Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T17:32:39.140Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Collective memory: An hourglass between the collective and the individual

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 March 2022

Aline Cordonnier*
Affiliation:
Psychological Science Research Institute, UCLouvain, 10 Place cardinal Mercier, 1348 Louvain-la-Neuve, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
Valérie Rosoux
Affiliation:
Institute of Political Science Louvain-Europe, UCLouvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium Fund for Scientific Research (FRS-FNRS), Brussels, Belgium
Anne-Sophie Gijs
Affiliation:
Institute for the Analysis of Change in Contemporary and Historical Societies, UCLouvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium Institute for the Study of Civilisations, Arts and Letters, UCLouvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
Olivier Luminet
Affiliation:
Psychological Science Research Institute, UCLouvain, 10 Place cardinal Mercier, 1348 Louvain-la-Neuve, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium Fund for Scientific Research (FRS-FNRS), Brussels, Belgium
*
Corresponding author: Aline Cordonnier, email: aline.cordonnier@uclouvain.be

Abstract

Collective memories are memories shared by a group that influence their social identity. The goal of this paper is to focus on two major limitations in current studies on collective memory and show how the hourglass metaphor can overcome those limitations. The first limitation concerns the partial nature of studies devoted to the analysis of collective memory. Studies tend to focus either on the choice of the past (how memory agents mobilise the past) or the weight of the past (how the past affects the individual or the group). The second limitation relates to the temporal dimension of research conducted so far. Most studies only assess memory over a single generation, yet it can have long-term effects. In this paper, we suggest considering memory work as an hourglass, with the collective and the individual at opposite ends and the sand of memories passing from one to the other, filtered through family values and representations. The hourglass metaphor thus provides a helpful tool to explain the formation of collective memories over time and the interactions between the macro, meso, and micro levels. We approach the study of collective memory from an interdisciplinary perspective, mainly involving psychology, political science, and history. We conclude by suggesting three challenges that future studies of memory will need to address: (1) the need to combine multiple approaches; (2) the need to consider the role of generations; and (3) the need to bridge discussion across disciplines.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Human memory makes it possible to encode, preserve, transform, and restore lived experiences and transmitted knowledge. It can refer to a set of psychological functions by which humans can update past impressions or information. Seen from this perspective, the study of memory is part of cognitive psychology, neurophysiology, biology and, for memory disorders, neuropsychology, clinical psychology, and psychiatry. Studies in these domains focus on individuals and examine ‘memory in the head’. From the perspective of the human and social sciences, memory is a subject of analysis for sociology, history, anthropology, philosophy, and communication studies. These fields investigate ‘memory in the wild’ and most experts in these fields stress that memory cannot be an exact and perfect reflection of the past: it is only its trace or evocation (Kensinger Reference Kensinger2009; Noiriel Reference Noiriel2004). Yet the links between memory in the head and in the wild remain unclear (Barnier and Hoskins Reference Barnier and Hoskins2018). The study of collective memory resides at the intersection of these two worlds, where individuals and the social context in which they live collide to form an ensemble of representations and memories shared by a group. Throughout this paper, we argue that both perspectives need to be considered jointly in order to fully grasp the scale of how collective memories are formed, transformed, and transmitted, as well as how they impact the individual and the collective.

Memories are not literally preserved, but they are reconstructed according to present conditions and the social context. It is from this premise that Halbwachs (Reference Halbwachs1997 [1950]) developed the concept of collective memory. Contesting the notion – which he considered an impossibility – of isolated individual memory, the French sociologist emphasised above all the influence of the social on the content of individual memories. In Les cadres sociaux de la mémoire (Reference Halbwachs1994 [1925]), he argued that, over the course of an era, it is a groups’ shared beliefs and its collective experiences which shape the meaning of individual memories and not the other way around. When the notion of collective memory resurfaced at the end of the 1970s, it was in the realm of history not sociology. According to Nora, collective memory is ‘what remains of the past in the groups’ experiences, or rather what these groups do with the past’ (Reference Nora, Goff, Charlier and Revel1978, 401). Since then, the notion has gradually spread throughout the humanities and social sciences (Baussant Reference Baussant2002; Gluck Reference Gluck, Jager and Mitter2007; Hirst and Manier Reference Hirst and Manier2008; Kim Reference Kim2016; Kurze and Lamont Reference Kurze and Lamont2019; Olick et al Reference Olick, Vinitzky-Seroussi and Levy2011; Winter Reference Winter2001).

Despite the wealth of existing literature, two major limitations remain in collective memory studies. The first concerns the relatively partial nature of many studies. They generally focus on only one of the two dimensions that constitute the subject under examination (Lavabre Reference Lavabre1994). The first of these is the choice of the past – which refers to how memory agents strategically mobilise the past (Assmann and Conrad Reference Assmann and Conrad2010; Foong Khong Reference Foong Khong1992; Langenbacher and Shain Reference Langenbacher and Shain2010; Todorov Reference Todorov1995) – and the second is the weight of the past – which investigates the traces or imprints left by the past on individuals and groups (Bell Reference Bell2010; Davoine and Gaudillière Reference Davoine and Gaudillière2006; Rosenblum Reference Rosenblum2009). The hypothesis underlying this paper is that it is crucial to understand the articulation between the macro level, most often reduced to the strategic dimension (the choice of the past), and the micro level, which has been often analysed through the prism of ‘trauma’, in the broadest sense (the weight of the past) (Margry and Sanchez-Carretero Reference Margry and Sanchez-Carretero2011; Resende and Budryte Reference Resende and Budryte2014). We argue that another important dimension also lies within the meso level, the smaller groups in which individuals interact, exchange stories, representations, and narratives. Within the scope of this paper, we examine the meso level through the role of families and how the family values and representations act as a memory and information filter.Footnote 1

We are not the first researchers to call for more studies to examine the interaction between the different levels of memory (e.g., Heinrich and Weyland Reference Heinrich and Weyland2016; Van de Putte Reference Van de Putte2021). Among those who have made similar calls, communication studies researchers Keightley, Pickering and Bisht (Reference Keightley, Pickering, Bisht, Maurantonio and Parks2019) have suggested an ‘interscalar approach’ to the study of memory, which takes experience as a starting point before analysing the different communicative processes between the individual and collective dimensions. For them, remembering at different scales is produced by ‘mnemonic imagination’, a mechanism that combines memory and imagination. This suggestion is closely related to current arguments in psychology, which assert that memory uses the same reconstructive process as imagining the future or considering counterfactual versions of past events (Cordonnier et al Reference Cordonnier, Barnier and Sutton2018; Schacter et al Reference Schacter, Addis and Buckner2007).

This is but one of many good examples that demonstrate how different research disciplines working in the field of memory would benefit from collaboration. Consequently, in order to examine and comprehend the intertwining mechanisms operating at the collective, interpersonal and individual levels, multi-disciplinary work is essential. Only by combining expertise and methodologies is it possible to identify phenomena related to the instrumentalisation of memory, its relational, cognitive, and emotional aspects, as well as its possible distortions over time.

