97)
Rio de Janeiro (46%) 50/393 6.71 (4.30-10.33) 14/393 3.83 (1.69-8.45)
Bangalore (83%) 30/398 3.83 (2.46-5.91) 95/398 17.12 (13.18-21.94)
Shanghai (99%) 44/576 3.63 (2.34-5.59) 47/576 5.98 (3.83-9.20)
Seattle (61%) 26/373 3.37 (1.90-5.90) 3/373 0.25 (0-0.78)
Verona (55%) 18/250 2.34 (1.41-3.86) 7/250 1.84 (0.54-6.06)
Ibadan (88%) 11/269 2.26 (1.17-4.33) 47/269 16.09 (11.03-22.88)
All centres (63%) 734/5438 7.99 (7.13-8.85) 438/5438 6.27 (5.47-7.18)
χ2=6.64, d.f.=14, P < 0.001 χ2=10.51, d.f.=14, P < 0.001

Prevalence of substantial unexplained fatigue differed fifteen-fold across primary care centres (P<0.001). Centres with high prevalence of substantial unexplained fatigue (>10%) were Manchester, Santiago, Berlin, Groningen, Paris and Mainz, and centres with low prevalence (<4%) were Bangalore, Shanghai, Seattle, Verona and Ibadan.

Fatigue as a presenting complaint (either weakness or lethargy) was reported by 6.27% of the subjects (95% CI 5.47-7.18) with wide variation across centres. Centres with high prevalence of fatigue as a presenting complaint (>10%) were Bangalore, Ibadan, Nagasaki, Ankara and Paris, whereas centres with low prevalence (<2%) were Manchester, Seattle, Mainz and Verona.

In the whole data-set, very few subjects with substantial unexplained fatigue presented with fatigue (11%; 95% CI 8.21-14.57).

Association with level of economic development

In the logistic regression analysis, unexplained fatigue was associated positively with GNI per head after adjustment for all other socio-demographic variables, psychiatric morbidity and chronic physical morbidity. High-income countries had an odds ratio of 2.62 (95% CI 1.67-4.11) compared with low-income countries (Table 2).

Table 2 Association of gross national income (GNI) per head and substantial unexplained fatigue or fatigue as a presenting complaint in international primary care

Unweighted number of cases/Total number interviewed Odds ratios (95% CI)1
GNI per head2 Substantial unexplained fatigue3 Fatigue as presenting complaint4
Low income 85/1243 189/1243
1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
Middle income 158/1067 63/1067
1.67 (1.04-2.66) 0.39 (0.23-0.68)
High income 491/3128 186/3128
2.62 (1.67-4.11) 0.38 (0.25-0.58)

In contrast, fatigue as a presenting complaint was associated negatively with GNI per head, and subjects from higher-income countries had an odds ratio of 0.38 (95% CI 0.25-0.58) compared with subjects from lower income countries.

DISCUSSION

In this multi-national study in primary care, we found that unexplained fatigue was present in all cultures but the prevalence varied widely. Subjects from higher-income countries were more likely to report fatigue in response to direct questions compared with subjects from lower-income countries. In contrast, subjects in higher-income countries were less likely to complain to their doctors of fatigue than those in poorer countries.

Limitations and strengths

These results should be interpreted in the context of the following limitations. First, this is a study carried out in primary care and therefore the results cannot be generalised to the general population. Second, response rates for the second phase baseline assessment were below 50% for five centres. Therefore, a systematic bias in either direction cannot be ruled out, even though participation was not related significantly to age, gender or screening GHQ—12 score. Third, although CIDI has been developed for use in cross-cultural epidemiological research, this does not prove its cultural validity. Fourth, medical causes of fatigue were excluded in a crude way by asking patients if a doctor had given them a definite diagnosis for their symptom or if there had been any abnormalities reported on examination or further investigation.

Despite these limitations our study had the advantage of investigating fatigue in a large multicultural sample using the same methodology. We are not aware of any other studies that used such a culturally diverse sample. The present research was therefore able to study fatigue independently of the confounding effect of the sociocultural context.

Prevalence of fatigue syndromes across centres

The prevalence of substantial unexplained fatigue differed significantly across centres, with an average prevalence of 7.99% (95% CI 7.13-8.85) in primary care but with a range between 2.26 and 15.05 in different countries.

The prevalence estimates reported from studies carried out in Western countries are generally consistent with the estimates provided here for similar countries. For example, Buchwald et al (Reference Buchwald, Sullivan and Komaroff1987) found a prevalence of unexplained chronic fatigue of 21% in an American primary care centre. In Australia, 24% of the primary care attenders reported substantial unexplained fatigue (Reference Hickie, Hooker and Hadzi-PavlovicHickie et al, 1996), whereas in Canada 14% reported this (Reference Cathebras, Robbins and KirmayerCathebras et al, 1992). In UK primary care the prevalence of unexplained fatigue has been found to be approximately 10% (Reference David, Pelosi and McDonaldDavid et al, 1990; Reference Wessely, Chalder and HirschWessely et al, 1997). By contrast, studies from the more-developed countries that used more culturally heterogeneous samples reported lower prevalence rates compared with the rates mentioned previously, for example 2% in a multiracial sample in San Francisco (Reference Steele, Dobbins and FukudaSteele et al, 1998) and 6.4% in a study of Chinese—Americans in Los Angeles (Reference Zheng, Lin and TakeuchiZheng et al, 1997). In the present study we also observed lower rates of fatigue in Asian countries.

