Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T22:09:31.816Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Deficits in executive functions among youths with autism spectrum disorders: an age-stratified analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 March 2016

S.-F. Chen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, National Taiwan University Hospital & College of Medicine, Taipei, Taiwan Department of Psychiatry, Taipei Tzu Chi General Hospital, Buddhist Tzu Chi Medical Foundation, Taipei, Taiwan School of Occupational Therapy, College of Medicine, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
Y.-L. Chien
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, National Taiwan University Hospital & College of Medicine, Taipei, Taiwan Graduate Institute of Clinical Medicine, College of Medicine, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
C.-T. Wu
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, National Taiwan University Hospital & College of Medicine, Taipei, Taiwan School of Occupational Therapy, College of Medicine, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
C.-Y. Shang
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, National Taiwan University Hospital & College of Medicine, Taipei, Taiwan
Y.-Y. Wu
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital-Linkou, Taoyuan, Taiwan
S. S. Gau*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, National Taiwan University Hospital & College of Medicine, Taipei, Taiwan School of Occupational Therapy, College of Medicine, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan Graduate Institute of Clinical Medicine, College of Medicine, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan Department of Psychology, Graduate Institute of Brain and Mind Sciences, Graduate Institute of Epidemiology and Preventive Medicine, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
*
*Address for correspondence: S. S.-F. Gau, MD, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, National Taiwan University Hospital and College of Medicine, No.7, Chung-Shan South Road, Taipei 10002, Taiwan. (Email: gaushufe@ntu.edu.tw)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Impaired executive function (EF) is suggested to be one of the core features in individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD); however, little is known about whether the extent of worse EF in ASD than typically developing (TD) controls is age-dependent. We used age-stratified analysis to reveal this issue.

Method

We assessed 111 youths with ASD (aged 12.5 ± 2.8 years, male 94.6%) and 114 age-, and sex-matched TD controls with Digit Span and four EF tasks of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB): Spatial Span (SSP), Spatial Working Memory (SWM), Stockings of Cambridge (SOC), and Intradimensional/Extradimensional Shift Test (I/ED).

Results

Compared to TD controls, youths with ASD performed poorer on the Digit Span, SWM, SOC, and I/ED tasks. The performance of all the tasks improved with age for both groups. Age-stratified analyses were conducted due to significant age × group interactions in visuospatial planning (SOC) and set-shifting (I/ED) and showed that poorer performance on these two tasks in ASD than TD controls was found only in the child (aged 8–12 years) rather than the adolescent (aged 13–18 years) group. By contrast, youths with ASD had impaired working memory, regardless of age. The increased magnitude of group difference in visuospatial planning (SOC) with increased task demands differed between the two age groups but no age moderating effect on spatial working memory.

Conclusions

Our findings support deficits in visuospatial working memory and planning in youths with ASD; however, worse performance in set-shifting may only be demonstrated in children with ASD.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a complex neurodevelopmental disorder with long-lasting neurocognitive dysfunctions in addition to impaired socio-communication and restricted, repetitive and stereotypical patterns of behavior, interests, and activities (APA, 2013). The prevalence of ASD has increased in recent decades causing much health burden (Matson & Kozlowski, Reference Matson and Kozlowski2011; Baxter et al. Reference Baxter, Brugha, Erskine, Scheurer, Vos and Scott2015). In Western society, the prevalence of ASD rose from 0.30% (Martin et al. Reference Martin, Volkmar and Lewis2007) to 2.00% (Blumberg et al. Reference Blumberg, Bramlett, Kogan, Schieve, Jones and Lu2013). The increased prevalence and incidence of ASD was also evidenced in Taiwan where the number of identified ASD individuals aged 3–17 years increased from 3995 to 8072 from 2004 to 2010, and the prevalence of all age groups increased over years (Lai et al. Reference Lai, Tseng, Hou and Guo2012, Reference Lai, Tseng and Guo2013; Lin, Reference Lin2015). These convergent evidences (Nakahachi et al. Reference Nakahachi, Iwase, Takahashi, Honaga, Sekiyama, Ukai, Ishii, Osterling and Dawson1994; Gabig, Reference Gabig2008; Robinson et al. Reference Robinson, Goddard, Dritschel, Wisley and Howlin2009) strongly suggest that ASD as a common disorder warrants much more studies to investigate the inherent deficits of ASD beyond the core symptoms.

Among the three main cognitive theories of ASD [i.e. theory of mind (Baron-Cohen et al. Reference Baron-Cohen, Tager-Flusberg and Cohen2000), weak central coherence (Baron-Cohen, Reference Baron-Cohen2002), and executive dysfunction (Hill, Reference Hill2004)], Hill's theory of executive dysfunction explains deficits in initiating new non-routine actions and links the behavioral repetition and perseveration to frontal lobe dysfunction in ASD. Such a theory is supported by several early studies (Damasio & Maurer, Reference Damasio and Maurer1978; Osterling & Dawson, Reference Osterling and Dawson1994; Dawson et al. Reference Dawson, Meltzoff, Osterling and Rinaldi1998; Salmond et al. Reference Salmond, De Haan, Friston, Gadian and Vargha-Khadem2003); for example, the lack of joint attention in young children with ASD is related to dysfunction of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Osterling & Dawson, Reference Osterling and Dawson1994). Damasio & Maurer (Reference Damasio and Maurer1978) were the first to report that individuals with ASD, similar to patients with frontal lobe damage, had difficulties in switching, planning and presenting adaptable social performance. Such early study has been supported by several subsequent studies (Minshew et al. Reference Minshew, Muenz, Goldstein and Payton1992; Goldstein et al. Reference Goldstein, Johnson and Minshew2001; Minshew & Goldstein, Reference Minshew and Goldstein2001; Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004; Yerys et al. Reference Yerys, Wallace, Harrison, Celano, Giedd and Kenworthy2009). The most consistent findings are deficits in working memory (Barnard et al. Reference Barnard, Muldoon, Hasan, O'Brien and Stewart2008), planning (Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004), and set-shifting (Minshew et al. Reference Minshew, Muenz, Goldstein and Payton1992; Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004) in ASD compared to typically developing (TD) individuals (Minshew et al. Reference Minshew, Muenz, Goldstein and Payton1992; Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004) or individuals with other neurodevelopmental disorders (Barnard et al. Reference Barnard, Muldoon, Hasan, O'Brien and Stewart2008).

