Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T00:36:45.336Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Relationship between subjective cognitive functioning and fluid and crystallized cognitive abilities in bipolar disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 October 2024

Ivan J. Torres*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada British Columbia Mental Health and Substance Use Services Research Institute, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Sylvia Mackala
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Sharon Ahn
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Erin E. Michalak
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Trisha Chakrabarty
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Raymond W. Lam
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Kamyar Keramatian
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Lakshmi N. Yatham
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
*
Corresponding author: Ivan J. Torres; Email: ivan.torres@ubc.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

People with bipolar disorder (BD) often show inaccurate subjective ratings of their objective cognitive function. However, it is unclear what information individuals use to formulate their subjective ratings. This study evaluated whether people with BD are likely using information about their crystallized cognitive abilities (which involve an accumulated store of verbal knowledge and skills and are typically preserved in BD) or their fluid cognitive abilities (which involve the capacity for new learning and information processing in novel situations and are typically impaired in BD) to formulate their subjective cognitive ratings.

Method:

Eighty participants diagnosed with BD and 55 control volunteers were administered cognitive tests assessing crystallized and fluid cognitive abilities. Subjective cognitive functioning was assessed with the Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ), daily functioning was rated using the Multidimensional Scale of Independent Functioning (MSIF) and the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale (GAF), and quality of life was assessed with the Quality of Life in Bipolar Disorder scale (QoL.BD).

Results:

The BD group exhibited considerably elevated subjective cognitive complaints relative to controls. Among participants with BD, CFQ scores were associated with fluid cognitive abilities including measures of memory and executive function, but not to crystallized abilities. After controlling for objective cognition and depression, higher cognitive complaints predicted poorer psychosocial outcomes.

Conclusions:

Cognitive self-reports in BD may represent a metacognitive difficulty whereby cognitive self-appraisals are distorted by a person’s focus on their cognitive weaknesses rather than strengths. Moreover, negative cognitive self-assessments are associated with poorer daily functioning and diminished quality of life.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Neuropsychological Society

Introduction

Cognitive impairment is a common clinical feature in people with bipolar disorder (BD) (Keramatian et al., Reference Keramatian, Torres and Yatham2021; Miskowiak et al., Reference Miskowiak, Burdick, Martinez‐Aran, Bonnin, Bowie, Carvalho, Gallagher, Lafer, López‐Jaramillo, Sumiyoshi, McIntyre, Schaffer, Porter, Purdon, Torres, Yatham, Young, Kessing and Vieta2018), and it is associated with diminished daily functioning and quality of life (QoL) (Baune & Malhi, Reference Baune and Malhi2015; Depp et al., Reference Depp, Mausbach, Harmell, Savla, Bowie, Harvey and Patterson2012; Koene et al., Reference Koene, Zyto, van der Stel, van Lang, Ammeraal, Kupka and van Weeghel2022; Mackala et al., Reference Mackala, Torres, Kozicky, Michalak and Yatham2014). However, cognitive impairment in BD is not uniform across different cognitive domains. Based on the two-component theory of intellectual development (Cattell, Reference Cattell1963; Horn & Cattell, Reference Horn and Cattell1966), contemporary assessment approaches distinguish between crystallized cognitive abilities (representing an accumulated store of verbal knowledge and skills and thus more heavily influenced by education and cultural exposure) and fluid cognitive abilities (involving the capacity for new learning and information processing in novel situations, and especially influenced by biological processes and less dependent on past exposure) (Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Akshoomoff, Tulsky, Mungas, Weintraub, Dikmen, Beaumont, Casaletto, Conway, Slotkin and Gershon2014; Kaufman & Kaufman, Reference Kaufman and Kaufman2004). Neuropsychological research in BD indicates that fluid cognitive abilities such as attention, memory, executive function, and processing speed are preferentially impaired relative to crystallized abilities such as vocabulary and verbal comprehension (Ko et al., Reference Ko, Park, Shin, Yu, Ryu and Lee2022; Mann-Wrobel et al., Reference Mann-Wrobel, Carreno and Dickinson2011; Torres et al., Reference Torres, Boudreau and Yatham2007).

Despite robust evidence of neuropsychological deficits on objective cognitive tests, subjective cognitive self-reports of people with BD generally correspond poorly or inconsistently to their objective cognitive performance (Burdick et al., Reference Burdick, Endick and Goldberg2005; Demant et al., Reference Demant, Vinberg, Kessing and Miskowiak2015; Lima et al., Reference Lima, Cardoso, Serafim, Martins, Solé, Martínez-Arán, Vieta and Rosa2018; Svendsen et al., Reference Svendsen, Kessing, Munkholm, Vinberg and Miskowiak2012; Toyoshima et al., Reference Toyoshima, Inoue, Masuya, Ichiki, Fujimura and Kusumi2019a; Van der Werf-Eldering et al., Reference Van der Werf-Eldering, Burger, Jabben, Holthausen, Aleman and Nolen2011). To a certain degree, the poor correspondence between objective and subjective cognitive measures may relate to the likelihood that these measures may not be tapping into fully overlapping constructs. That is, objective cognitive assessments sensitively and specifically delineate cognitive processes in a controlled laboratory environment, whereas subjective cognitive questionnaires probe cognitive functioning in everyday and home/living environments. Nevertheless, the discrepancy between objective and subjective cognitive functioning in BD may also be related to several interrelated factors. First, cognitive self-ratings in BD may reflect underlying mood symptoms which bias individuals to view themselves or their abilities in a negative light. The finding that subjective cognitive ratings frequently correlate with depressive symptoms provides some support for this explanation (Harvey et al., Reference Harvey, Paschall and Depp2015; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Lu, Huang, Chen, Luo and Zhu2019; Van der Werf-Eldering et al., Reference Van der Werf-Eldering, Burger, Jabben, Holthausen, Aleman and Nolen2011). A further possibility is that the poor correspondence between objective and subjective cognition in BD may reflect poor insight or awareness into one’s own cognitive functioning, which can be conceptualized as a metacognitive failure that results in inaccurate cognitive self-ratings (David et al., Reference David, Bedford, Wiffen and Gilleen2012; Roux et al., Reference Roux, Faivre, Cannavo, Brunet-Gouet and Passerieux2021; Torres et al., Reference Torres, Mackala, Kozicky and Yatham2016; Torres et al., Reference Torres, Hidiroglu, Mackala, Ahn, Yatham, Ozerdem and Michalak2021; Van Camp et al., Reference Van Camp, Sabbe and Oldenburg2019). The degree to which depression may underlie or contribute to a metacognitive deficit, however, remains unclear. Nevertheless, the finding that even euthymic individuals with BD tend to underestimate their cognitive functioning suggests that metacognitive difficulties may not be fully driven by depressed mood (Torres et al., Reference Torres, Hidiroglu, Mackala, Ahn, Yatham, Ozerdem and Michalak2021).

