Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T00:50:48.784Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The interface of autism and (borderline) personality disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 September 2024

Orestis Zavlis*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK; and Department of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, London, UK
Peter Tyrer
Affiliation:
Centre of Psychiatry, Imperial College London, London, UK
*
Correspondence: Orestis Zavlis. Email: orestis.research@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Summary

Prominent clinical perspectives posit that the interface of autism and (borderline) personality disorder manifests as either a misdiagnosis of the former as the latter or a comorbidity of both. In this editorial, we integrate these disparate viewpoints by arguing that personality difficulties are inherent to the autistic spectrum.

Type
Guest Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Royal College of Psychiatrists

The interface of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and personality disorder, particularly borderline personality disorder, has been a topic of recent debate.Reference May, Pilkington, Younan and Williams1 The two conditions were shown to overlap in various domains, including the interpersonal (relational instabilities and detachment), intrapersonal (instability in self-perception), anankastic (obsessional rigidity) and affective (mood instability).Reference May, Pilkington, Younan and Williams1Reference Anckarsäter, Stahlberg and Larson3 How these overlaps ought to be interpreted, though, remains unclear. Some researchers have suggested that putative comorbidities between ASD and personality disorder should be read as cautionary tales of misdiagnoses, since the two conditions are fundamentally different.Reference May, Pilkington, Younan and Williams1 Yet others have argued that the two conditions are sufficiently similar to warrant the possibility of comorbidity.Reference Lugnegård, Hallerbäck and Gillberg2 In this editorial, we closely examine these perspectives and present an alternative that can unify them. Namely, the view that personality psychopathology is an inherent feature of the autistic spectrum, but not vice versa.

A note about terminology and focus

Before elaborating this alternative viewpoint, we make some notes on the terminology and focus of our editorial. First, with ASD we refer to high-functioning autism (that is, autism without intellectual disability – formerly known as Asperger’s syndrome), since this is the kind of autism that is frequently misdiagnosed as (borderline) personality disorder, particularly in females.Reference Allely, Woodhouse and Mukherjee4 Second, by female/male, we mean people who were assigned female/male sex at birth (although it is worth noting that most researchers do not distinguish between gender and sex and no research on transgender individuals exists in this line of inquiry, at the time of writing). Finally, although most research in this area has focused on 'borderline' personality disorder, in this editorial we focus on its dimensional counterpart for at least two reasons. First, borderline personality disorder is so heterogenous that it falls on the border of virtually all other psychiatric conditions, not just ASD.Reference Tyrer and Mulder5 Second, accruing evidence suggests that borderline personality disorder can be subsumed under a general personality disorder spectrum (much like Asperger’s can be subsumed under the autistic spectrum).Reference Tyrer and Mulder5 Thus, focusing on the broader personality disorder spectrum, as opposed to borderline personality or any other putative personality 'type', presents a fruitful conceptual shift. With these notes being made, we move on to examine the two alternative viewpoints on the interface of ASD and general personality pathology.

Misdiagnosis thesis

According to the first viewpoint, cases of high-functioning ASD can sometimes be misdiagnosed as (borderline) personality disorder, given the inherent overlap of the two conditions (particularly in females).Reference May, Pilkington, Younan and Williams1,Reference Allely, Woodhouse and Mukherjee4 Since being first expressed in the early 2000s, this argument has been supported by diagnostic asymmetries in the two conditions. In particular, the relative underdiagnosis of ASD (and overdiagnosis of ‘borderline’ personality disorder) in females versus males has prompted several researchers to argue that ‘female autism’ could be conflated with (borderline) personality disorder.Reference May, Pilkington, Younan and Williams1,Reference Allely, Woodhouse and Mukherjee4 Indeed, compared with male autism, female autism is less well-defined, more camouflaged and has a higher likelihood of being misdiagnosed (with borderline personality disorder being among the main misdiagnoses).Reference May, Pilkington, Younan and Williams1,Reference Allely, Woodhouse and Mukherjee4

The misdiagnosis thesis clearly bears merit and is supported not only by diagnostic asymmetries but also by notable phenomenological overlaps across the two conditions (e.g. in relational and emotional instabilities).Reference May, Pilkington, Younan and Williams1 Nevertheless, extreme variations of this viewpoint may perhaps better be avoided since they risk promoting the notion that ASD and personality disorder can never co-occur, something that cannot be justified by recent clinical findings.

