Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T20:18:01.907Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

White matter, cognition and psychotic-like experiences in UK Biobank

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 November 2021

M. J. Bosma*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
S. R. Cox
Affiliation:
School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
T. Ziermans
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
C. R. Buchanan
Affiliation:
School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
X. Shen
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Edinburgh, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Edinburgh, Scotland, UK
E. M. Tucker-Drob
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, USA
M. J. Adams
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Edinburgh, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Edinburgh, Scotland, UK
H. C. Whalley
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Edinburgh, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Edinburgh, Scotland, UK
S. M. Lawrie
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Edinburgh, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Edinburgh, Scotland, UK
*
Author for correspondence: M. J. Bosma, E-mail: minke.bosma@student.uva.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Psychotic-like experiences (PLEs) are risk factors for the development of psychiatric conditions like schizophrenia, particularly if associated with distress. As PLEs have been related to alterations in both white matter and cognition, we investigated whether cognition (g-factor and processing speed) mediates the relationship between white matter and PLEs.

Methods

We investigated two independent samples (6170 and 19 891) from the UK Biobank, through path analysis. For both samples, measures of whole-brain fractional anisotropy (gFA) and mean diffusivity (gMD), as indications of white matter microstructure, were derived from probabilistic tractography. For the smaller sample, variables whole-brain white matter network efficiency and microstructure were also derived from structural connectome data.

Results

The mediation of cognition on the relationships between white matter properties and PLEs was non-significant. However, lower gFA was associated with having PLEs in combination with distress in the full available sample (standardized β = −0.053, p = 0.011). Additionally, lower gFA/higher gMD was associated with lower g-factor (standardized β = 0.049, p < 0.001; standardized β = −0.027, p = 0.003), and partially mediated by processing speed with a proportion mediated of 7% (p = < 0.001) for gFA and 11% (p < 0.001) for gMD.

Conclusions

We show that lower global white matter microstructure is associated with having PLEs in combination with distress, which suggests a direction of future research that could help clarify how and why individuals progress from subclinical to clinical psychotic symptoms. Furthermore, we replicated that processing speed mediates the relationship between white matter microstructure and g-factor.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Psychotic experiences are not only symptoms of clinically severe psychiatric disorders like schizophrenia, but also occur in the general population with an estimated prevalence of 5–10%, in which case they can be called psychotic-like experiences (PLEs). Although there is variability in the definition of PLEs (Seiler, Nguyen, Yung, & O'Donoghue, Reference Seiler, Nguyen, Yung and O'Donoghue2020), they generally refer to subclinical hallucinations or delusions. Unlike clinical psychotic symptoms, PLEs are usually transient and often not associated with distress. However, people who have PLEs are at higher risk for schizophrenia and other mental health disorders, especially if they are accompanied by distress (Hanssen, Bak, Bijl, Vollebergh, & van Os, Reference Hanssen, Bak, Bijl, Vollebergh and van Os2005). Elucidating the pathophysiology of PLEs with and without distress could therefore help to understand the development of psychotic symptoms and disorders, and inform measures to prevent the transition from subclinical to clinical symptoms.

White matter disturbances in schizophrenia have been consistently reported, with studies finding reduced white matter integrity and connectivity in the whole brain (Alloza et al., Reference Alloza, Bastin, Cox, Gibson, Duff, Semple and Lawrie2017; Ashtari et al., Reference Ashtari, Cottone, Ardekani, Cervellione, Szeszko, Wu and Kumra2007; Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Jahanshad, Zalesky, Kochunov, Agartz, Alloza and Donohoe2018; Skudlarski et al., Reference Skudlarski, Jagannathan, Anderson, Stevens, Calhoun, Skudlarska and Pearlson2010, Reference Skudlarski, Schretlen, Thaker, Stevens, Keshavan, Sweeney and Pearlson2013) and in specific tracts (Burns et al., Reference Burns, Job, Bastin, Whalley, Macgillivray, Johnstone and Lawrie2003; McIntosh et al., Reference McIntosh, Maniega, Lymer, McKirdy, Hall, Sussmann and Lawrie2008). Studies have also shown white matter integrity deficits in at-risk populations (Cooper, Alm, Olson, & Ellman, Reference Cooper, Alm, Olson and Ellman2018; Muñoz Maniega et al., Reference Muñoz Maniega, Lymer, Bastin, Marjoram, Job, Moorhead and Lawrie2008; Roalf et al., Reference Roalf, de la Garza, Rosen, Calkins, Moore, Quarmley and Gur2019; Tang et al., Reference Tang, Pasternak, Kubicki, Rathi, Zhang, Wang and Seidman2019), which include people that do not yet have schizophrenia but experience attenuated symptoms (Cornblatt et al., Reference Cornblatt, Lencz, Smith, Correll, Auther and Nakayama2003; Klosterkötter & Schultze-Lutter, Reference Klosterkötter and Schultze-Lutter2000). Other research has also found relationships between having PLEs and widespread functional dysconnectivity, altered white matter microstructure or decreased FA in the auditory hemispheric pathway (Oestreich, Randeniya, & Garrido, Reference Oestreich, Randeniya and Garrido2019; O'Hanlon et al., Reference O'Hanlon, Leemans, Kelleher, Clarke, Roddy, Coughlan and Cannon2015; Orr, Turner, & Mittal, Reference Orr, Turner and Mittal2014). Furthermore, Schoorl et al. (Reference Schoorl, Barbu, Shen, Harris, Adams, Whalley and Lawrie2021) found a significant association between localized white matter integrity in the splenium of the corpus callosum and a specific PLE (visual hallucinatory experiences), especially when accompanied by distress. Connectome approaches to the study of cognition and PLEs have not yet delivered any consistent results.