Regardless of whether driven by depression, poor metacognition, or both, the basis for the inaccurate cognitive self-reports in people with BD remains unclear. Specifically, it is not known what information people with BD utilize to formulate their subjective cognitive self-ratings. It is possible that individuals with BD may be relying on two potential primary sources to make their cognitive judgments, including either (1) their more impaired fluid cognitive skills/functioning or (2) their more intact crystallized cognitive skills/functioning. Clarification of the information that individuals use to make their subjective cognitive judgments could provide insights into the nature of metacognitive difficulties in BD and how these problems should be addressed. For example, people with BD may be exclusively focusing on their impaired fluid cognitive difficulties when they formulate their cognitive self-appraisals, possibly at the expense of considering their own cognitive strengths. Alternatively (and less likely), if subjective cognitive judgments are based exclusively on perceived intact crystallized cognitive skills, these judgments may be inaccurate because they are not reflective of the person’s current cognitive struggles or functioning. Each of these scenarios may underlie faulty metacognition in BD, which could in turn lead to poor daily outcomes. Unfortunately, existing studies assessing the relationship between objective and subjective cognitive functioning have almost exclusively utilized fluid cognitive tests that tend to be more sensitive to cognitive impairments associated with the illness (Burdick et al., Reference Burdick, Endick and Goldberg2005; Demant et al., Reference Demant, Vinberg, Kessing and Miskowiak2015; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Lu, Huang, Chen, Luo and Zhu2019; Luo et al., Reference Luo, Zhu, Lu, Zong and Lin2020; Toyoshima et al., Reference Toyoshima, Fujii, Mitsui, Kako, Asakura, Martinez-Aran, Vieta and Kusumi2017, Reference Toyoshima, Inoue, Masuya, Ichiki, Fujimura and Kusumi2019a), whereas use of crystallized cognitive tests has been rare.

The purpose of the present study was to investigate the type of information that people with BD likely utilize in formulating subjective cognitive ratings about their general cognitive functioning. To accomplish this, we evaluated the magnitude of correlation between subjective ratings of general cognitive function and objective measures of crystallized cognitive function (which tend to be more intact in BD), as well as between subjective ratings and fluid cognitive abilities (which are most impaired in BD). We hypothesized that subjective–objective correlations would be significant for cognitive domains in which people with BD show more pronounced cognitive deficits (fluid abilities), relative to cognitive domains where they show less impairment (crystallized abilities). This would imply that people with BD are focusing on their cognitive limitations, rather than their cognitive strengths, when generating subjective cognitive appraisals. A second objective was to assess whether subjective cognitive ratings would be predictive of psychosocial outcomes including daily functioning and QoL, even after controlling for mood symptoms and objective cognitive functioning.

Method

Participants

The study was conducted at the Mood Disorders Centre in the Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia (UBC). Participants were recruited from outpatient and inpatient hospital clinics, regional mood disorder groups and associations, local community mental health centers, and online advertisements. People with BD were required to be aged 17 years or older, have a primary Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5), diagnosis of BD type I or II, to be fluent in English, and to be sufficiently clinically stable to undergo cognitive assessment. Exclusion criteria included a history of serious neurological disorder or brain injury or alcohol or substance misuse in the past month. Participants completed a standardized protocol which included documentation of clinical history, symptom ratings and other clinical measures, and a structured clinical interview. Individuals with BD had received a documented chart diagnosis of BD by a treating psychiatrist or physician, and the diagnosis was confirmed by a clinical psychologist (IJT) and trained research assistant during a structured clinical interview utilizing strict DSM-5 criteria.

A healthy volunteer comparison group was recruited from the community through online and community postings. Healthy volunteers were also given a structured interview and were required to be aged 17 or older and fluent in English, but were excluded if they had a history of serious neurological disorder/brain injury, psychiatric disorder, diagnosed psychiatric disorder in first-degree relatives, or history of alcohol or substance use in the past month. All participants provided written informed consent, and the study received ethics approval from the UBC Clinical Research Ethics Board. This study was completed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. The sample for the present analysis overlapped with a previous study investigating prediction and postdiction cognitive ratings on specific neuropsychological tests (Torres et al., Reference Torres, Hidiroglu, Mackala, Ahn, Yatham, Ozerdem and Michalak2021); however, the present study involved a larger sample size and included data from the Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ; Broadbent et al., Reference Broadbent, Cooper, FitzGerald and Parkes1982).

Objective cognitive functioning

A neuropsychological battery assessing both crystallized and fluid cognitive skills was utilized, and the categorization of cognitive measures into each of these two broad domains was based on prior work utilizing similar neuropsychological batteries (Cassetta et al., Reference Cassetta, Menon, Carrion, Pearce, DeGraaf, Leonova, White, Stowe, Honer, Woodward and Torres2020; Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Akshoomoff, Tulsky, Mungas, Weintraub, Dikmen, Beaumont, Casaletto, Conway, Slotkin and Gershon2014).

Three tests were used to assess crystallized abilities. To estimate premorbid intellectual functioning through single-word reading ability, the North American Adult Reading Test (NAART) estimated full-scale IQ score was utilized (Blair & Spreen, Reference Blair and Spreen1989). Verbal knowledge/vocabulary was assessed using the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test–2 (K-BIT-2) (Kaufman & Kaufman, Reference Kaufman and Kaufman2004) Verbal Knowledge subtest, and verbal comprehension/reasoning was assessed using the K-BIT-2 Riddles subtest.