The second limitation relates to the temporal dimension of collective memory research. Understanding the mechanisms of memory transmission and evolution necessarily implies a broadening of the timescale (Joutard Reference Joutard1977). Rather than limiting itself to the study of one generation of actors, future research should attempt to identify tensions, gaps, and even contradictions from one generation to the next, as well as investigate the active role that different generations take in the sharing of memories and knowledge. Although there have already been studies that have examined intergenerational transmission within families or in schools (Billaud et al Reference Billaud, Gollac, Oeser and Pagis2015; Buono Reference Buono2004; Cordonnier et al Reference Cordonnier, Bouchat, Hirst and Luminet2021; Fivush et al Reference Fivush, Bohanek, Duke and Sani2008; Leonhard Reference Leonhard2002; Stone et al Reference Stone, van der Haegen, Luminet and Hirst2014), very few, so far, have tackled the articulation of memory at macro, meso, and micro levels over three generations (one of the rare examples being Welzer Reference Welzer2005).

In order to counter these limitations, we need more interconnected, ambitious approaches that strive to combine the methodological tools and analytical lenses of multiple disciplines to interpret the multifaceted concept that is collective memory. Within this article, we propose the hourglass metaphor to facilitate discussions across disciplines that wish to investigate the formation, transmission, and evolution of collective memories over time, as well as the interactions between the macro, meso, and micro levels.

Memory work as an hourglass

The malleability of memory is one of the main objects of consensus across all fields of memory studies. Memory functions in ‘a shadow land of nuance and subtlety’ (Storrie Reference Storrie2014), depending on the needs and requirements of the present. Indeed, the notion of collective memory is based on the interactions between public narratives of the past (whether presented by official representatives or historians) and individual memories (experienced and/or transmitted by the population) that operate according to their own logic but affect each other. Their interactions can be approached in a top-down manner, examining how the weight of the collective bears on the individual, or from a bottom-up perspective, by assuming that extant collective memory must take its root in the collection of individual memories (Hirst and Manier Reference Hirst and Manier2008). In this sense, we suggest that memory research can be seen as an hourglass, with the collective and the individual at opposite ends and the sand of memories passing from one to the other, depending on the way it has been turned (see Figure 1). In the next section, we will start by briefly describing the metaphor, before delving into each approach in more detail.

Figure 1. The hourglass metaphor.

Beyond the apt allegory of the passing of time visualised as the accumulation of sand, both the shape of the hourglass and its invertible nature can be used as a representation of our approach. At the top of the hourglass lie the historical, political, and social contexts (macro). This is a large area that combines an ensemble of predominant collective notions, beliefs, and perceptions of the past. In the centre, the narrow passage between the two bulbs represents the particular case of family memory (meso). It acts as a filter, a meeting point between the official (or public) history and the lived (or private, intimate) history. Its filtering capacity – represented by the width of the neck – changes depending on the peculiarities of each family, but also the evolution of the role of the family across different generations. At the bottom rests the individual (micro). However, the individual is not an empty receptacle. In this broad space lies the individuals’ idiosyncrasies, such as their personal beliefs, attitudes, or social identifications, all of which will influence the way they apprehend and reconstruct the past. But each grain of sand – each memory – collected will have a certain impact, direct or indirect, on the individual. This neatly reflects Halbwachs’ notion of memory (Reference Halbwachs1994 [1925]), where the individual is always dependent on the collective and the social framework in which it evolves.

Conversely, inverting the hourglass allows us to put the individual and their family at the top. After all, it is the individuals themselves – not the groups to which they belong – who remember, and it is the weight of their memories that is brought to bear on the way history and societies represent the past. When discussing memories of the First World War, Winter has insisted that political leaders and academic scholars do not shape historical narratives; rather families are the ones doing the work of remembrance (Winter Reference Winter2014, 166).

Finally, turning the hourglass on its side reveals its resemblance to the infinity symbol (∞), hinting to the never-ending loop between the two sides. Top-down and bottom-up influences interact continuously (Rothberg Reference Rothberg2009), through discussions but also through various media, both traditional and digital. Memory is never set in stone. It evolves and develops, depending on the availability of information, the perception of the generation, the identity, the needs, the goals, the functions, and the roles of the individual and the collective. As Wang said: ‘Just as one cannot separate a dance from the dancer, so one cannot separate a collective memory from the collective and its individual members who are the creators and carriers of the memory’ (Wang Reference Wang2008, 305).

Collective memory studies: Across disciplines and across generations

The hourglass metaphor suggests that, in order to obtain a well-rounded understanding of the interactions between the different levels (macro, meso, and micro), top-down and bottom-up approaches must be examined side-by-side. We argued above that the best way to tackle this challenge is to combine the expertise of multiple disciplines. For instance, whereas psychologists have excelled in developing paradigms to investigate the processes of memory through a bottom-up perspective, other fields of research in the humanities have established the importance of the roles played by the media, the elite, culture, and context in shaping collective memory and, thus, have mainly focused on top-down analyses.

Within the next section, we review what history, political science, and psychology can bring to the study of collective memory through the hourglass metaphor. It is important to note that by analysing these three branches of memory studies (that correspond to the disciplines of the authors), we do not wish to exclude any other discipline. Indeed, the contribution from other disciplines is highly valuable and welcome, but it would be pretentious of us to attempt to describe their expertise in the field of memory studies within the scope of this paper.

Looking through the hourglass: top-down approaches

Top-down approaches are generally interested in the content of collective memories shared across members of broad groups – the most studied one being the nation (Liu and Khan Reference Liu and Khan2021) – but also in the way these memories impact society and its individuals. They focus on what is common to the community depending on a group's characteristics and its cultural and historical context. The transmission of these collective memories generally occurs through traditional media, where the press can widely broadcast prevailing public opinions. Furthermore, collective memories can be reactivated or given more prominence depending on their presence in the media, for example, during times of commemoration (Kligler-Vilenchik et al Reference Kligler-Vilenchik, Tsfati and Meyers2014). This approach has been used extensively within history and political science, but also within social psychology.

Historians have argued that memory differs from history, but the former should not be viewed in direct opposition to the latter. In principle, history attempts to maintain an objective distance from its subject matter to explain past events through the scholarly analysis of mentalities and contexts. Memory, on the other hand, is inherently associated with social perceptions and beliefs within a group (Nora Reference Nora1984). Its objective is not to recount events as they happened, but to create or maintain an identity (Bedarida Reference Bedarida2003; Roekens Reference Roekens and van Ypersele2006). This does not mean, however, that historical scholarship is not informed by memory and vice versa (Noiriel Reference Noiriel2004). The selective and fluctuating character of memory should not be considered a negative attribute. It is, on the contrary, inherent to any situation in which a person resorts to memory: narratives of the past are constantly updated and adapted to present circumstances. Thus, following Pierre Nora's pioneering work, historians have interrogated the history of ‘collective memories’ as social representations of the past that have been maintained by communities of varying sizes. Using top-down approaches, they have endeavoured to identify the actors or social mechanisms responsible for shaping collective memories. For example, historians tracing ‘the history of representations’ or those working on ‘new cultural history’ examine how groups understand the world and events at a given time, and how their collective memories then impact the emotions and attitudes of group members. They analyse the power relations by reviewing how groups’ representations, practices, and discourses influence the identities of the ‘dominant’ and the ‘dominated’. In other words, they investigate how representations can be imposed from the top and also how individuals can modify and reclaim them (Chartier Reference Chartier2003; Veyne Reference Veyne2010). They do so through the analysis of a broad range of sources, including written, iconographic and artistic sources, and, more recently, online sources, such as social media.

