Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-956mj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-24T14:03:31.657Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Maternal depression during the perinatal period and its relationship with emotion regulation in young adulthood: An fMRI study in a prenatal birth cohort

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 February 2025

Klara Mareckova*
Affiliation:
Brain and Mind Research, Central European Institute of Technology (CEITEC), Masaryk University, Brno, Czech Republic First Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine, Masaryk University and St. Anne’s University Hospital, Brno, Czech Republic
Filip Trbusek
Affiliation:
Brain and Mind Research, Central European Institute of Technology (CEITEC), Masaryk University, Brno, Czech Republic
Radek Marecek
Affiliation:
Brain and Mind Research, Central European Institute of Technology (CEITEC), Masaryk University, Brno, Czech Republic First Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine, Masaryk University and St. Anne’s University Hospital, Brno, Czech Republic
Jan Chladek
Affiliation:
Brain and Mind Research, Central European Institute of Technology (CEITEC), Masaryk University, Brno, Czech Republic First Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine, Masaryk University and St. Anne’s University Hospital, Brno, Czech Republic Institute of Scientific Instruments, The Czech Academy of Sciences, Brno, Czech Republic
Zuzana Koscova
Affiliation:
Institute of Scientific Instruments, The Czech Academy of Sciences, Brno, Czech Republic
Filip Plesinger
Affiliation:
Institute of Scientific Instruments, The Czech Academy of Sciences, Brno, Czech Republic
Lenka Andrysková
Affiliation:
RECETOX, Faculty of Science, Masaryk University, Brno, Czech Republic
Milan Brazdil
Affiliation:
Brain and Mind Research, Central European Institute of Technology (CEITEC), Masaryk University, Brno, Czech Republic First Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine, Masaryk University and St. Anne’s University Hospital, Brno, Czech Republic
Yuliya S. Nikolova*
Affiliation:
Centre for Addiction and Mental Health (CAMH), University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto
*
Corresponding authors: Klara Mareckova and Yuliya Nikolova; Emails: klara.mareckova@ceitec.muni.cz; yuliya.nikolova@camh.ca
Corresponding authors: Klara Mareckova and Yuliya Nikolova; Emails: klara.mareckova@ceitec.muni.cz; yuliya.nikolova@camh.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Maternal perinatal mental health is essential for optimal brain development and mental health of the offspring. We evaluated whether maternal depression during the perinatal period and early life of the offspring might be selectively associated with altered brain function during emotion regulation and whether those may further correlate with physiological responses and the typical use of emotion regulation strategies.

Methods

Participants included 163 young adults (49% female, 28–30 years) from the ELSPAC prenatal birth cohort who took part in its neuroimaging follow-up and had complete mental health data from the perinatal period and early life. Maternal depressive symptoms were measured mid-pregnancy, 2 weeks, 6 months, and 18 months after birth. Regulation of negative affect was studied using functional magnetic resonance imaging, concurrent skin conductance response (SCR) and heart rate variability (HRV), and assessment of typical emotion regulation strategy.

Results

Maternal depression 2 weeks after birth interacted with sex and showed a relationship with greater brain response during emotion regulation in a right frontal cluster in women. Moreover, this brain response mediated the relationship between greater maternal depression 2 weeks after birth and greater suppression of emotions in young adult women (ab = 0.11, SE = 0.05, 95% CI [0.016; 0.226]). The altered brain response during emotion regulation and the typical emotion regulation strategy were also as sociated with SCR and HRV.

Conclusions

These findings suggest that maternal depression 2 weeks after birth predisposes female offspring to maladaptive emotion regulation skills and particularly to emotion suppression in young adulthood.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Emotion regulation is an important part of daily life and is necessary for adequate social functioning and overall mental health of individuals (McRae & Gross, Reference McRae and Gross2020). Cognitive reappraisal and emotion suppression have been described as two main strategies of emotion regulation (Gross & Barrett, Reference Gross and Barrett2011; Hu et al., Reference Hu, Zhang, Wang, Mistry, Ran and Wang2014; Morawetz et al., Reference Morawetz, Bode, Derntl and Heekeren2017). The use of cognitive reappraisal is a more effective strategy in regulating emotional outcomes (Webb et al., Reference Webb, Miles and Sheeran2012) and has been associated with better overall mental health outcomes than suppression of emotional expression (Hu et al., Reference Hu, Zhang, Wang, Mistry, Ran and Wang2014; John & Gross, Reference John and Gross2004). Difficulties in emotion regulation are common across various mental disorders (Sheppes et al., Reference Sheppes, Suri and Gross2015), including affective disorders such as depression and anxiety disorders (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010), and spanning also neurodevelopmental conditions such as Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Stringaris, Nigg and Leibenluft2014) and autism spectrum disorder (Mazefsky et al., Reference Mazefsky, Herrington, Siegel, Scarpa, Maddox, Scahill and White2013). According to Li et al (Li et al., Reference Li, Li, Wu and Wang2019), one’s emotion regulation and lability is associated with parental emotion dysregulation and this intergenerational effect is transmitted indirectly through parental reactions to children’s negative emotions. Recent research in almost 2000 adolescents (Maruyama et al., Reference Maruyama, Valente, Tovo-Rodrigues, Santos, Barros, Munhoz and Matijasevich2023) then linked worse self-esteem and emotion regulation problems in adolescents with maternal depression.