Seven tests were used to measure fluid abilities. To assess fluid nonverbal reasoning, the K-BIT-2 Matrices subtest was used. Verbal learning/memory was assessed with the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) (Lezak, Reference Lezak2004), utilizing total words recalled across learning trials 1–3. Nonverbal memory was assessed using the Extended Complex Figure Test (ECFT) Delayed Recall Score (Fastenau, Reference Fastenau2003). Visual–spatial construction ability was assessed using the ECFT Copy Trial score. Mental processing speed was assessed with the Trailmaking Test part A, and attentional shifting ability was measured using Trails B (Reitan & Wolfson, Reference Reitan and Wolfson1985). To assess abstract conceptual learning ability, a computerized version of the Halstead Category Test was employed (Choca et al., Reference Choca, Garside, Gupta and Fenstermacher2009). This test requires participants to determine the underlying concept for each of 208 stimuli that are presented consecutively on a computer screen. The task was slightly modified (for purposes of another study) by requiring participants to provide confidence ratings of their performance after each trial (data not presented here). The total correct score out of 208 items was utilized.

Subjective cognitive functioning

Subjective cognitive functioning was measured with the CFQ, a self-report questionnaire that assesses failures and lapses in everyday cognitive functioning (Broadbent et al., Reference Broadbent, Cooper, FitzGerald and Parkes1982). The CFQ has also been commonly used to study subjective cognition in BD (Burdick et al., Reference Burdick, Endick and Goldberg2005; Demant et al., Reference Demant, Vinberg, Kessing and Miskowiak2015; Groenman et al., Reference Groenman, van der Werf and Geurts2022; Van der Werf-Eldering et al., Reference Van der Werf-Eldering, Burger, Jabben, Holthausen, Aleman and Nolen2011). Each of the 25 items on the scale is rated from 0 (never) to 4 (very often), for a total score ranging between 0 and 100 with higher scores reflecting poorer subjective ratings of cognitive functioning. The CFQ has been demonstrated to load onto a single factor (Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Timpano, Llabre and Bainter2022).

Daily functional outcome

Functional outcome was assessed with the MSIF, a structured interview scale assessing a person’s functioning during the previous month in the primary environments of work, educational, and residential. The MSIF was developed for use in patients with psychiatric disorders and validated in samples with BD (Berns et al., Reference Berns, Uzelac, Gonzalez and Jaeger2007; Jaeger et al., Reference Jaeger, Berns and Czobor2003). The MSIF requires the rater to consider the client’s role expectations, level of support, and performance in order to derive a global rating for each of the three environments. We used the Overall Global rating which represents a summary rating across the three major functional environments, using the following anchors: 1 = essentially normal functioning, 2 = very mild disability, 3 = somewhat disabled, 4 = moderately disabled, 5 = significantly disabled, 6 = extremely disabled, 7 = totally disabled. As a secondary measure of functioning, we utilized the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale (GAF), a clinician-rated scale of psychological, social, and occupational functioning which is rated from 0 (lowest) to 100 (highest) based on anchors describing varying levels of functioning (Endicott et al., Reference Endicott, Spitzer, Fleiss and Cohen1976).

QoL

QoL was assessed with the Quality of Life in Bipolar Disorder (QoL.BD) scale, a self-report questionnaire measuring well-being across multiple life domains in individuals living with BD (Michalak & Murray, Reference Michalak and Murray2010). The items are answered on a 5-point Likert scale, with higher scores reflecting higher QoL. We used the total score based on the sum of items across the 12 primary domains (four questions per domain), which yields scores ranging from a minimum of 48 to a maximum of 240.

Mood symptoms

Mood symptoms were assessed by a trained rater utilizing the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD) (Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1960) and the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) (Young et al., Reference Young, Biggs, Ziegler and Meyer1978).

Statistical analysis

Raw scores for each of the 10 cognitive measures were converted into z-scores based on demographic-adjusted normative data derived from the cognitive test manuals. Composite cognitive scores were computed for crystallized ability (by averaging z-scores for the three crystallized measures) and for fluid abilities (by averaging z-scores for the seven fluid measures). Group differences in objective and subjective cognitive scores were assessed using t-tests. Based on the presence of 10 primary objective cognitive measures, a Bonferonni adjusted alpha level of p < .005 was used. Pearson correlation coefficients were utilized to evaluate the relationship between the CFQ and objective cognitive measures. Partial correlations were also calculated, controlling for depressive symptom scores and for fluid composite scores (for the individual crystallized measures) or crystallized composite scores (for the fluid measures). In order to assess the relative contributions of the crystallized and fluid composite scores to predicting CFQ scores, a regression model was constructed entering these two predictors as independent variables and the CFQ as the dependent variable.

Hierarchical multiple regressions were utilized to assess predictors of the three primary psychosocial outcomes, including MSIF, GAF, and QoL.BD scores. In the first model, HAMD scores and fluid and crystallized composite scores were entered as predictors of outcome. The CFQ was subsequently entered into the model to assess this variable’s unique contribution to predicting each of the three primary outcomes. Assumptions for regression were assessed by visualizing scatterplots between independent variables and the dependent variable, visualizing residual plots, and utilizing collinearity, Durbin–Watson, and Cook’s Distance statistics.

Results

Demographics and clinical characteristics

The BD group had a mean age of 38.4 years (SD = 10.2), mean duration of illness of 21.6 years (SD = 11.3), mean education of 15.0 years (SD = 2.1), and 76% had a diagnosis of BD type I. Mood rating scales indicated that participants with BD showed a low level of depressive and manic symptoms (Table 1). A total of 18%, 3%, and 0% of the sample were in a current depressive, hypomanic, or manic episode. A substantial proportion of people in the BD group had a history of psychosis as well as history of alcohol/substance use comorbidity, and the majority were treated with mood stabilizers or antipsychotics. Details of demographic and clinical features are presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Demographics and clinical variables for participants with BD and healthy controls

*p < .05; ***p < .001 difference between patients and controls.

Individuals with BD were comparable to controls with regard to mean age, years of education, premorbid IQ, and sex (Table 1). There was a slightly higher proportion of non-white individuals among controls relative to patients (X 2 = 7.09, p = 0.03); however, the proportion of individuals who learned English as their first language was comparable between groups (X 2 = 1.85, p = 0.17). People with BD had lower current employment/schooling rates than controls (X 2 = 6.95, p = 0.03).

Group differences in objective and subjective cognitive functioning

Table 2 summarizes the objective and subjective cognitive data between groups.

Table 2. Objective and subjective cognition in participants with BD and controls

Note. NAART = North American Adult Reading Test, K-BIT-2 = Kaufmann Brief Intelligence Test Second Edition, VK = verbal knowledge, RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, ECFT = Extended Complex Figures Test, CFQ = Cognitive Failures Questionnaire

a n = 45 in control group.

b n = 45 in control group; Mann–Whitney U reported.

c n = 54 in control group.

