Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T05:15:19.372Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Can impulsivity evolve in response to childhood environmental harshness?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 May 2022

Atsushi Kometani
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Graduate School of Humanities, Kobe University, Kobe, Japan
Yohsuke Ohtsubo*
Affiliation:
Department of Social Psychology, Graduate School of Humanities and Sociology, University of Tokyo, 7-3-1 Hongo, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo, 113-0033, Japan
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: yohtsubo@l.u-tokyo.ac.jp

Abstract

Previous studies have suggested that human impulsivity is an adaptive response to childhood environmental harshness: individuals from families of low socioeconomic status (SES) tend to be more impulsive. However, no studies have tested the evolvability of this reaction norm. This study examined whether (a) impulsivity is associated with higher fitness among individuals from low SES families, while (b) it is associated with lower fitness among individuals from high SES families. We assessed three indices of impulsivity (temporal discounting, risk taking and fast/slow life history strategy), childhood SES and five proxy indices of fitness (number of children, lifelong singlehood, annual household income, subjective SES and life satisfaction) of 692 middle-aged participants (40–45 years old). None of the results supported the evolvability of the impulsivity reaction norm, although low childhood SES was associated with lower fitness on every proxy measure. Impulsivity (operationalised as the fast life history strategy) was associated with lower fitness regardless of childhood SES.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social media summary: Impulsivity is not an adaptive response to childhood environmental harshness among Japanese middle-aged adults.

Introduction

Phenotypic plasticity has long been recognised but has remained underappreciated until recently in evolutionary biology (Sommer, Reference Sommer2020). Although many instances of plasticity may be by-products of physical and chemical developmental processes, certain kinds of plasticity are adaptive reactions to environmental variation (Nijhout, Reference Nijhout2003). Recently, psychological studies on phenotypic plasticity have increased under the rubric of life history theory (Del Giudice et al., Reference Del Giudice, Gangestad, Kaplan and Buss2015; Nettle & Frankenhuis, Reference Nettle and Frankenhuis2020). However, phenotypic plasticity and life history theory do not necessarily go together in evolutionary biology. Consequently, less attention has been paid to the literature on biological phenotypic plasticity in evaluating purported phenotypic plasticity in humans. In this study, drawing on a biological model of phenotypic plasticity, we critically examine whether an oft-cited human phenotypic plasticity (i.e. early-life adversity causing impulsivity in adulthood) is an evolvable reaction norm.

Low socioeconomic status and impulsivity

According to Nettle and Frankenhuis (Reference Nettle and Frankenhuis2020), the core idea of life history theory in evolutionary biology is that natural selection acts on phenotypes associated with different components of fitness (e.g. survival, reproduction). Its goal is to understand the population-specific optimisation of trade-offs between different fitness components under different environments. Hence, it is often used to explain across-species/population differences. In contrast, life history theory in psychology almost exclusively focuses on individual differences along the so-called fast–slow continuum: whether each individual prioritises reproduction (leaning towards fast strategies) or survival (leaning towards slow strategies) in response to their local environment.

Such an empirical focus of psychological life history theory has its roots in abundant evidence that low socioeconomic status (SES) is associated with a set of apparently problematic behaviours (or behavioural constellation of deprivation, BCD), such as saving less for the future, having teen pregnancies, investing less in their children, having poorer diets, using illicit drugs, consuming an excessive amount of alcohol and smoking more (Pepper & Nettle, Reference Pepper and Nettle2017). Although BCD is often viewed as maladaptive, Pepper and Nettle maintain that BCD may be more accurately understood as contextually appropriate responses to environmental uncertainty experienced by low SES individuals (e.g. if you do not expect to survive until next year, there is little point in saving for the distant future). Accordingly, Pepper and Nettle consider that BCD may represent evolved responses to environmental uncertainty.