Comorbidity thesis

Indeed, in light of recent findings revealing genuine comorbidities of ASD and personality disorder,Reference Allely, Woodhouse and Mukherjee4 researchers have argued that personality disorder and autism can often present together.Reference Lugnegård, Hallerbäck and Gillberg2 Unlike those endorsing the misdiagnosis perspective, proponents of this viewpoint emphasise points of convergence, rather than divergence, of the two phenotypes and attempt to shed light on their clinical presentation. Two notable findings can be reported from this literature. First, epidemiologically, although the comorbidity rate of the two disorders matches their population estimates (1–5%), the prevalence of personality disorder traits (such as instability of self, emotional reactivity and impulsivity) in those with ASD is higher than the prevalence of ASD features (such as alexithymic or systemising tendencies) in those with (borderline) personality disorder.Reference May, Pilkington, Younan and Williams1,Reference Lugnegård, Hallerbäck and Gillberg2 Second, clinically speaking, in comorbid cases, the neurodevelopmental symptoms of ASD are considered primary and the personality features secondary (since they could stem from autistic neurodivergence).Reference May, Pilkington, Younan and Williams1,Reference Allely, Woodhouse and Mukherjee4

In this sense, the personality disorder phenotype can be thought to be closer to the ASD one (than vice versa). In light of this observation, the comorbidity and misdiagnosis theses can be fruitfully united.

The autistic and personality disorder spectra

Given that the comorbidity and misdiagnosis accounts are not mutually exclusive (and both may be endorsed in conjunction by most clinicians), they could be conjoined by embracing recent advances in the field of personality disorder.

First, contemporary classification systems (including both ICD-11, with its new personality disorder classification, and DSM-5, with its alternative model of personality dysfunction) diagnose personality disorder in dimensional, not categorical, terms.Reference Tyrer and Mulder5 ICD-11, in particular, highlights that personality disorder falls on a spectrum of severity that ranges from personality difficulty (in the subsyndromal case) to mild, moderate and severe personality disorder.Reference Tyrer and Mulder5

Second, maladaptive personality traits (including the ICD-11 ones and their DSM-5 counterparts) are omnipresent in various psychopathologies, particularly neurodevelopmental ones.Reference Anckarsäter, Stahlberg and Larson3

Finally, although somewhat controversial, the diagnosis of personality disorder can now be made at any age once a pattern of personality pathology has been demonstrated for at least 2 years.Reference Tyrer and Mulder5 This is in keeping with the new lifespan approach to diagnosis that is being followed in both ICD-11 and DSM-5. The option of making an assessment of personality pathology across severity and age groups allows many relational psychopathologies (particularly neurodevelopmental ones such as ASD or attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder) to be characterised by levels of (at least) personality difficulty, which if recognised during early development can be addressed before major personality disorder becomes manifest.Reference Lugnegård, Hallerbäck and Gillberg2,Reference Anckarsäter, Stahlberg and Larson3

Given these points, we argue that some level of personality disorder is intrinsic to ASD, but not vice versa. In other words, certain personality problems (that is, maladaptive ways of relating to oneself and others) are inherent to the autistic phenotype; but autistic traits are not inherent to those with personality disorder. This conclusion is supported by (a) epidemiological findings indicating high rates (48–68%) of personality difficulties in those with ASD (but not vice versa)Reference May, Pilkington, Younan and Williams1,Reference Lugnegård, Hallerbäck and Gillberg2 and (b) developmental findings showing that personality difficulties are a common clinical sequela of neurodivergence.Reference Anckarsäter, Stahlberg and Larson3

This conclusion unifies the abovementioned perspectives by highlighting that even though neurodiversity could give rise to maladaptive personality traits (potentially birthing a comorbid personality disorder),Reference Anckarsäter, Stahlberg and Larson3 (sub)threshold personality pathology could also dominate the clinical picture (yielding a misdiagnosis of ASD as personality disorder).Reference May, Pilkington, Younan and Williams1