Reported p-value is prior to correction for multiple comparisons.

People with BD showed numerically poorer performance than controls on most objective cognitive measures, especially fluid abilities. However, after correction for multiple comparisons, these differences did not reach statistically significant levels. Nevertheless, within the bipolar group, mean crystallized composite scores were significantly higher than mean composite fluid scores, t(79) = 2.08, p = .04. Regarding subjective cognition, there was a significant difference between groups in CFQ scores indicating more subjective cognitive complaints in participants with BD relative to controls, t(133) = 7.15, p < .001.

Associations between CFQ and objective cognitive functioning

Figure 1 displays the Pearson correlations between CFQ scores and each of the objective cognitive measures in participants with BD. There was no significant correlation between the CFQ and any of the three crystallized cognitive measures (all p > 0.05). In contrast, there were significant correlations between the CFQ score and most fluid cognitive measures, including K-BIT-2 Matrices, RAVLT Learning, ECFT Copy, and ECFT Delay scores (Figure 1). In all cases, increased cognitive complaints were associated with poorer cognitive performance. As above, the partial correlation analyses controlling for depression and the fluid composite score failed to reveal a significant association between any of the crystallized tests and CFQ (all p > .25). However, after controlling for depression and crystallized composite score, there was a significant association between CFQ and K-BIT-2 Matrices, r(76) = −0.25, p = .03, RAVLT Learning, r(76) = −0.23, p = .04, ECFT Copy, r(76) = −0.37, p < .001, and a trend for ECFT Delay r(76) = −0.21, p = .06. With regard to fluid and crystallized composite scores, there was a significant association between CFQ scores and fluid skills (r = −0.30, p = .008), but not between CFQ scores and crystallized skills (r = −0.13, p = 0.25). Moreover, in the regression model, fluid, t(77) = −2.45, p = .02, but not crystallized composite scores, t(77) = −0.18, p = .86, significantly predicted CFQ scores. There was no significant relationship between CFQ scores and any of the cognitive variables in the control group.

Figure 1. Correlations between Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) scores and objective cognitive functions in bipolar disorder sample. Light-shaded bars represent crystallized cognitive skills, and dark-shaded bars represent fluid cognitive skills. NAART = North American Adult Reading Test, K-BIT-2 = Kaufmann Brief Intelligence Test Second Edition, RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, ECFT = Extended Complex Figures Test.

Predictors of psychosocial outcomes

Table 3 displays the correlation matrix between predictors (depressive symptoms, crystallized and fluid composite scores, CFQ score) and the three psychosocial outcomes in people with BD. Most of the predictors were significantly associated with all the psychosocial outcomes, with the exception that crystallized ability was not associated with any of the three outcomes, and fluid ability was not associated with QoL.BD scores (Table 3).

Table 3. Correlation matrix between predictors and psychosocial outcomes

*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

Table 4 summarizes the results of the multiple regression analyses for each of the three primary psychosocial outcomes. After controlling for the effects of residual depressive symptoms and crystallized and fluid composite scores, CFQ was a significant predictor of MSIF scores, such that increased cognitive complaints were associated with poorer functioning. These findings were replicated when GAF served as the dependent variable (Table 4). Similarly, after controlling for depression and fluid and crystallized composite scores, CFQ was a significant predictor of QoL.BD scores, such that increased cognitive complaints were associated with poorer quality of life.

Table 4. Hierarchical regression models for predictors of MSIF, GAF, and quality of life

Note. MSIF = Multidimensional Scale of Independent Functioning, HAMD = Hamilton Depression Rating Scale, CFQ = Cognitive Failures Questionnaire, GAF = Global Assessment of Functioning, QoL.BD = Quality of Life in Bipolar Disorder Scale.

Discussion

The primary goal of this study was to investigate the association between subjective cognitive functioning and objective cognitive performance across different cognitive domains to clarify how people with BD formulate their cognitive self-appraisals. We found that individuals with BD showed highly elevated cognitive complaints relative to people without BD that were out of proportion to objective cognitive functioning differences between the patient and control groups. This finding is consistent with prior studies that demonstrate elevated cognitive complaints even in euthymic BD (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Lu, Huang, Chen, Luo and Zhu2019; Luo et al., Reference Luo, Zhu, Lu, Zong and Lin2020; Martinez-Aran et al., Reference Martínez-Arán, Vieta, Colom, Torrent, Reinares, Goikolea, Benabarre, Comes and Sánchez-Moreno2005; Simjanoski et al., Reference Simjanoski, Jansen, Mondin, Pedrotti Moreira, Vieira, da Silva, Souza, Frey, Cardoso and Kapczinski2021).

Furthermore, we found that cognitive complaints were preferentially associated with fluid cognitive abilities including verbal and nonverbal learning/memory, as well as some aspects of executive functioning (fluid nonverbal reasoning, visual construction). In contrast, there was no significant association between cognitive complaints and crystallized cognitive skills. This suggests that individuals with BD base their cognitive self-appraisals on impaired fluid cognitive skills, without reference to their generally preserved crystallized skills. Moreover, these self-assessments may reflect a person’s perception that their cognition is diminished relative to a prior level. This is consistent with the finding that in the BD group crystallized skills, which often index premorbid abilities, were higher than fluid skills, which are likely to represent acquired cognitive problems.