With regards to working memory, individuals with ASD may have impaired spatial working memory (Williams et al. Reference Williams, Goldstein, Carpenter and Minshew2005; Russo et al. Reference Russo, Flanagan, Iarocci, Berringer, Zelazo and Burack2007; Steele et al. Reference Steele, Minshew, Luna and Sweeney2007), yet intact verbal working memory (Williams et al. Reference Williams, Goldstein, Carpenter and Minshew2005; Cui et al. Reference Cui, Gao, Chen, Zou and Wang2010). The impaired spatial working memory in ASD is consistently evident across child (Goldberg et al. Reference Goldberg, Mostofsky, Cutting, Mahone, Astor, Denckla and Landa2005; Williams et al. Reference Williams, Goldstein, Carpenter and Minshew2005; Corbett et al. Reference Corbett, Constantine, Hendren, Rocke and Ozonoff2009; Cui et al. Reference Cui, Gao, Chen, Zou and Wang2010), adolescent (Steele et al. Reference Steele, Minshew, Luna and Sweeney2007) and adult (Williams et al. Reference Williams, Goldstein, Carpenter and Minshew2005) populations. Among the neuropsychological tests, the Spatial Span (SSP) and the Spatial Working Memory (SWM) of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB; Goldberg et al. Reference Goldberg, Mostofsky, Cutting, Mahone, Astor, Denckla and Landa2005; Corbett et al. Reference Corbett, Constantine, Hendren, Rocke and Ozonoff2009) have been used to assess working memory in children with ASD. Visuospatial planning is also impaired in children and adolescents (Hughes et al. Reference Hughes, Russell and Robbins1994; Hughes, Reference Hughes1996; Ozonoff & Jensen, Reference Ozonoff and Jensen1999; Geurts et al. Reference Geurts, Verté, Oosterlaan, Roeyers and Sergeant2004; Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004, Reference Ozonoff, Pennington and Rogers2006; Landa & Goldberg, Reference Landa and Goldberg2005) as well as adults (Barnard et al. Reference Barnard, Muldoon, Hasan, O'Brien and Stewart2008) with ASD. For example, Ozonoff et al. (Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004) found youths with ASD took more thinking time and moves to complete trials on the Stockings of Cambridge task (SOC) than TD youths. Set-shifting reflecting cognitive flexibility is among the most consistent neuropsychological deficits in ASD, which is commonly assessed by the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Ozonoff & Jensen, Reference Ozonoff and Jensen1999; Tsuchiya et al. Reference Tsuchiya, Oki, Yahara and Fujieda2005; Barnard et al. Reference Barnard, Muldoon, Hasan, O'Brien and Stewart2008; Kaland et al. Reference Kaland, Smith and Mortensen2008; Sumiyoshi et al. Reference Sumiyoshi, Kawakubo, Suga, Sumiyoshi and Kasai2011) and the Intradimensional/Extradimensional Shift Test (I/ED) of the CANTAB (Hughes et al. Reference Hughes, Russell and Robbins1994; Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004; Corbett et al. Reference Corbett, Constantine, Hendren, Rocke and Ozonoff2009; Yerys et al. Reference Yerys, Wallace, Harrison, Celano, Giedd and Kenworthy2009).

The chronological change of executive function (EF) is of particular interest. Most studies have been conducted in samples with limited age range, except for a few studies including both children and adults with ASD (Williams et al. Reference Williams, Goldstein, Carpenter and Minshew2005; Happé et al. Reference Happé, Booth, Charlton and Hughes2006; Rosenthal et al. Reference Rosenthal, Wallace, Lawson, Wills, Dixon, Yerys and Kenworthy2013; Pugliese et al. Reference Pugliese, Anthony, Strang, Dudley, Wallace and Kenworthy2014; Van den Bergh et al. Reference Van den Bergh, Scheeren, Begeer, Koot and Geurts2014). Rosenthal and colleagues (2013) recruited 185 participants with ASD and divided them into four age groups (5–7, 8–10, 11–13, 14–18 years). Their results showed that older children with ASD had greater problems of initiating, working memory and organization than younger participants with ASD. Another study stratified ASD participants into four age groups (6–8, 9–11, 12–14, 15–18 years) and found that the impairments of inhibition and cognitive flexibility were most significant in childhood, while the planning deficit was apparent in early adolescence (12–14 years age group) (Van den Bergh et al. Reference Van den Bergh, Scheeren, Begeer, Koot and Geurts2014). The findings of the study stratifying ASD participants into six age groups (4–5, 6–7, 8–9, 10–11, 12–13, 14–23 years) showed the EFs examined by the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Parent Form was age-related and affected the performance of adaptive skills in children and youths with ASD (Pugliese et al. Reference Pugliese, Anthony, Strang, Dudley, Wallace and Kenworthy2014). Happé et al. (Reference Happé, Booth, Charlton and Hughes2006) reported that the older subjects with ASD performed as well as normal controls on the tasks of response selection/inhibition, flexibility, and planning/working memory, suggesting an age-dependent improvement of EFs in ASD. However, most studies assessing planning ability in youths aged 9–15 showed worse performance in ASD than normal controls (Hughes et al. Reference Hughes, Russell and Robbins1994; Hughes, Reference Hughes1996; Ozonoff & Jensen, Reference Ozonoff and Jensen1999; Geurts et al. Reference Geurts, Verté, Oosterlaan, Roeyers and Sergeant2004; Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004, Reference Ozonoff, Pennington and Rogers2006; Landa & Goldberg, Reference Landa and Goldberg2005); similar deficits were also noted in adult ASD population (Barnard et al. Reference Barnard, Muldoon, Hasan, O'Brien and Stewart2008). Likewise, impaired working memory is consistently found across different age groups from children (Ozonoff & Jensen, Reference Ozonoff and Jensen1999; Goldberg et al. Reference Goldberg, Mostofsky, Cutting, Mahone, Astor, Denckla and Landa2005; Williams et al. Reference Williams, Goldstein, Carpenter and Minshew2005; Corbett et al. Reference Corbett, Constantine, Hendren, Rocke and Ozonoff2009; Cui et al. Reference Cui, Gao, Chen, Zou and Wang2010), adolescents (Bennetto et al. Reference Bennetto, Pennington and Rogers1996; Williams et al. Reference Williams, Goldstein, Carpenter and Minshew2005; Steele et al. Reference Steele, Minshew, Luna and Sweeney2007) to adults (Williams et al. Reference Williams, Goldstein, Carpenter and Minshew2005; Barnard et al. Reference Barnard, Muldoon, Hasan, O'Brien and Stewart2008) with ASD. In contrast, although youths with ASD aged 7–16 showed impaired set-shifting ability on the WCST (Ozonoff & Jensen, Reference Ozonoff and Jensen1999; Tsuchiya et al. Reference Tsuchiya, Oki, Yahara and Fujieda2005; Kaland et al. Reference Kaland, Smith and Mortensen2008; Sumiyoshi et al. Reference Sumiyoshi, Kawakubo, Suga, Sumiyoshi and Kasai2011) and CANTAB (Hughes et al. Reference Hughes, Russell and Robbins1994; Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004; Corbett et al. Reference Corbett, Constantine, Hendren, Rocke and Ozonoff2009; Yerys et al. Reference Yerys, Wallace, Harrison, Celano, Giedd and Kenworthy2009) compared to normal controls and clinical controls, such group difference in set-shifting ability was not demonstrated in the studies of adults with ASD on the WCST (Barnard et al. Reference Barnard, Muldoon, Hasan, O'Brien and Stewart2008; Sumiyoshi et al. Reference Sumiyoshi, Kawakubo, Suga, Sumiyoshi and Kasai2011). Some of previous studies have proved the developmental importance of the period of adolescence that the synaptic pruning of excitatory contact happened in the late brain maturation during adolescence (Selemon, Reference Selemon2013), and the reduction of cortical thickness in the dorsal prefrontal region and maturity of gray-matter volume of temporal structure was found to be age-related during childhood to adulthood (Sowell et al. Reference Sowell, Delis, Stiles and Jernigan2001). Luna and colleagues also found that age-related changes in establishing large-range connections could support more functional neural processing for maturing EFs (Luna et al. Reference Luna, Padmanabhan and O'Hearn2010). Adolescence is a critical period for brain development in TD and ASD populations as well (Begley, Reference Begley2000; Giedd, Reference Giedd2008; Selemon, Reference Selemon2013), particularly the prefrontal systems which play a significant role in the executive processes (Luna et al. Reference Luna, Padmanabhan and O'Hearn2010). Related studies have shown evidence of age-related neurodevelopment from childhood to early adolescent in ASD (O'Hearn et al. Reference O'Hearn, Asato, Ordaz and Luna2008). EFs in this transition stage warrant investigation (Selemon, Reference Selemon2013).