In addition, life history theory in psychology tends to emphasise the role of childhood environments (Nettle & Frankenhuis, Reference Nettle and Frankenhuis2020). Its tenet is that early-life adversity, such as childhood economic harshness, causes accelerated reproductive timing and other traits associated with BCD. Empirical studies have supported the effects of early-life adversity (e.g. Griskevicius et al., Reference Griskevicius, Ackerman, Cantú, Delton, Robertson, Simpson and Tybur2013; Mittal & Griskevicius, Reference Mittal and Griskevicius2014). For example, Griskevicius et al. (Reference Griskevicius, Ackerman, Cantú, Delton, Robertson, Simpson and Tybur2013) exposed half of the participants to a series of images indicative of economic recession (recession condition), while the other half was exposed to a series of images of nature (control condition). Participants then engaged in two tasks relevant to BCD (i.e. temporal discounting and risk taking), which Griskevicius et al. referred to as ‘impulsivity’. In the present article, we follow this terminology and use a loose definition of impulsivity that refers to a group of traits that are theorised as outcomes of childhood adversity. Dividing participants into high vs. low childhood SES groups by the criterion of one standard deviation (SD) above or below the mean, Griskevicius et al. found a 2 (childhood SES) × 2 (economic threat priming) interaction effect on impulsivity. In the recession condition, individuals from low SES families exhibited greater temporal discounting and risk-taking tendencies than individuals from high SES families, but no such difference was observed in the control condition. If we assume that an immediate economic threat reveals one's disposition, the result is congruent with life history theory in psychology, which predicts that individuals grow more impulsive in response to early-life adversity (but see Amir et al., Reference Amir, Jordan and Rand2018, for a replication failure).

Evolution of reaction norms

Although the results of Griskevicius et al. (Reference Griskevicius, Ackerman, Cantú, Delton, Robertson, Simpson and Tybur2013) and other similar findings (see Del Giudice et al., Reference Del Giudice, Gangestad, Kaplan and Buss2015, for a review) are congruent with life history theory in psychology, the psychology version of life history theory has been criticised in various aspects. Critics question, for example, whether there are, in fact, trade-offs in reproduction and survival in humans and whether various traits necessarily cluster together along the fast–slow continuum (see articles in the special issue of Evolution and Human Behavior on ‘current debates in human life history research’ edited by Frankenhuis & Nettle, Reference Frankenhuis and Nettle2020). Although many criticisms have been directed at its validity as a branch of life history theory, some have paid critical attention to another aspect: an evolutionary account of human plasticity (e.g. Galipaud & Kokko, Reference Galipaud and Kokko2020; Nettle et al., Reference Nettle, Frankenhuis and Rickard2013).

For example, it may not be adaptive for any long-lived species, such as humans, to set their phenotypes based on their childhood environments because their environments may change over their long lifespans (Nettle et al., Reference Nettle, Frankenhuis and Rickard2013). In this case, reversible lifelong plasticity may be more adaptive (Ratikainen & Kokko, Reference Ratikainen and Kokko2019). However, there are also reasons to expect that childhood SES may be predictive of one's later environments (Nettle et al., Reference Nettle, Frankenhuis and Rickard2013). For example, early-life malnutrition may have lasting effects on one's health status. Educational disadvantage during childhood may also accentuate initial disparities later in life. Although such prolonged effects of early-life adversity may make accelerated reproductive timing adaptive, a non-adaptationist account is also possible: detrimental psychological effects of early-life adversity may be a by-product of disruption of normal brain development (e.g. McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014). Therefore, no firm answer exists for the question of whether apparent human phenotypic plasticity is in fact an instance of an evolved reaction norm.

As a first step to answering this question, we drew on a mathematical model of adaptive reaction norms developed in biology (Hazel et al., Reference Hazel, Smock and Johnson1990; see also Kokko, Reference Kokko2007, for a simplified version). One application of this model is the predator-induced polymorphism in the acorn barnacle Chthamalus anisopoma (Lively, Reference Lively1986a, Reference Livelyb). Although they normally develop conical shells, some develop bent shells in the presence of the predatory snail Acanthina angelica. This predator-induced polymorphism is adaptive because the bent morph is costly (i.e. less fecund), but more resistant to predators’ attacks. This logic can be readily applied to the impulsivity reaction norm (Table 1). In the absence of childhood environmental harshness (high SES, conceptually corresponding to predator-absence), impulsivity is costly: high impulsivity is associated with lower fitness (α 0) than low impulsivity (β 0) is. In the presence of childhood environmental harshness (low SES, corresponding to predator presence), impulsivity confers fitness benefits: high impulsivity is associated with higher fitness (α 1) than low impulsivity (β 1) is. In addition, it is plausible to assume that environmental harshness adversely influences fitness. Thus, the inequality β 0 > α 0 > α 1 > β 1 must hold for the reaction norm of impulsivity to be evolvable.