Importantly, reduced white matter quality has also been linked to diminished cognitive ability in the healthy population, people with first-episode psychosis and in people with chronic schizophrenia (Alloza et al., Reference Alloza, Bastin, Cox, Gibson, Duff, Semple and Lawrie2017; Holleran et al., Reference Holleran, Kelly, Alloza, Agartz, Andreassen, Arango and Donohoe2020; Penke et al., Reference Penke, Mañiega, Bastin, Hernández, Murray, Royle and Deary2012a, Reference Penke, Muñ Oz Maniega, Valdé, Herná Ndez, Murray, Royle, Starr and Dearyb; Pérez-Iglesias et al., Reference Pérez-Iglesias, Tordesillas-Gutiérrez, McGuire, Barker, Roiz-Santiañez, Mata and Crespo-Facorro2010; Power et al., Reference Power, Su, Wu, Reid, Jack, Knopman and Mosley2019; Voineskos et al., Reference Voineskos, Felsky, Kovacevic, Tiwari, Zai, Mallar Chakravarty and Kennedy2013). Impaired cognitive functioning in domains like memory and executive functions is a core problem in schizophrenia, but also occurs in at-risk populations and in healthy people who report PLEs (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Deste, Smieskova, Barlati, Yung, Howes and Borgwardt2012; Mohamed, Paulsen, O'Leary, Arndt, & Andreasen, Reference Mohamed, Paulsen, O'Leary, Arndt and Andreasen1999; Whyte et al., Reference Whyte, Brett, Harrison, Byrne, Miller, Lawrie and Johnstone2006). Furthermore, longitudinal studies have shown that cognitive deficits are present in people who will later report PLEs or develop schizophrenia (Barnett et al., Reference Barnett, McDougall, Xu, Croudace, Richards and Jones2012; Khandaker, Barnett, White, & Jones, Reference Khandaker, Barnett, White and Jones2011; MacCabe, Reference MacCabe2008).

The quality of white matter can be assessed through its microstructure, which can be estimated with diffusion MRI (dMRI). In white matter, the directional coherence of water molecular diffusion (known as fractional anisotropy, or FA) is affected by multiple microstructural properties, including axonal myelination, fiber density and axonal ordering (Jones, Knösche, & Turner, Reference Jones, Knösche and Turner2013). An often used complementary estimate of microstructure is mean diffusivity (MD), an inverse measure of membrane density (Alexander et al., Reference Alexander, Hurley, Samsonov, Adluru, Hosseinbor, Mossahebi and Field2011). In addition, the connectome approach provides a more detailed pattern of brain connectivity beyond the major white matter tracts. A whole-brain structural connectome can be constructed in vivo from dMRI, resulting in a network formed from nodes (brain areas) and edges (white matter tracts) (Sotiropoulos & Zalesky, Reference Sotiropoulos and Zalesky2019). Aspects of network topology can then be computed using graph theory, a technique which characterizes a variety of global properties from these complex networks (Bullmore & Sporns, Reference Bullmore and Sporns2009). It has been proposed that the brain functions effectively because these nodes and edges have the properties of a small-world network, which means that the brain is highly integrated but also maintains some level of segregation (Gong et al., Reference Gong, He, Concha, Lebel, Gross, Evans and Beaulieu2009; Griffa, Baumann, Thiran, & Hagmann, Reference Griffa, Baumann, Thiran and Hagmann2013). This high level of integration is thought to be reduced in schizophrenia, often called a ‘dysconnectivity’ disorder. In accordance, several studies have found that a graph-theory measure of network efficiency, called global efficiency, was reduced in participants with schizophrenia compared to controls (Griffa et al., Reference Griffa, Baumann, Thiran and Hagmann2013).

Reduced network efficiency is also associated with less effective cognitive functioning – in terms of both general cognitive ability (Li et al., Reference Li, Liu, Li, Qin, Li, Yu and Jiang2009; Van Den Heuvel, Stam, Kahn, & Hulshoff Pol, Reference Van Den Heuvel, Stam, Kahn and Hulshoff Pol2009) and processing speed (Pineda-Pardo, Martínez, Román, & Colom, Reference Pineda-Pardo, Martínez, Román and Colom2016; Wen et al., Reference Wen, Zhu, He, Kochan, Reppermund, Slavin and Sachdev2011). Moreover, processing speed is a key mechanism for higher-order cognition and therefore general cognitive ability (Kail & Salthouse, Reference Kail and Salthouse1994). Indeed, studies have shown that the relationship between white matter integrity and general intelligence is (partially) mediated by processing speed in healthy participants (Penke et al., Reference Penke, Mañiega, Bastin, Hernández, Murray, Royle and Deary2012a, Reference Penke, Muñ Oz Maniega, Valdé, Herná Ndez, Murray, Royle, Starr and Dearyb) and in people with schizophrenia (Alloza et al., Reference Alloza, Cox, Duff, Semple, Bastin, Whalley and Lawrie2016).

Given the well-established relationships between white matter and both processing speed and general cognitive functioning, the observation of reduced cognitive functioning in people with PLEs and the previously found association between white matter microstructure and PLEs, we sought to investigate a comprehensive framework of the relationships between white matter, cognition and PLEs. We hypothesized that general cognitive ability mediates the relationship between white matter microstructure or network efficiency and reporting PLEs. Further, aiming to replicate the findings of Penke et al. (Reference Penke, Mañiega, Bastin, Hernández, Murray, Royle and Deary2012a, Reference Penke, Muñ Oz Maniega, Valdé, Herná Ndez, Murray, Royle, Starr and Dearyb), we hypothesized that processing speed acts as an intermediate mediator between white matter properties (white matter microstructure and network efficiency) and general cognitive ability. These hypotheses were investigated through path analysis, first in a population-based sample of N = 6170 participants who had detailed (connectomic) data available. We then tried to replicate these findings with less detailed white matter indicates in a larger population-based sample of N = 19 891 participants. The model that was tested can be seen in Fig. 1. The preregistration of this study can be found on https://osf.io/2jkty and deviations from this protocol are reported in the supplementary materials.

Fig. 1. Path diagram of the model that was tested. Note. Path diagram specifies that the association between ostensibly poorer (higher MD, lower FA and lower global efficiency) white matter metrics and having PLEs is mediated via processing speed and general cognitive ability. Though prior work suggests that processing speed may underpin g, we also allow for a separate independent path directly from speed to PLEs.

Methods

Sample

The data were provided by UK Biobank (UKB), a cohort of approximately 500 000 middle-aged and older community-dwelling participants from the United Kingdom and Northern Ireland, under project number 4844 linked to projects 10 279 and 16 124. More information on the recruitment of participants and their sociodemographic and health-related characteristics can be found in Fry et al. (Reference Fry, Littlejohns, Sudlow, Doherty, Adamska, Sprosen and Allen2017) and on http://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/key-documents/. UKB has received ethical approval from the Research Ethics Committee (reference 11/NW/0382) and all participants provided informed consent.

A head MRI scan was available in a subset of participants. 6170 of these participants completed the online Mental Health Questionnaire and had individual connectomes constructed following the methods of Buchanan et al. (Reference Buchanan, Bastin, Ritchie, Liewald, Madole, Tucker-Drob and Cox2020). This sample will henceforth be referred to as the connectome sample. Twenty of the participants in this sample reported having a psychotic disorder (F20 – F29 in ICD-10). The results of all analyses reported below are excluding these individuals, but analyses with these individuals included are reported in online Supplementary Tables S11–S16, S19, S20, S23, S24 in the supplementary materials.