Emotion regulation is supported by a relatively well-characterized neural circuitry involving frontoparietal and subcortical regions. According to a meta-analysis by Frank et al. (Frank et al., Reference Frank, Dewitt, Hudgens-Haney, Schaeffer, Ball, Schwarz and Sabatinelli2014), successful emotion regulation is associated with decreased response in the amygdala, parahippocampal cortex, and inferior parietal lobe (IPL), and increased response in superior, middle, and inferior frontal gyrus, supplementary motor area (SMA), left middle temporal and angular gyrus, and left anterior cingulate cortex. Morawetz et al. (Morawetz et al., Reference Morawetz, Bode, Derntl and Heekeren2017) studied different emotion regulation strategies and reported that while reappraisal strategies were associated with greater response of superior and inferior frontal gyrus, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, superior and middle temporal gyri, IPL, SMA, and preSMA, emotion suppression was associated with converging response in the right IPL and left inferior frontal gyrus. Consistently, research from our group (Mareckova et al., Reference Mareckova, Holsen, Admon, Makris, Seidman, Buka and Goldstein2016) eliciting negative affect using negative images from the International Affective Picture System showed a greater inability to regulate the amygdala as mood became more dysphoric. Research in patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) versus healthy controls then concluded that MDD patients display abnormalities in higher-level cognitive as well as subcortical limbic structures, which manifest as an inability to appropriately regulate one’s emotions (Park et al., Reference Park, Rosenblat, Lee, Pan, Cao, Iacobucci and McIntyre2019). Diminished ability to downregulate emotions in MDD was also linked with lower activity of lateral prefrontal cortex (Ebneabbasi et al., Reference Ebneabbasi, Mahdipour, Nejati, Li, Liebe, Colic and Tahmasian2021).

Better emotion regulation has also been associated with greater heart rate variability (Appelhans & Luecken, Reference Appelhans and Luecken2006; Park & Thayer, Reference Park and Thayer2014), which is defined as the variation in time between the heartbeats. The autonomic nervous system (ANS), which influences the heart rate, consists of the sympathetic (SNS) and parasympathetic (PNS) nervous system. While the SNS predominates during stress and is related to an increase in heart rate and skin conductivity, the PNS is the “relaxed response” system, which predominates during the quiet and relaxing states and is related to a decrease in heart rate and skin conductance (Zaehringer et al., Reference Zaehringer, Jennen-Steinmetz, Schmahl, Ende and Paret2020). Heart rate variability (HRV) has been associated with emotional arousal and anxiety (Jonsson, Reference Jonsson2007; Nickel & Nachreiner, Reference Nickel and Nachreiner2003) and it has been demonstrated that HRV can be modulated by the prefrontal cortex (Napadow et al., Reference Napadow, Dhond, Conti, Makris, Brown and Barbieri2008). In contrast, patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) are known to have lower levels of skin conductivity (Iacono & Lykken, Reference Iacono and Lykken1983; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Jang, Choi, Jeon, Byun, Sim and Yu2019) and higher high-frequency power (HFn) during HRV measurements (An et al., Reference An, Han, Jeong, Kim, Lee, Lee and Kim2020) than healthy controls.

According to the theory of prenatal programming (Cao-Lei et al., Reference Cao-Lei, de Rooij, King, Matthews, Metz, Roseboom and Szyf2020; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Bale and Epperson2015), the uteroplacental environment of the developing fetus has a long-lasting impact on the offspring. Longitudinal data from several birth cohort studies showed that prenatal maternal anxiety and/or depression were associated with a higher risk for emotional and behavioral problems in childhood (consistent across measurements from 4 to 13 years) and clinical levels of anxiety and depression in adulthood (Acosta et al., Reference Acosta, Tuulari, Scheinin, Hashempour, Rajasilta, Lavonius and Karlsson2019; Monk et al., Reference Monk, Lugo-Candelas and Trumpff2019; O’Donnell et al., Reference O’Donnell, Glover, Barker and O’Connor2014; O’Donnell & Meaney, Reference O’Donnell and Meaney2017). Moreover, these effects could not be explained by maternal prenatal smoking and substance use, birth weight and gestational age of the child, postpartum symptoms of anxiety or depression, maternal parenting, or social circumstances such as maternal education and socioeconomic status (O’Donnell et al., Reference O’Donnell, Glover, Barker and O’Connor2014). Recent research also linked perinatal maternal symptoms of depression and anxiety with self-regulation (Schwarze et al., Reference Schwarze, von der Heiden, Wallwiener and Pauen2024) and emotion regulation (Amani et al., Reference Amani, Krzeczkowski, Schmidt and Van Lieshout2024) in their infants. Within the current project, we focused on the effects of maternal depression during the perinatal period and early life of the offspring on emotion regulation skills in young adult offspring.

Numerous studies pointed out the importance of the timing of the exposure during the perinatal period (Mareckova et al., Reference Mareckova, Miles, Andryskova, Brazdil and Nikolova2020; Teicher et al., Reference Teicher, Tomoda and Andersen2006), because different brain regions have unique periods when they are maximally sensitive to the effects of early adversity. Based on this literature, we hypothesized that perinatal maternal depression and particularly depression during the prenatal period will be associated with altered brain function during emotion regulation and manifest as worse emotion regulation skills in young adulthood. Given previous research, which demonstrated greater vulnerability of women to affective disorders (Nolen-Hoeksema, Reference Nolen-Hoeksema2012; Seedat et al., Reference Seedat, Scott, Angermeyer, Berglund, Bromet, Brugha and Kessler2009), sex differences in emotion regulation and psychopathology (Nolen-Hoeksema, Reference Nolen-Hoeksema2012), and sex differences in brain response during emotion regulation (McRae et al., Reference McRae, Ochsner, Mauss, Gabrieli and Gross2008), we hypothesized that these effects might be particularly pronounced in women. We also hypothesized that the altered brain function during emotion regulation as well as the typical use of specific emotion regulation strategies will be reflected by altered skin conductance and heart rate variability during emotion regulation. Finally, we hypothesized that the altered brain response during emotion regulation will mediate the relationship between maternal depression and worse emotion regulation skills.