Despite extensive research of EFs in ASD, the sample sizes of most studies are relatively too small to establish a conclusion (Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004; Barnard et al. Reference Barnard, Muldoon, Hasan, O'Brien and Stewart2008; Sumiyoshi et al. Reference Sumiyoshi, Kawakubo, Suga, Sumiyoshi and Kasai2011). Second, most studies recruited participants from a particular age range (Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004; Barnard et al. Reference Barnard, Muldoon, Hasan, O'Brien and Stewart2008; Kaland et al. Reference Kaland, Smith and Mortensen2008; Corbett et al. Reference Corbett, Constantine, Hendren, Rocke and Ozonoff2009), and the chronological changes of executive dysfunctions in individuals with ASD are still unclear. Third, the relevant research is dominated by Western society (e.g. Goldberg et al. Reference Goldberg, Mostofsky, Cutting, Mahone, Astor, Denckla and Landa2005; Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Pennington and Rogers2006). Hence, we investigated the chronological difference of a wide range of EFs with varied task difficulty in a large-scale sample of Taiwanese youths with ASD and TD youths. Our hypothesis is that the impairments of EFs in youths with ASD will be moderated by age and task difficulty, and that the group difference in school age may decrease in adolescence and the group difference may increase with increased task difficulty.

Method

Participants and procedure

The Research Ethics Committee of National Taiwan University Hospital approved the study prior to the recruitment of participants (201201006RIB; ClinicalTrials.gov number, NCT01582256). We explained the purpose and procedure of the study, and obtained the written informed consent from all participants and their parents.

The sample consisted of 111 youths with clinical diagnosis of ASD (age 12.5 ± 2.8 years, male 94.6%) according to DSM-IVdiagnostic criteria of autistic disorder or Asperger's disorder (APA, 1994) from the child psychiatric clinics of National Taiwan University Hospital, and 114 TD youths (age 12.3 ± 2.3 years, male 94.7%) from schools and colleges referred by teachers according to the gender and age distribution of the ASD group. Among the ASD participants, 28 (25.22%) had ever been treated with methylphenidate, which had been discontinued at least 24 h before performing the tasks. Parents of both groups received the Kiddie Epidemiologic Version of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (K-SADS-E) to screen for any neuropsychiatric disorders (Gau et al. Reference Gau, Chong, Chen and Cheng2005). The interview training and best-estimate procedure have been described in detail elsewhere (Gau & Shang, Reference Gau and Shang2010a ; Shang & Gau, Reference Shang and Gau2011).

The ASD diagnosis was further confirmed by interviewing parents of ASD youths using the Chinese version of the Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised (ADI-R; Gau et al. Reference Gau, Lee, Lai, Chiu, Huang, Kao and Wu2011; Chen et al. Reference Chen, Huang, Cheng, Chiu, Tsai, Wu, Liu and Gau2014). Exclusion criteria for all participants included lifetime neurological or severe medical illness, learning disorder, substance use disorder, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, major depression and current anxiety disorders. In order to complete the CANTAB, participants who were aged <8 years or who had full-scale IQ scores <80 were excluded from the study.

Measures

ADI-R

The ADI-R is a standardized semi-structured assessment of the three core symptoms of autism in individuals aged >18 months including qualitative abnormalities in reciprocal social interaction, communication, and restricted interests and repetitive and stereotyped behaviors. The Chinese Version of the ADI-R was officially approved by Western Psychological Services (WPS) in May 2007 (Gau et al. Reference Gau, Lee, Lai, Chiu, Huang, Kao and Wu2011, Reference Gau, Liu, Wu, Chiu and Tsai2013; Lau et al. Reference Lau, Gau, Chiu, Wu, Chou, Liu and Chou2013) and extensively used for clinical research in Taiwan (Gau et al. Reference Gau, Chou, Lee, Wong, Chou, Chen, Soong and Wu2010; Chien et al. Reference Chien, Gau, Chen, Tsai, Wu, Chen, Shang and Chen2013a , Reference Chien, Gau, Liao, Chiu, Wu, Huang, Tsai, Tsai and Chen b ; Liao et al. Reference Liao, Gau, Tsai, Fang, Su, Chou, Liu, Chou, Wu and Chen2013; Lin et al. Reference Lin, Kuo, Chen, Wu, Gau, Wu and Liu2013; Lo et al. Reference Lo, Chou, Fan, Gau, Chiu and Tseng2013; Chen et al. Reference Chen, Huang, Cheng, Chiu, Tsai, Wu, Liu and Gau2014). All the interviewers reached agreement over 90%, ranging from 98.25 ± 1.91 to 99.38 ± 1.06 against the rating of each item in the Chinese ADI-R by a qualified ADI-R cross-site trainer (Y. Y. Wu) and on-site trainer (S. S. Gau) before implementation of this study.

Digit Span

Participants were instructed to repeat orally presented digits in order, or backward recall digits in the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – 3rd edition (WISC-III). The Forward Digit Span test assesses verbal sustained attention and working memory, while the Backward Digit Span test assesses verbal working memory.

CANTAB

The CANTAB is a set of computerized tests to examine non-verbal neuropsychological functions (Robbins et al. Reference Robbins, James, Owen, Sahakian, Lawrence, McInnes and Rabbitt1998). Four subtests (SSP, SWM, SOC, I/ED) of the CANTAB were used to assess EFs (Gau & Shang, Reference Gau and Shang2010a , Reference Gau and Shang b ).

The SSP evaluates spatial short-term memory. At the start of the test the screen shows nine white boxes in fixed locations. Afterwards, the boxes change colors one after the other. Participants were asked to recall the sequence of color-changing boxes, and respond by touching boxes in order. The task begins with 2-box problems, and ends with 9-box problems if the participants are able to complete the task. This study presents span length (the longest sequence participants remember successfully) and total errors (the number of times participants choose incorrect boxes).

The SWM test for spatial working memory consists of three types of errors: ‘within error’ occurs when participants search the same box more than once in the same trial; ‘between error’ represents a mistake of searching boxes which have already shown blue tokens; and ‘double error’ can be categorized as both a within error and a between error. Two indexes are presented: strategy utilization (the number of searching strategies used in 6- and 8-box problems) and total errors in 4-, 6-, and 8-box problems (subtracting the number of double errors from the combination of within and between errors).