Other scholars prefer to focus on official memory agents. In their seminal book The Invention of Tradition, Hobsbawm and Ranger (Reference Hobsbawm and Ranger1983) discussed how the elites of the nation actively manipulate collective narratives for their own purposes. Political authorities can attempt to structure memory transmitted privately through textbooks, monuments, or commemorations (Citron Reference Citron1987; Namer Reference Namer1983; Warland Reference Warland2019).

This instrumentalist approach to collective memory finds a similar resonance within the field of political sciences. Officials, including foreign policy actors, constantly reshape the meaning given to past events according to varying national and international circumstances (Rosoux Reference Rosoux2001, Reference Rosoux and Butler2019). Of course, the facts of the past cannot be altered. No one can undo or alter what has been done. But the meaning of what happened is never completely set in stone (Ricoeur Reference Ricoeur2000). A key concern in political science approaches has been the role of memory in conflict resolution. Most authors working in this field consider that only a change in the meaning given to the past can ultimately alleviate the emotional burden of painful past events (Bracka Reference Bracka, Belavusau and Gliszczynska-Grabias2017; Krondorfer Reference Krondorfer2018). In the absence of such changes, some groups appear to be characterised by ‘unfinished grief’ (Baussant Reference Baussant2006; Tison Reference Tison2011), reliving past events without end. What matters then is not to establish the truth with a capital ‘T’, but to elaborate narratives that have the potential to bring different groups closer together. The objective is to tell the story in another way, in other words, to tell it from the other's point of view (Ricoeur Reference Ricoeur2000). In this way, a plurality of memories can be established, turning the work of memory into the work of memories (see Irwin-Zarecka Reference Irwin-Zarecka2017; Olick Reference Olick2003; Zerubavel Reference Zerubavel, Olick, Adams and Steinmetz2003). This effort to open the past up to a plurality of representations appears to be the only way to elaborate a narrative that may not be shared by all but is ultimately ‘shareable’ (Passerini Reference Passerini2014).

Indeed, political scientists and, more broadly, experts in transitional justice often stress the importance of forms of recognition for past crimes, either through requests for forgiveness, official apologies or truth, and reconciliation commissions (Brooks Reference Brooks1999; Brophy Reference Brophy2006; Daase et al Reference Daase, Engert, Horelt, Renner and Strassner2016; Lind Reference Lind2008). Some argue in favour of a ‘reconstructive approach’ between representatives of various national groups in order to bring former adversaries together (Ferry Reference Ferry1996). This approach is based on actors assuming historical responsibility for past crimes in a critically measured manner. The official representatives of formerly war-torn communities are called upon to describe the wrongs suffered and committed, so that future generations can turn over the bloody pages of the past and work towards a shared future. From this perspective, the different types of media that reach and influence people widely (such as radio, television, and social media) are often seen as critical forms of communication that can help gradually develop a vision of the past which is acceptable to all sides (Gilboa Reference Gilboa2009).

Although much research has been carried on the subjects of transitional justice and reparation politics (e.g., Butt Reference Butt2008; de Greiff Reference De Greiff2006; Kritz Reference Kritz1995; Thompson Reference Thompson2002; Torpey Reference Torpey2006), one particular element remains under-analysed: the long-term effects of a critical assessment of the past. The negotiation processes that lead to the recognition or non-recognition of historical responsibility have been the subject of numerous analyses. But little in-depth research has been devoted to the impact of such gestures across not only one, but two or even three generations, even though all case studies demonstrate that the transformation of relationships between former enemies has to be understood in terms of generations, and not of years (Rosoux and Anstey Reference Rosoux and Anstey2017).

Finally, psychologists – and more specifically social psychologists – have also applied top-down approaches to their studies of collective memory mostly through the application of the social representation theory developed by Liu and Hilton (Reference Liu and Hilton2005). According to these authors, large groups – such as nations, political, cultural, and religious communities – define themselves through shared representations of their history. Collective memory not only provides a strong social identity for groups, but also influences their beliefs, perceptions, attitudes, and behaviours (Pennebaker et al Reference Pennebaker, Rimé and Paez2013; Tajfel and Turner Reference Tajfel, Turner, Worchel and Austin1986). In order to investigate these relationships, researchers using top-down approaches map out the social representations of history of specific groups by surveying a large sample of their members and then correlate these representations with the behaviours or attitudes expressed by the participants. For example, researchers have examined how the perceived victimisation of a group in history may still encourage its members to act in certain ways or hold negative attitudes towards other groups (e.g., Bar-Tal and Antebi Reference Bar-Tal and Antebi1992; Bouchat et al Reference Bouchat, Licata, Rosoux, Allesch, Ammerer, Bovina, Bruckmüller, Cabecinhas, Chryssochoou, Cohrs, Csertő, Delouvée, Durante, Ernst-Vintila, Flassbeck, Hilton, Kesteloot, Kislioglu, Krenn, Macovei, Mari, Petrovic, Pólya, Sá, Sakki, Turjacanin, van Ypersele, Volpato, Bilewicz and Klein2017).

Studies on social representations of history assert that collective memory serves three principal functions (Hirst et al Reference Hirst, Yamashiro and Coman2018; Liu and Hilton Reference Liu and Hilton2005). First, as already mentioned, sharing similar interpretations of the past helps create and maintain a feeling of belonging to the same group by providing examples of what the group is and how it should act. It thus serves the essential function of building social identity. The second function is social at its core. Through their shared representations of history, groups keep track of other groups they interact with and define these relationships: Are they friends or foes? What are the intergroup politics? The last function of collective memory is not only to provide frames of the interpretation of the world but also to help us make ‘better’ decisions in the future by learning from past mistakes.

Overall, communities draw on their histories to elaborate narratives that can unify them and guide their behaviour. In order to thrive, these narratives should have memorable characteristics, such as the presence of narrative patterns or distinctive protagonists (heroes, villains, or victims), and they also should be plausible, although factual accuracy is less important (László Reference László2008; Olick Reference Olick1999). They need to be widely broadcast through traditional media, while social and digital media have also become increasingly significant in allowing for the rapid and wide-ranging transmission of narratives. Some important events, such as foundational myths, can even become ‘historical charters’ that account for a group's historical mission but can be negotiated and amended over time often by the elites (Liu and Hilton Reference Liu and Hilton2005). It is, thus, through these top-down studies that we can perceive, evaluate, and investigate how the choice of the past weighs on the members of the group.

Looking through the hourglass: bottom-up approaches

Bottom-up approaches start with the individual and tend to focus on the processes that influence the creation, distribution, and the evolution of collective memories, as well as on the consequences of such memories have for the person. They question how the small, the insignificant, the discrete manages the weight of the past but also, at their own level, influences it. Especially since the rise of social media, individuals have found new platforms to counter dominant memories broadcast by mainstream media and foster new perceptions of the past (Birkner and Donk Reference Birkner and Donk2020).