Methods and materials

Participants

We studied young adults from the Czech Republic who participated in the European Longitudinal Study of Pregnancy and Childhood (ELSPAC) (Piler et al., Reference Piler, Kandrnal, Kukla, Andryskova, Svancara, Jarkovsky and Klanova2017) and its neuroimaging follow-up study Health Brain Age (HBA). The ELSPAC study comprises a prenatal cohort consisting of 5151 individuals, whose members were born between 1991 and 1992. The initial ELSPAC questionnaires were administered to the mothers upon enrolment into the study at the 20th week of pregnancy, while the final ELSPAC questionnaire was completed by the child at the age of 19. The HBA neuroimaging follow-up was conducted in a subgroup of the ‘ELSPAC children’ (n = 262, 49% females, all of White European ancestry, as indicated by self-report questionnaire) at the age of 28–30 years. All participants provided written informed consent to participate in the HBA study, including the agreement to merge data from HBA with their historical data from ELSPAC. Ethical approval for the HBA study was obtained from the ELSPAC ethics committee. However, only a total of 163 participants (49% females; see Demographics Table in Supplementary Table 1) had maternal depression data from all 4 timepoints during the perinatal and early life period as well as fMRI data at the age of 28 to 30 years and thus could have been included in the current study.

Procedures

Maternal depression during the perinatal period and early life of the offspring

Maternal depression was assessed using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) (Cox et al., Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky1987; Gibson et al., Reference Gibson, McKenzie-McHarg, Shakespeare, Price and Gray2009) at 4 time points during the perinatal period and early life of the offspring: (a) mid-pregnancy (approx. Week 20), (b) 2 weeks after birth, (c) 6 months after birth, and (d) 18 months after birth.

Acquisition and analysis of MRI and fMRI data in young adulthood

Regulation of negative affect in the offspring was studied using the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) fMRI task. Participants observed negative and neutral images from the IAPS database (Bradley & Lang, Reference Bradley, Lang, Zeigler-Hill and Shackelford2017), which were selected based on ratings of evoked affect. A total of 66 images were presented to probands during the measurement, 1 image per trial. These 66 trials were divided into 3 conditions (22 images each) – neutral observe, negative observe, and negative regulate. In each trial, the image exposure lasted for the first 8 seconds. The trial started by displaying an image and text “Observe image” for 3 seconds. After 3 seconds the text changed either to “Keep observing the image” (neutral/negative observation trial) or “Regulate the emotions evoked by the image” (negative regulate trial) while the image was still displayed for another 5 seconds. Therefore, the affective response to negative pictures was regulated between the 3rd and 8th second. The trial continued by the display of a blank screen for 1 second and another screen for 3 seconds that asked for a rating by the query “How uncomfortable do you feel right now?” and a scale “not at all – a little – quite a bit – very much.” The trial ends with observing the text “Relax” for 9 seconds to prevent carry-over effects and set up the baseline for neural as well as heart rate and electrodermal activity. Overall, the trial duration was 21 seconds and the complete task took 23 minutes. The order of the trials was pseudo-random, creating 12 variations of the task. The order was set to avoid two trials from the same condition appearing consecutively. Further, the first three trials contained all three conditions in random order. This was the same for the second three trials and all ensuing threes of trials. During the neutral and negative observation conditions, participants were instructed to observe the images. During the negative regulate condition, participants were asked to regulate their affective response.

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of the brain was conducted using a 3 T Siemens Prisma MRI scanner. T1-weighted (T1w) whole-brain MPRAGE images were acquired using 64 channel head/neck coil with acquisition parameters: voxel size 1 mm3, repetition time (TR) 2,300 ms, echo time (TE) 2.34 ms, inversion time (TI) 900 ms, flip angle 8°. Functional MRI images were acquired using the following acquisition parameters: voxel size: 3 × 3 × 3 mm; whole-brain coverage, repetition Time: 0.7 s, FlipAngle: 47°, Effective Echo Spacing: 0.000285002 s, Phase Encoding Direction: “y-,” Multiband Acceleration Factor: 6, Parallel Reduction Factor In Plane: 2, Total slices: 2,121, EchoTime: 0.0164 s (Echo1); 0.03766 s, (Echo 2); 0.05892 s (Echo 3). For each measurement, 3 frames were captured with different Echo time parameters, which were subsequently combined into 1 frame as part of the data preprocessing.

The fMRI data were processed using SPM12 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/) toolbox running under MATLAB (The MathWorks, Inc.). The preprocessing comprised steps as follows: All echo 2 data were realigned and resliced and echo 1 and 3 data were resliced using parameters from echo 2 data. Voxel displacement map was estimated according to the fieldmap data and applied to correct data from echoes 1, 2, and 3. Next, the 3 echo datasets were merged using temporal SNR weighting approach. Subsequently, the mean fMRI image was registered to the structural image, which was spatially normalized to the MNI152 template. Data were smoothed using FWHM of 5 mm and all pre-processed data were checked for subject motion. One subject was excluded due to more than 20% of scans with a framewise displacement of 0.5 mm (Power et al., Reference Power, Mitra, Laumann, Snyder, Schlaggar and Petersen2014; Power et al., Reference Power, Schlaggar and Petersen2015; Pupikova et al., Reference Pupikova, Simko, Lamos, Gajdos and Rektorova2022). There was no difference in framewise displacement between men and women (Supplementary Figure 1).