Table 1. Four types of individuals (groups 1–4) divided by childhood environment (harsh vs. not harsh) and impulsivity (high vs. low)

Note: the underline indicates greater fitness in each column.

The present study

To test whether the above inequality holds, we conducted a modified replication of Griskevicius et al. (Reference Griskevicius, Ackerman, Cantú, Delton, Robertson, Simpson and Tybur2013). In addition to assessing each participant's childhood SES and their impulsivity, we had participants report five proxy indices of fitness (i.e. number of children, marriage experience, annual household income, subjective SES, life satisfaction). We specifically recruited participants in their early 40s because our indices of fitness included the number of children. The set of three measures allowed us to test the aforementioned inequality. The first two measures, which were independent variables, were used to divide participants into four groups: high childhood SES individuals exhibiting low impulsivity (group 1); high childhood SES individuals exhibiting high impulsivity (group 2); low childhood SES individuals exhibiting low impulsivity (group 3); and low childhood SES individuals exhibiting high impulsivity (group 4). We then tested whether the inequality held for each of the five proxy indices of fitness. In particular, we tested whether impulsivity was associated with higher fitness among individuals from low SES families (α 1 > β 1) but lower fitness among individuals from high SES families (β 0 > α 0).

This study has three features that merit additional explanation. First, the proxy indices of fitness included variables that are not usually considered relevant to biological fitness. The straightforward index of fitness is number of children. However, owing to the availability of contraceptive methods in modern environments, successful individuals may not necessarily have more children (e.g. Pérusse, Reference Pérusse1993; Vining, Reference Vining1986; but see Stulp & Barrett, Reference Stulp and Barrett2016, for counterevidence). Accordingly, we assessed participants’ marriage experiences, which would have led to having children in ancestral environments. Financial success, which is associated with high status, would also have resulted in more children and enable greater parental investment in offspring in ancestral environments. Therefore, we measured participants’ annual incomes and subjective evaluations of their SES. In addition, the overall evaluation of one's quality of life may reflect some components that would have been associated with fitness in ancestral environments. In summary, to fill the gap between modern and ancestral environments, we included a wide range of variables, but recognised that some may not be relevant to fitness.

Second, unlike Griskevicius et al.'s (2013) study, we did not include economic recession priming. Our study was conducted in the midst of the COVID-19 pandemic, and people were continually exposed to real-world news about economic crises. Instead of including the priming task, we assessed whether participants’ incomes had decreased from the year 2019 (one year before data collection and the COVID-19 pandemic) to 2020. Assuming that a real decrease in annual income would have a greater psychological effect than priming tasks, we separately analysed the data from participants whose incomes had decreased from the previous year. However, the analyses of this subset of the sample did not change the general pattern of the results. Therefore, we report the results based on this subsample in the Supplementary Material.

Third, as an additional measure of impulsivity, as it was loosely defined in this study, we included the Mini-K scale (Figueredo et al., Reference Figueredo, Vásquez, Brumbach, Schneider, Sefcek, Tal and Jacobs2006), which was designed to measure individual differences in fast vs. slow life history strategies. We decided to expand the measures of ‘impulsivity’ because human life history research encompasses a wide range of traits as dependent variables, despite a lack of evidence that those traits do in fact cluster together (Sear, Reference Sear2020). For example, risky behaviours may refer to at least two distinct constructs: activities that would increase the likelihood of undesirable outcomes (e.g. unprotected sex, drug use) and willingness to accept outcome variance, the latter being standard operationalisation in laboratory studies (Pepper & Nettle, Reference Pepper and Nettle2017). Therefore, we considered it better to expand our independent variables to cover as many purported life history traits as possible. Although some scholars question its psychometric properties (e.g. Richardson et al., Reference Richardson, Chen, Dai, Brubaker and Nedelec2017), the Mini-K scale is often used to measure position on the fast–slow continuum (e.g. Figueredo et al., Reference Figueredo, Wolf, Olderbak, Gladden, Fernandes, Wenner and Rushton2014). Accordingly, we included it as an additional measure.