These findings are broadly in accord with previous studies in BD that have also reported significant associations between subjective cognitive functioning and fluid cognitive skills including various aspects of memory and/or executive function (Aydemir & Kaya, Reference Aydemir and Kaya2009; Demant et al., Reference Demant, Vinberg, Kessing and Miskowiak2015; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Lu, Huang, Chen, Luo and Zhu2019; Luo et al., Reference Luo, Zhu, Lu, Zong and Lin2020; Rosa et al., Reference Rosa, Mercadé, Sánchez-Moreno, Solé, Mar Bonnin, Torrent, Grande, Sugranyes, Popovic, Salamero, Kapczinski, Vieta and Martinez-Aran2013; Xiao et al., Reference Xiao, Lin, Wang, Lu and Tang2015). Nevertheless, not all studies report significant correlations between subjective cognition and fluid memory or executive skills (Burdick et al., Reference Burdick, Endick and Goldberg2005; Lima et al., Reference Lima, Cardoso, Serafim, Martins, Solé, Martínez-Arán, Vieta and Rosa2018; Toyoshima et al., Reference Toyoshima, Inoue, Masuya, Ichiki, Fujimura and Kusumi2019a; Van der Werf-Eldering et al., Reference Van der Werf-Eldering, Burger, Jabben, Holthausen, Aleman and Nolen2011). This variation in results may be related to methodological differences among studies along characteristics including power/sample size, symptom status of patient sample, subjective cognition measure used, objective neuropsychological tests used, and component of memory or executive function that was studied. Even within our study, not all fluid tests correlated with the CFQ. We note that all of the fluid measures that showed significant correlations with CFQ were measures that were untimed and that two of the three fluid measures that did not correlate with CFQ were timed. It may be that if patients can still complete a task accurately even if it takes longer for them to do it, their subjective perception may be that they don’t have a problem. In contrast, if they struggle on a task that has no time limits they may be more inclined to perceive their performance as problematic, and hence their ratings may correlate more with their diminished performance on these untimed fluid tasks. Regardless, consistent with our findings, most prior work does support a consistent association between subjective cognition and objective fluid skills of memory and executive functioning.

A novel contribution of the present study is that crystallized skills were not associated with subjective cognition. In one of the few previous studies that utilized a crystallized cognitive measure, Rosa et al. (Reference Rosa, Mercadé, Sánchez-Moreno, Solé, Mar Bonnin, Torrent, Grande, Sugranyes, Popovic, Salamero, Kapczinski, Vieta and Martinez-Aran2013) also failed to find a significant association between subjective cognitive complaints and expressive vocabulary ability. In sum, the finding that cognitive self-ratings correlate with fluid abilities suggests that individuals with BD utilize information about their cognitive weaknesses (e.g., memory and executive functioning) in formulating their cognitive self-ratings. In contrast, they do not appear to focus on their intact crystallized skills such as vocabulary/verbal knowledge, verbal reasoning/comprehension, or single-word reading.

The preferential focus on cognitive weaknesses, which leads to highly elevated cognitive complaints, coupled with the absence of focus on cognitive strengths, is consistent with the view that subjective ratings in BD may reflect a metacognitive failure. That is, people with BD may have a distorted, negatively biased sense of their own cognitive functioning. Moreover, this negatively oriented metacognitive difficulty may be reflective of a broader bias toward the processing of negatively valenced information in BD (Gopin et al., Reference Gopin, Burdick, Derosse, Goldberg and Malhotra2011), or the presence of implicit negative self-associations or self-schemas that characterize individuals with BD even in the euthymic state (Granger et al., Reference Granger, Pavlis, Collett and Hallam2021; Jabben et al., Reference Jabben, de Jong, Kupka, Glashouwer, Nolen and Penninx2014).

To what extent does this negative cognitive self-view relate to depressive symptoms? On the one hand, consistent with prior work, depression was strongly correlated with cognitive complaints, indicating that even subsyndromal depression associates with cognitive complaints (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Lu, Huang, Chen, Luo and Zhu2019; Van der Werf-Eldering et al., Reference Van der Werf-Eldering, Burger, Jabben, Holthausen, Aleman and Nolen2011). Nevertheless, the hierarchical regression analyses consistently revealed that even when depression and objective cognitive functioning (both crystallized and fluid) were controlled, elevated cognitive complaints in BD still had a negative impact on both objective indices of daily functioning (MSIF, GAF) as well as self-reports of QoL (QoL.BD). Prior studies in BD have also reported a relationship between elevated cognitive complaints and poorer functioning (Grover et al., Reference Grover, Avasthi, Chakravarty, Dan, Chakraborty, Neogi, Desouza, Nayak, Praharaj, Menon, Deep, Bathla, Subramanyam, Nebhinani, Ghosh, Lakdawala and Bhattacharya2023; Jensen et al., Reference Jensen, Støttrup, Nayberg, Knorr, Ullum, Purdon, Kessing and Miskowiak2015) and diminished QoL (O’Rourke et al., Reference O’Rourke, Sixsmith, Kirshner and Osher2021; Tatay-Manteiga et al., Reference Tatay-Manteiga, Cauli, Tabarés-Seisdedos, Michalak, Kapczinski and Balanzá-Martínez2019), even after controlling for depressive symptoms (Demant et al., Reference Demant, Vinberg, Kessing and Miskowiak2015; Luo et al., Reference Luo, Zhu, Lu, Zong and Lin2020; Toyoshima et al., Reference Toyoshima, Kako, Toyomaki, Shimizu, Tanaka, Nakagawa, Inoue, Martinez-Aran, Vieta and Kusumi2019b; Xiao et al., Reference Xiao, Gao, Zhang, Chen and Sun2016).

The present findings are also in line with prior work indicating that euthymic individuals with BD predominantly underestimate their cognitive ability/performance relative to controls (Torres et al., Reference Torres, Hidiroglu, Mackala, Ahn, Yatham, Ozerdem and Michalak2021) and that underestimations are associated with poorer functioning (Miskowiak et al., Reference Miskowiak, Petersen, Ott, Knorr, Kessing, Gallagher and Robinson2016). Together, these findings provide support for the hypothesis that negative cognitive self-appraisals are not exclusively a function of depressive symptoms, as they still exert a negative impact on peoples’ lives and functioning even when depression is controlled. As such, these negative cognitive self-views may represent a behavioral marker for a trait-related feature associated with BD. The question relating the source of this trait (i.e., whether it reflects a premorbid vulnerability marker or a “scar” related to cumulative mood episodes) cannot be addressed in the present study and would likely require longitudinal investigation. Regardless of the source, the strong and consistent association between CFQ scores and psychosocial outcomes suggests that efforts to address or modify these negative cognitive self-referents through cognitive remediation (CR) and similar therapies should be prioritized. Increasingly, CR therapies developed for use in BD emphasize the need to incorporate assessment and modification of metacognitive components (Tsapekos et al., Reference Tsapekos, Strawbridge, Cella, Wykes and Young2020, Tsapekos et al., Reference Tsapekos, Strawbridge, Cella, Goldsmith, Kalfas, Taylor, Swidzinski, Marwaha, Grey, Newton, Shackleton, Harrison, Browning, Harmer, Hartland, Cousins, Barton, Wykes and Young2023). Based on the findings from the present study, one approach may be to assess for negative cognitive self-appraisals so that they can be evaluated and modified to better align with a person’s goals or actual cognitive functioning/performance. This might entail (1) conducting objective and subjective cognitive assessments; (2) reviewing and comparing these with the client, highlighting discrepancies; (3) allowing the client to identify strengths rather than exclusively focusing on weaknesses; and (4) helping the client challenge negative self-appraisals by testing these assumptions through simulated or real-life scenarios. Such realignment may have the potential to lessen the impact of negative cognitive self-appraisals and improve overall functioning and QoL.