The SOC is used to evaluate planning ability. In this task, the screen is divided into upper and lower half parts. Each part presents three balls (red, blue, green) held in three suspended stockings. Participants were asked to plan and execute sequences of moves to achieve the desired arrangement. The two conditions are the test condition, in which participants were instructed to move balls in the lower part to match the arrangement in the upper part; and the control condition, which is designed to provide baseline measures for initiation time. This study presents problems solved in minimum moves (the number of times participants solved problems in the minimum number of moves) and ‘(2) “mean moves”’ (the number of moves within the range between minimum and maximum number of moves).

The I/ED is designed to evaluate mental flexibility. In this task, there are stimuli composed of unfamiliar pink shapes and white lines. Participants are instructed to select one of the two stimuli freely at the beginning. After they make the decision, the screen gives them feedbacks to assist in forming particular rules. The rules will be changed when participants have selected correct stimuli for six consecutive trials. To complete the task, participants have to pass through nine stages. The task ends if participants fail to select correct stimuli for 50 consecutive trials. This study presents (1) pre-extradimensional shift errors: participants select the correct stimuli depending on previously ignored dimension of stimuli; and (2) the number of completed stage trials.

Data analyses

We used SAS v. 9.2 software (SAS Institute Inc., USA) to perform data analyses. To acquire descriptive data, categorical variables were presented with frequency and percentage and analyzed by χ2 test, the continuous variables were presented with mean and standard deviation (s.d.) and analyzed by analysis of variance (ANOVA) and analysis of covariance with age and sex as covariates for multivariate analyses.

To investigate whether the extent of worse EF performance in ASD than TD  individuals would vary between the child (age 8–12 years) and adolescent (age 13–18 years) groups, we tested the interactions between the diagnostic group and age group, and also conducted stratifying analysis by the child and adolescent groups. We also calculated Cohen's d for the effect size, which is graded by three levels, small (0.2–0.5), medium (0.5–0.8), and large (>0.8). To further investigate the effect of task difficulty, we used the mixed procedure of SAS to address the repeated measure within the same participants on the performances of the SWM and SOC.

Results

Sample characteristics

While the ages between adolescents with ASD and TD individuals were compatible, the mean ages between children with ASD and TD children were significantly different. Children with ASD had younger mean age (9.96 ± 1.37 years) and lower full-scale IQ (108.58 ± 15.97) than TD children (mean age 10.65 ± 1.31 years, IQ 114.94 ± 8.92) (Table 1). In general, the age, education level, and employment status of the parents were compatible between ASD and TD for both child and adolescent groups except for mothers’ age and employment status (Table 1).

Table 1. Demographics and IQ of youth with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and typically developing (TD) youth, stratified by ages 8–12 and 13–18 years

IQ, Intelligence quotient; s.d., standard deviation.

* p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Age-stratified analysis (Table 2, Supplementary Table S1)

Both ASD groups recalled significantly fewer forward and backward digits than TD youths with large and medium effect sizes for the child (Cohen's d = −0.78, −0.98) and adolescent (Cohen's d = −0.68, −0.77) groups. Youths with ASD, regardless of age, recalled significantly shorter span sequences of SSP than TD youths (Cohen's d = −0.76, −0.60). For SWM, compared to TD youth, both ASD groups needed to use more strategies to complete their tasks (Cohen's d = 0.83, 0.78) but only adolescents with ASD had more total errors (Cohen's d = 0.51). For SOC, both ASD groups needed more total moves to complete the task (Cohen's d = 0.69, 0.42) than TD children but only children with ASD solved fewer problems in minimum moves (Cohen's d = −0.54). For I/ED, overall, the passing percentage of ASD youth fell strongly during the last two stages (Supplementary Fig. S1), yet only children with ASD made more pre-extra-dimensional shift errors (Cohen's d = 0.39) and completed fewer stages (Cohen's d = −0.37) than TD children without such group difference in the adolescent groups.

Table 2. Comparisons of executive functions among youth with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and typically developing (TD) youth, stratified by ages 8–12 and 13–18 years

s.d., Standard deviation.

a Controlling for age and sex.

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Diagnosis, age and their interactions

For the whole sample, youths with ASD showed worse performance in the Digit Span, SSP, SWM, SOC, but not I/ED (Supplementary Table S1 and Table 2). There were significantly better performance in all the EF measures in the adolescent group than the child group.

The effect of task difficulty

When testing the effect of task difficulty in the whole sample (Supplementary Table S3), we found significant interactions between the diagnosis group (ASD v. TD) and task difficulty on the number of total errors (6- v. 4-box problem, 8- v. 4-box problem) in SWM and on the mean moves (5- v. 2-move problem) in SOC.

When stratified by age (Table 3), we found that the younger stratum showed significant interactions between the diagnosis group and all the difficulty levels of the SWM total errors; whereas, the elder stratum only revealed a significant interaction between diagnosis and the 8- v. 4-box problem. Fig. 1 also presents the magnitude of the difference between ASD and TD groups on the SWM total errors, enlarged as the task difficulty increased for the child and adolescent groups.

Fig. 1. Total errors of the spatial working memory task for the ASD child and adolescent groups as well as the TD child and adolescent groups.

Table 3. A model integrating task difficulties, the diagnosis groups and their interactions, controlling for sex and age and stratifying by ages 8–12 and 13–18 years

CI, confidence interval; β, regression coefficient estimates; ASD, autism spectrum disorder.

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

In the mean moves of the SOC, there was significant interaction between the diagnosis and 5- v. 2-move problem for the child group (Table 3). Fig. 2 shows that the magnitude of the difference between ASD and TD groups on total moves of the SOC, enlarged as the task difficulty increased.

Fig. 2. Total moves of the Stockings of Cambridge for the ASD child and adolescent groups as well as the TD child and adolescent groups.

The correlation between strategy utilization and the errors (Supplementary Table S5)

We further computed Pearson's correlation between strategy utilization and the number of errors of the SWM and found significant correlations between strategy utilization and three types of errors (total error, within error and between error) in both ASD and TD groups (Supplementary Table S5). Moreover, the correlations between errors and strategy use were greater with increasing level of task difficulty. The correlations did not significantly differ between ASD and TD groups (Supplementary Table S5)

The correlation between EFs and ASD symptom severity in youth with ASD

To investigate the correlation between EFs and ASD symptom severity, we calculated Pearson's correlations to quantify the relationship between the CANTAB performance and the ADI-R subscores in youth with ASD for the child (Supplementary Table S6) and adolescent (Supplementary Table S7) groups. The results showed that while children with ASD showed significant correlations between the SOC and verbal communication deficits assessed by the ADI-R at the current and most severe status (Supplementary Table S6), there were significant correlations between the performance of the SOC and the verbal and non-verbal communication deficits assessed by the ADI-R at the most severe status in adolescents with ASD (Supplementary Table S7).

Discussion

This study is the first to employ the comprehensive, standardized and computerized test battery, CANTAB, to assess EFs in an ethnic Chinese population, and is also one of the few studies recruiting a larger sample of ASD youths. The results demonstrated impaired EF in youths with ASD on short-term memory, spatial working memory, planning, and set-shifting, with an age-moderating effect mainly on planning and set-shifting, which were found impaired only in children with ASD but not in adolescents with ASD in our age-stratified analysis. We also found greater group difference with increased task difficulty in the SWM (Fig. 1) and SOC (Fig. 2) for both the child and adolescent groups.