Political scientists and historians have observed and analysed these discrepancies between official interpretations and individuals’ perceptions of an event (Margry and Sanchez-Carretero Reference Margry and Sanchez-Carretero2011). In this regard, the plurality of representations of the Algerian War of Independence is an emblematic example. This war led to the definitive secession of an integral part of the French territory and the exodus of an estimated one million refugees of European descent alongside pro-French Muslims to the other side of the Mediterranean Sea. From 1954 to 1962, more than two million French soldiers served in Algeria. Approximately 20,000 of them died, while hundreds of thousands of Algerians were killed during the war (Pervillé Reference Pervillé, Harbi and Stora2004). These figures show the tragic impact of the war on individuals from both sides of the Mediterranean. However, in the French strategic narrative, this devastating conflict remained a war without a name for almost four decades (Gacon Reference Gacon and Nicolaïdis1994). In refusing categorically to recognise the fighting in Algerian as a war, the French government caused a gap to develop between the sanitised narrative that was emphasised officially and public awareness of the violence that characterised the conflict. This example reminds us that, even if institutional actors can attempt to modify narratives of the past to fit their own objectives, the dissemination of a sole, monolithic historical interpretation cannot simply be imposed from above. Citizens exposed to official discourses are not merely empty vessels waiting to imbibe state-sponsored narratives without hesitation. Rather, they co-construct the messages conveyed to them.

In order to bring these alternative narratives of the past to light, oral historians have focused on what Assmann and Czaplicka (Reference Assmann and Czaplicka1995) termed ‘communicative memory’, which are collective memories based on everyday communications. These memories are neither formal nor stable, and they evolve and exist within individuals and their idiosyncrasies. Collections of oral testimonies can provide fascinating insights into how topics are represented and discussed within the broader population, as well as the specific emotions associated with them. According to Descamps, oral history is a useful tool to deal with the tension between memory and history, as it provides ‘a vivid, psychologically and affectively sensitive memory’ of an event alongside clear and rationalised historical knowledge of that same event (Descamps Reference Descamps2019, 85). This approach is clearly inspired by the pioneering work of scholars such as Thompson (Reference Thompson1963) and his concept of ‘History from Below’ and Ginzburg (Reference Ginzburg, Bentley, Subrahmanyam and Wiesner-Hanks2015), who advocated for historians to reduce their scale of observation and to favour ‘individual appropriations’ and ‘microstoria’. And now that these stories can be easily shared through social media, historians increasingly refer to the digital sphere to include these new sources in their analyses (Clavert Reference Clavert2021).

Experiences of trauma and tragedy have been a particularly fertile ground for the research of both historians (Audoin-Rouzeau Reference Audoin-Rouzeau2013; Conan and Rousso Reference Conan and Rousso2013; Tison Reference Tison2011; van Ypersele Reference van Ypersele and Horne2010) and political scientists (Cobb Reference Cobb2013; Cobban Reference Cobban2007; Hayner Reference Hayner2001; Pouligny et al Reference Pouligny, Chesterman and Schnabel2007). In public debate, issues related to the legacies of war are often described in a binary way, i.e. remembrance versus oblivion. Nevertheless, one of the most fundamental interrogations common to this topic is not whether to remember or forget, but how to remember and forget in order to move forward. From this perspective, the critical question is not only ‘what happened?’, but also – and above all – ‘what shall we do with the past?’. The challenge is immense. Despite the rich literature devoted to the topic, there is no standard list of specific techniques and ingredients for processing problematic past events and correcting historical injustices. However, there is a list of constraints that inevitably limit the room for manoeuvre. The most challenging obstacle is the irreversible nature of the crimes that were committed in the past. From this vantage point, it is worth questioning the notion of ‘post-conflict’. This concept is widely used in handbooks, dictionaries, and encyclopaedias, yet the duration of so-called ‘post-conflict’ contexts remains uncertain. What are the basic criteria that determine when a conflict is over? How long do the notions of victim and perpetrator make sense? When does the victors/vanquished dichotomy lose its meaning? Until when are the labels ‘occupiers/occupied’ relevant? All these questions intersect with individual and collective levels of memory. Case studies indicate that the answers given to this series of interrogations determine, to some extent, individual perceptions (in terms of self-esteem – or lack of self-esteem – and otherness), as well as political positions (in terms of national interest).

Beyond these important questions, cognitive psychologists have focused their research on understanding the processes that explain how individuals can influence and construct collective memories. In order to do so, they have applied methodologies previously used to evaluate interpersonal communication (generally at the dyadic level) to bigger groups of people. They postulate that psychological processes which occur at the micro level can have repercussions all the way to the macro level. Four main processes are generally studied from a bottom-up perspective: how memories are reinforced (usually through repetition across time, e.g., Roediger et al Reference Roediger, Zaromb, Butler, Boyer and Wertsch2009); how they are shared across networks (Coman et al Reference Coman, Momennejad, Drach and Geana2016); how they are manipulated (consciously or not) by other people or contaminated by information from other sources (Maswood and Rajaram Reference Maswood and Rajaram2019); and how they are forgotten (Abel and Bäuml Reference Abel and Bäuml2015; Cuc et al Reference Cuc, Koppel and Hirst2007). For example, researchers have applied a well-known psychological method used to evaluate how particular silences in individual recall can promote forgetting (the method is called retrieval-induced forgetting) in socially shared contexts (Coman et al Reference Coman, Manier and Hirst2009). Stone et al (Reference Stone, Luminet, Jay, Klein, Licata and Hirst2020) even tested this theory in a real-life situation by examining how the omission of particular topics from the speeches of public figures (in this case, the Belgian king) can actively promote collective forgetting.

One field of research that has been well developed in psychology using a bottom-up approach is that of flashbulb memories. Flashbulb memories are detailed memories of the reception context where one learns about an important, surprising and consequential public event (Brown and Kulik Reference Brown and Kulik1977; Luminet and Curci Reference Luminet and Curci2018). One of the main reasons why we form these flashbulb memories is essentially to be part of the collective. They allow us to align our lives with that of other members of the group and share memories even though we were not present at the event (Hirst and Meksin Reference Hirst, Meksin, Luminet and Curci2018). Social identity thus influences the formation and the retention of these particular memories (Cordonnier and Luminet Reference Cordonnier and Luminet2021), which, when shared, foster bonding across members of the groups. However, flashbulb memories can also be modified by the collective and the media. One famous example is that of George W. Bush's memory of when he first learnt about the 9/11 terrorist attacks (Greenberg Reference Greenberg2004). On separate occasions, he described watching the footage of the first plane hitting the tower on television before walking into a classroom, yet this memory cannot be true as there was no such footage available at the time. A likely explanation is that he integrated the vivid and memorable images of the first plane crash, which were widely broadcast in the media in the days following the attack, into his own memory. This example shows how suggestible our memory can be, but also how the media can influence it (Schacter Reference Schacter2022).

Looking through the hourglass: families and generations

One strength of the hourglass metaphor is to suggest an additional – yet important – layer to the study of collective memory: families and generations. Although families were central to Halbwachs’ model of collective memory (Reference Halbwachs1994 [1925]) and the Assmanns’ notion of communicative memory (Assmann Reference Assmann, Goodin and Tilly2009, Reference Assmann, Meusburger, Heffernan and Wunder2011), they have received far less attention than groups such as nations. Yet, they are important hubs of information and representations. Notably, through the sharing of family memories, parents, and grandparents convey to their (grand)children important moral and social values that are essential for the formation and maintenance of a cohesive family identity (Green Reference Green2019; Merrill et al Reference Merrill, Booker and Fivush2019; Pratt and Fiese Reference Pratt and Fiese2004). These values can act as filters for family members, which may lead them to question, reinterpret or reject information they receive. However, this filtering capacity of families depends on the perceived importance of their role in establishing values. Thus, they will differ depending on the culture of the family (Wang et al Reference Wang, Song and Kim Koh2017) as well as on the evolution of the role of family through time (Radica Reference Radica2013).

