Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T06:20:57.719Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Outcome of cosmetic surgery and ‘DIY’ surgery in patients with body dysmorphic disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

David Veale*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Sciences, Royal Free and University College Medical School, University College, c/o Grovelands Priory Hospital, The Bourne, Southgate, London N14 6RA
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aims and Method

Little is known about the outcome of cosmetic surgery in patients with body dysmorphic disorder (BDD). Self-reported outcome was collected on 25 patients with BDD who at the time of psychiatric assessment had reported that they had had cosmetic surgery in the past.

Results

Twenty-five patients with BDD had a total of 46 procedures. The worst outcome was found in those who had had rhinoplasty and those with repeated operations. Mammoplasty and pinnaplasty was associated with higher degrees of satisfaction. Nine patients with BDD, either in desperation at being turned down for cosmetic surgery or because they could not afford it, had performed their own ‘DIY’ surgery in which they attempted by their own hand to alter their appearance dramatically.

Clinical Implications

Cosmetic surgery cannot at present be recommended for patients with BDD. However, patients turned down for surgery or who cannot afford it, may try to alter their appearance by themselves. The study contains a selection bias of patients in favour of treatment failures in cosmetic surgery and prospective studies are required on BDD patients who obtain cosmetic surgery or dermatological treatment.

Type
Original Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © 2000, The Royal College of Psychiatrists

Body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) is characterised by a preoccupation with an imagined defect or a markedly excessive concern, where there is a slight physical anomaly (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Many patients with BDD crave to change their appearance by cosmetic surgery or by dermatological treatments. I have previously reported that in 50 patients with BDD in the UK, 26% had managed to obtain one or more cosmetic operations (Reference Veale, Boocock and GournayVeale et al, 1996a ). There are no outcome studies of cosmetic surgery in BDD apart from Phillips et al (Reference Phillips, McElroy and Keek1993) who reported on eight patients who had between them undergone 25 plastic or dental surgery procedures. Only two procedures led to an improvement of remission of symptoms while 20 procedures resulted in an exacerbation of symptoms. BDD is not uncommon in cosmetic surgery clinics. In a recent survey of 100 women seeking cosmetic surgery in the USA, 5% met the diagnostic criteria for BDD (Reference Sarwer, Wadden and PertschukSarwer et al, 1998). This paper reports both on BDD patients seen in the UK about their experience of cosmetic surgery and on the occurrence of DIY cosmetic surgery in patients unable to obtain the surgery they crave.

The study

Data were collected on 25 patients with BDD who reported at the time of psychiatric assessment that they had had cosmetic surgery in the past. Referrals were from their general practitioner (n=8), a consultant psychiatrist (n=7), or self-referral or via a patient support group (n=10). Patients were asked to rate:

  1. (a) the degree of satisfaction with the outcome of each operation they had had on a scale between zero and 10, where zero represented extreme dissatisfaction and 10 extreme satisfaction;

  2. (b) any change in the overall amount of time they took and distress they experienced thinking about their appearance after the operation on a scale between one and seven where one represented ‘very much less’ and seven was ‘very much more’ (Reference GuyGuy, 1976);

  3. (c) any change in the overall degree of handicap in relation to their occupational and social life after the operation on the same scale as above.

Patients were also asked if after an operation the preoccupation transferred to another area of the body.

Findings

The sample consisted of 25 patients with BDD who had had a total of 46 procedures. Ten patients had undergone one procedure, 10 patients had undergone two procedures, four patients had undergone three procedures, and one patient had undergone four procedures. They had a mean age of 36.0 years (range 22-63) and 12% of the sample was male. The operations had occurred a mean of 5.6 years ago. Twenty-six per cent of the operations were performed on the NHS. Many of those who had had private cosmetic surgery had taken out loans or borrowed from relatives.

Three patients claimed that they were not preoccupied by their appearance prior to the surgery and that their symptoms of BDD developed only after surgery, which they believed had been done badly. The satisfaction ratings and any change in preoccupation and overall handicap are listed in Table 1. The numbers of several procedures are small but the satisfaction ratings tended to be higher for mammoplasty, rhytidectomy (face-lift) and pinnaplasty. Mammoplasty and pinnaplasty (but not rhytidectomy) tended to lead to an overall decrease in preoccupation and handicap.