Next, a whole-brain voxel-wise analysis used a general linear model (GLM) that was estimated for each subject. The GLM design matrix contained 3 regressors for each condition: a regressor that modelled initial phase of a trial (“observe image”) another for the second phase of the trial (“keep observing” or “regulate”) and another for the last phase of the trial (the rating). Each condition was modeled as an boxcar function convolved with canonical hemodynamic response function provided by SPM12 (HRF). The GLM was amended by additional regressors that modeled (a) low frequency fluctuations (a cutt-off frequency of 1/128 s), (b) signal variability caused by subject’s movements (transitions, rotations and their differencies) and (c) neural activity that corresponded to execution of key strokes (a brief events convolved with HRF). These additional regressors effectively filtered nuisance variability comprised in the data (e.g. physiological noise and neural activity linked to response execution). After GLM estimation we set the following contrasts based on effects of the regressors that modelled the second phase of the trials: (1) negative observe > neutral observe and (2) negative regulate > negative observe. These contrasts estimates entered the group-level statistics.

Acquisition and analysis of skin conductance data in young adulthood

Skin conductance response (SCR) data were collected during the fMRI task using the ExG MR compatible system – BrainProducts BrainAmp ExG MR, allowing SCR data to be calculated for the negative observe versus neutral observe as well as the negative regulate versus negative observe contrasts. Similarly to the fMRI data analysis, the SCR data analysis focused on the 5-second window when participants saw the negative or neutral image with instructions to either simply observe or regulate the emotion it elicits in them.

First, EDA signal quality was assessed based on the recommendations from previous research (Benedek & Kaernbach, Reference Benedek and Kaernbach2010; Boucsein, 2012). A semi-automatic classifier included the following steps: (1) An independent expert evaluator (JCH) excluded data with incomplete measurements and easily visible artifacts – e.g. noisy data with no visible signal, signals with abrupt level changes; (2) An automatic artifact detector “ft_artifact_jump” implemented in the Fieldtrip Toolbox (Oostenveld et al., Reference Oostenveld, Fries, Maris and Schoffelen2011) was used to detect “jumps” in the data stream based on z-scores and data containing these “jump’ artifacts were excluded from further analysis, except for isolated spikes, which were removed using LedaLab Toolbox; (3) Since the lack of contact between an electrode and the skin results in a fall of the EDA signal to a minimal constant value (Böttcher et al., Reference Böttcher, Vieluf, Bruno, Joseph, Epitashvili, Biondi and Loddenkemper2022; Nasseri et al., Reference Nasseri, Nurse, Glasstetter, Böttcher, Gregg, Laks Nandakumar and Brinkmann2020), flat segments in the EDA data were identified by an expert evaluator (JCH) and data with more than 50% of flat record were excluded from further analysis. This lack of contact between an electrode and the skin was the most common reason for data exclusion (cca 75% of the excluded data).

Preprocessing of the SCR data was performed using Brain Vision Analyzer 2.1 software (Brain Products GmbH, Germany) to suppress MR gradient artifacts. Next, data were filtered by a low-pass Finite Impulse Response (FIR) filter with a cut-off frequency 40 Hz and downsampled to 250 Hz. The FIR filter was a Raised-Cosine filter (beta = 1) designed by the windowing method using a Hanning window. Further preprocessing was done using the LedaLab v3.4.9, a MATLAB Toolbox (Benedek & Kaernbach, Reference Benedek and Kaernbach2010). SCR data were low-pass filtered, and resampled to 25 Hz, the remaining artifacts were corrected by adaptive smoothing with a Gaussian window and subsequently decomposed into its tonic and phasic components using a Continuous Decomposition Analysis (CDA) strategy. The number of optimization initial sets was in the range of 2–4. Next, the event-related sympathetic activity was analyzed relative to stimulus onset while the minimum amplitude criterion used was 0.01 muS. Next, standard trough-to-peak (TTP) analysis was used to extract the number of significant SCR within the response window (nSCR), the response latency of the first significant SCR within the response window (latency), and the sum of the SCR amplitudes within the response window (sum of amplitudes). Good quality skin conductance data (e.g. data with a complete set of trials, without a flat curve or technical artifacts due to movement or bad electrode contact) were available for 144 participants (57 women, 137 men) with ERQ data and 86 of these participants (37 women, 49 men) had also complete data on maternal depression during the perinatal period and early life of the offspring and fMRI data.

Acquisition and analysis of HRV in young adulthood

During the fMRI task, the electrocardiogram (ECG) signals were collected and, subsequently, interbeat (RR) intervals were extracted. Next, the freely available Python package “hrv-analysis” (https://pypi.org/project/hrv-analysis/) was used to pre-process RR intervals and extract HRV features. Briefly, the data underwent outlier removal, interpolation of missing values, and ectopic beat removal, ensuring the integrity of the signal for further HRV analysis. Finally, time-domain HRV features were extracted for the negative observe versus neutral observe as well as the negative regulate versus negative observe contrasts. We focused on the high-frequency band (HF; 0.15–0.50 Hz), which reflects the parasympathetic modulation, and calculated the normalized high-frequency bands (HFn), expressed as percentages and calculated as the absolute power area of the high-frequency band divided by the sum of absolute power areas of both low and high-frequency bands multiplied by 100. Good quality HRV data were available for 243 participants (114 women, 129 men) with ERQ data and 155 of these participants (75 women, 80 men) had also complete data on maternal depression during the perinatal period and early life of the offspring and fMRI data.