To try to replicate any findings in a larger sample, we also performed all analyses on participants who had UKB processed diffusion MRI (dMRI) data available from a head MRI scan and who completed the Mental Health Questionnaire. This resulted in a sample of N = 25 945 participants with dMRI but no connectome data. Since this sample overlapped with the connectome sample, we removed all participants that were also in the connectome sample. This resulted in a sample of N = 19 891, which we will henceforth call the dMRI sample. Seventy seven of the participants in this sample reported having a psychotic disorder (F20 – F29 in ICD-10). See Fig. 2 for a flowchart of the participants.

Fig. 2. Participant flow chart.

Materials

Details about the exact data fields and a link to the corresponding data dictionary can be found in the supplementary methods, online Supplementary Table S1.

Cognitive tests

To assess general intelligence and processing speed, we used results from the UKB cognitive battery (for a description of the used tasks, see supplementary materials). This battery does not include any standardized tests, but a recent study concluded that their concurrent validity and short-term test-retest reliability was moderate to good (Fawns-Ritchie & Deary, Reference Fawns-Ritchie and Deary2020). The results regarding the validity and reliability of the measures that were used can be found in the supplementary methods. We used the score on the UK Biobank reaction task (RT, field ID 20023 http://biobank.ndph.ox.ac.uk/showcase/field.cgi?id=20023) to assess processing speed. To create a measure of general cognitive ability, the test scores on UKB pairs memory (field ID 399, http://biobank.ndph.ox.ac.uk/showcase/field.cgi?id=399), prospective memory (field ID 20018, http://biobank.ndph.ox.ac.uk/showcase/field.cgi?id=20018), fluid intelligence (field ID 20016, http://biobank.ndph.ox.ac.uk/showcase/field.cgi?id=20016) and numeric memory (field ID 4282, http://biobank.ndph.ox.ac.uk/showcase/field.cgi?id=4282) were entered in a principal component analysis (PCA).

Psychotic-like experiences

PLEs were assessed with a Mental Health Questionnaire (MHQ; UKBiobank Category: 144. Four different questions asked whether participants had ever had a visual or auditory hallucination, persecutory delusion or delusion of reference. We merged the people who answered yes to at least one of these questions into one group, and thus ended up with two groups: participants with at least one PLE and participants without any PLE. Finally, the participants were asked how distressing they found their experience, to create a variable that was used in the exploratory analyses. Details of this questionnaire can be found in the supplementary methods, online Supplementary Table S2.

Imaging protocol and connectome derived variables

T1 weighted and diffusion MRI images were acquired using a Siemens Skyra 3 T scanner. More detailed information on imaging acquisition, preprocessing and quality control can be found in UK Biobank (2014) and in Alfaro-Almagro et al. (Reference Alfaro-Almagro, Jenkinson, Bangerter, Andersson, Griffanti, Douaud and Smith2018). Cortical reconstruction and segmentation were conducted in-house using FreeSurfer 5.3 with default settings, and visual quality control procedures to identify and remove major processing fails, skull strip errors and tissue segmentation issues. Each T1 weighted image was segmented into 85 regions of interest (ROI). Cortical ROIs were consequently identified according to the Desikan−Killiany atlas (Desikan et al., Reference Desikan, Ségonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson, Blacker and Killiany2006), resulting in 34 cortical structures per hemisphere. Eight subcortical gray matter ROIs per hemisphere and the brain stem were also identified based on Fischl et al. (Reference Fischl, Salat, Busa, Albert, Dieterich, Haselgrove and Dale2002).

Subsequently, brain structural connectomes were derived. Whole-brain probabilistic tractography was performed and weighted connectomes were constructed by identifying all connections between ROIs. The network weightings were computed for FA and MD and recorded in 85 × 85 connectivity matrices. Further information on the construction of the connectomes can be found in Buchanan et al. (Reference Buchanan, Bastin, Ritchie, Liewald, Madole, Tucker-Drob and Cox2020). We applied a consistency thresholding method (Roberts, Perry, Roberts, Mitchell, & Breakspear, Reference Roberts, Perry, Roberts, Mitchell and Breakspear2017) to retain 30% of all connections with weights that were consistent across subjects (Buchanan et al., Reference Buchanan, Bastin, Ritchie, Liewald, Madole, Tucker-Drob and Cox2020).

The weights of all individual network connections which survived the consistency threshold were entered in a PCA to obtain general FA (gFA) and general MD (gMD) as measures of the first independent variable; white matter microstructure (Alexander et al., Reference Alexander, Hurley, Samsonov, Adluru, Hosseinbor, Mossahebi and Field2011). The graph theory metric global efficiency, the inverse of the average shortest path length between all pairs of nodes in the network (Rubinov & Sporns, Reference Rubinov and Sporns2010), was calculated from both weighted networks. This resulted in FA derived global efficiency (GeFA) and MD-derived global efficiency (GeMD), as measures of the second independent variable, network efficiency.

UKB imaging derived phenotypes (IDPs) variables

UK Biobank provides minimally processed, ready-to-use measures of white matter integrity, called imaging derived phenotypes (IDPs). We used the weighted FA and MD values of 27 white matter tracts derived through diffusion MRI probabilistic tractography to obtain a second set of general FA and MD measures, which will henceforth be referred to as the IDP-derived gFA and gMD measures. Further information on how the IDPs (27 tracts) derived gFA and gMD measures where constructed can be found in http://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/crystal/docs/brain_mri.pdf.

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were performed in R, version 3.5.3 and version 3.6.1.

Principal component analyses

To account for missing values in cognitive measures, individual scores were imputed using the missMDA package (Josse & Husson, Reference Josse and Husson2016). A PCA was then performed on the four cognitive variables and the scores on the first unrotated component (accounting for ~43% of the total variance, loadings all >0.30) were extracted as a measure of general cognitive ability (g). Details on the results of this PCA can be found in the supplementary materials online Supplementary Table S3 and online Supplementary Figs. S2–S3. We also performed a PCAs on the IDP derived variables and extracted the first unrotated component of FA and MD (both accounting for ~40% of total variance) as measures of IDP derived gFA and gMD. Details on the results of these PCAs, can be found in the supplementary materials online Supplementary Figs. S6–S9 and online Supplementary Table S4. The above mentioned PCAs were performed separately in all samples. Finally, we performed PCAs on the FA and MD weighted white matter network connections which remained after network thresholding (Madole et al., Reference Madole, Ritchie, Cox, Buchanan, Hernández, Maniega and Tucker-Drob2020). The scores of the first unrotated component for FA and MD were extracted as a measure of gFA and gMD (respectively accounting for 18.3% and 22.4% of the total variance). Details on the results of these PCAs can be found in supplementary materials online Supplementary Figs. S4–S5.