The notion of ‘generation’ has been studied by several sociologists (Mannheim Reference Mannheim and Kecskemeti1952; Pichler Reference Pichler1994). These sociologists generally focused on the concept of ‘social generations’ (also called cohorts), which included groups people born around the same time on the premise that because of their common historical–biographical past, they would have a shared world view and a generational consciousness (Scherger as cited in Timonen and Conlon Reference Timonen and Conlon2015). Yet, the concept of ‘family generations’ has been the subject of far less scrutiny in the field of collective memory, except for the large body of literature focusing on the multigenerational transmission of memories of the Holocaust. According to Hirsch (Reference Hirsch2012) – to take only one example – the notion of ‘postmemory’ describes ‘the relationship that the “generation after” bears to the personal, collective, and cultural trauma of those who came before’ (Hirsch Reference Hirsch2012, p. 5). So far, however, work on postmemory has mainly focused on literary accounts of intergenerational transmission rather than systematic assessments of the processes and mechanisms underlying such transmission. In recent years, scholars have begun to look beyond the Holocaust as an area of study of intergenerational mnemonic transmission and undertake more systematic studies of other events that may be less emotionally charged. Some of this work has included personal events from times of conflict which are clearly historically relevant. For instance, Svob and Brown (Reference Svob and Brown2012) found that, in Croatia, transmission of memories between generations was more likely to occur if the events described took place during a war. In his study of ‘third’ generation Germans, Welzer (Reference Welzer2005) found that grandchildren knew personal events from their grandfather's Second World War experiences, but they often misremembered them. This handful of studies demonstrates the originality and considerable potential of multi-disciplinary approaches to the intergenerational transmission of memories, especially given the current dearth of literature in this particular field.

In psychology, research on intergenerational transmission has thus far been restricted principally to values and trauma (Albert and Ferring Reference Albert and Ferring2012; Nauck Reference Nauck2001), with a few exceptions. Robyn Fivush and her team have closely examined family reminiscing and how it influences the way autobiographical memories are transformed into narratives of the self (Fivush Reference Fivush2008). They argue that ‘children are born into storied worlds’ (Fivush and Merrill Reference Fivush and Merrill2016, 308) and that family stories, informed by cultural master narratives, influence the way children and adolescents come to construct their view of themselves and the world (Fivush and Merrill Reference Fivush and Merrill2016; Fivush et al Reference Fivush, Habermas, Waters and Zaman2011). Similar to our perspective, this approach is interscalar. They propose that family stories can be divided into three ecological systems: micro (co-constructed narratives of shared events), exo (communicative family narratives) and macro (master narratives and cultural history). These different systems constantly interact and influence the construction of autobiographical memories. Yet, few other researchers in cognitive sciences have included family narratives within their studies of memory.

Finally, a few recent psychological studies have examined the intergenerational transmission of historical memories (Cordonnier et al Reference Cordonnier, Bouchat, Hirst and Luminet2021; Stone et al Reference Stone, van der Haegen, Luminet and Hirst2014). They found that overall, the transmission of family historical memories of Second World War-related events was very limited and that, by the third generation, most family memories were lost or stripped back to the general gist without many specific details. Yet, even if detail-rich memories are not transmitted easily, feelings of victimisation can seep from one generation to the next (Bouchat et al Reference Bouchat, Licata, Rosoux, Allesch, Ammerer, Bovina, Bruckmüller, Cabecinhas, Chryssochoou, Cohrs, Csertő, Delouvée, Durante, Ernst-Vintila, Flassbeck, Hilton, Kesteloot, Kislioglu, Krenn, Macovei, Mari, Petrovic, Pólya, Sá, Sakki, Turjacanin, van Ypersele, Volpato, Bilewicz and Klein2017).

Conclusion

The duration of the transmission processes observed raises several methodological questions. How can we assess the ‘effects’ and ‘after-effects’ of an important event for the community? How can we measure the transformation of representations from one generation to the next? How can we detect emotional and even unconscious processes? These questions underline the need to work with interdisciplinary teams of scholars, so that future research on collective memory can be studied from numerous angles, because as of now, different disciplines have mostly excelled in their own approaches. For a long time, historians have mostly focused on the macro and meso scales, leaving generally little space for the everyday person's role in the construction of collective memory. Political scientists have highlighted the functional role of memory for the community and shown how memory's malleability is a tool that can be used to achieve set goals. Psychologists, on the other hand, have mostly limited their scope to individuals and their personal memories, generally ignoring the broader cultural and societal contexts. In this paper, we have suggested to consider memory work as an hourglass, with the collective and the individual at two possible entry points and the sand of memories passing from one to the other, filtered through family memories, values, and representations. This metaphor poses several challenges for future research.

The first challenge is the need to examine side-by-side research from top-down and bottom-up perspectives. Indeed, as an hourglass needs to be turned upside-down from time to time, so does our understanding of the processes, content and context of collective memory. Interactions between the one and the plural are reciprocal: individuals always evolve within social frameworks, yet the collective is nothing without its members. One way to overcome this challenge is to work on long-term research projects that provide the necessary time to run different types of studies as well as to analyse their results side-by-side.

The second challenge relates to the importance of the passing of time in memory studies, and that this time should be counted in generations. On the one hand, social generations allow researchers to situate a person in time by specifying the social, political, and cultural context in which they have lived. It forces the researchers to take a non-normative approach by considering the evolution of the norms and the emotions associated with a particular event through time. On the other hand, family generations provide an estimation of the personal importance of the event for the individual. For this reason, we believe that studies on the intergenerational transmission of memory are essential to developing our understanding of collective memory.

The final challenge is to bridge discussions across disciplines in order to take advantage of the diversity of expertise in the field of memory studies. Scrutinising the creation, transmission, and evolution of collective memory requires a deep understanding of psychological processes, while being able to ground these within broader political, social, and historical contexts. It requires not only time, but also a form of intellectual curiosity open to examining a memory from other vantage points and, to a certain extent, a form of intellectual humility that accepts the theoretical and methodological shortcomings of one's own discipline.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Koen Aerts, Pierre Bouchat, Bruno De Wever, Chantal Kesteloot, Florence Rasmont, Laurence van Ypersele, Geneviève Warland, and Nico Wouters for their insightful contributions to the RE-Member project. We also thank Matthew Haultain-Gall for his careful edits to the text.

Funding

The authors are the members of a project titled ‘The transmission of memories related to stigmatisation: Official and family memories related to collaboration and colonisation in Belgium’ (RE-Member), funded by an UCLouvain Concerted Research Action grant (ARC 20/25-110). Valérie Rosoux and Olivier Luminet are both supported by the Fund for Scientific Research (FRS/FNRS).

Disclosure Statement

The authors declare that this manuscript is an original work and have not been published before, not is it being considered for publication elsewhere either in printed or electronic form. However, the original idea for this paper was presented in a grant proposal for an UCLouvain Concerted Research Action project titled ‘The transmission of memories related to stigmatisation: Official and family memories related to collaboration and colonisation in Belgium’ (RE-Member – ARC 20/25-110).