Emotion regulation questionnaire in young adulthood

The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) (Gross & John, Reference Gross and John2003) was used to assess participants’ typical emotion regulation strategy. The ERQ questionnaire includes 10 questions on a 7-point Likert scale to evaluate (1) one’s ability to cognitively reappraise emotions and thus being able to change the actual emotional experience when regulating emotions (e.g. “When I want to feel a less negative emotion such as sadness or anger, I change what I’m thinking about.”) and to evaluate (2) one’s ability of expressive suppression and thus being able to suppress the emotions in their talk, gestures or behavior (e.g. “When I want to feel less negative emotions, I make sure not to express them”). This self-assessment questionnaire was done on the same day as the MRI session.

Statistical analyses

First, we conducted a voxelwise analysis using multiple regression in SPM12 to assess the impact of maternal depression during the perinatal period and early life of the offspring on brain response during emotion regulation (the negative regulate versus negative observe contrast). The multiple regression included the 4 EPDS measurements (mid-pregnancy, 2 weeks after birth, 6 months after birth, 18 months after birth) and the interaction with sex was added as a covariate for each contrast. The level of statistical significance for this voxelwise analysis was set to p < 0.05 and was corrected for multiple comparisons using the Family-Wise Error (FWE) correction. Next, we used a general linear model in JMP version 10.0.0 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) to test the relationships between the maternal depression-associated brain response during emotion regulation and the typical use of emotion regulation strategy (cognitive reappraisal, emotion suppression). Multiple comparisons were corrected using the False Discovery Rate (FDR) method. Finally, we used SPSS version 27 to conduct a mediation analysis to determine whether the brain response during emotion regulation, which showed a significant relationship with emotion regulation skills (the use of cognitive reappraisal and emotion suppression strategies), might mediate the relationship between maternal depression and the emotion regulation skills in young adulthood. The significance of the indirect effect (Preacher & Hayes, Reference Preacher and Hayes2004) was determined using a bootstrap procedure. A total of 5,000 bootstrap resamples were used to provide stable estimates of the direct, indirect, and total effects. We determined 95% confidence intervals from the bootstrap resamples and any interval that did not include 0 was considered significantly different from 0. All these analyses were conducted in n = 163.

Subsequently, a general linear model in JMP version 10.0.0 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) evaluated the relationship of the maternal depression-related brain response during the emotion regulation task, which showed a significant relationship with emotion regulation skills, and (1) skin conductance (n = 86) and (2) heart rate variability (n = 155). Finally, we assessed the potential relationship of skin conductance (n = 144) and heart rate variability (n = 243) with the typical use of emotion regulation skills in young adulthood. Sex was considered as a potential moderator. Multiple comparisons were corrected using the FDR method within each family of analyses.

Results

Information on the range of maternal depression at the 4 different time points during the perinatal period and early life of the offspring is available in Supplementary Table 2. As detailed in Supplementary Table 3, the Cronbach alpha values were > 0.7, indicating internal consistency and reliability of the measures. The correlations between the different measures are provided in Supplementary Table 4. The variance inflation factors (VIF) for these relationships (VIF = <1), indicating no multicollinearity issues.

Patterns of brain response to the 2 contrasts of interest – (1) Negative observe versus neutral observe and (2) Negative regulate versus negative observe are also presented in the Supplementary Materials.

Maternal depression during the perinatal period and early life of the offspring and brain function during emotion regulation in young adulthood

Multiple regression showed two statistically significant interactions between maternal depression and offspring sex on brain response during the negative regulate versus negative observe contrast. In women (but not men), maternal depression in mid-pregnancy was associated with lower brain response in one cluster of voxels including the left middle occipital gyrus, cuneus, and occipital superior gyrus (68 voxels, peak T = 4.33, MNI coordinates x = −24, y = −91, z = 17, cluster FWEp = 0.001; see Figure 1a). In women (but not men), maternal depression 2 weeks after birth was associated with greater brain response in a cluster including superior frontal gyrus and anterior orbitofrontal cortex (11 voxels, peak T = 4.50, MNI coordinates x = 12, y = −71, z = 10, cluster FWEp = 0.004; see Figure 1b).

Figure 1. Maternal mental health during the perinatal period and brain response during emotion regulation task in young adulthood. Greater maternal depression during pregnancy was associated with lower brain response in the left occipital cluster in women (blue) during the regulate vs observe negative contrast (1A). Greater maternal depression after birth was associated with greater brain response in the right frontal cluster in women (blue) during the regulate vs observe negative contrast (1B). The box represents the standard error, the whiskers represent the standard deviation, and the square represents the mean activity.

We did not find any statistically significant evidence of an association of maternal depression during the perinatal period and early life of the offspring and brain activity during the condition observe negative > neutral.

Maternal depression-related brain response during emotion regulation fMRI task in young adult women and its relationship with typical emotion regulation strategy

Greater brain response in the right frontal cluster associated in young adult women with maternal depression 2 weeks after birth predicted greater use of emotion suppression (beta = 0.27, p = 0.006, FDRp = 0.02, R2 = 0.07) but not emotion reappraisal (beta = −0.14, p = 0.22, FDRp = 0.22). There was no relationship between the brain response in the occipital cluster associated in young adult women with maternal depression during pregnancy and emotion suppression (beta = 0.19, p = 0.09, FDRp = 0.15) or emotion reappraisal (beta = −0.18, p = 0.11, FDRp = 0.15).

Does brain response in the right frontal cluster mediate the relationship between maternal depression 2 weeks after birth and the typical emotion regulation strategy in young adult women?