White matter analyses

First, we performed a log transformation on the reaction time task scores and excluded participants that did not have any score on this task. We then identified and excluded participants who had scores of 3.5 standard deviations above or below the group mean (having PLE or not) for all derived predicting and mediating variables (reaction time, g-factor, gFA, gMD, GeFA, GeMD, IDP derived gFA and IDP derived gMD). The extreme scores on the imaging variables were removed because we believe that they mostly represented artifacts and processing errors that can be missed by the automated quality control pipeline. We also chose to exclude extreme values on the cognitive variables that might have arisen due to the unsupervised nature of the cognitive tests.

Next, we tested the model proposed in the introduction, using path analysis with lavaan version 0.6–5 (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012). In this model, we specified that the association between ostensibly poorer white network efficiency/microstructure and having PLEs is mediated via processing speed and general cognitive ability (see Fig. 1). Though previous research suggests that processing speed may underpin g, we also allowed for a separate independent path directly from processing speed to PLEs. Following our hypotheses, we were especially interested in the possible significance of the total effects of white matter measures on PLE (path c + (a×b) + (d×f) + (d×e×b)) and white matter measures on g (path a + (d×e)) and the confidence intervals of the indirect effects through cognition and processing speed (d×e and (a×b) + (d×f) + (d×e×b)). The exact defined model can be found in supplementary methods online in Supplementary Fig. S9. Per analysis, only the predicting variable changed between connectome derived gFA, gMD, GeMD, GeFA and IDP derived gFA and gMD. Age and sex were included as covariates and we used diagonal weighted least squares (DWLS) as an estimator because of the dichotomous nature of the outcome variable (PLE). Due to the asymmetrical nature of the sampling distribution of the indirect effect, given that they involve the multiplication of other effects, we also calculated a thousand iterations bootstrapped confidence intervals for all estimates (Bollen & Stine, Reference Bollen and Stine1990). This whole process was repeated in the dMRI sample with only IDP derived gFA and gMD. Here scan site was included as an extra covariate because the dMRI sample was acquired on multiple scanners whereas the smaller connectome sample was acquired on a single scanner.

Distress analyses

We also wanted to explore the role of distress in our analyses. Therefore, we performed exploratory analyses to test the model in the full sample available (N = 24 774), with only participants who indicated to have felt a little or more distress associated with their PLEs (a score of 2 or higher on the distress question) included in the PLE group. In these analyses, participants with distress and PLEs were coded as one, and participants without PLEs were coded as 0. Before analysis, outliers were removed in the same fashion as in the other samples. Because of low power due to few participants reporting a distress score of 2 or higher, we only performed these analyses in the full available sample.

All p values were corrected for false-discovery rate (FDR) (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995) over the different samples that were tested and all reported betas are standardized. We corrected for different paths and different white matter measures separately, meaning that we corrected different FA measures over all samples, different MD measures over all samples etc. For example, we corrected for the FA measures of the path ‘a’ over all confirmatory analyses, so we performed FDR correction over the six p values (FA connectome, FA IDP and FA including and excluding participants with a psychotic disorder).

Results

Descriptive statistics

We excluded 20 participants with psychotic disorders and 105 outliers from the connectome sample. From the dMRI sample, we excluded 77 participants with psychotic disorders and 315 outliers. Details on the final sample size, demographics and descriptive statistics of all participants can be found in Table 2.

Table 1. Table indicating outcomes of interest per hypothesis

Table 2. Demographics and descriptive statistics of participants, divided per sample and group.

Note. Abbreviations: GeFA = FA derived global efficiency, GeMD = MD derived global efficiency, gFA = general FA, gMD = general MD, log RT = log reaction time, IDP = Imaging derived phenotype.

a Derived through two-sample t test;.

b Derived through chi-square test.

Psychotic-like experiences

Below, we will refer to our outcomes of interest as total and indirect effects; for the exact specifications see Table 1. All analyses below are excluding participants with a psychotic disorder. Results of all analyses, also containing analysis including participants with psychotic disorder, can be found in supplementary materials online Supplementary Tables S5–S20.

Our analyses showed no significant total effects between PLEs and network efficiency or white matter microstructure in all the samples tested (β < 0.046, p > 0.30). The indirect effects, as an indication of the mediation effect of cognition on the relationship between white matter variables and PLEs were also non-significant (β < 0.0012, p > 0.35). More details on the results of these analyses can be found in supplementary materials, online Supplementary Tables S5–S20.

Importantly, we also ran the exploratory path analysis in the full available sample with only participants included in the PLEs group that also reported distress score >1 associated with their PLEs. There, we found that those with distressing PLEs had significantly lower gFA (β = −0.055, p = 0.008). However, they did not have significantly lower cognitive functioning (β = −0.0322, p = 0.2277) and there was no evidence of any mediation effect (β = −0.002, p = 0.1203). Tables with output from the above analyses can be found in supplementary materials, online Supplementary Tables S21–S24.

White matter and cognition

In the connectome sample, analyses showed that higher g-factor was significantly related to lower IDP derived gMD (β = −0.03, p = 0.040) and higher IDP derived gFA (β = 0.042, p = 0.026). This same effect was found in the dMRI sample, with IDP derived gMD (β = −0.027, p = 0.003) and with IDP derived FA (β = 0.049, p < 0.001). This means that there was a significant total effect of the IDP derived white matter variables on g-factor.

Another effect that replicated across samples was the relationships between lower gMD, higher gFA derived from both connectome or IDP derived data and slower reaction time, with absolute beta's ranging from β = 0.0272 to β = 0.048 and p values ranging from p = 0.036 to p < 0.001. Higher MD derived global efficiency also related significantly to slower reaction times (β = 0.049, p = 0.001).

Further, analyses showed that faster reaction time was related to higher g-factor in the connectome sample (β = −0.10, p < 0.001) and the dMRI sample (β = −0.08, p = < 0.001).

Because the direct effect and both indirect effects of the mediation model proved to be significant when tested with IDP derived gFA and gMD, we evaluated whether the relationship between both white matter parameters and g-factor was indeed partially mediated by reaction time. In support of our hypothesis, the confidence intervals of the indirect effects did not contain zero in both samples, indicating partial mediation. In the connectome sample, the proportion mediated was 8.3% (p = 0.020) for gFA and 15% (p = 0.005) for gMD. This effect replicated in the dMRI sample, with a proportion mediated of 7% for gFA (p < 0.001) and 11% (p < 0.001) for gMD. In summary, we found a total effect of higher gFA and lower gMD related to an increase in g-factor. We also found significantly higher gFA and lower gMD related to faster reaction times, faster reaction times related to higher g-factor and a significant indirect effect, all indicating a partial mediation.