Table 1. Ratings of satisfaction, preoccupation and handicap for each type of operation

Operation n Satisfaction, range 0-10, mean (s.d.) Preoccupation, range 1-7, mean (s.d.) Handicap, range 1-7, mean (s.d.)
All operations 46 3.5 (3.0) 4.4 (1.7) 4.1 (1.67)
Rhinoplasty 22 2.9 (2.7) 4.4 (1.4) 4.5 (1.5)
Rhytidectomy (face-lift) 4 6.0 (3.6) 3.7 (2.5) 4.0 (2.0)
Mammoplasty augmentation 3 6.3 (3.5) 3.7 (2.3) 2.0 (1.0)
Mammoplasty reduction 1 8.0 4.0 2.0
Blepharoplasty (eye-lid) 2 3.7 (1.2) 5.5 (0.7) 5.0 (1.4)
Pinnaplasty (pinning of ears) 4 5.3 (3.8) 2.0 (1.4) 2.0 (1.4)
Abdominoplasty (tummy-tuck) 1 2.0 3.0 4.0
Chemical peel (to face) 1 2.0 3.0 3.0
Laser resurfacing (to face) 1 2.0 3.0 4.0
Mentoplasty augmentation (chin implant) 1 6.0 3.0 3.0
Collagen injection to lips 1 0.0 7.0 7.0
Cosmetic diathermy to skin 1 0.0 7.0 7.0
Electrocautery to thread veins 1 1.0 4.0 4.0
Hair transplant 1 0.0 7.0 6.0
Removal of mole on face 1 2.0 N/A N/A
Removal of thread veins by sclerotherapy 1 2.0 3.0 4.0

The satisfaction rating was relatively low for rhinoplasty, which was the most common procedure. The nose is also the most usual location for complaint by patients with BDD (Reference Veale, Boocock and GournayVeale et al, 1996a ). Rhinoplasty tended to be associated with an increase in preoccupation and handicap. Most of the patients in the study had multiple concerns about their appearance and reported that after 50% of the procedures the preoccupation transferred tc another area of their body. After one procedure, the mean satisfaction rating was 3.9 (see Table 2). By the second or third procedure, this had dropped to 2.8. The second or third procedure was not always a revision of the first procedure. For example, of the 17 patients who had rhinoplasty, only three underwent one or more revisions and 14 had a different procedure when their preoccupation moved to a different area of the body. When patients were dissatisfied with their operation, they often felt guilty or angry with themselves or the surgeon for having made their appearance worse, thus further fuelling their depression and a failure to achieve their ideal. This in turn tended to increase mirror gazing and craving for more surgery. Six patients rated themselves as satisfied with their procedure (defined as a rating of 7-10). Four of these patients went on to have one or more further procedures or were dissatisfied with another area of their body but did not have further surgery.

Table 2. Satisfaction with repeated operation

Operation n Satisfaction, range 0-10, mean (s.d.) Preoccupation, range 1-7, mean (s.d.) Handicap, range 1-7, mean (s.d.)
First 25 3.9 (3.3) 4.4 (1.5) 4.1 (1.5)
Second 15 2.8 (2.5) 4.5 (2.0) 4.2 (1.9)
Third 5 2.8 (1.9) 4.7 (1.3) 4.5 (1.7)

‘DIY’ cosmetic surgery

Nine patients with BDD (of whom two were included in the sample above) reported performing their own cosmetic surgery in which they attempted to alter their appearance dramatically. This was either in desperation at being turned down for cosmetic surgery or because they could not afford it. The procedures were often associated with many hours of mirror gazing and intense disgust at their perceived defect. Examples of the procedures that were undertaken are shown in the Appendix. All cases were dissatisfied with the results of their own attempts at surgery.

Comment

Most patients with BDD reported a high degree of dissatisfaction with cosmetic surgery and an increase in their symptoms of BDD or overall handicap. Repeated surgery tended to lead to increasing dissatisfaction. Some operations, such as rhinoplasty appear to be associated with higher degrees of dissatisfaction. Mammoplasty and pinnaplasty tended to have relatively higher satisfaction ratings. These operations tend to be unambiguous in that the patient can usually describe the problem that concerns them and their desired outcome and the cosmetic surgeon can understand their expectations.

Even when patients were partially or wholly satisfied with the results then the preoccupation transferred to a different area of the body and they remained significantly handicapped in their symptoms of BDD. There is a suggestion that the onset of BDD for some patients occurred after cosmetic surgery although this needs confirmation in a larger prospective study of patients before cosmetic surgery.

The main weaknesses of this study and that of Phillips et al (Reference Phillips, McElroy and Keek1993) are:

  1. (a) The data are retrospective and there is a selection bias of patients in favour of treatment failures. Mental health practitioners are unlikely to see patients with BDD who might be satisfied with their cosmetic surgery and have overcome their symptoms of BDD. However, at this stage, it is not known what factors might predict satisfaction in patients with BDD who undergo surgery and in whom the preoccupation does not transfer repeatedly to another area of the body.