The present findings should be viewed in the context of several study limitations. First, the mean education level for both groups was relatively high, which in part reflects the demographic profile of individuals who are typically interested in participating in research studies. This may impact the generalizability of our findings and suggests that further work should be conducted in samples with a wider range of education level. This factor may also account for the fact that the objective cognitive differences between people with BD and controls in the present study were not as large as is typically observed in other studies. Nevertheless, this fact underscores the robustness of the findings herein. That is, prominent patient–control differences in subjective cognitive functioning were observed even when groups were relatively similar with regard to their objective cognitive performance. Finally, another potential interpretation of these data might be that correlations between fluid abilities and CFQ scores are significant because the content of items on the CFQ (or similar scales) reflects daily cognitive mishaps that are more related to these types of abilities (e.g., memory, executive function). However, the fact that fluid cognition–CFQ correlations were not observed in our control group, and are not typically observed in healthy populations (Carrigan & Barkus, Reference Carrigan and Barkus2016; Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Timpano, Llabre and Bainter2022), argues against the idea that the item content is driving the observed findings. Despite this, it is likely that fluid skills are indeed preferentially involved in many everyday functions, and thus decrements in fluid skills that are observed in clinical populations are likely to correlate strongly with diminished daily functioning (Cassetta et al., Reference Cassetta, Menon, Carrion, Pearce, DeGraaf, Leonova, White, Stowe, Honer, Woodward and Torres2020).

In sum, these data support the idea that cognitive self-reports in BD may represent a metacognitive difficulty, as cognitive self-appraisals appear distorted by self-focus on cognitive weaknesses, rather than cognitive strengths. Findings also confirm that subjective cognitive ratings are associated with subsyndromal depressive symptoms. However, depression does not fully explain this metacognitive difficulty, as even when depression and objective cognitive function are accounted for, more subjective cognitive complaints are still predictive of poor psychosocial outcomes in terms of daily functioning and quality of life.

Acknowledgements

We are thankful to all participants who were a part of this study.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (IJT, grant number MOP-115166).

Competing interests

IT has served as consultant for Boehringer Ingelheim Canada and received grant funding from Michael Smith Health Research/BC Schizophrenia Society Foundation. EM has received support from Otsuka-Lundbeck for patient education activities. TC has received grant funding from Michael Smith Health Research BC and the National Research Council Canada. RL has received honoraria for ad hoc speaking or advising/consulting or received research funds from Abbvie, Asia-Pacific Economic Cooperation, Bausch, BC Leading Edge Foundation, Brain Canada, Canadian Institutes of Health Research, Canadian Network for Mood and Anxiety Treatments, Carnot, CB Solutions, Genome BC, Grand Challenges Canada, Healthy Minds Canada, Janssen, Lundbeck, Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research, MITACS, Neurotorium, Ontario Brain Institute, Otsuka, Shanghai Mental Health Center, Unity Health, Vancouver Coastal Health Research Institute, and VGH-UBCH Foundation. KK is supported by a Tier 2 Canada Research Chair in Early Interventions in Psychosis/Bipolar Disorder and has previously served on the scientific advisory board of AbbVie. LY reports consultant/speaker fees from Alkermes, Allergan (currently Abbvie), Sumitomo Pharma, Intracellular Therapies, LivaNova, Merck, Sanofi, and Sunovion, and grants from Allergan (now AbbVie), CIHR, and Sumitomo, outside the submitted work, over the last 3 years. No other authors report conflicts.

Ethical standard

This study was approved by the UBC Clinical Research Ethics Board including the informed consent practices used herein.