Competing Interest

The authors Aline Cordonnier, Valérie Rosoux, Anne-Sophie Gijs and Olivier Luminet declare that there are no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Aline Cordonnier is a postdoctoral researcher at UCLouvain. Her current research focuses on intergenerational transmission of family historical memories associated with the resistance or the collaboration during the Second World War, or the Colonization of the Congo. She is particularly interested in questions relating to memory functions and transmission processes.

Valérie Rosoux is a Research Director at the Belgian National Fund for Scientific Research (FNRS). She teaches International Negotiation and Transitional Justice at UCLouvain. She is a member of the Belgian Royal Academy. She has a Licence in Philosophy and a Ph.D. in Political Sciences.

Anne-Sophie Gijs, Ph.D. in History, is a Professor at UCLouvain. Her research focuses on colonial and postcolonial history, as well as on current interactions between the European Union and African countries. In this regard, she is interested in the dynamics between history and memory, and in processes of identity building.

Olivier Luminet is a research director at the Fund for Scientific Research and professor of psychology at UCLouvain, Belgium. His research examines the links between emotion, identity, and memories. He studies flashbulb memories and their impact on collective memory. He is co-editor of Flashbulb memories: New challenges and future perspectives (2018).

Footnotes

1 Other groups at the meso level could also be included, such as the role of specific associations or simply the impact of peers and friends. More recently, groups on social media have become an important source of memory transmission that should be included in the meso level. We choose to focus on the role of families for two main reasons: First, research carried out so far shows that families play a crucial role in transmitting emotions related to past political violence (Muxel Reference Muxel2002; see also project TRANSMEMO). Second, from a methodological perspective, the presence of families is more universal and more operationalisable than other potential variables.