We preregistered the protocol for this study on the Open Science Framework (OSF; https://osf.io/tuav4/). This study was approved by the institutional review board at the first author's institute.

Method

Participants

A sample of 4,579 Japanese community-based participants was recruited in September 2020 through an online survey service (Cross Marketing Inc., Japan). Given that our primary dependent variable was number of children, we specifically recruited participants aged 40–45 years, surpassing the Japanese average first-marriage age of 30.7 and 29.0 years for men and women, respectively (Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare, 2016). As preregistered, we included a stringent attention check (i.e. asking participants to ignore an entire page of the survey; Miura & Kobayashi, Reference Miura and Kobayashi2015) along with two standard attention checks (i.e. asking participants to choose a specific option). Consequently, a large number of participants (3,446 participants) failed. We also excluded 441 participants, most of whom were not in the required age range. We retained 692 participants (352 women, 337 men and 3 not reported; 42.72 ± 1.73 years old), which was slightly below the preregistered sample size of 700, for subsequent analyses.

Independent variables

The childhood SES measure consisted of three items (e.g. ‘My family usually had enough money for things when I was growing up’), rated on a nine-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 9 = strongly agree; Cronbach's α = 0.79). The temporal discounting task consisted of 20 choices between an immediate small reward (e.g. receiving JPY 4100 tomorrow) and a larger delayed reward (e.g. receiving JPY 5,100 after 33 days). The risk-taking task consisted of 20 choices between definitely earning a fixed amount of money and earning a larger amount of money with a certain probability (e.g. earning JPY 5,600 for sure vs. a 54% chance of earning JPY 6500). The frequency of choosing the immediate reward options and gamble options was used as the index of impulsivity for the temporal discounting task and risk taking task, respectively. These materials, translated into Japanese by the authors, were adapted from Griskevicius et al. (Reference Griskevicius, Ackerman, Cantú, Delton, Robertson, Simpson and Tybur2013).

In addition, participants filled out the Japanese version 20-item Mini-K scale (Figueredo et al., Reference Figueredo, Vásquez, Brumbach, Schneider, Sefcek, Tal and Jacobs2006; Kawamoto, 2015), designed to measure individual differences in fast vs. slow life history strategies (e.g. ‘I often make plans in advance’). Participants rated each item on a seven-point scale (−3 = strongly disagree to 3 = strongly agree). We excluded two items that assess participants’ childhood experiences, which have some overlap with the independent variable (i.e. childhood SES). The Mini-K score (Cronbach's α = 0.84) represents orientation towards the slow life history strategy; higher scores indicate lower impulsivity.

Dependent variables

The dependent variables were proxy indices of participants’ fitness (i.e. number of children, marriage experience, annual household income, subjective SES, life satisfaction). A straightforward fitness index was reproductive success, operationalised as the number of children that participants had throughout their lives. We explicitly asked them to include any children from previous marriages. Marriage experience (i.e. lifelong singlehood vs. having at least one marriage) was also included; lifelong singles are considered less reproductively successful. Annual household income and subjective SES were included because wealth enables greater investment in offspring. Participants reported their household income last year, broken down into six ranges: <1.5, [1.5, 3), [3, 5), [5, 7), [7, 10) and ≥10 (units of million JPY). Subjective SES was measured using the MacArthur Scale (Adler et al., Reference Adler, Epel, Castellazzo and Ickovics2000), requiring participants to choose one of 10 SES-level rungs on a ladder. Life satisfaction was included, assuming that their assessment of their own quality of life may be associated with some aspects of biological fitness. It was measured using the Satisfaction With Life Scale (Diener et al., Reference Diener, Emmons, Larsen and Griffin1985) consisting of five items (e.g. ‘In most ways my life is close to my ideal’), rated on a seven-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree; Cronbach's α = 0.90).