Brain response in the right frontal cluster mediated the relationship between greater maternal depression 2 weeks after birth and greater suppression of emotions in young adulthood (ab = 0.11, SE = 0.05, 95% CI [0.016; 0.226]; see Figure 2).

Figure 2. Brain response in the right frontal cluster mediated the relationship between greater maternal depression after birth and greater suppression of emotions in young adult women (ab = 0.11, SE = 0.05, 95% CI [0.016; 0.226]). The a path coefficient refers to the effect of maternal depression after birth on brain response in the right frontal cluster during emotion regulation in young adulthood. The b path coefficient refers to the effect of brain response in the right frontal cluster during emotion regulation on emotion suppression in young adulthood. The indirect effect of maternal depression after birth on emotion suppression in young adulthood through the brain response in the right frontal cluster during emotion regulation in young adulthood is obtained by multiplying a and b path coefficients. The c’ path coefficient refers to the direct effect of the maternal depression after birth on the emotion suppression in young adulthood. Unstandardized regression weights are provided and followed by standard errors (in brackets). n.s. means not significant.

Skin conductance and its relationship with brain response during emotion regulation in young adult women

In young adult women, the greater brain response in the right frontal cluster previously linked with maternal depression 2 weeks after birth was associated with greater nSCR (beta = 0.39, FDRp = 0.01, R2 = 0.16; Figure 3A) and greater skin conductance latency (beta = 0.36, FDRp = 0.04, R2 = 0.13; Figure 3B), but not greater sum of amplitudes (beta = 0.13, p = 0.44) during the emotion regulation.

Figure 3. Skin conductance and emotion regulation in young adulthood. Greater nSCR (3A; R2 = 0.16, FDRp = 0.01) and greater skin conductance latency (3B; R2 = 0.13, FDRp = 0.04) during emotion regulation in women was associated with greater brain response in the right frontal cluster associated with maternal depression after birth. Further, greater nSCR during emotion regulation in women (3C; R2 = 0.10, p = 0.02) and greater skin conductance latency during emotion regulation irrespective of sex (3D; AdjR2 = 0.04, FDRp = 0.02) predicted more emotion reappraisal. Women are depicted in blue, men are depicted in red.

Skin conductance and its relationship with typical emotion regulation strategy

Sex interacted with nSCR (AdjR2 = 0.04, beta = 0.25, FDRp = 0.02; Figure 3c) and predicted greater emotion reappraisal. Posthoc analyses revealed that the relationship between greater nSCR during emotion regulation and more emotion reappraisal was significant in women (beta = 0.31, p = 0.02, R2 = 0.10) but not men (beta = −0.15, p = 0.16). In addition, greater skin conductance latency predicted greater emotion reappraisal irrespective of sex (AdjR2 = 0.04, beta = 0.24, FDRp = 0.02; Figure 3d). No relationship appeared with the sum of amplitudes (beta = 0.05, FDRp = 0.69). Also, no similar relationships appeared with emotion suppression (p > 0.19).

Heart rate and its relationship with brain response during emotion regulation in young adult women

There was no relationship between the HFn during emotion regulation and brain response in the right frontal cluster associated with maternal depression 2 weeks after birth in young adult women (beta = 0.11, p = 0.36).

Heart rate and its relationship with typical emotion regulation strategy in young adulthood

Greater HFn during emotion regulation interacted with sex and predicted more emotion suppression (beta = −0.15, FDRp = 0.01) in young adulthood. Posthoc analyses revealed that the relationship between greater HFn during emotion regulation and more emotion suppression was significant in women (R2 = 0.08, beta = 0.29, p = 0.002; Figure 4) but not men (beta = −0.02, p = 0.83). There was no similar interaction between HFn and sex on emotion reappraisal (beta = 0.03, FDRp = 0.45) or a main effect of HFn on reappraisal (beta = 0.09, p = 0.16).

Figure 4. Greater HFn during emotion regulation was associated with more emotion suppression in women (blue; R 2 = 0.08, beta = 0.29, p = 0.002). Women are depicted in blue, men are depicted in red.

Participants’ feelings during the observe versus regulate conditions of the fMRI task

As indicated in the graph provided in Supplementary Figure 4 and in the statistics overview table provided in Supplementary Table 7, the regulate condition was associated with significantly more „not at all “answers (z = 4.83, p < 0.001) than the observe condition, indicating that the emotion regulation helped participants not to feel uncomfortable. Consistently, the observe condition was associated with significantly more „quite a bit “(z = −3.35, p = 0.001) and „very much “(z = −2.79, p = 0.005) answers than the regulate condition, indicating that simple observation of the negative images made the participants feel uncomfortable.

Discussion

We have conducted a neuroimaging follow-up of a prenatal birth cohort and studied the relationships between maternal depression during the perinatal period and early life of the offspring and brain response during an emotion regulation task in young adulthood. We also assessed whether these relationships might be reflected by typical emotion regulation strategy (emotion suppression versus emotion reappraisal) in young adulthood and particularly whether the altered brain response might explain the relationship between maternal perinatal depression and the typical emotion regulation strategy in the young adult offspring. Moreover, we also tested whether the altered brain response might be reflected by altered skin conductance and heart rate variability during the emotion regulation task and whether the altered skin conductance and heart rate variability could predict the typical emotion regulation strategy.