References

Abel, M and Bäuml, KHT (2015) Selective memory retrieval in social groups: When silence is golden and when it is not. Cognition 140, 4048. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albert, I and Ferring, D (2012) Intergenerational value transmission within the family and the role of emotional relationship quality. Family Science 3, 412. doi:10.1080/19424620.2011.671496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Assmann, A (2009) Memory, individual and collective. In Goodin, RE and Tilly, C (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Contextual Political Analysis. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 210225.Google Scholar
Assmann, J (2011) Communicative and cultural memory. In Meusburger, P, Heffernan, M and Wunder, E (eds), Cultural Memories. The Geographical Point of View. Dordrecht: Springer, 1527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Assmann, A and Conrad, S (2010) Memory in a Global Age: Discourses, Practices and Trajectories. London: Palgrave.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Assmann, J and Czaplicka, J (1995) Collective memory and cultural identity. New German Critique 65, 125133. doi:10.2307/488538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Audoin-Rouzeau, S (2013) Cinq Deuils De Guerre, 1914–1918. Paris: Tallandier.Google Scholar
Bar-Tal, D and Antebi, D (1992) Beliefs about negative intentions of the world: A study of the Israeli siege mentality. Political Psychology 13, 633645. doi:10.2307/3791494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnier, AJ and Hoskins, A (2018) Is there memory in the head, in the wild? Memory Studies 11, 386390. doi:10.1177/1750698018806440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baussant, M (2002) Pieds-Noirs. Mémoires d'Exil. Paris: Stock.Google Scholar
Baussant, M (2006) Exils et construction de la mémoire généalogique: L'exemple des pieds-noirs. Pôle Sud 24, 2944. doi:10.3917/psud.024.0029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedarida, F (2003) Histoire, Critique et Responsabilité. Paris/Brussels: IHTP-CNRS/Complexe.Google Scholar
Bell, D (2010) Memory, Trauma and World Politics: Reflections on the Relationship between Past and Present. London: Palgrave.Google Scholar
Billaud, S, Gollac, S, Oeser, A and Pagis, J (eds) (2015) Histoires de Famille. Les Récits du Passé dans la Parenté Contemporaine. Paris: Editions de la rue d'Ulm.Google Scholar
Birkner, T and Donk, A (2020) Collective memory and social media: Fostering a new historical consciousness in the digital age? Memory Studies 13, 367383. doi:10.1177/1750698017750012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouchat, P, Licata, L, Rosoux, V, Allesch, C, Ammerer, H, Bovina, I, Bruckmüller, S, Cabecinhas, R, Chryssochoou, X, Cohrs, JC, Csertő, I, Delouvée, S, Durante, F, Ernst-Vintila, A, Flassbeck, C, Hilton, D, Kesteloot, C, Kislioglu, R, Krenn, A, Macovei, I, Mari, S, Petrovic, N, Pólya, T, , A, Sakki, I, Turjacanin, V, van Ypersele, L, Volpato, C, Bilewicz, M and Klein, O (2017) A century of victimhood: Antecedents and current impacts of victimization associated to World War I across Europe. European Journal of Social Psychology 47, 195208. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bracka, J (2017) From banning Nakba to bridging narratives: The collective memory of 1948 and transitional justice for Israelis and Palestinians. In Belavusau, U and Gliszczynska-Grabias, A (eds), Law and Memory: Towards Legal Governance of History. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 348373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R (1999) When Sorry isn't Enough: The Controversy over Apologies and Reparations for Human Injustice. New York: New York University Press.Google Scholar
Brophy, A (2006) Reparations, Pro & Con. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R and Kulik, J (1977) Flashbulb memories. Cognition 5, 7399. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(77)90018-X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buono, C (2004) Pieds-noirs de père en fils. Paris: Baland.Google Scholar
Butt, D (2008) Rectifying International Injustice. Principles of Compensation and Restitution between Nations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chartier, R (2003) La nouvelle histoire culturelle existe-t-elle ? Cahiers du Centre de Recherches Historique 31. doi:10.4000/ccrh.291.Google Scholar
Citron, S (1987) Le Mythe National. L'Histoire De France En Question. Paris: Editions ouvrières.Google Scholar
Clavert, F (2021) History in the era of massive data: Online social media as primary sources for historians. Geschichte und Gesellschaft 47, 175194. doi:10.13109/gege.2021.47.1.175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cobb, S (2013) Speaking of Violence. The Politics and Poetics of Narrative in Conflict Resolution. Oxford: OUP.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cobban, H (2007) Amnesty After Atrocity? Healing Nations After Genocide and War Crimes. Boulder: Paradigm Publishers.Google Scholar
Coman, A, Manier, D and Hirst, W (2009) Forgetting the unforgettable through conversation: Socially shared retrieval-induced forgetting of September 11 memories. Psychological Science 20, 627633. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02343.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coman, A, Momennejad, I, Drach, RD and Geana, A (2016) Mnemonic convergence in social networks: The emergent properties of cognition at a collective level. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113, 81718176. doi:10.1073/pnas.1525569113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conan, E and Rousso, H (2013) Vichy, un Passé Qui ne Passe pas. Paris: Fayard/Pluriel.Google Scholar
Cordonnier, A and Luminet, O (2021) Consistency and social identification: A test-retest study of flashbulb memories collected on the day of the 2016 Brussels bombings. Memory 29, 305318. doi:10.1080/09658211.2021.1891253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cordonnier, A, Barnier, AJ and Sutton, J (2018) Phenomenology in autobiographical thinking: Underlying features of prospection and retrospection. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice 5, 295311. doi:10.1037/cns0000156.Google Scholar
Cordonnier, A, Bouchat, P, Hirst, W and Luminet, O (2021) Intergenerational transmission of World War II family historical memories of the resistance. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 24, 302314. doi:10.1111/ajsp.12436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuc, A, Koppel, J and Hirst, W (2007) Silence is not golden: A case for socially shared retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychological Science 18, 727733. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01967.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daase, C, Engert, S, Horelt, M-A, Renner, J and Strassner, R (2016) Apology and Reconciliation in International Relations. The Importance of Being Sorry. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Davoine, F and Gaudillière, J-M (2006) Histoire et Trauma. La Folie des Guerres. Paris: Stock.Google Scholar
De Greiff, P (2006) The Handbook of Reparations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Descamps, F (2019) Archiver la Mémoire. De l'Histoire Orale au Patrimoine Immatériel. Paris: EHESS.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferry, J-M (1996) L’Éthique Reconstructive. Paris: Cerf.Google Scholar
Fivush, R (2008) Remembering and reminiscing: How individual lives are constructed in family narratives. Memory Studies 1, 4958. doi:10.1177/1750698007083888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R and Merrill, N (2016) An ecological systems approach to family narratives. Memory Studies 9, 305313. doi:10.1177/1750698016645264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R, Bohanek, JG and Duke, M (2008) The intergenerational self: Subjective perspective and family history. In Sani, F (ed.), Individual and Collective Self-Continuity. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 131143.Google Scholar
Fivush, R, Habermas, T, Waters, TEA and Zaman, W (2011) The making of autobiographical memory: Intersections of culture, narratives and identity. International Journal of Psychology 46, 321345. doi:10.1080/00207594.2011.596541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foong Khong, Y (1992) Analogies at War: Korea, Munich, Dien Bien Phu, and the Vietnam Decisions of 1965. Princeton: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gacon, S (1994) L'oubli institutionnel. In Nicolaïdis, D (ed), Oublier Nos Crimes. L'amnésie Nationale, une Spécificité Française ? Paris: Editions Autrement, 98111.Google Scholar
Gilboa, E (2009) Media and Conflict Resolution: A Framework for Analysis. Marquette Law Review 93, 87110.Google Scholar
Ginzburg, C (2015) Microhistory and world history. In Bentley, JH, Subrahmanyam, S and Wiesner-Hanks, M (eds), The Cambridge World History, vi, The Construction of a Global World, 1400–1800 CE. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 446473.Google Scholar
Gluck, C (2007) Operations of memory: ‘Comfort Women’ and the World. In Jager, SM and Mitter, R (eds), Ruptured Histories: War, Memory, and the Post-Cold War in Asia. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, pp. 4777.Google Scholar
Green, A (2019) Grandparents, communicative memory and narrative identity. Oral History 47, 8191.Google Scholar
Greenberg, DL (2004) President Bush's False ‘Flashbulb’ Memory of 9/11/01. Applied Cognitive Psychology 18, 363370. doi:10.1002/acp.1016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halbwachs, M (1994 [1925]) Les Cadres Sociaux De La Mémoire. Paris: Albin Michel.Google Scholar
Halbwachs, M (1997 [1950]) La Mémoire Collective. Paris: Albin Michel.Google Scholar
Hayner, P (2001) Unspeakable Truths: Confronting State Terror and Atrocity. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Heinrich, HA and Weyland, V (2016) Communicative and cultural memory as a micro-meso-macro relation. International Journal of Media & Cultural Politics 12, 2741. doi:10.1386/macp.12.1.27_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirsch, M (2012) The Generation of Postmemory: Writing and Visual Culture After the Holocaust. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Hirst, W and Manier, D (2008) Towards a psychology of collective memory. Memory 16, 183200. doi:10.1080/09658210701811912.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirst, W and Meksin, R (2018) Aligning flashbulb and collective memories. In Luminet, O and Curci, A (eds), Flashbulb Memories: New Challenges and Future. London: Perspectives Psychology Press, pp. 201218.Google Scholar
Hirst, W, Yamashiro, JK and Coman, A (2018) Collective memory from a psychological perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22, 438451. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2018.02.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobsbawm, E and Ranger, T (eds) (1983) The Invention of Tradition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Irwin-Zarecka, I (2017) Frames of Remembrance: The Dynamics of Collective Memory. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joutard, J (1977) La Légende des Camisards. Une Sensibilité au Passé. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
Keightley, E, Pickering, M and Bisht, P (2019) Interscalarity and the memory spectrum. In Maurantonio, N and Parks, DW (eds), Communicating Memory & History. Peter Lang, 1738. doi:10.3726/b14522.Google Scholar