Clinical and research implications

Based on the evidence synthesis above, several research and clinical implications may follow. First and foremost, ASD may be considered as a differential diagnosis of personality disorder, particularly for females, for whom ASD is currently under-recognised.Reference Allely, Woodhouse and Mukherjee4 Although this point may seem intuitive clinically, it is worth reiterating because personality problems are often a consequence of other issues, including complex trauma, social deprivation and, of course, neurodiversity.Reference Tyrer and Mulder5 Examining these other issues (and often formulating them as primary) is crucial for an accurate personality diagnosis (particularly when the medical history of a patient is unknown).Reference Tyrer and Mulder5

This point leads to the second implication of the reviewed evidence above: a more scientifically accurate and clinically helpful way of conceiving the interface of ASD and personality disorder is in dimensional, not categorical terms. That is, researchers should focus on identifying the personality traits, not personality ‘disorders’, that are most likely to ensue in people with ASD. For instance, affective volatility, relational instability, behavioural rigidity and detached relationships are found in many psychiatric conditions, including ASD.Reference Anckarsäter, Stahlberg and Larson3,Reference Tyrer and Mulder5 Examining how and why these, as well as related, traits proliferate in some, but not all, individuals with ASD could help clarify the intimate link between neurodiversity and personality psychopathology.Reference Lugnegård, Hallerbäck and Gillberg2,Reference Anckarsäter, Stahlberg and Larson3

Finally, in light of this intimate link, the treatment of ASD could be extended to interventions that have been constrained to personality disorder. This includes environment-based interventions (which focus on altering patients’ environments rather than their lifelong illnesses)Reference Tyrer, Tarabi and Bassett6 but also dialectical behavioural interventions (which focus on teaching coping skills and acceptance practices). Although thought to be inapplicable to people with autism, such interventions are increasingly found (in modified forms) to be useful.Reference May, Pilkington, Younan and Williams1

Conclusions

In sum, conceptualising autism and personality disorder as dimensional spectra appears to unify disparate viewpoints on their interface. Doing so further reveals that personality difficulties are somewhat inherent to the autistic spectrum (but not vice versa) and can yield either a misdiagnosis (when they dominate the clinical picture) or a comorbidity (when they reach diagnostic status that is acknowledged alongside neurodiversity). In light of these points, a fruitful way forward can be to examine the interface of ASD and personality disorder dimensionally, identifying the personality difficulties that individuals with ASD typically face, as well as the conditions under which they emerge. Treating those ASD difficulties using therapies that have been constrained to personality disorder appears a promising clinical avenue.

Data availability

Data availability is not applicable to this article as no new data were created or analysed in this study.

Author contributions

O.Z.: writing – original draft; O.Z. and P.T.: writing – review and editing; P.T.: supervision.

Funding

This study received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Declaration of interest

P.T. is the chair of a registered charity, NIDUS-UK, that promotes the development of nidotherapy.

References

May, T, Pilkington, PD, Younan, R, Williams, K. Overlap of autism spectrum disorder and borderline personality disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Autism Res 2021; 14: 2688–710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lugnegård, T, Hallerbäck, MU, Gillberg, C. Personality disorders and autism spectrum disorders: what are the connections? Compr Psychiatry 2012; 53: 333–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anckarsäter, H, Stahlberg, O, Larson, T, Hakansson C, Jutblad SB, Niklasson L, et al. The impact of ADHD and autism spectrum disorders on temperament, character, and personality development. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163: 1239–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allely, CS, Woodhouse, E, Mukherjee, RA. Autism spectrum disorder and personality disorders: how do clinicians carry out a differential diagnosis? Autism 2023; 27: 1847–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyrer, PJ, Mulder, R. Personality Disorder: From Evidence to Understanding. Cambridge University Press, 2022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyrer, P, Tarabi, S, Bassett, P, Liedtka N, Hall R, Nagar J, et al. Nidotherapy compared with enhanced care programme approach training for adults with aggressive challenging behaviour and intellectual disability (NIDABID): cluster-randomised controlled trial. J Intellect Disabil Res 2017; 61: 521–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.