Our findings of short-term spatial memory deficits in youth with ASD were consistent with some previous studies (Bowler et al. Reference Bowler, Matthews and Gardiner1997; Minshew & Goldstein, Reference Minshew and Goldstein2001; Poirier et al. Reference Poirier, Martin, Gaigg and Bowler2011) but not others (Prior & Chen, Reference Prior and Chen1976; Williams et al. Reference Williams, Happé and Jarrold2008). Such discrepant findings may be due to the different levels of task difficulty used across studies.

Compatible with the most previous studies showing impaired spatial working memory on various tasks in ASD (Williams et al. Reference Williams, Goldstein, Carpenter and Minshew2005, Reference Williams, Goldstein and Minshew2006; Joseph et al. Reference Joseph, McGrath and Tager-Flusberg2005a ; Gras-Vincendon et al. Reference Gras-Vincendon, Bursztejn and Danion2008; Kenworthy et al. Reference Kenworthy, Yerys, Anthony and Wallace2008), our study demonstrated spatial working memory deficit in both age groups of ASD measured by the CANTAB. Some researchers explained the impairment by the utilization of strategy (Joseph et al. Reference Joseph, Steele, Meyer and Tager-Flusberg2005b ; Gras-Vincendon et al. Reference Gras-Vincendon, Bursztejn and Danion2008) that TD youth, but not youth with ASD, use verbal mediation strategies to locate positions in spatial working memory tasks. Nevertheless, there are few studies which recruit participants with high-functioning autism (Williams et al. Reference Williams, Goldstein, Carpenter and Minshew2005) or Asperger's disorder (Cui et al. Reference Cui, Gao, Chen, Zou and Wang2010) showing no impairment of spatial working memory (Nakahachi et al. Reference Nakahachi, Iwase, Takahashi, Honaga, Sekiyama, Ukai, Ishii, Osterling and Dawson1994; Williams et al. Reference Williams, Goldstein, Carpenter and Minshew2005, Reference Williams, Goldstein and Minshew2006). Functional level of participants with ASD is one possible reason for the inconsistency. Moreover, our finding of the impairment in both age groups is similar to the previous reports that individuals with ASD recalled shorter length or had more inefficient strategy utilization across the child and adolescent populations (Goldberg et al. Reference Goldberg, Mostofsky, Cutting, Mahone, Astor, Denckla and Landa2005; Steele et al. Reference Steele, Minshew, Luna and Sweeney2007; Barnard et al. Reference Barnard, Muldoon, Hasan, O'Brien and Stewart2008; Corbett et al. Reference Corbett, Constantine, Hendren, Rocke and Ozonoff2009), suggesting that the spatial working memory deficit may persist into late adolescence as a trait marker for ASD.

Our findings of planning deficits in youths with ASD were similar to the results of most previous studies using either the same measurement (Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004; Landa & Goldberg, Reference Landa and Goldberg2005) or not (Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Rogers and Pennington1991; Hughes et al. Reference Hughes, Russell and Robbins1994; Ozonoff & Jensen, Reference Ozonoff and Jensen1999; Geurts et al. Reference Geurts, Verté, Oosterlaan, Roeyers and Sergeant2004). Interestingly, some other studies evaluating planning by traditional measurements, such as the Tower of Hanoi (ToH) and the Tower of London (ToL), demonstrated that individuals with ASD had fewer passes and less efficient strategy than both healthy and comparison individuals with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Rogers and Pennington1991; Hughes et al. Reference Hughes, Russell and Robbins1994; Ozonoff & Jensen, Reference Ozonoff and Jensen1999; Geurts et al. Reference Geurts, Verté, Oosterlaan, Roeyers and Sergeant2004). Regarding the age effect, although deficits in planning ability (total moves of SOC) were noted in both ASD groups with greater effect size in younger youths, only younger ASD youths demonstrated poorer problem-solving ability assessed by the SOC than their counterparts. Such findings indicate that ASD youths may improve their planning/problem-solving ability over time throughout adolescence. Our findings are in line with some evidence from previous studies of younger ASD individuals (Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Rogers and Pennington1991; Hughes et al. Reference Hughes, Russell and Robbins1994; Hughes, Reference Hughes1996; Ozonoff & Jensen, Reference Ozonoff and Jensen1999; Geurts et al. Reference Geurts, Verté, Oosterlaan, Roeyers and Sergeant2004; Landa & Goldberg, Reference Landa and Goldberg2005; Happé et al. Reference Happé, Booth, Charlton and Hughes2006); whereas there is scarce evidence in older ASD individuals (Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004). Hill's review concluded that planning ability in ASD was positively correlated with non-verbal mental age (El-Bassel et al. Reference El-Bassel, Gilbert, Frye, Wu, Go, Hill and Richman2004) that the planning deficits were remarkable in childhood but would become lessen with age. In general, there was no significant diagnosis  ×  age interaction on SOC performance (Supplementary Table S2), suggesting a declined planning ability was noted in children, adolescents and young adults with ASD.

In contrast to previous studies which reported significant impairments of set-shifting in ASD as assessed by the I/ED of the CANTAB (Hughes et al. Reference Hughes, Russell and Robbins1994; Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004; Corbett et al. Reference Corbett, Constantine, Hendren, Rocke and Ozonoff2009; Yerys et al. Reference Yerys, Wallace, Harrison, Celano, Giedd and Kenworthy2009), the dimension-change card sort task (Dichter et al. Reference Dichter, Radonovich, Turner-Brown, Lam, Holtzclaw and Bodfish2010), WCST (Ozonoff & Jensen, Reference Ozonoff and Jensen1999; Tsuchiya et al. Reference Tsuchiya, Oki, Yahara and Fujieda2005; Kaland et al. Reference Kaland, Smith and Mortensen2008; Sumiyoshi et al. Reference Sumiyoshi, Kawakubo, Suga, Sumiyoshi and Kasai2011) and abstracting tests (Minshew et al. Reference Minshew, Muenz, Goldstein and Payton1992), our findings only revealed poorer cognitive flexibility as assessed by the I/ED task in children with ASD (aged 8–12) but no difference in adolescents with ASD (aged 13–18). The observed discrepancies may be explained by different chronological age of the participants between studies. ASD participants recruited in previous studies were younger and showed more errors and different completion rates while performing the I/ED task (Hughes et al. Reference Hughes, Russell and Robbins1994; Yerys et al. Reference Yerys, Wallace, Harrison, Celano, Giedd and Kenworthy2009; Dichter et al. Reference Dichter, Radonovich, Turner-Brown, Lam, Holtzclaw and Bodfish2010). However, findings in adolescents and young adults with ASD were inconsistent. Sumiyoshi et al. (Reference Sumiyoshi, Kawakubo, Suga, Sumiyoshi and Kasai2011) found that adults with ASD (mean age 26 years) performed significantly worse than TD individuals on the WCST. Another study did not find a significant difference (mean age 30 years) in comparison to participants with learning disabilities (Barnard et al. Reference Barnard, Muldoon, Hasan, O'Brien and Stewart2008). The discrepancy in adults with ASD may also be attributed to the nature of the set-shifting paradigms. Researchers have suggested that the widely administered measurement, i.e. the WCST, is more complex in that subjects have to recognize the patterns of the cards (including color, number, and shape), form abstract concepts, sustain the principle in mind and transfer the principle timely (Kaland et al. Reference Kaland, Smith and Mortensen2008), thus it evaluates functions more than set-shifting. In contrast, the CANTAB simplified the principles (i.e. line and figure) and presented them in a graded format, thus the results could acquire more reliable evidences of set-shifting ability (Robbins et al. Reference Robbins, James, Owen, Sahakian, Lawrence, McInnes and Rabbitt1998). Taken together, adults with ASD may not have set-shifting problems but that depends on which task was chosen. Whether adolescents with ASD have set-shifting problems warrants further investigation.