We showed that maternal depression 2 weeks after birth (above and beyond maternal depression experienced at other times during the perinatal period and early life of the offspring) predicted greater response in the ventromedial prefrontal region including superior frontal gyrus and anterior orbitofrontal cortex during emotion regulation in young adult women but not men. Moreover, this altered brain response during emotion regulation in young adult women was associated with greater habitual use of emotion suppression, which is typically a maladaptive emotion regulation strategy. In addition, the increased brain response in the right frontal cluster mediated the relationship between maternal depression 2 weeks after birth and emotion suppression in the young adult daughters. We speculate that the greater brain response in the frontal regions might reflect greater inefficiency of the prefrontal circuitry during emotion regulation and that young adult women, whose mothers suffered from more depressive symptoms 2 weeks after birth, had to make more cognitive effort to regulate their negative affect. These findings also suggest that women are more vulnerable to the long-lasting changes of early adversity and might also, at least in part, explain the greater prevalence of depression in women.

These findings substantially extend the research of Maruyama et al (Maruyama et al., Reference Maruyama, Valente, Tovo-Rodrigues, Santos, Barros, Munhoz and Matijasevich2023), who linked maternal depression during the offspring’s childhood to worse emotion regulation skills in adolescence, and the research of Li et al (Li et al., Reference Li, Li, Wu and Wang2019), who linked parental emotion dysregulation in offspring’s childhood to worse emotion regulation in childhood. They point out the importance of timing of the exposure and that the early postpartum period (e.g 2 weeks after birth) might be particularly important not only for mother–infant attachment (Śliwerski et al., Reference Śliwerski, Kossakowska, Jarecka, Świtalska and Bielawska-Batorowicz2020), but also for the development of emotion regulation skills in the offspring. The sex-specificity of our findings then supports the research of Nolen-Hoeksema (Nolen-Hoeksema, Reference Nolen-Hoeksema2012) and McRae et al (McRae et al., Reference McRae, Ochsner, Mauss, Gabrieli and Gross2008), who reported sex differences in emotion regulation and brain response during emotion regulation.

The relationship between maternal depression during pregnancy (above and beyond maternal depression experienced at other times during the perinatal period and early life of the offspring) and greater brain response in the occipital cluster in women might be related to altered sensory development in utero. However, activity in this cluster was not further associated with altered skin conductance, heart rate variability, or the typical emotion regulation strategy. These findings are suggestive of a temporal specificity, whereby maternal depression 2 weeks after birth but not during pregnancy particularly affects emotion regulation skills in young adulthood. Based on this time-specificity, we speculate that the relationship between maternal depression 2 weeks after birth and worse emotion regulation skills in young adulthood might not stem from a direct effect of altered glucocorticoids or maternal immune activation during the prenatal development but may instead be mediated by socio-emotional developmental factors such as an insecure attachment or an indirect effect on cortisol levels in young adulthood. In fact, it has been demonstrated that cortisol might exert beneficial effects on the cognitive downregulation of affect elicited by intensive negative pictures (Jentsch et al., Reference Jentsch, Merz and Wolf2019; Langer et al., Reference Langer, Jentsch and Wolf2022).

We also demonstrated that the altered brain response during emotion regulation in women was further associated with skin conductance and heart rate variability during emotion regulation and that this altered skin conductance and heart rate variability predicted the typical emotion regulation strategy. These findings suggest that exposure to maternal depression may impact emotion regulation on the neural and physiological levels, which may in turn translate into broader patterns of habitual use of adaptive versus maladaptive emotion regulation skills. The fact that we found associations between greater emotion reappraisal, the more adaptive emotion regulation strategy, and greater skin conductance latency as well as greater nSCR is consistent with previous research that found lower skin conductance among depressed patients versus healthy controls. (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Jang, Choi, Jeon, Byun, Sim and Yu2019; Ward et al., Reference Ward, Doerr and Storrie1983). The relationship between greater HFn, an index of parasympathetic modulation, and greater emotion suppression in women is similarly consistent with An et al (An et al., Reference An, Han, Jeong, Kim, Lee, Lee and Kim2020), who reported an association between greater HFn and increased risk of depression. The concurrent acquisition of skin conductance, heart rate variability and brain response during fMRI, as done in our current study, is rare, mostly due to the difficult pre-processing of the physiology data and thus our study provides important evidence that the trough to peak (TTP) analysis is well suited for the analysis of skin conductance response during fMRI task and that the altered emotion regulation manifests particularly in the nSCR, skin conductance latency, and HFn.

Our findings are limited by the design of the current study and do not allow us to make any conclusions about the relationship between maternal depression occurring before or after these 4 sensitive periods and emotion regulation skills in the offspring or about the relationship between emotion regulation skills in the mother and the young adult offspring. We also did not assess the genetics of the mother and the offspring and thus could not test to what extent the observed time-specific relationships between maternal depression 2 weeks after birth and the brain function and physiology during the emotion regulation task as well as the emotion regulation skills in the offspring might be mediated by genes. Still, the current study has a number of methodological strengths including (1) prospective longitudinal study design using data from a prenatal birth cohort with detailed information on maternal depression during the perinatal period and early life of the offspring assessed at four different timepoints throughout the pregnancy and after birth, (2) assessment of emotion regulation skills in young adulthood using both fMRI emotion regulation task as well as behavioral data regarding the typical emotion regulation strategy, (3) combination of the brain response with skin conductance and heart rate variability data collected during the fMRI task, and (4) even distribution of sex within the sample, enabling us to investigate the role of sex in these relationships. Therefore, our findings regarding the time-specific relationship between maternal depression during the perinatal period and brain function during an emotion regulation task as well as the typical emotion regulation strategy in young adulthood provide unique insight into the developmental programming of mental health.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291725000042.