Differences in the prevalence

In order to explain the reported differences in the prevalence of unexplained fatigue we examined whether economic factors at the aggregate level influence rates of fatigue. The classification of centres according to the GNI per head for the country of location showed that subjects coming from middle- or high-income countries were more likely to report substantial unexplained fatigue compared with subjects from low-income countries. Therefore, there is an indication that economic development might influence the reporting of unexplained fatigue. It is worth noting that this pattern was not found when similar analyses were carried out for other unexplained somatic symptoms such as pain, headache, dizziness, excessive flatulence and palpitations (data on file). These symptoms, unlike fatigue, tended to be less prevalent in higher-income countries. Therefore, this finding cannot be attributed to a general tendency for unexplained functional symptoms to be reported more commonly in well-developed countries.

This is a difficult finding to interpret because economic development might be associated with many other (confounding) variables such as organisation of primary health care or local diagnostic preferences. To find out whether a similar pattern is observed for primary care patients, who presented to primary care physicians with complaints of weakness or lethargy (‘presenting fatigue’), we carried out a similar analysis with the presenting complaint of fatigue as the dependent variable. This showed a very different picture; subjects from high- or middle-income countries were less likely to present with fatigue compared with those from low-income countries.

Past research in developed countries, in both the community and primary care, has shown that subjects with fatigue usually attribute their symptoms to psychosocial causes. For example, in a community study in the UK almost half of the subjects attributed fatigue to psychosocial causes such as work, family and lifestyle (Reference Pawlikowska, Chalder and HirschPawlikowska et al, 1994) and similar findings have been reported in primary care (Reference David, Pelosi and McDonaldDavid et al, 1990). If psychosocial explanations are prevalent then it seems reasonable that fatigue will not be a presenting complaint in primary care in developed countries. In that case, fatigue is more of a social ‘metaphor’ rather than a legitimate or useful medical complaint (Reference Lee and WongLee & Wong, 1995). By contrast, in less-developed countries a somatic presentation might ensure an appropriate medical examination. This reminds us of the process of ‘facultative somatisation’ described by Goldberg & Bridges (Reference Goldberg and Bridges1988) where patients present with somatic symptoms as a ‘ticket of admission’ to the primary care clinic. Simon et al (Reference Simon, Von Korff and Piccinelli1999) have reported a similar finding in their study of the relation between somatic symptoms and depression. Analysing the same dataset used in the present study, they found that a somatic presentation of depression was more common at centres where patients lacked an ongoing relationship with a primary care physician. All the centres from low-income countries and most of the centres from middle-income countries were of this type.

Given the strong association of psychiatric disorders with fatigue (Reference Skapinakis, Lewis and MeltzerSkapinakis et al, 2000), we think that this finding might have important clinical implications. In less-developed countries, the complaint of fatigue might be an indicator of hidden psychiatric morbidity. By contrast, in more-developed countries, although syndromes of fatigue are common, they should not be always considered as evidence of unmet need as they might represent a common expression of psychosocial distress.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. Prevalence of unexplained fatigue in primary care differs widely across countries.

  2. Patients from well-developed countries are more likely to report fatigue in response to direct questions but are less likely to present with fatigue to physicians compared with patients from less-developed countries.

  3. Presenting fatigue in less-developed countries might be an important indicator of hidden psychiatric need.

LIMITATIONS

  1. The study was carried out in primary care and results cannot be generalised to the general population.

  2. Participation rates for the baseline assessment were relatively low for some centres.

  3. Medical causes of fatigue were excluded in a crude way by asking patients if a doctor had given them a definite diagnosis for their symptom or if there had been any abnormalities reported on examination or further investigation.

Acknowledgements

The data reported in this article were collected as part of a World Health Organization's Psychological Problems in General Health Care project. Participating investigators include: O. Ozturk and M. Rezaki (Ankara, Turkey); C. Stefanis and V. Mavreas (Athens, Greece); S. M. Channabasavana and T. G. Sriram (Bangalore, India); H. Helmchen and M. Linden (Berlin, Germany); W. van der Brink and B. Tiemens (Groningen, The Netherlands); M. Olatawura and O. Gureye (Ibadan, Nigeria); O. Benkert and W. Maier (Mainz, Germany); R. Gater and S. Kisely (Manchester, UK); Y. Nakane and S. Michitsuji (Nagasaki, Japan); Y. Lecrubier and P. Boyer (Paris, France); J. Costa e Silva and L. Villano (Rio de Janeiro, Brazil); R. Florenzano and J. Acuna (Santiago, Chile); G. E. Simon and M. von Korff (Seattle, USA); Y. He-Quin and X. Shi Fu (Shanghai, China); and M. Tansella and C. Bellantuono (Verona, Italy). The study advisory group includes J. Costa e Silva, D. P. Goldberg, Y. Lecrubier, M. von Korff and H.-U. Wittchen. Coordinating staff at World Health Organization headquarters include N. Sartorius and T. B. Ustun. This paper was started while P.S. was studying for a PhD degree at the University of Wales College of Medicine. P.S. was funded throughout his studies by the Alexander S. Onassis Public Benefit Foundation.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