To summarize the chronological effect on EF, the results of current study showed impaired spatial working memory, planning, and set-shifting in children with ASD but improved in adolescents with ASD except for spatial working memory. Although the age-moderating effect is generally insignificant on planning and set-shifting, we found a trend that set-shifting ability in youth with ASD turned to parallel that of TD individuals as they grew up. Our findings were consistent with a few related studies that showed impaired cognitive flexibility and planning ability were age-dependent and apparent in childhood and early adolescence of ASD (Pugliese et al. Reference Pugliese, Anthony, Strang, Dudley, Wallace and Kenworthy2014; Van den Bergh et al. Reference Van den Bergh, Scheeren, Begeer, Koot and Geurts2014). Happé et al. (Reference Happé, Booth, Charlton and Hughes2006) also provided evidence that the performance of flexibility and planning in adolescents (aged 11–16) with ASD caught up with the performance of age-matched healthy controls. In line with one similar study investigating response preparation, inhibition and working memory in ASD samples (Luna et al. Reference Luna, Doll, Hegedus, Minshew and Sweeney2007), our findings imply that there were both typical and atypical developmental progressions of distinctive EFs in individuals with autism. The impaired EF such as planning and set-shifting which only presents in early childhood would imply a developmental delay and brain maturational processes which compensates for the inherent impairment (Luna et al. Reference Luna, Doll, Hegedus, Minshew and Sweeney2007). The study providing evidence of a reduction in cortical thickness in the dorsal prefrontal region between childhood and adolescence might explain a part of brain maturational processes and the related intact performance of planning and set-shifting task in older youth with ASD (Sowell et al. Reference Sowell, Delis, Stiles and Jernigan2001). Instead of age-dependent planning and set-shifting ability, the persistent spatial working memory may serve as a trait marker for ASD and may nest in the core pathology of the ASD brain.

Consistent with previous studies using verbal working memory tasks (Rapin, Reference Rapin1996; Williams et al. Reference Williams, Goldstein and Minshew2006) or nonsense words (Gabig, Reference Gabig2008), we found a significant effect of task difficulty in both age stratifications that youths with ASD showed more errors on SWM relative to TD individuals when the tasks imposed greater demands, suggesting that spatial working memory deficits are fundamental in ASD regardless of task difficulty and aging effect (Fig. 1). Poor usage of compensatory strategies may lead to poorer performance. Although performing better in simple tasks, individuals with ASD failed to organize strategies to support memory in the tasks with increasing complexity of materials (Gras-Vincendon et al. Reference Gras-Vincendon, Bursztejn and Danion2008). Similarly, increased moves to complete the task were noted in SOC as the task demands increased, but planning deficits in ASD, relative to TD individuals, appeared only in the most difficult task (5-move problems) (Supplementary Table S3, Fig. 2). Unlike TD individuals who used fewer moves to solve problems, youths with ASD required more moves to complete the task, suggesting that the latter group tends to use trial-and-error, rather than taking time to develop an effective strategy before action. Other studies using the same tasks (Ozonoff et al. Reference Ozonoff, Cook, Coon, Dawson, Joseph, Klin, McMahon, Minshew, Munson and Pennington2004) or the ToL task (Hughes et al. Reference Hughes, Russell and Robbins1994; Geurts et al. Reference Geurts, Verté, Oosterlaan, Roeyers and Sergeant2004) also found that planning deficits were evident on trials requiring longer sequences of moves (4- and 5-move problems) in youths with ASD. Consistently, more discrepancy with increased task demands were also demonstrated in other planning paradigms such as a transferring task (Hughes, Reference Hughes1996), which requires subjects to pick up and transfer pegs from one hole to the other, and the Zoo Map test (Hill & Bird, Reference Hill and Bird2006), which requires subjects to plan a route to destinations. More interestingly, when we stratified the sample by age, children with ASD used more moves across the four levels of task difficulty, while adolescents with ASD demonstrated planning deficits only in the most difficult task, implying that the task difficulty effect was more pronounced in older ASD youths rather than younger. Taken together, in contrast to the task difficulty effect in SWM regardless of age stratification, an interaction between age group and task difficulty was found in SOC, again implying that planning ability on simple tasks in ASD youths comes closer to that in the TD group, but impaired working memory persisted across developmental stages from childhood to adulthood. Hill's review article supports our statement that the planning ability in ASD participants improved as they became older, and the planning deficit was evident only on tasks requiring heavier demands (Hill, Reference Hill2004).

Several limitations should be taken into consideration while interpreting our findings. First, due to a cross-sectional study design, we could only examine the age difference on EFs rather than the developmental trajectory of EFs. A longitudinal study is warranted to advance our knowledge about the chronological change of EFs. Second, the treatments effects on executive dysfunction were not considered in this study. Since most autistic participants in our study had received early intervention, the frequency and quantity of interventions would possibly affect participants’ performance, which should be inquired and controlled during data analyses. Last, this study only recruited subjects with an IQ >80 so they were able to perform the CANTAB tasks, our findings may not be applicable to ASD individuals with a lower IQ.

The findings in our study contribute not only to academia, but also to clinical practice. In past decades, with increasing government support and health education, the public has paid much closer attention to neurodevelopmental disorders. The interventions of ASD, in general, focus on improving communication and social skills, which are apparent and strongly educated in the public, while other functional domains that deserve more concerns have lacked attention. The results of our study provide evidence for caregivers and clinicians that some EFs (e.g. visuospatial planning and set-shifting) may improve to some extent in adolescence, yet working memory remains a weakness and warrants more coaching and assistance. Therapists may provide training aimed at strengthening specific EFs. Furthermore, considering the difficulties that increase with task demands, applying therapeutic activities with applicably graded task demands to both children and adolescents with ASD may potentially help increase the effect of interventions. For instance, providing activities such as asking children with ASD to recall words, sentences, and paragraphs in order could effectively improve their working memory ability (Rapin, Reference Rapin1996; Williams et al. Reference Williams, Goldstein and Minshew2006).

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715002238.

Acknowledgements

This work is supported by grants from National Science Council, Taiwan (NSC98-3112-B-002-004, NSC99-2627-B-002-015, NSC100-2627-B-002-014, NSC101-2627-B-002-002, NSC 101-2314-B-002-136-MY3) and AIM for Top University Excellent Research Project (10R81918-03,101R892103, 102R892103). The authors thank all the participants and their parents.