Analyses

We divided the participants into four groups by bivariate median splits, combining childhood SES with one of the three impulsivity indices (i.e. temporal discounting, risk taking, or life history strategy). The four groups were: low impulsivity, high childhood SES (group 1, fitness denoted as β 0 in Table 1); high impulsivity, high childhood SES (group 2, α 0); low impulsivity, low childhood SES (group 3, β 1); and high impulsivity, low childhood SES (group 4, α 1). Based on the hypothesised inequality, we ran a set of three planned contrasts with weights of (1, −1, 0, 0), (0, 0, 1, −1) and (0.5, 0.5, −0.5, −0.5) for (group 1, group 2, group 3, group 4). The first two contrasts were designed to test β 0 > α 0 and α 1 > β 1, respectively. The third contrast corresponded to the comparison between high and low childhood SES groups, included to test the basic presumption that childhood economic harshness adversely influences later fitness. The data used in the reported analyses and the R code are available from OSF (https://osf.io/tuav4/).

Results

Tables 2 and 3 show descriptive statistics and correlation matrices of the variables of interest (Table 2 for the independent variables and Table 3 for the dependent variables). We first examined whether childhood SES was negatively associated with impulsivity. Contrary to previous studies, childhood SES was not significantly associated with temporal discounting (r 668 = −0.06) or risk taking (r 673 = 0.01). However, it was significantly associated with a self-rated slow life history strategy (i.e. the Mini-K score; r 663 = 0.26, p < 0.001).

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and correlation matrix of the independent variables

*** p < 0.001.

Table 3. Descriptive statistics and correlation matrix of the dependent variables

Note: Marriage experience – 0 = lifetime singlehood, 1 = having at least one marriage.

*** p < 0.001.

Figure 1 shows the distributions of the five fitness indices of groups 1–4. The four groups were divided by childhood SES and impulsivity, which are operationalised as temporal discounting in Figure 1a–e, risk-taking tendency in Figure 1f–j and Mini-K score in Figure 1k–q. Although the hypothesised inequality predicts a horizontally mirrored J-shaped pattern, none of the figures shows this (except Figure 1a, which exhibits a slight tendency towards a horizontally mirrored J-shape).

Figure 1. Distribution of fitness indices of four groups divided by medians of childhood socioeconomic status (SES) and impulsivity. Note: the hypothesis predicts a horizontally mirrored J-shape – highest, second highest, lowest and second lowest for groups 1–4, respectively. Results based on childhood SES × temporal discounting grouping, childhood SES × risk-taking grouping, and childhood SES × Mini-K grouping are presented as (a–e), (f–j) and (k–q), respectively. The dependent variables were number of children for (a), (f) and (k), marriage experience for (b), (g) and (m); annual household income for (c), (h) and (n); subjective SES for (d), (i) and (p); and life satisfaction for (e), (j) and (q).

When impulsivity was operationalised by temporal discounting (Figures 1a–e), contrasts 1 and 2 yielded no significant results for any of the five fitness indices (see also Table S1). When impulsivity was operationalised by risk-taking tendency (Figures 1f–j), although contrast 2 revealed no significant results, contrast 1 showed significant effects opposite to the hypothesis: group 2 reported having more children (b = −0.24, SE = 0.06, p < 0.001), higher annual household income (b = −0.17, SE = 0.08, p < 0.05), higher subjective SES (b = −0.35, SE = 0.10, p < 0.001) and higher life satisfaction (b = −0.15, SE = 0.07, p < 0.05) than group 1 (see Table S1 for details).

When impulsivity was operationalised by Mini-K score (Figures 1k–q, see also Table S1), regardless of childhood SES (i.e. in both contrasts 1 and 2), lower impulsivity was consistently associated with higher fitness: more children (b = 0.22, SE = 0.07, p < 0.001 and b = 0.41, SE = 0.07, p < 0.001 for contrasts 1 and 2, respectively), higher likelihood of marriage (b = 0.61, SE = 0.12, p < 0.001 and b = 0.61, SE = 0.12, p < 0.001), higher annual household income (b = 0.24, SE = 0.08, p < 0.01 and b = 0.39, SE = 0.08, p < 0.001), higher subjective SES (b = 0.38, SE = 0.10, p < 0.001 and b = 0.50, SE = 0.09, p < 0.001) and higher life satisfaction (b = 0.39, SE = 0.07, p < 0.001 and b = 0.52, SE = 0.06, p < 0.001). This pattern predicts that impulsivity was selected against regardless of childhood economic harshness.