Discussion

White matter and psychotic-like experiences

The results of the confirmatory analyses showed no support for the relationship between white matter microstructure/network efficiency and PLEs, and thus for our hypotheses about the mediating effect of general cognitive ability on this relationship. One possible explanation could be that unlike clinical psychotic symptoms, PLEs are transient and often of low impact on the person who experiences them. It is thus plausible that white matter disturbances are minimal in people with PLEs, especially in our relatively old UKB sample. Furthermore, the mean age of around 55 in the current sample is well beyond the peak age of developing a psychotic disorder (Hafner et al., Reference Hafner, Maurer, Loffler, Fatkenheuer, An der Heiden, Riecher- Rossler and Gattaz1994), which suggests that the PLEs experienced by people in this sample are probably less likely to have high clinical significance. This reinforces the view that PLEs in this sample may not severe enough to be associated with marked white matter disturbances.

Importantly, we did find that participants in the PLEs group who had experienced distress in relation to their PLEs showed significant relationships between lower gFA and PLEs. This relationship was not mediated by cognition. It does suggest that people who experience PLEs may have altered white matter microstructure compared to people who do not experience PLEs, but especially or perhaps only if these PLEs are severe and/or long enough to cause distress. This association may reflect a neurodevelopmental vulnerability to more severe/uncontrollable PLEs. However, it is also possible that distress in combination with PLEs represents an individual's general tendency toward maladaptive emotion regulation (Osborne, Willroth, DeVylder, Mittal, & Hilimire, Reference Osborne, Willroth, DeVylder, Mittal and Hilimire2017) or other psychopathology (Brañas, Barrigón, Lahera, Canal-Rivero, & Ruiz-Veguilla, Reference Brañas, Barrigón, Lahera, Canal-Rivero and Ruiz-Veguilla2017), instead of the occurrence of more severe PLEs. Regardless, the current finding is in line with previous studies showing that distress associated with PLEs is an important predictor for progression to more severe mental health issues (Hanssen, Bijl, Vollebergh, & Os, Reference Hanssen, Bijl, Vollebergh and Os2003). It is also in keeping with previous studies in high-risk populations and participants with psychotic disorders, which have reported associations between white matter microstructure and psychotic symptoms (Alloza et al., Reference Alloza, Bastin, Cox, Gibson, Duff, Semple and Lawrie2017; Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Alm, Olson and Ellman2018; Roalf et al., Reference Roalf, de la Garza, Rosen, Calkins, Moore, Quarmley and Gur2019; Skudlarski et al., Reference Skudlarski, Jagannathan, Anderson, Stevens, Calhoun, Skudlarska and Pearlson2010, Reference Skudlarski, Schretlen, Thaker, Stevens, Keshavan, Sweeney and Pearlson2013; Tang et al., Reference Tang, Pasternak, Kubicki, Rathi, Zhang, Wang and Seidman2019). Notably, it was previously found that the association between white matter integrity in the forceps major and visual hallucinations was stronger in people who reported distress (Schoorl et al., Reference Schoorl, Barbu, Shen, Harris, Adams, Whalley and Lawrie2021).

Together, these results suggest that distress is an important factor when investigating neurobiological markers of PLEs. Since people who experience distress are more likely to develop a diagnosable psychotic disorder (Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Moore, Satterthwaite, Wolf, Turetsky, Roalf and Gur2017), it is possible that their white matter disturbances are more evident than those of people without distress. However, we cannot, at this stage say whether distress may be a marker of content, severity or impact. This should be examined in future studies of younger participants who experience distress associated with their PLEs, or in participants who have a psychotic disorder.

Cognition

Our analyses showed that the quality of white matter microstructure was associated with a higher general cognitive ability score. This finding is in accordance with a study by Cox, Ritchie, Fawns-Ritchie, Tucker-Drob, and Deary (Reference Cox, Ritchie, Fawns-Ritchie, Tucker-Drob and Deary2019), who also found an association between white matter macro- and microstructure and g-factor, in a UKB sample with an enhanced cognitive battery. Importantly, we also found that higher FA/lower MD was associated with quicker reaction time and that quick reaction time was related to a higher general cognitive ability score. We thus conclude that our results are in keeping with the hypothesis that altered white matter microstructure is associated with lower general cognitive ability, and that this relationship is partially mediated by processing speed. We thus replicate the results by (Penke et al., Reference Penke, Mañiega, Bastin, Hernández, Murray, Royle and Deary2012a, Reference Penke, Muñ Oz Maniega, Valdé, Herná Ndez, Murray, Royle, Starr and Dearyb), who found this same mediation effect in a smaller sample. Our study, therefore, adds to the certainty and universality of the mediation effect of processing speed on the relationship between white matter microstructure and general cognitive ability.

Limitations

Several limitations should be taken into account when interpreting our results. First of all, we note that all the effect sizes found in our study are small, possibly reducing their clinical relevance. However, small effect sizes are common in large population studies and likely represent reliable, true effects rather than unreliable big effects often found in smaller studies (Button et al., Reference Button, Ioannidis, Mokrysz, Nosek, Flint, Robinson and Munafò2013). Further, it is important to mention that FA and MD are not direct measures of white matter quality; rather, they measure multiple microstructural properties, including axonal myelination and fibre density (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Knösche and Turner2013). The interpretation of reduced or increased gFA and gMD is therefore not straightforward and one should consider this when interpreting a decrease of gFA or an increase of gMD as a sign of white matter disruption (Alexander et al., Reference Alexander, Hurley, Samsonov, Adluru, Hosseinbor, Mossahebi and Field2011). Additionally, network variables are known to be sensitive to the network construction methodology used (Qi, Meesters, Nicolay, ter Haar Romeny, & Ossenblok, Reference Qi, Meesters, Nicolay, ter Haar Romeny and Ossenblok2015), which may affect the generalizability of our findings to other samples. Lastly, we chose our graph theory metric (global efficiency) a priori, but realize that other graph theory metrics might have been similarly, or even more predictive of our outcome variables.