References

APA (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Press: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
APA (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Association: Arlington, VA.Google Scholar
Barnard, L, Muldoon, K, Hasan, R, O'Brien, G, Stewart, M (2008). Profiling executive dysfunction in adults with autism and comorbid learning disability. Autism 12, 125141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6, 248254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, SE, Tager-Flusberg, HE, Cohen, DJ (2000). Understanding other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. Oxford University Press: Oxford.Google Scholar
Baxter, AJ, Brugha, TS, Erskine, HE, Scheurer, RW, Vos, T, Scott, JG (2015). The epidemiology and global burden of autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Medicine 45, 601613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begley, S (2000). Getting inside a teen brain. Hormones aren't the only reason adolescents act crazy. Their gray matter differs from children's and adults’. Newsweek 135, 5859.Google ScholarPubMed
Bennetto, L, Pennington, BF, Rogers, SJ (1996). Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Development 67, 18161835.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blumberg, SJ, Bramlett, MD, Kogan, MD, Schieve, LA, Jones, JR, Lu, MC (2013). Changes in prevalence of parent-reported autism spectrum disorder in school-aged US children: 2007 to 2011–2012. National Health Statistics Reports 65, 111.Google Scholar
Bowler, DM, Matthews, NJ, Gardiner, JM (1997). Asperger's syndrome and memory: similarity to autism but not amnesia. Neuropsychologia 35, 6570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, CH, Huang, CC, Cheng, MC, Chiu, YN, Tsai, WC, Wu, YY, Liu, SK, Gau, SS (2014). Genetic analysis of GABRB3 as a candidate gene of autism spectrum disorders. Molecular Autism 5, 36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chien, WH, Gau, SS, Chen, CH, Tsai, WC, Wu, YY, Chen, PH, Shang, CY, Chen, CH (2013 a). Increased gene expression of FOXP1 in patients with autism spectrum disorders. Molecular Autism 4, 23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chien, WH, Gau, SS, Liao, HM, Chiu, YN, Wu, YY, Huang, YS, Tsai, WC, Tsai, HM, Chen, CH (2013 b). Deep exon resequencing of DLGAP2 as a candidate gene of autism spectrum disorders. Molecular Autism 4, 26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbett, BA, Constantine, LJ, Hendren, R, Rocke, D, Ozonoff, S (2009). Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and typical development. Psychiatry Research 166, 210222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cui, J, Gao, D, Chen, Y, Zou, X, Wang, Y (2010). Working memory in early-school-age children with Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 40, 958967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A, Maurer, R (1978). A neurological model for childhood autism. Archive of Neurology 35, 777786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, G, Meltzoff, AN, Osterling, J, Rinaldi, J (1998). Neuropsychological correlates of early symptoms of autism. Child Development 69, 12761285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dichter, GS, Radonovich, KJ, Turner-Brown, LM, Lam, KS, Holtzclaw, TN, Bodfish, JW (2010). Performance of children with autism spectrum disorders on the dimension-change card sort task. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 40, 448456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
El-Bassel, N, Gilbert, L, Frye, V, Wu, E, Go, H, Hill, J, Richman, BL (2004). Physical and sexual intimate partner violence among women in methadone maintenance treatment. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 18, 180183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabig, CS (2008). Verbal working memory and story retelling in school-age children with autism. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 39, 498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gau, SS, Chong, MY, Chen, TH, Cheng, AT (2005). A 3-year panel study of mental disorders among adolescents in Taiwan. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 13441350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gau, SS, Liu, LT, Wu, YY, Chiu, YN, Tsai, WC (2013). Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the social responsiveness scale. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 7, 349360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gau, SS, Shang, CY (2010 a). Executive functions as endophenotypes in ADHD: evidence from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Battery (CANTAB). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 51, 838849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gau, SS, Shang, CY (2010 b). Improvement of executive functions in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an open-label follow-up study with once-daily atomoxetine. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 13, 243256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gau, SSF, Chou, MC, Lee, JC, Wong, CC, Chou, WJ, Chen, MF, Soong, WT, Wu, YY (2010). Behavioral problems and parenting style among Taiwanese children with autism and their siblings. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 64, 7078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gau, SS-F, Lee, C-M, Lai, M-C, Chiu, Y-N, Huang, Y-F, Kao, J-D, Wu, Y-Y (2011). Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the social communication questionnaire. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5, 809818.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geurts, HM, Verté, S, Oosterlaan, J, Roeyers, H, Sergeant, JA (2004). How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45, 836854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giedd, JN (2008). The teen brain: insights from neuroimaging. Journal of Adolescent Health 42, 335343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, M, Mostofsky, S, Cutting, L, Mahone, E, Astor, B, Denckla, M, Landa, R (2005). Subtle executive impairment in children with autism and children with ADHD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 35, 279293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, G, Johnson, CR, Minshew, NJ (2001). Attentional processes in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 31, 433440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gras-Vincendon, A, Bursztejn, C, Danion, JM (2008). Functioning of memory in subjects with autism. Encephale 34, 550556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Happé, F, Booth, R, Charlton, R, Hughes, C (2006). Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: examining profiles across domains and ages. Brain and Cognition 61, 2539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, EL (2004). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review 24, 189233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, EL, Bird, CM (2006). Executive processes in Asperger syndrome: patterns of performance in a multiple case series. Neuropsychologia 44, 28222835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C (1996). Brief report: planning problems in autism at the level of motor control. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 26, 99107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C, Russell, J, Robbins, TW (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia 32, 477492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joseph, RM, McGrath, LM, Tager-Flusberg, H (2005 a). Executive dysfunction and its relation to language ability in verbal school-age children with autism. Developmental Neuropsychology 27, 361378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joseph, RM, Steele, SD, Meyer, E, Tager-Flusberg, H (2005 b). Self-ordered pointing in children with autism: failure to use verbal mediation in the service of working memory? Neuropsychologia 43, 14001411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaland, N, Smith, L, Mortensen, EL (2008). Brief report: cognitive flexibility and focused attention in children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism as measured on the computerized version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 38, 11611165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenworthy, L, Yerys, BE, Anthony, LG, Wallace, GL (2008). Understanding executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world. Neuropsychology Review 18, 320338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, DC, Tseng, YC, Guo, HR (2013). Trends in the prevalence of childhood disability: analysis of data from the national disability registry of Taiwan, 2000–2011. Research in Developmental Disabilities 34, 37663772.Google ScholarPubMed
Lai, DC, Tseng, YC, Hou, YM, Guo, HR (2012). Gender and geographic differences in the prevalence of autism spectrum disorders in children: analysis of data from the National Disability Registry of Taiwan. Research in Developmental Disabilities 33, 909915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landa, RJ, Goldberg, MC (2005). Language, social, and executive functions in high functioning autism: a continuum of performance. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 35, 557573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lau, WY, Gau, SS, Chiu, YN, Wu, YY, Chou, WJ, Liu, SK, Chou, MC (2013). Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the autism spectrum quotient (AQ). Research in Developmental Disabilities 34, 294305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liao, HM, Gau, SS, Tsai, WC, Fang, JS, Su, YC, Chou, MC, Liu, SK, Chou, WJ, Wu, YY, Chen, CH (2013). Chromosomal abnormalities in patients with autism spectrum disorders from Taiwan. American Journal of Medical Genetics, B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics 162B, 734741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, LY (2015). Coping strategies, caregiving burden, and depressive symptoms of Taiwanese mothers of adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 15, 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, PI, Kuo, PH, Chen, CH, Wu, JY, Gau, SS, Wu, YY, Liu, SK (2013). Runs of homozygosity associated with speech delay in autism in a Taiwanese Han population: evidence for the recessive model. PLoS ONE 8, e72056.Google Scholar