References

Aydemir, O., & Kaya, E. (2009). What does the subjective assessment of cognitive functioning measure in bipolar disorder? Correlation with the objective assessment of cognitive functioning. Turkish Journal of Psychiatry, 20(4), 332338.Google ScholarPubMed
Baune, B. T., & Malhi, G. S. (2015). A review on the impact of cognitive dysfunction on social, occupational, and general functional outcomes in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 17(S2), 4155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berns, S., Uzelac, S., Gonzalez, C., & Jaeger, J. (2007). Methodological considerations of measuring disability in bipolar disorder: Validity of the multidimensional scale of independent functioning. Bipolar Disorders, 9(1-2), 310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, J. R., & Spreen, O. (1989). Predicting premorbid IQ: A revision of the national adult reading test. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 3(2), 129136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broadbent, D. E., Cooper, P. F., FitzGerald, P., & Parkes, K. R. (1982). The cognitive failures questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21(1), 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burdick, K. E., Endick, C. J., & Goldberg, J. F. (2005). Assessing cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder: Are self-reports valid? Psychiatry Research, 136(1), 4350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrigan, N., & Barkus, E. (2016). A systematic review of cognitive failures in daily life: Healthy populations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 63, 2942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassetta, B. D., Menon, M., Carrion, P. B., Pearce, H., DeGraaf, A., Leonova, O., White, R. F., Stowe, R. M., Honer, W. G., Woodward, T. S., & Torres, I. J. (2020). Preliminary examination of the validity of the NIH toolbox cognition battery in treatment-resistant psychosis. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 34(5), 9811003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54(1), 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choca, J. L., Garside, D., Gupta, R., & Fenstermacher, J. (2009). The computer category test (3rd ed.). Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
David, A. S., Bedford, N., Wiffen, B., & Gilleen, J. (2012). Failures of metacognition and lack of insight in neuropsychiatric disorders. Philosophical Transactions Of The Royal Society Of London Series B, Biological Sciences, 367(1594), 13791390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demant, K. M., Vinberg, M., Kessing, L. V., & Miskowiak, K. W. (2015). Assessment of subjective and objective cognitive function in bipolar disorder: Correlations, predictors and the relation to psychosocial function. Psychiatry Research, 229(1-2), 565571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Depp, C. A., Mausbach, B. T., Harmell, A. L., Savla, G. N., Bowie, C. R., Harvey, P. D., & Patterson, T. L. (2012). Meta-analysis of the association between cognitive abilities and everyday functioning in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 14(3), 217226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Endicott, J., Spitzer, R. L., Fleiss, J. L., & Cohen, J. (1976). The global assessment scale: A procedure for measuring overall severity of psychiatric disturbance. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33(6), 766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fastenau, P. S. (2003). Extended complex figure test (ECFT) manual. Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Goodman, Z. T., Timpano, K. R., Llabre, M. M., & Bainter, S. A. (2022). Revisiting the factor structure and construct validity of the cognitive failures questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 34(7), 671683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopin, C. B., Burdick, K. E., Derosse, P., Goldberg, T. E., & Malhotra, A. K. (2011). Emotional modulation of response inhibition in stable patients with bipolar I disorder: A comparison with healthy and schizophrenia subjects. Bipolar Disorders, 13(2), 164172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granger, S., Pavlis, A., Collett, J., & Hallam, K. T. (2021). Revisiting the “manic defence hypothesis”: Assessing explicit and implicit cognitive biases in euthymic bipolar disorder. Clinical Psychologist, 25(2), 212222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Groenman, A. P., van der Werf, S., & Geurts, H. M. (2022). Subjective cognition in adults with common psychiatric classifications; a systematic review. Psychiatry Research, 308, 114374114374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grover, S., Avasthi, A., Chakravarty, R., Dan, A., Chakraborty, K., Neogi, R., Desouza, A., Nayak, O., Praharaj, S., Menon, V., Deep, R., Bathla, M., Subramanyam, A. A., Nebhinani, N., Ghosh, P., Lakdawala, B., & Bhattacharya, R. (2023). Subjective cognitive deficits and its correlates among patients with bipolar disorder: Findings from the bipolar disorder course and outcome study from India (BiD-CoIN study). Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 65(3), 345355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 23(1), 5662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, P. D., Paschall, G., & Depp, C. (2015). Factors influencing self-assessment of cognition and functioning in bipolar disorder: A preliminary study. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 20(4), 361371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, R. K., Akshoomoff, N., Tulsky, D., Mungas, D., Weintraub, S., Dikmen, S., Beaumont, J., Casaletto, K. B., Conway, K., Slotkin, J., & Gershon, R. (2014). Reliability and validity of composite scores from the NIH toolbox cognition battery in adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20(6), 588598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, J. L., & Cattell, R. B. (1966). Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallized general intelligences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 57(5), 253270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jabben, N., de Jong, P. J., Kupka, R. W., Glashouwer, K. A., Nolen, W. A., & Penninx, B. W. (2014). Implicit and explicit self-associations in bipolar disorder: A comparison with healthy controls and unipolar depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 215(2), 329334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaeger, J., Berns, S. M., & Czobor, P. (2003). The multidimensional scale of independent functioning: A new instrument for measuring functional disability in psychiatric populations. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 29(1), 153168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen, J. H.øy, Støttrup, M. M., Nayberg, E., Knorr, U., Ullum, H., Purdon, S. E., Kessing, L. V., & Miskowiak, K. W. (2015). Optimising screening for cognitive dysfunction in bipolar disorder: Validation and evaluation of objective and subjective tools. Journal of Affective Disorders, 187, 1019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman brief intelligence test (2nd ed.). Pearson.Google Scholar
Keramatian, K., Torres, I. J., & Yatham, L. N. (2021). Neurocognitive functioning in bipolar disorder: What we know and what we don’t. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 23(1), 2938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ko, H., Park, D., Shin, J., Yu, R., Ryu, V., & Lee, W. (2022). Cognitive profiles in bipolar I disorder and associated risk factors: Using Wechsler adult intelligence scale-IV. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 951043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koene, J., Zyto, S., van der Stel, J., van Lang, N., Ammeraal, M., Kupka, R. W., & van Weeghel, J. (2022). The relations between executive functions and occupational functioning in individuals with bipolar disorder: A scoping review. International Journal of Bipolar Disorders, 10(1), 8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lezak, M. D. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment (4th ed.). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lima, F. M., Cardoso, T. A., Serafim, S. D. D., Martins, D. S., Solé, B., Martínez-Arán, A., Vieta, E., & Rosa, A. R. (2018). Validity and reliability of the cognitive complaints in bipolar disorder rating assessment (COBRA) in Brazilian bipolar patients. Trends in Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 40(2), 170178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, X., Lu, D., Huang, Z., Chen, W., Luo, X., & Zhu, Y. (2019). The associations between subjective and objective cognitive functioning across manic or hypomanic, depressed, and euthymic states in Chinese bipolar patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 249, 7381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, X., Zhu, Y., Lu, D., Zong, K., & Lin, X. (2020). Subjective cognitive dysfunction in patients with bipolar disorder: The prevalence, related factors and effects on predicting psychosocial functioning and suicidal ideation. Psychiatry Research, 284, 112669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackala, S. A., Torres, I. J., Kozicky, J., Michalak, E. E., & Yatham, L. N. (2014). Cognitive performance and quality of life early in the course of bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 168, 119124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann-Wrobel, M. C., Carreno, J. T., & Dickinson, D. (2011). Meta-analysis of neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: An update and investigation of moderator variables. Bipolar Disorders, 13(4), 334342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martínez-Arán, A., Vieta, E., Colom, F., Torrent, C., Reinares, M., Goikolea, J. M., Benabarre, A., Comes, M., & Sánchez-Moreno, J. (2005). Do cognitive complaints in euthymic bipolar patients reflect objective cognitive impairment? Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 74(5), 295302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michalak, E. E., & Murray, G. (2010). Development of the QoL.BD: A disorder-specific scale to assess quality of life in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 12(7), 727740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miskowiak, K. W., Burdick, K. E., Martinez‐Aran, A., Bonnin, C. M., Bowie, C. R., Carvalho, A. F., Gallagher, P., Lafer, B., López‐Jaramillo, C., Sumiyoshi, T., McIntyre, R. S., Schaffer, A., Porter, R. J., Purdon, S., Torres, I. J., Yatham, L. N., Young, A. H., Kessing, L. V., & Vieta, E. (2018). Assessing and addressing cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder: The international society for bipolar disorders targeting cognition task force recommendations for clinicians. Bipolar Disorders, 20(3), 184194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miskowiak, K. W., Petersen, J. Z., Ott, C. V., Knorr, U., Kessing, L. V., Gallagher, P., & Robinson, L. (2016). Predictors of the discrepancy between objective and subjective cognition in bipolar disorder: A novel methodology. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 134(6), 511521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Rourke, N., Sixsmith, A., Kirshner, G., & Osher, Y. (2021). Perceived cognitive failures and quality of life for older adults with bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 287, 433440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1985). The halstead-reitan neuropsychological test battery. Neuropsychology Press.Google Scholar
Rosa, A. R., Mercadé, C., Sánchez-Moreno, J., Solé, B., Mar Bonnin, C. D., Torrent, C., Grande, I., Sugranyes, G., Popovic, D., Salamero, M., Kapczinski, F., Vieta, E., & Martinez-Aran, A. (2013). Validity and reliability of a rating scale on subjective cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder (COBRA). Journal of Affective Disorders, 150(1), 2936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roux, P., Faivre, N., Cannavo, A. S., Brunet-Gouet, E., & Passerieux, C. (2021). Exploring clinical correlates of metacognition in bipolar disorders using moderation analyses: The role of antipsychotics. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 10(19), 4349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simjanoski, M., Jansen, K., Mondin, T. D. C., Pedrotti Moreira, F., Vieira, I. S., da Silva, R. A., Souza, L. D. M., Frey, B. N., Cardoso, T. A., & Kapczinski, Fávio (2021). Cognitive complaints in individuals recently diagnosed with bipolar disorder: A cross-sectional study. Psychiatry Research, 300, 113894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Svendsen, A. M., Kessing, L. V., Munkholm, K., Vinberg, M., & Miskowiak, K. W. (2012). Is there an association between subjective and objective measures of cognitive function in patients with affective disorders? Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 66(4), 248253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tatay-Manteiga, A., Cauli, O., Tabarés-Seisdedos, R., Michalak, E. E., Kapczinski, F., & Balanzá-Martínez, V. (2019). Subjective neurocognition and quality of life in patients with bipolar disorder and siblings. Journal of Affective Disorders, 245, 283288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torres, I. J., Boudreau, V. G., & Yatham, L. N. (2007). Neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 116(s434), 1726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torres, I. J., Hidiroglu, C., Mackala, S. A., Ahn, S., Yatham, L. N., Ozerdem, E., & Michalak, E. E. (2021). Metacognitive knowledge and experience across multiple cognitive domains in euthymic bipolar disorder. European Psychiatry, 64(1), e36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torres, I. J., Mackala, S. A., Kozicky, J. M., & Yatham, L. N. (2016). Metacognitive knowledge and experience in recently diagnosed patients with bipolar disorder. Journal of Clinical And Experimental Neuropsychology, 38(7), 730744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toyoshima, K., Fujii, Y., Mitsui, N., Kako, Y., Asakura, S., Martinez-Aran, A., Vieta, E., & Kusumi, I. (2017). Validity and reliability of the cognitive complaints in bipolar disorder rating assessment (COBRA) in Japanese patients with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research, 254, 8589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toyoshima, K., Inoue, T., Masuya, J., Ichiki, M., Fujimura, Y., & Kusumi, I. (2019a). Evaluation of subjective cognitive function using the cognitive complaints in bipolar disorder rating assessment (COBRA) in Japanese adults. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 15, 29812990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toyoshima, K., Kako, Y., Toyomaki, A., Shimizu, Y., Tanaka, T., Nakagawa, S., Inoue, T., Martinez-Aran, A., Vieta, E., & Kusumi, I. (2019b). Associations between cognitive impairment and quality of life in euthymic bipolar patients. Psychiatry Research, 271, 510515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsapekos, D., Strawbridge, R., Cella, M., Goldsmith, K., Kalfas, M., Taylor, R. H., Swidzinski, S., Marwaha, S., Grey, L., Newton, E., Shackleton, J., Harrison, P. J., Browning, M., Harmer, C., Hartland, H., Cousins, D., Barton, S., Wykes, T., & Young, A. H. (2023). Cognitive remediation in bipolar (CRiB2): Study protocol for a randomised controlled trial assessing efficacy and mechanisms of cognitive remediation therapy compared to treatment as usual. BMC Psychiatry, 23(1), 842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsapekos, D., Strawbridge, R., Cella, M., Wykes, T., & Young, A. H. (2020). Do we really need a “new” cognitive therapy for bipolar disorder? Paradigm refinements and treatment mechanisms for cognitive remediation. Bipolar Disorders, 22(3), 213215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Camp, L., Sabbe, B. G. C., & Oldenburg, J. F. E. (2019). Metacognitive functioning in bipolar disorder versus controls and its correlations with neurocognitive functioning in a cross-sectional design. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 92, 712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Werf-Eldering, M., Burger, H., Jabben, N., Holthausen, E. A. E., Aleman, A., & Nolen, W. A. (2011). Is the lack of association between cognitive complaints and objective cognitive functioning in patients with bipolar disorder moderated by depressive symptoms? Journal of Affective Disorders, 130(1-2), 306311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xiao, L., Gao, Y., Zhang, L., Chen, P., & Sun, X. (2016). The relationship between cognitive function and quality of life in euthymic Chinese patients with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research, 246, 427431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xiao, L., Lin, X., Wang, Q., Lu, D., & Tang, S. (2015). Adaptation and validation of the cognitive complaints in bipolar disorder rating assessment (COBRA) in Chinese bipolar patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 173, 226231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, R. C., Biggs, J. T., Ziegler, V. E., & Meyer, D. A. (1978). A rating scale for mania: Reliability, validity and sensitivity. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The journal of mental science, 133(5), 429435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographics and clinical variables for participants with BD and healthy controls

Figure 1

Table 2. Objective and subjective cognition in participants with BD and controls

Figure 2

Figure 1. Correlations between Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) scores and objective cognitive functions in bipolar disorder sample. Light-shaded bars represent crystallized cognitive skills, and dark-shaded bars represent fluid cognitive skills. NAART = North American Adult Reading Test, K-BIT-2 = Kaufmann Brief Intelligence Test Second Edition, RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, ECFT = Extended Complex Figures Test.

Figure 3

Table 3. Correlation matrix between predictors and psychosocial outcomes

Figure 4

Table 4. Hierarchical regression models for predictors of MSIF, GAF, and quality of life