Contrast 3 (high vs. low childhood SES) was tested three times, although redundant, for the three grouping schemes. Therefore, as shown in Table S1, despite grouping schemes, groups 1 and 2 (high childhood SES groups) were associated with higher fitness than groups 3 and 4 (low childhood SES groups) on every measure, confirming the basic presumption that low childhood SES adversely influenced later fitness. Parenthetically, this result suggests the criterion validity of our childhood SES measure as well as fitness indices: despite contemporary Japan's low fertility rate (1.36 in 2019) and ever increasing lifetime unmarried rate (23.4 and 14.1 for males and females, respectively, in 2015; Ministry of Health, Labour, and Welfare, 2020), the influence of the theoretically relevant variable (i.e. childhood SES) was observed for the number of children and marriage experience.

We conducted four sets of robustness checks. First, we redefined the four groups using the upper and lower tertiles of childhood SES and impulsivity scores (Table S2, Figure S1). Second, we focused on participants experiencing real economic threats whose annual household income decreased from the previous year, possibly owing to COVID-19 (Table S3, Figure S2). Third, the same analyses were conducted separately by sex (Tables S4 and S5, Figures S3 and S4). Fourth, we tested the hypothesis without splitting the participants into four groups. In a series of regression analyses that treated childhood SES and impulsivity measures as continuous variables, each of the five fitness measures was regressed on childhood SES, one of the three impulsivity measures (either temporal discounting, risk-taking tendency or Mini-K score) and their interaction term (Table S6 for the entire sample, Table S7 for the subsample of participants whose annual income decreased from the previous year, Table S8 for the male sample and Table S9 for the female sample). These robustness checks revealed a mostly identical pattern. In addition, since the fast strategy may result in a greater likelihood of divorce and remarriage, we conducted the three planned contrasts using the number of marriages as the dependent variable. The results were mostly similar to the planned contrasts that included the other fitness indices as the dependent variables (see Table S10).

Discussion

We examined the evolvability of the impulsivity reaction norm. Although the results confirmed the basic presumption that childhood economic harshness adversely influenced participants’ later fitness, none of the other results supported the impulsivity reaction norm's evolvability: when operationalised as risk-taking tendency, impulsivity was associated with higher fitness among individuals with high, but not low, childhood SES (this pattern, however, was not replicated in our subsequent unpublished study including only male participants). When operationalised by Mini-K score, it was associated with lower fitness regardless of childhood SES.

The present study failed to replicate the results of previous studies (Griskevicius et al., Reference Griskevicius, Ackerman, Cantú, Delton, Robertson, Simpson and Tybur2013). In particular, childhood SES was not significantly associated with either risk taking or temporal discounting. Given the robust association between childhood SES and BCD (Pepper & Nettle, Reference Pepper and Nettle2017), the present study's operationalisation of impulsivity might have provided inadequate indices of impulsivity. For example, one could argue that the temporal discounting and risk-taking tasks should have been incentivised (but see Amir et al., Reference Amir, Jordan and Rand2018, which reported no systematic differences between incentivised and non-incentivised risk-taking tasks). Mishra et al. (Reference Mishra, Barclay and Sparks2017) recently proposed a model of risk-taking (relative state model) that distinguishes two types of risk-taking behaviours, need-based and ability-based risk-taking; the former is motivated by poor environments, while the latter is motivated by superior abilities (i.e. the prospect of successful risk-taking). In future studies, it is worthwhile not only to incentivise risk-taking tasks but also to distinguish subtypes of risk-taking and impulsivity based on such a nuanced model.

One limitation is that we assessed fitness in a modern, industrialised society that is largely different from the environment of evolutionary adaptedness (EEA). For example, if low SES conditions in contemporary Japan are still more benign compared with harsh conditions in EEA, the present study may not be a fair test of the hypothesised phenotypic plasticity. Nonetheless, it is worth noting that childhood SES was in fact positively associated with every measure of fitness in this study. Moreover, careful analyses revealed some comparability of the modern and ancestral environments (i.e. positive association between wealth and the number of children in both modern and ancestral environments; Nettle & Pollet, Reference Nettle and Pollet2008). Nevertheless, this particular result does not necessarily imply that the comparability between the modern and ancestral environments extends to other aspects. For example, one could argue that impulsivity is an effective strategy for disadvantaged individuals only in EEA but not in the modern environment. Since there is a wide range of differences between the modern environment and EEA, or the so-called evolutionary mismatch problem (Li et al., Reference Li, van Vugt and Colarelli2018), it is informative to replicate this study in populations that maintain traditional lifestyles.