Bates, D. W., Schmitt, W., Buchwald, D., et al (1993) Prevalence of fatigue and chronic fatigue syndrome in a primary care practice. Archives of Internal Medicine, 153, 27592765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchwald, D., Sullivan, J. L. & Komaroff, A. L. (1987) Frequency of ‘chronic active Epstein-Barr virus infection’ in a general medical practice. Journal of the American Medical Association, 257, 23032307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cathebras, P. J., Robbins, J. M., Kirmayer, L. J., et al (1992) Fatigue in primary care: prevalence, psychiatric comorbidity illness behavior, and outcome. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 7, 276286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
David, A., Pelosi, A., McDonald, E., et al (1990) Tired, weak, or in need of rest: fatigue among general practice attenders. BMJ, 301, 11991202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, G., Pickles, A., Tansella, M., et al (1999) Two-phase epidemiological surveys in psychiatric research. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 95100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuhrer, R. & Wessely, S. (1995) The epidemiology of fatigue and depression: a French primary-care study. Psychological Medicine, 25, 895905.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, D. & Bridges, K. (1988) Somatic presentations of psychiatric illness in primary care setting. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 32, 137144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, D. & Williams, P. (1988) A User's Guide to the General Health Questionnaire. Windsor: NFER–Nelso.Google Scholar
Hickie, I. B., Hooker, A. W., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., et al (1996) Fatigue in selected primary care settings: sociodemographic and psychiatric correlates. Medical Journal of Australia, 164, 585588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, S. & Wong, K. C. (1995) Rethinking neurasthenia: the illness concepts of shenjing shuairuo among Chinese undergraduates in Hong Kong. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 19, 91111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, E., David, A. S., Pelosi, A. J., et al (1993) Chronic fatigue in primary care attenders. Psychological Medicine, 23, 987998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pawlikowska, T., Chalder, T., Hirsch, S. R., et al (1994) Population based study of fatigue and psychological distress. BMJ, 308, 763766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sartorius, N., Ustun, T. B., Costa e Silva, J. A., et al (1993) An international study of psychological problems in primary care. Preliminary report from the World Health Organization Collaborative Project on ‘Psychological Problems in General Health Care’. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 819824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, G. E., Von Korff, M., Piccinelli, M., et al (1999) An international study of the relation between somatic symptoms and depression. New England Journal of Medicine, 341, 13291335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skapinakis, P., Lewis, G. & Meltzer, H. (2000) Clarifying the relationship between unexplained chronic fatigue and psychiatric morbidity: results from a community survey in Great Britain. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 14921498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skapinakis, P., Lewis, G. & Mavreas, V. (2003) Unexplained fatigue syndromes in a multinational primary care study. American Journal of Psychiatry, in press.Google Scholar
Stata Corporation (1999) Stata Reference Manual, release 6.0. College Station, TX: Stata Press.Google Scholar
Steele, L., Dobbins, J. G., Fukuda, K., et al (1998) The epidemiology of chronic fatigue in San Francisco. American Journal of Medicine, 105, 83S90S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Von Korff, M. & Üstün, T. B. (1995) Methods of the WHO Collaborative study on ‘Psychological Problems in General Health Care’. In Mental Illness in General Health Care. An International Study (eds Üstün, T. B. & Sartorius, N.). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Walker, E. A., Katon, W. J. & Jemelka, R. P. (1993) Psychiatric disorders and medical care utilization among people in the general population who report fatigue. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 8, 436440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wessely, S., Chalder, T., Hirsch, S., et al (1997) The prevalence and morbidity of chronic fatigue and chronic fatigue syndrome: a prospective primary care study. American Journal of Public Health, 87, 14491455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittchen, H., Robins, L. N., Cottler, L. B., et al (1991) Cross-cultural feasibility, reliability and sources of variance of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). The multicentre WHO/ADAMHA Field Trials. British Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 645653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Bank (2000) World Development Indicators 2000. http://www.worldbank.org.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1998) The ICD–10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Zheng, Y. P., Lin, K. M., Takeuchi, D., et al (1997) An epidemiological study of neurasthenia in Chinese-Americans in Los Angeles. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 38, 249259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Prevalence of substantial unexplained fatigue and fatigue as a presenting complaint in international primary care (n=5438)

Figure 1

Table 2 Association of gross national income (GNI) per head and substantial unexplained fatigue or fatigue as a presenting complaint in international primary care

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.