We should also note several limitations about the cognitive measures that were used. First of all, due to restricted cognitive data available in initial UKB samples, we only used one test score as an indication of the latent construct processing speed. Ideally, latent constructs should be determined based on at least three indicators. However, research by Fawns-Ritchie and Deary (Reference Fawns-Ritchie and Deary2020) does show that the UKB reaction time test correlated moderately to highly with other reference tests of processing speed. Secondly, the cognition tasks we used to infer general cognitive ability had relatively low test-retest reliability. Low test-retest reliability means more random error in a variable, causing the variable to relate less strongly to other variables. This limitation is partially mitigated by our latent variable approach. Namely, research by Fawns-Ritchie and Deary (Reference Fawns-Ritchie and Deary2020) showed that the first principal component of a PCA over five cognitive measures (including the measure for processing speed) correlated highly (r = 0.74) with a reference test for the assessment of general intelligence. However, this limitation could still have contributed to some of the null findings regarding the white matter and general cognitive ability. Finally, due to a lot of missing values on the cognition tasks, we have imputed scores before executing the PCA to derive a g-factor, which could have led to an inflated explained variance. However, we made sure to also perform the PCA without imputed values and the explained variance and loadings were still satisfactory (see supplementary results, online Supplementary Table S3 for details).

We have also used data from self-report questionnaires to establish whether participants had PLEs, whether they experienced distress and whether they have been diagnosed with a psychotic disorder. For the PLE and psychotic disorder variables, this yielded binary scores. In future studies, a more precise clinician rating could yield scores with a wider range of values, therefore creating a higher variance in the variable. This would allow for a more reliable indication of these phenomena and a more rigorous and generalizable test (Lakes, Reference Lakes2013) of the relation between white matter and PLEs. Related to this, an interesting suggestion for further research would be to include the amount of PLEs participants experience in the analyses.

The relatively old age of our sample should also be taken into account when interpreting the results of our study. Apart from the fact that our study participants are well beyond the peak age of developing psychosis, the older age could also have influenced the results of the Mental Health Questionnaire. The Mental Health Questionnaire asked questions about experiences that could have occurred over 30 years ago. It is thus plausible that some participants forgot about a PLE that they experienced a long time ago, or about the exact circumstances, such as drug use or distress, surrounding the PLE. However, results from the Mental Health Questionnaire show that approximately 5% of the participants indicated that they had experienced a PLE. Since this is in accordance with a previously reported prevalence of PLE (Van Os, Linscott, Myin-Germeys, Delespaul, & Krabbendam, Reference Van Os, Linscott, Myin-Germeys, Delespaul and Krabbendam2009), it does not appear to have been influenced by the sub-optimal memory of the participants.

Lastly, it is possible that psychotropic medication could have confounded our findings; e.g. that white matter differences could have been linked to psychotropic medication use instead of having PLEs with distress. However, we note that similar findings were reported including and excluding the small number of participants most likely to be taking medication, i.e. those with a diagnosis of a psychotic disorder (97 out of 24 774).

Conclusion

To conclude, this study presents a novel investigation of the relationships between white matter quality, cognition and PLEs; which has given new insights into the possible importance of distress in these relationships, as well as the validity of connectome measures. Furthermore, we corroborated earlier findings of the mediating role of processing speed on the relationship between white matter microstructure and general cognitive ability, in a big general population sample. Most importantly, we showed that PLEs in combination with distress are associated with reduced global white matter microstructure, which suggests a direction of research that could clarify how and why individuals progress from subclinical to clinical psychotic symptoms.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721004244.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the UK Biobank and all its voluntary participants. The UK Biobank study was conducted under generic approval from the NHS National Research Ethics Service (approval letter dated June 17, 2011, Ref 11/NW/0382). All participants gave full informed written consent.

Financial support

The authors acknowledge funding support from the Wellcome Trust (Wellcome Trust Strategic Award ‘STratifying Resilience and Depression Longitudinally’ (STRADL) Reference 104036/Z/14/Z). SRC is supported by Age UK (Disconnected Mind project) and the UK Medical Research Council [MR/M013111/1, MR/R024065/1]. SCR, CRB and EMT-D are supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) research grant R01AG054628

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Footnotes

*

These authors contributed equally to this work.