Kensinger, E (2009) Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emotion Review 1(2), 99113. doi:10.1177/1754073908100432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, M (2016) Routledge Handbook of Memory and Reconciliation in East Asia. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kligler-Vilenchik, N, Tsfati, Y and Meyers, O (2014) Setting the collective memory agenda: Examining mainstream media influence on individuals’ perceptions of the past. Memory Studies 7, 484499. doi:10.1177/1750698014523443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kritz, N (1995) Transitional Justice: How Emerging Democracies Reckon with Former Regimes. Washington, DC: United States Institute of Peace.Google Scholar
Krondorfer, B (2018) Reconciliation in Global Context. New York: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Kurze, A and Lamont, C (2019) New Critical Spaces in Transitional Justice. Gender, Art, and Memory. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Langenbacher, E and Shain, Y (2010) Power and the Past. Collective Memory and International Relations. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
László, J (2008) The Science of Stories: An Introduction to Narrative Psychology. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lavabre, M-C (1994) Le Fil Rouge. Sociologie De La Mémoire Communiste. Paris: Presses de la Fondation nationale des sciences politiques.Google Scholar
Leonhard, N (2002) Politik- Und Geschichtsbewusstsein Im Wandel. Die Politische Bedeutung Der Nationalsozialis- Tischen Vergangenheit Im Verlauf Von Drei Generationen In Ost- Und Westdeutschland. Münster: Lit.Google Scholar
Lind, J (2008) Sorry States: Apologies in International Politics. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Liu, JH and Hilton, DJ (2005) How the past weighs on the present: Social representations of history and their role in identity politics. British Journal of Social Psychology 44, 537556. doi:10.1348/014466605X27162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, JH and Khan, SS (2021) If the past weighs on the present, then the present also weighs on the past: Collective remembering as an open system for human science. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 24, 263271. doi:10.1111/ajsp.12494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luminet, O and Curci, A (2018) Flashbulb Memories: New Challenges and Future Perspectives. London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mannheim, K (1952) The sociological problem of generations. In Kecskemeti, P (ed), Karl Mannheim: Essays. London: Routledge, 276322.Google Scholar
Margry, PJ and Sanchez-Carretero, C (2011) Grassroots Memorials. The Politics of Memorializing Traumatic Death. New York: Berghahn Books.Google Scholar
Maswood, R and Rajaram, S (2019) Social transmission of false memory in small group and large networks. Topics in Cognitive Science 11, 687709. doi:10.1111/tops.12348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merrill, N, Booker, JA and Fivush, R (2019) Functions of parental intergenerational narratives told by young people. Topics in Cognitive Science 11, 752773. doi:10.1111/tops.12356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muxel, A (2002) La participation politique des jeunes: Soubresauts, fractures et ajustements. Revue Française De Science Politique 52, 521544. doi:10.3917/rfsp.525.0521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Namer, G (1983) Batailles Pour la Mémoire. Commémorations En France De 1945 À Nos Jours. Paris: Papyrus.Google Scholar
Nauck, B (2001) Intercultural contact and intergenerational transmission in immigrant families. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 32, 159173. doi:10.1177/0022022101032002004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noiriel, G (2004) Histoire, mémoire, engagement civique. Hommes Et Migrations 1247, 1726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nora, P (1978) Mémoire collective. In Goff, JL, Charlier, R and Revel, J (eds), La Nouvelle Histoire. Paris: C.E.P.L., 398401.Google Scholar
Nora, P (1984) Les Lieux de Mémoire, I, La République. Paris: Gallimard, XIX.Google Scholar
Olick, JK (1999) Collective memory: The two cultures. Sociological Theory 17, 333348. doi:10.1111/0735-2751.00083.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olick, JK (2003) States of Memory: Continuities, Conflict and Transformations in National Retrospection. Durham: Duke University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olick, JK, Vinitzky-Seroussi, V and Levy, D (2011) The Collective Memory Reader. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Passerini, L (2014) Affective narratives for Europe (Brussels, 9 April 2014). In Madame de Stael Prize for Cultural Values, vol. 37. Berlin: ALLEA. https://doczz.net/doc/8613454/luisa-passerini--affectivenarratives-for-europe-speech-h.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, JW, Rimé, B and Paez, D (eds) (2013) Collective Memory of Political Events: Social Psychological Perspectives. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pervillé, G (2004) La guerre d'Algérie : Combien de morts? In Harbi, M and Stora, B (eds), La Guerre d'Algérie, 1954–2004. La Fin de l'Amnistie. Paris: Laffont, 477493.Google Scholar
Pichler, J (1994) Mannheim's sociology of generations: An undervalued legacy. British Journal of Sociology 45, 481495.Google Scholar
Pouligny, B, Chesterman, S and Schnabel, A (2007) After Mass Crime. Rebuilding States and Communities. New York: United Nations University.Google Scholar
Pratt, MW and Fiese, BH (2004) Families, Stories, and the Life Course: An Ecological Context. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radica, G (2013) Philosophie de la Famille. Communauté, Normes et Pouvoirs. Paris: Vrin.Google Scholar
Resende, E and Budryte, D (2014) Memory and Trauma in International Relations. Theories, Cases and Debates. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ricoeur, P (2000) La mémoire, l'Histoire, l'Oubli. Paris: Seuil.Google Scholar
Roediger, HL, Zaromb, FM and Butler, AC (2009) The role of repeated retrieval in shaping collective memory. In Boyer, P and Wertsch, JV (eds), Memory in Mind and Culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2958.Google Scholar
Roekens, A (2006) Les identités collectives, l'apport des sciences sociales. In van Ypersele, L (ed), Histoire Culturelle Et Conflits Contemporains. Quelques Outils Conceptuels À L'usage Des Jeunes Chercheurs. Paris: PUF, 7788.Google Scholar
Rosenblum, R (2009) Postponing trauma: The dangers of telling. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis 90, 13191340. doi:10.1111/j.1745-8315.2009.00171.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosoux, V (2001) Les Usages de la Mémoire en Relations Internationales. Brussels: Bruylant.Google Scholar
Rosoux, V (2019) The role of memory in inter-societal conflicts. In Butler, M (ed), Revisiting Securitization: Contemporary Applications and Insights. New York: Routledge, 194216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosoux, V and Anstey, M (2017) Negotiating Reconciliation in Peacemaking. Cham: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothberg, M (2009) Multidirectional Memory. Standford: Standford University Press.Google Scholar
Schacter, DL (2022) Media, technology, and the sins of memory. Memory, Mind & Media 1, e1, 1–15. doi:10.1017/mem.2021.3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schacter, DL, Addis, DR and Buckner, RL (2007) Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8, 657661. doi:10.1038/nrn2213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stone, CB, van der Haegen, A, Luminet, O and Hirst, W (2014) Personally relevant vs. nationally relevant memories: An intergenerational examination of World War II memories across and within Belgian French-speaking families. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 3, 280286. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.08.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stone, CB, Luminet, O, Jay, AC, Klein, O, Licata, L and Hirst, W (2020) Do public speeches induce “collective” forgetting? The Belgian King's 2012 summer speech as a case study. Memory Studies. doi:1177/1750698019900949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storrie, B (2014) Serbia – Back to the Future. Transconflict, 16 October. Available at http://www.transconflict.com/2014/10/serbia-back-future-160/.Google Scholar
Svob, C and Brown, N (2012) Intergenerational transmission of the reminiscence bump and biographical conflict knowledge. Psychological Science 23, 14041409. doi:10.1177/0956797612445316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tajfel, H and Turner, JC (1986) The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In Worchel, S and Austin, WG (eds), The Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Chicago: Nelson Hall, pp. 724.Google Scholar
Thompson, EP (1963) The Making of the English Working Class. London: Victor Gollancz Ltd.Google Scholar
Thompson, J (2002) Taking Responsibility for the Past. Cambridge: Polity.Google Scholar
Timonen, V and Conlon, C (2015) Beyond Mannheim: Conceptualising how people ‘talk’ and ‘do’ generations in contemporary society. Advances in Life Course Research 24, 19. doi:10.1016/j.alcr.2015.03.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tison, S (2011) Comment Sortir de la Guerre? Deuil, Mémoire et Traumatisme. 1870–1940. Rennes: Presses universitaires de Rennes.Google Scholar
Todorov, T (1995) Les Abus de la Mémoire. Paris: Arléa.Google Scholar
Torpey, J (2006) Making Whole What Has Been Smashed: On Reparations Politics. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Van de Putte, T (2021) “Let me tell you what we already know”: Collective memory between culture and interaction. Memory Studies, 116. doi:10.1177/17506980211044709.Google Scholar
van Ypersele, L (2010) Mourning and memory, 1919–1945. In Horne, J (ed), A Companion to World War I. London: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 576590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veyne, P (2010) His Thought, His Character. Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Wang, Q (2008) On the cultural constitution of collective memory. Memory 16, 305317. doi:10.1080/09658210701801467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Q, Song, Q and Kim Koh, JB (2017) Culture, memory, and narrative self-making. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 37, 199223. doi:10.1177/0276236617733827.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warland, G (2019) Experience and Memory of the First World War in Belgium. Comparative and Interdisciplinary Insights. Munster: Waxmann Verlag.Google Scholar
Welzer, H (2005) Grandpa wasn't a Nazi: The Holocaust in German Family Remembrance. New York: American Jewish Committee.Google Scholar
Winter, J (2001) The generation of memory: Reflections on the ‘memory boom’ in contemporary historical studies. Canadian Military History 10, 5766.Google Scholar
Winter, J (2014) Commemorating catastrophe: Remembering the Great War 100 years on. Materiaux pour l'Histoire de notre Temps 1, 166174. doi:10.3917/mate.113.0166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zerubavel, E (2003) Calendars and history: A comparative study of the social organization of national memory. In Olick, J, Adams, J and Steinmetz, G (eds), States of Memory. Durham: Duke University Press, pp. 315338.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. The hourglass metaphor.