  2. (b) A variety of different cosmetic operations were under scrutiny and it is difficult to interpret the success of some operations which have only one or two procedures in this sample.

  3. (c) There is no control group of psychiatric patients who have undergone cosmetic surgery but do not have BDD. However, the majority of individuals (who do not have BDD) are very satisfied with cosmetic surgery and their self-esteem and other psychological measures improve (Reference Klassen, Fitzpatrick and JenkinsonKlassen et al, 1996; Reference Carr, Harris and JamesCarr et al, 2000).

There is, however, never likely to be a randomised-controlled trial of cosmetic surgery in patients with BDD comparing, for example, cognitive-behavioural therapy (Reference Veale, Gournay and DrydenVeale et al, 1996b ) or a serotonin reuptake inhibitor (Reference Hollander, Allen and KwonHollander et al, 1999). At best, a prospective study of patients with BDD undergoing specific cosmetic surgery operations with a long-term follow-up is required to determine efficacy in the long term and whether patients eventually become satisfied after several operations.

The motivation for DIY surgery is complex, but it appears primarily either to camouflage a perceived defect or to try to achieve an unrealistic ideal. It does not fit the existing classification of self-harm by Favazza & Rosenthal (Reference Favazza and Rosenthal1993). It has a similar poor psychological outcome in so far as nearly all the patients were dissatisfied with their handiwork and found their symptoms of BDD were exacerbated. It reflects the extreme measures that some patients take and is mirrored in the high rate of attempted suicide in this population (Reference Veale, Boocock and GournayVeale et al, 1996a ).

Appendix

Examples of DIY cosmetic surgery

A man preoccupied by his skin who believed that it was too ‘loose’. He used a staple gun on both sides of his face to try to keep his skin taut. The staples fell out after 10 minutes and he narrowly missed damaging his facial nerve.

A woman preoccupied by her skin and the shape of her face. She filed down her teeth in order to alter the appearance of her jaw-line.

A man who was preoccupied with the appearance of his chin deliberately cycled into the back of a lorry in an attempt to fracture his jaw so that it could be reset in a way that he wanted. At the last moment, he decided against the plan, dropped his head, cut his forehead and fractured his skull.

A man preoccupied by his facial skin said he had used sandpaper as a form of dermabrasion to remove scars and to lighten his skin.

A man who was preoccupied by redness on his skin repeatedly exsanguinated himself by a syringe and needle or when he was accepted, by going to blood transfusion clinics to ‘make himself look paler’.

A woman who was preoccupied by the ugliness of multiple areas of her body who desired liposuction but could not afford it, used a knife to cut her thighs and attempted to squeeze out the fat.

Acknowledgement

I would like to thank Susan Riley, Assistant Psychologist, for help in collection of the data and Dr Marc Serfaty for his comments.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM–IV) (4th edn). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Carr, A. T., Harris, D. L. & James, C. (2000) The Derriford Appearance Scale (DAS59): a new rated self-report questionnaire. British Journal of Health Psychology, in press.Google Scholar
Favazza, A. R. & Rosenthal, R. J. (1993) Diagnostic issues in self-mutilation. Hospital and Community Psychiatry 44, 134140.Google Scholar
Guy, W. (1976) Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology. Washington, DC: National Institute of Mental Health, US Department of Health, Education and Welfare.Google Scholar
Hollander, E., Allen, A., Kwon, J., et al (1999) Clomipramine vs desipramine crossover trial in body dismorphic disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 56, 10331042.Google Scholar
Klassen, A., Fitzpatrick, R., Jenkinson, C., et al (1996) Should breast surgery be rationed? A comparison of the health status of patients before and after treatment: postal questionnaire survey. British Medical Journal, 313, 454457.Google Scholar
Phillips, K. A., McElroy, S. L., Keek, P. E., et al (1993) Body dysmorphic disorder: 30 cases of imagined ugliness. American Journal of Psychiatry 150, 302308.Google Scholar
Sarwer, D. B., Wadden, T. A., Pertschuk, M. J., et al (1998) Body image dissatisfaction and bodydysmorphicdisorderin100 cosmetic surgery patients. Plastic & Reconstructive Surgery, 101, 16441649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veale, D., Boocock, A., Gournay, K., et al (1996a) Body dysmorphic disorder. A survey of fifty cases. British Journal of Psychiatry 169, 196201.Google Scholar
Veale, D., Gournay, K., Dryden, W., et al (1996b) Body Dysmorphic Disorder: a cognitive behavioural model and pilot randomised-controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy 34, 717729.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Ratings of satisfaction, preoccupation and handicap for each type of operation

Figure 1

Table 2. Satisfaction with repeated operation

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.