References

Alexander, A. L., Hurley, S. A., Samsonov, A. A., Adluru, N., Hosseinbor, A. P., Mossahebi, P., ... Field, A. S. (2011). Characterization of cerebral white matter properties using quantitative magnetic resonance imaging stains. Brain Connectivity, 1, 423446. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0071CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alfaro-Almagro, F., Jenkinson, M., Bangerter, N. K., Andersson, J. L. R., Griffanti, L., Douaud, G., … Smith, S. M. (2018). Image processing and quality control for the first 10000 brain imaging datasets from UK Biobank. NeuroImage, 166, 400424. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.034CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alloza, C., Bastin, M. E., Cox, S. R., Gibson, J., Duff, B., Semple, S. I., … Lawrie, S. M. (2017). Central and non-central networks, cognition, clinical symptoms, and polygenic risk scores in schizophrenia. Human Brain Mapping, 38(12), 59195930. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23798CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloza, C., Cox, S. R., Duff, B., Semple, S. I., Bastin, M. E., Whalley, H. C., & Lawrie, S. M. (2016). Information processing speed mediates the relationship between white matter and general intelligence in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research - Neuroimaging, 254, 2633. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.05.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashtari, M., Cottone, J., Ardekani, B. A., Cervellione, K., Szeszko, P. R., Wu, J., … Kumra, S. (2007). Disruption of white matter integrity in the inferior longitudinal fasciculus in adolescents with schizophrenia as revealed by fiber tractography. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(11), 12701280. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1270CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnett, J. H., McDougall, F., Xu, M. K., Croudace, T. J., Richards, M., & Jones, P. B. (2012). Childhood cognitive function and adult psychopathology: Associations with psychotic and non-psychotic symptoms in the general population. British Journal of Psychiatry, 201(2), 124130. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.102053CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.xGoogle Scholar
Bollen, K. A., & Stine, R. (1990). Direct and indirect effects: Classical and bootstrap estimates of variability. Sociological Methodology, 20, 115. doi: 10.2307/271084CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brañas, A., Barrigón, M. L., Lahera, G., Canal-Rivero, M., & Ruiz-Veguilla, M. (2017). Influence of depressive symptoms on distress related to positive psychotic-like experiences in women. Psychiatry Research, 258, 469475. doi: 10.1016/J.PSYCHRES.2017.08.094CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchanan, C. R., Bastin, M. E., Ritchie, S. J., Liewald, D. C., Madole, J. W., Tucker-Drob, E. M., … Cox, S. R. (2020). The effect of network thresholding and weighting on structural brain networks in the UK Biobank. NeuroImage, 211, 116443. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116443CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bullmore, E., & Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 186198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burns, J., Job, D., Bastin, M. E., Whalley, H., Macgillivray, T., Johnstone, E. C., & Lawrie, S. M. (2003). Structural disconnectivity in schizophrenia: A diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182(MAY), 439443. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.5.439CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P. A., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S. J., & Munafò, M. R. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(5), 365376. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calkins, M. E., Moore, T. M., Satterthwaite, T. D., Wolf, D. H., Turetsky, B. I., Roalf, D. R., … Gur, R. E. (2017). Persistence of psychosis spectrum symptoms in the Philadelphia neurodevelopmental cohort: A prospective two-year follow-up. World Psychiatry, 16(1), 6276. doi: 10.1002/WPS.20386CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, S., Alm, K. H., Olson, I. R., & Ellman, L. M. (2018). White matter alterations in individuals experiencing attenuated positive psychotic symptoms. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 12(3), 372379. doi: 10.1111/eip.12306CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornblatt, B. A., Lencz, T., Smith, C. W., Correll, C. U., Auther, A. M., & Nakayama, E. (2003). The schizophrenia prodrome revisited: A neurodevelopmental perspective. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 29(4), 633651. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, S. R., Ritchie, S. J., Fawns-Ritchie, C., Tucker-Drob, E. M., & Deary, I. J. (2019). Structural brain imaging correlates of general intelligence in UK Biobank. Intelligence, 76, 101376. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2019.101376CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desikan, R. S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., … Killiany, R. J. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage, 31(3), 968980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fawns-Ritchie, C., & Deary, I. J. (2020). Reliability and validity of the UK Biobank cognitive tests. PLoS One, 15(4), e0231627. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0231627CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., … Dale, A. M. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33(3), 341355. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fry, A., Littlejohns, T. J., Sudlow, C., Doherty, N., Adamska, L., Sprosen, T., … Allen, N. E. (2017). Comparison of sociodemographic and health-related characteristics of UK Biobank participants with those of the general population. American Journal of Epidemiology, 186(9), 10261034. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwx246CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fusar-Poli, P., Deste, G., Smieskova, R., Barlati, S., Yung, A. R., Howes, O., … Borgwardt, S. (2012). Cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: A meta-analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(6), 562571. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1592CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gong, G., He, Y., Concha, L., Lebel, C., Gross, D. W., Evans, A. C., & Beaulieu, C. (2009). Mapping anatomical connectivity patterns of human cerebral cortex using in vivo diffusion tensor imaging tractography. Cerebral Cortex, 19(3), 524536. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffa, A., Baumann, P. S., Thiran, J. P., & Hagmann, P. (2013). Structural connectomics in brain diseases. NeuroImage, 80, 515526. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.056CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hafner, H., Maurer, K., Loffler, W., Fatkenheuer, B., An der Heiden, W., Riecher- Rossler, A., … Gattaz, W. F. (1994). The epidemiology of early schizophrenia. Influence of age and gender on onset and early course. British Journal of Psychiatry, 164(APR. SUPPL. 23), 2938. doi:10.1192/s0007125000292714CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanssen, M., Bak, M., Bijl, R., Vollebergh, W., & van Os, J. (2005). The incidence and outcome of subclinical psychotic experiences in the general population. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(2), 181191. doi: 10.1348/014466505X29611CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanssen, M. S. S., Bijl, R. V., Vollebergh, W., & Os, J. V. (2003). Self-reported psychotic experiences in the general population: A valid screening tool for DSM-III-R psychotic disorders? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 107(5), 369377. doi: 10.1034/J.1600-0447.2003.00058.XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holleran, L., Kelly, S., Alloza, C., Agartz, I., Andreassen, O. A., Arango, C., … Donohoe, G. (2020). The relationship between white matter microstructure and general cognitive ability in patients with schizophrenia and healthy participants in the ENIGMA consortium. American Journal of Psychiatry, 177(6), 537547. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19030225CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, D. K., Knösche, T. R., & Turner, R. (2013). White matter integrity, fiber count, and other fallacies: The do's and don'ts of diffusion MRI. NeuroImage, 73, 239254. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.081CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Josse, J., & Husson, F. (2016). missMDA: A package for handling missing values in multivariate data analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 70(1), 131. doi: 10.18637/JSS.V070.I01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R., & Salthouse, T. A. (1994). Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica, 86(2–3), 199225. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(94)90003-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, S., Jahanshad, N., Zalesky, A., Kochunov, P., Agartz, I., Alloza, C., … Donohoe, G. (2018). Widespread white matter microstructural differences in schizophrenia across 4322 individuals: Results from the ENIGMA schizophrenia DTI Working Group. Molecular Psychiatry, 23(5), 12611269. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.170CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khandaker, G. M., Barnett, J. H., White, I. R., & Jones, P. B. (2011). A quantitative meta-analysis of population-based studies of premorbid intelligence and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 132(2–3), 220227. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klosterkötter, J., & Schultze-Lutter, F. (2000). Diagnosing schizophrenia in the initial prodromal phase. Schizophrenia Research, 41(1), 1011. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(00)90330-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakes, K. D. (2013). Restricted sample variance reduces generalizability. Psychological Assessment, 25(2), 643650. doi: 10.1037/A0030912CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, Y., Liu, Y., Li, J., Qin, W., Li, K., Yu, C., … Jiang, T. (2009). Brain anatomical network and intelligence. PLoS Computational Biology, 5(5), e1000395. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000395CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacCabe, J. H. (2008). Population-based cohort studies on premorbid cognitive function in schizophrenia. Epidemiologic Reviews, 30, 7783. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxn007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madole, J. W., Ritchie, S. J., Cox, S. R., Buchanan, C. R., Hernández, M. V., Maniega, S. M., … Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2020). Aging-sensitive networks within the human structural connectome are implicated in late-life cognitive declines. Biological Psychiatry, 89(9), 795–806. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.06.010Google ScholarPubMed