We admit that this study does not disprove the evolvability of human reaction norms as a whole. This study only tested the evolvability of impulsivity in response to childhood economic harshness. There are other independent and dependent variables that have attracted researchers’ attention in the context of life history theory in psychology. For example, timing of puberty and parental strategies are oft-studied life history traits (i.e. dependent variables), and childhood mortality/morbidity and unpredictability are oft-studied environmental (independent) variables (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Figueredo, Brumbach and Schlomer2009). Therefore, future studies need to include a wider range of measures of childhood environments and life history traits in order to fully test the evolvability of any form of phenotypic plasticity in response to early environments.

In summary, this study does not reveal any evidence of the evolvability of the impulsivity reaction norm in response to childhood economic harshness. Therefore, we urge researchers to critically assess the impulsivity reaction norm, especially whether the adaptationist explanation is better supported by empirical data than by-product explanations.

Acknowledgements

One of the reviewers of this article recommended that we drop the Mini-K scale from the analyses because it was developed based on a scientifically flawed theory (i.e. Rushton's differential K theory). Although we sincerely appreciate the reviewer directing our attention to this aspect of the Mini-K scale, we decided to retain it because it was included in the preregistered questionnaire. We will be more cautious in measuring human life history strategies in our future works.

Author contributions

AK and YO conceived and designed the study, and conducted data gathering. AK performed statistical analyses. AK and YO wrote the article.

Financial support

This study was supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science KAKENHI grants to YO (18K03006, 21H00931).

Conflicts of interest declarations in manuscripts

AK and YO declare none.

Data availability statement

The materials, protocol, data and analytical codes that support the findings of this study are openly available from the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/tuav4/.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2022.22