Lo, YC, Chou, TL, Fan, LY, Gau, SS, Chiu, YN, Tseng, WY (2013). Altered structure-function relations of semantic processing in youths with high-functioning autism: a combined diffusion and functional MRI Study. Autism Research 12, 561570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luna, B, Doll, SK, Hegedus, SJ, Minshew, NJ, Sweeney, JA (2007). Maturation of executive function in autism. Biological Psychiatry 61, 474481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luna, B, Padmanabhan, A, O'Hearn, K (2010). What has fMRI told us about the development of cognitive control through adolescence? Brain and Cognition 72, 101113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A, Volkmar, FR, Lewis, M (eds) (2007). Lewis's Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: A Comprehensive Textbook. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins: Philadelphia.Google Scholar
Matson, JL, Kozlowski, AM (2011). The increasing prevalence of autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5, 418425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minshew, NJ, Goldstein, G (2001). The pattern of intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42, 10951101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minshew, NJ, Muenz, LR, Goldstein, G, Payton, JB (1992). Neuropsychological functioning in nonmentally retarded autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 14, 749761.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakahachi, T, Iwase, M, Takahashi, H, Honaga, E, Sekiyama, R, Ukai, S, Ishii, R, Osterling, J, Dawson, G (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: a study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24, 247257.Google Scholar
O'Hearn, K, Asato, M, Ordaz, S, Luna, B (2008). Neurodevelopment and executive function in autism. Development and Psychopathology 20, 11031132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osterling, J, Dawson, G (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: a study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24, 247257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ozonoff, S, Cook, I, Coon, H, Dawson, G, Joseph, RM, Klin, A, McMahon, WM, Minshew, N, Munson, JA, Pennington, BF (2004). Performance on Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery subtests sensitive to frontal lobe function in people with autistic disorder: evidence from the Collaborative Programs of Excellence in Autism network. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34, 139150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ozonoff, S, Jensen, J (1999). Brief report: specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 29, 171177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ozonoff, S, Pennington, BF, Rogers, SJ (2006). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 32, 10811105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ozonoff, S, Rogers, SJ, Pennington, BF (1991). Asperger's syndrome: evidence of an empirical distinction from high-functioning Autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 32, 11071122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poirier, M, Martin, JS, Gaigg, SB, Bowler, DM (2011). Short-term memory in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 120, 247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pugliese, CE, Anthony, L, Strang, JF, Dudley, K, Wallace, GL, Kenworthy, L (2014). Increasing adaptive behavior skill deficits from childhood to adolescence in autism spectrum disorder: role of executive function. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 45, 15791587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prior, MR, Chen, C (1976). Short-term and serial memory in autistic, retarded, and normal children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia 6, 121131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapin, I (1996). Preschool Children with Inadequate Communication: Developmental Language Disorder, Autism, Low IQ. Mac Keith Press: Suffolk.Google Scholar
Robbins, TW, James, M, Owen, AM, Sahakian, BJ, Lawrence, AD, McInnes, L, Rabbitt, PM (1998). A study of performance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volunteers: implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 4, 474490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, S, Goddard, L, Dritschel, B, Wisley, M, Howlin, P (2009). Executive functions in children with autism spectrum disorders. Brain and Cognition 71, 362368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, M, Wallace, GL, Lawson, R, Wills, MC, Dixon, E, Yerys, BE, Kenworthy, L (2013). Impairments in real-world executive function increase from childhood to adolescence in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychology 27, 13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russo, N, Flanagan, T, Iarocci, G, Berringer, D, Zelazo, PD, Burack, JA (2007). Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: implications for cognitive neuroscience. Brain and Cognition 65, 7786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salmond, C, De Haan, M, Friston, K, Gadian, D, Vargha-Khadem, F (2003). Investigating individual differences in brain abnormalities in autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 358, 405413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selemon, LD (2013). A role for synaptic plasticity in the adolescent development of executive function. Translational Psychiatry 3, e238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shang, CY, Gau, SS (2011). Visual memory as a potential cognitive endophenotype of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Medicine 40, 26032614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sowell, ER, Delis, D, Stiles, J, Jernigan, TL (2001). Improved memory functioning and frontal lobe maturation between childhood and adolescence: a structural MRI study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 7, 312322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steele, SD, Minshew, NJ, Luna, B, Sweeney, JA (2007). Spatial working memory deficits in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37, 605612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sumiyoshi, C, Kawakubo, Y, Suga, M, Sumiyoshi, T, Kasai, K (2011). Impaired ability to organize information in individuals with autism spectrum disorders and their siblings. Neuroscience Research 69, 252257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsuchiya, E, Oki, J, Yahara, N, Fujieda, K (2005). Computerized version of the Wisconsin card sorting test in children with high-functioning autistic disorder or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain and Development 27, 233236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van den Bergh, SF, Scheeren, AM, Begeer, S, Koot, HM, Geurts, HM (2014). Age related differences of executive functioning problems in everyday life of children and adolescents in the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 44, 19591971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, D, Happé, F, Jarrold, C (2008). Intact inner speech use in autism spectrum disorder: evidence from a short-term memory task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49, 5158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, DL, Goldstein, G, Carpenter, PA, Minshew, NJ (2005). Verbal and spatial working memory in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 35, 747756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, DL, Goldstein, G, Minshew, NJ (2006). The profile of memory function in children with autism. Neuropsychology 20, 21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yerys, BE, Wallace, GL, Harrison, B, Celano, MJ, Giedd, JN, Kenworthy, LE (2009). Set-shifting in children with autism spectrum disorders reversal shifting deficits on the Intradimensional/Extradimensional Shift Test correlate with repetitive behaviors. Autism 13, 523538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographics and IQ of youth with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and typically developing (TD) youth, stratified by ages 8–12 and 13–18 years

Figure 1

Table 2. Comparisons of executive functions among youth with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and typically developing (TD) youth, stratified by ages 8–12 and 13–18 years

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Total errors of the spatial working memory task for the ASD child and adolescent groups as well as the TD child and adolescent groups.

Figure 3

Table 3. A model integrating task difficulties, the diagnosis groups and their interactions, controlling for sex and age and stratifying by ages 8–12 and 13–18 years

Figure 4

Fig. 2. Total moves of the Stockings of Cambridge for the ASD child and adolescent groups as well as the TD child and adolescent groups.

Supplementary material: Image

Chen supplementary material

Figure S1

Download Chen supplementary material(Image)
Image 108.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

Chen supplementary material

Tables S1-S7

Download Chen supplementary material(File)
File 47.4 KB