McIntosh, A. M., Maniega, S. M., Lymer, G. K. S., McKirdy, J., Hall, J., Sussmann, J. E. D., … Lawrie, S. M. (2008). White matter tractography in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 64(12), 10881092. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.07.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohamed, S., Paulsen, J. S., O'Leary, D., Arndt, S., & Andreasen, N. (1999). Generalized cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: A study of first-episode patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56(8), 749754. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.8.749CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muñoz Maniega, S., Lymer, G. K. S., Bastin, M. E., Marjoram, D., Job, D. E., Moorhead, T. W. J., … Lawrie, S. M. (2008). A diffusion tensor MRI study of white matter integrity in subjects at high genetic risk of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 106(2–3), 132139. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.09.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oestreich, L. K. L., Randeniya, R., & Garrido, M. I. (2019). Auditory prediction errors and auditory white matter microstructure associated with psychotic-like experiences in healthy individuals. Brain Structure and Function, 224(9), 32773289. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01972-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Hanlon, E., Leemans, A., Kelleher, I., Clarke, M. C., Roddy, S., Coughlan, H., … Cannon, M. (2015). White matter differences among adolescents reporting psychotic experiences: A population-based diffusion magnetic resonance imaging study. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(7), 668677. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0137CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orr, J. M., Turner, J. A., & Mittal, V. A. (2014). Widespread brain dysconnectivity associated with psychotic-like experiences in the general population. NeuroImage: Clinical, 4, 343351. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.01.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osborne, K. J., Willroth, E. C., DeVylder, J. E., Mittal, V. A., & Hilimire, M. R. (2017). Investigating the association between emotion regulation and distress in adults with psychotic-like experiences. Psychiatry Research, 256, 6670. doi: 10.1016/J.PSYCHRES.2017.06.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penke, L., Mañiega, S. M., Bastin, M. E., Hernández, M. C. V., Murray, C., Royle, N. A., … Deary, I. J. (2012a). Brain-wide white matter tract integrity is associated with information processing speed and general intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry, 17, 955. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.127CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penke, L., Muñ Oz Maniega, S., Valdé, S., Herná Ndez, M. C., Murray, C., Royle, N. A., Starr, J. M., … Deary, I. J. (2012b). Brain white matter tract integrity as a neural foundation for general intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry, 17, 10261030. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.66CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérez-Iglesias, R., Tordesillas-Gutiérrez, D., McGuire, P. K., Barker, G. J., Roiz-Santiañez, R., Mata, I., … Crespo-Facorro, B. (2010). White matter integrity and cognitive impairment in first-episode psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(4), 451458. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09050716CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pineda-Pardo, J. A., Martínez, K., Román, F. J., & Colom, R. (2016). Structural efficiency within a parieto-frontal network and cognitive differences. Intelligence, 54, 105116. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2015.12.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Power, M. C., Su, D., Wu, A., Reid, R. I., Jack, C. R., Knopman, D. S., … Mosley, T. H. (2019). Association of white matter microstructural integrity with cognition and dementia. Neurobiology of Aging, 83, 6372. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.08.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qi, S., Meesters, S., Nicolay, K., ter Haar Romeny, B. M., & Ossenblok, P. (2015). The influence of construction methodology on structural brain network measures: A review. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 253, 170182. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.06.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roalf, D. R., de la Garza, A. G., Rosen, A., Calkins, M. E., Moore, T. M., Quarmley, M., … Gur, R. E. (2019). Alterations in white matter microstructure in individuals at persistent risk for psychosis. Molecular Psychiatry, 25(10), 2441–2454. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0360-1Google ScholarPubMed
Roberts, J. A., Perry, A., Roberts, G., Mitchell, P. B., & Breakspear, M. (2017). Consistency-based thresholding of the human connectome. NeuroImage, 145, 118129. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.053CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 136. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubinov, M., & Sporns, O. (2010). Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations. NeuroImage, 52(3), 10591069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoorl, J., Barbu, M. C., Shen, X., Harris, M. R., Adams, M. J., Whalley, H. C., … Lawrie, S. M. (2021). Grey and white matter associations of psychotic-like experiences in a general population sample (UK Biobank). Translational Psychiatry, 11(1), 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-01131-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seiler, N., Nguyen, T., Yung, A., & O'Donoghue, B. (2020). Terminology and assessment tools of psychosis: A systematic narrative review. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 74, 226246. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12966CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skudlarski, P., Jagannathan, K., Anderson, K., Stevens, M. C., Calhoun, V. D., Skudlarska, B. A., & Pearlson, G. (2010). Brain connectivity is not only lower but different in schizophrenia: A combined anatomical and functional approach. Biological Psychiatry, 68(1), 6169. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.03.035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skudlarski, P., Schretlen, D. J., Thaker, G. K., Stevens, M. C., Keshavan, M. S., Sweeney, J. A., … Pearlson, G. D. (2013). Diffusion tensor imaging white matter endophenotypes in patients with schizophrenia or psychotic bipolar disorder and their relatives. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(8), 886898. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12111448CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sotiropoulos, S. N., & Zalesky, A. (2019). Building connectomes using diffusion MRI: Why, how and but. NMR in Biomedicine, 32, e3752. doi: 10.1002/nbm.3752CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tang, Y., Pasternak, O., Kubicki, M., Rathi, Y., Zhang, T., Wang, J., … Seidman, L. J. (2019). Altered cellular white matter but not extracellular free water on diffusion MRI in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 176(10), 820828. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.18091044CrossRefGoogle Scholar
UK Biobank. (2014, September 25). Brain Scan Protocol. Retrieved from https://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/crystal/docs/bmri_V4_23092014.pdfGoogle Scholar
Van Den Heuvel, M. P., Stam, C. J., Kahn, R. S., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2009). Efficiency of functional brain networks and intellectual performance.Journal of Neuroscience, 29(23), 7619–7624. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1443-09.2009Google Scholar
Van Os, J., Linscott, R. J., Myin-Germeys, I., Delespaul, P., & Krabbendam, L. (2009). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: Evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychological Medicine, 39(2), 179195. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003814CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voineskos, A. N., Felsky, D., Kovacevic, N., Tiwari, A. K., Zai, C., Mallar Chakravarty, M., … Kennedy, J. L. (2013). Oligodendrocyte genes, white matter tract integrity, and cognition in schizophrenia. Cerebral Cortex, 23(9), 20442057. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs188CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wen, W., Zhu, W., He, Y., Kochan, N. A., Reppermund, S., Slavin, M. J., … Sachdev, P. (2011). Discrete neuroanatomical networks are associated with specific cognitive abilities in old age. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(4), 1204–1212. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4085-10.2011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whyte, M. C., Brett, C., Harrison, L. K., Byrne, M., Miller, P., Lawrie, S. M., & Johnstone, E. C. (2006). Neuropsychological performance over time in people at high risk of developing schizophrenia and controls. Biological Psychiatry, 59(8), 730739. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.08.028CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Path diagram of the model that was tested. Note. Path diagram specifies that the association between ostensibly poorer (higher MD, lower FA and lower global efficiency) white matter metrics and having PLEs is mediated via processing speed and general cognitive ability. Though prior work suggests that processing speed may underpin g, we also allow for a separate independent path directly from speed to PLEs.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Participant flow chart.

Figure 2

Table 1. Table indicating outcomes of interest per hypothesis

Figure 3

Table 2. Demographics and descriptive statistics of participants, divided per sample and group.

Supplementary material: File

Bosma et al. supplementary material

Bosma et al. supplementary material

Download Bosma et al. supplementary material(File)
File 443.7 KB