References

Adler, N. E., Epel, E. S., Castellazzo, G., & Ickovics, J. R. (2000). Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy, White women. Health Psychology, 19(6), 586592. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.586CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amir, D., Jordan, M. R., & Rand, D. G. (2018). An uncertainty management perspective on long-run impacts of adversity: The influence of childhood socioeconomic status on risk, time, and social preferences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 79, 217226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.07.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Del Giudice, M., Gangestad, S. W., & Kaplan, H. S. (2015). Life history theory and evolutionary psychology. In Buss, D. M. (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (2nd ed., pp. 88114). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125563.evpsych102Google Scholar
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 7175. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: The impact of harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Human Nature, 20(2), 204268. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-009-9063-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figueredo, A. J., Vásquez, G., Brumbach, B. H., Schneider, S. M. R., Sefcek, J. A., Tal, I. R., … Jacobs, W. J. (2006). Consilience and life history theory: From genes to brain to reproductive strategy. Developmental Review, 26(2), 243275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2006.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figueredo, A. J., Wolf, P. S. A., Olderbak, S. G., Gladden, P. R., Fernandes, H. B. F., Wenner, C., … Rushton, J. P. (2014). The psychometric assessment of human life history strategy: A meta-analytic construct validation. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 8(3), 148185. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0099837CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frankenhuis, W. E., & Nettle, D. (2020). Current debates in human life history research. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(6), 469473. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.09.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galipaud, M., & Kokko, H. (2020). Adaptation and plasticity in life-history theory: How to derive predictions. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(6), 493501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.06.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griskevicius, V., Ackerman, J. M., Cantú, S. M., Delton, A. W., Robertson, T. E., Simpson, J. A., … Tybur, J. M. (2013). When the economy falters, do people spend or save? Responses to resource scarcity depend on childhood environments. Psychological Science, 24(2), 197205. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612451471CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hazel, W. N., Smock, R., & Johnson, M. D. (1990). A polygenic model for the evolution and maintenance of conditional strategies. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 242(1305), 181187. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1990.0122Google ScholarPubMed
Kawamoto (2015). The translation and validation of the Mini-K scale in Japanese. Japanese Psychological Research, 57(3), 254267. https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12083CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kokko, H. (2007). Modelling for field biologists and other interesting people. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, N. P., van Vugt, M., & Colarelli, S. M. (2018). The evolutionary mismatch hypothesis: Implications for psychological science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(1), 3844. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417731378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lively, C. M. (1986a). Predator-induced shell dimorphism in the acorn barnacle Chthamalus anisopoma. Evolution, 40(2), 232242. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1986.tb00466.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lively, C. M. (1986b). Competition, comparative life histories, and maintenance of shell dimorphism in a barnacle. Ecology, 67(4), 858864. https://doi.org/10.2307/1939808CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., & Lambert, H. K. (2014). Childhood adversity and neural development: Deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 578591. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare (2016). Specified Report of Vital Statistics in FY2016. https://www.mhlw.go.jp/english/database/db-hw/vs06.htmlGoogle Scholar
Ministry of Health, Labour, and Welfare (2020). Annual health, labour and welfare report 2020. https://www.mhlw.go.jp/stf/wp/hakusyo/kousei/19/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
Mishra, S., Barclay, P., & Sparks, A. (2017). The relative state model: Integrating need-based and ability-based pathways to risk-taking. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21(2), 176198. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868316644094CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mittal, C., & Griskevicius, V. (2014). Sense of control under uncertainty depends on people's childhood environment: A life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(4), 621637. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037398CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miura, A., & Kobayashi, T. (2015). Mechanical Japanese: Survey satisficing of online panels in Japan. Japanese Journal of Social Psychology, 31(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.14966/jssp.31.1_1Google Scholar
Nettle, D., & Frankenhuis, W. E. (2020). Life history theory in psychology and evolutionary biology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 375, article 20190490. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0490CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nettle, D., Frankenhuis, W. E., & Rickard, I. J. (2013). The evolution of predictive adaptive responses in human life history. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 280(1766), article 20131343. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.1343Google ScholarPubMed
Nettle, D., & Pollet, T. V. (2008). Natural selection on male wealth in humans. American Naturalist, 172(5), 658666. https://doi.org/10.1086/591690CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nijhout, H. F. (2003). Development and evolution of adaptive polyphenisms. Evolution and Development, 5(1), 918. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1525-142X.2003.03003.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pepper, G., & Nettle, D. (2017). The behavioural constellation of deprivation: Causes and consequences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, Article E314. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1600234XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérusse, D. (1993). Cultural and reproductive success in industrial societies: Testing the relationship at the proximate and ultimate levels. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(2), 267322. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00029939CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratikainen, I. I., & Kokko, H. (2019). The coevolution of lifespan and reversible plasticity. Nature Communications, 10, article 538. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-08502-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, G. B., Chen, C.-C., Dai, C.-L., Brubaker, M. D., & Nedelec, J. L. (2017). The psychometrics of the Mini-K: Evidence from two college samples. Evolutionary Psychology, 15(1). https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704916682034CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sear, R. (2020). Do human ‘life history strategies’ exist? Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(6), 513526. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.09.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommer, R. J. (2020). Phenotypic plasticity: From theory and genetics to current and future challenges. Genetics, 215(1), 113. https://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.120.303163CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stulp, G., & Barrett, L. (2016). Wealth, fertility and adaptive behaviour in industrial populations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371(1692), article 20150153. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0153CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vining, D. R. (1986). Social versus reproductive success: The central theoretical problem of human sociobiology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9(1), 167216. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00021968CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Four types of individuals (groups 1–4) divided by childhood environment (harsh vs. not harsh) and impulsivity (high vs. low)

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and correlation matrix of the independent variables

Figure 2

Table 3. Descriptive statistics and correlation matrix of the dependent variables

Figure 3

Figure 1. Distribution of fitness indices of four groups divided by medians of childhood socioeconomic status (SES) and impulsivity. Note: the hypothesis predicts a horizontally mirrored J-shape – highest, second highest, lowest and second lowest for groups 1–4, respectively. Results based on childhood SES × temporal discounting grouping, childhood SES × risk-taking grouping, and childhood SES × Mini-K grouping are presented as (a–e), (f–j) and (k–q), respectively. The dependent variables were number of children for (a), (f) and (k), marriage experience for (b), (g) and (m); annual household income for (c), (h) and (n); subjective SES for (d), (i) and (p); and life satisfaction for (e), (j) and (q).

Supplementary material: PDF

Kometani and Ohtsubo supplementary material

Kometani and Ohtsubo supplementary material

Download Kometani and Ohtsubo supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 2.7 MB