Data availability statement

Data are available from the first author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

This work has received funding from Czech Science Foundation, project no. 24–12183 M, Czech Health Research Council (No. NU20J-04-00022), the Czech Ministry of Education, Youth and Sports (MEYS CR) (CEITEC 2020, LQ1601, LM2023069), and by project nr. LX22NPO5107 (MEYS): Funded by European Union – Next Generation EU. Support with obtaining scientific data presented in this paper came from the core facility Multimodal and Functional Imaging Laboratory of Central European Institute of Technology, Masaryk University supported by the Czech-BioImaging large RI project (No. LM2018129, funded by MEYS CR). Dr. Plesinger and Ing. Chladek were supported by the Czech Academy of Sciences, RVO: 68081731. Dr Nikolova is supported by a Koerner New Scientist Award and a Paul Garfinkel New Investigator Catalyst Award administered by the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health Foundation.

Author contribution

KM: Conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, writing the original draft, funding acquisition; FT: Formal analysis, writing the original draft; RM: Analysis of fMRI data, Software; JCH: Analysis of skin conductance data; Software; ZK: Analysis of heart rate variability data, Software; FP: Supervision of the analysis of heart rate variability data, Validation; LA: Resources; MB: Resources; YN: Writing -review and editing.

Competing interest

All authors reported no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

References

Acosta, H., Tuulari, J. J., Scheinin, N. M., Hashempour, N., Rajasilta, O., Lavonius, T. I.,…Karlsson, H. (2019). Maternal pregnancy-related anxiety is associated with sexually dimorphic alterations in amygdala volume in 4-year-old children. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 13, 175. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00175CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amani, B., Krzeczkowski, J. E., Schmidt, L. A., & Van Lieshout, R. J. (2024). Public health nurse-delivered cognitive behavioral therapy for postpartum depression: Assessing the effects of maternal treatment on infant emotion regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 19. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579423001566Google ScholarPubMed
An, H., Han, J. W., Jeong, H. G., Kim, T. H., Lee, J. J., Lee, S. B.,…Kim, K. W. (2020). Parasympathetic predominance is a risk factor for future depression: A prospective cohort study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 260, 232237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.09.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Appelhans, B. M., & Luecken, L. J. (2006). Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Review of General Psychology, 10(3), 229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., & Kaernbach, C. (2010). Decomposition of skin conductance data by means of nonnegative deconvolution. Psychophysiology, 47(4), 647658. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00972.xGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boucsein. (2012). Electrodermal Activity. Springer. ISBN 978-1-4614-1125-3 e-ISBN 978-1-4614-1126-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2017). International affective picture system. In Zeigler-Hill, V. & Shackelford, T. K. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences (pp. 14). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_42-1Google Scholar
Böttcher, S., Vieluf, S., Bruno, E., Joseph, B., Epitashvili, N., Biondi, A.,…Loddenkemper, T. (2022). Data quality evaluation in wearable monitoring. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 21412. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-25949-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cao-Lei, L., de Rooij, S. R., King, S., Matthews, S. G., Metz, G. A. S., Roseboom, T. J., & Szyf, M. (2020). Prenatal stress and epigenetics. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 117, 198210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.05.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, J. L., Holden, J. M., & Sagovsky, R. (1987). Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item edinburgh postnatal depression scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 782786. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.150.6.782CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, D., Li, D., Wu, N., & Wang, Z. (2019). Intergenerational transmission of emotion regulation through parents’ reactions to children’s negative emotions: Tests of unique, actor, partner, and mediating effects. Children and Youth Services Review, 101, 113122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.03.038.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ebneabbasi, A., Mahdipour, M., Nejati, V., Li, M., Liebe, T., Colic, L.,…Tahmasian, M. (2021). Emotion processing and regulation in major depressive disorder: A 7T resting-state fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 42(3), 797810. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25263CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, D. W., Dewitt, M., Hudgens-Haney, M., Schaeffer, D. J., Ball, B. H., Schwarz, N. F.,…Sabatinelli, D. (2014). Emotion regulation: Quantitative meta-analysis of functional activation and deactivation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 45, 202211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.06.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, J., McKenzie-McHarg, K., Shakespeare, J., Price, J., & Gray, R. (2009). A systematic review of studies validating the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale in antepartum and postpartum women. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 119(5), 350364. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01363.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, J. J., & Barrett, L. F. (2011). Emotion generation and emotion regulation: One or two depends on your point of view. Emotion Review, 3(1), 816. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073910380974CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348362. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, T., Zhang, D., Wang, J., Mistry, R., Ran, G., & Wang, X. (2014). Relation between emotion regulation and mental health: A meta-analysis review. Psychological Reports, 114(2), 341362. https://doi.org/10.2466/03.20.PR0.114k22w4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iacono, W. G., & Lykken, D. T. (1983). The effects of instructions on electrodermal habituation. Psychophysiology, 20(1), 7180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1983.tb00905.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jentsch, V. L., Merz, C. J., & Wolf, O. T. (2019). Restoring emotional stability: Cortisol effects on the neural network of cognitive emotion regulation. Behavioural Brain Research, 374, 111880. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2019.03.049CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 13011333. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00298.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jonsson, P. (2007). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia as a function of state anxiety in healthy individuals. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 63(1), 4854. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.08.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, A. Y., Jang, E. H., Choi, K. W., Jeon, H. J., Byun, S., Sim, J. Y.,…Yu, H. Y. (2019). Skin conductance responses in Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) under mental arithmetic stress. PLoS One, 14(4), e0213140. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213140CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, D. R., Bale, T. L., & Epperson, C. N. (2015). Prenatal programming of mental illness: Current understanding of relationship and mechanisms. Current Psychiatry Reports, 17(2), 5. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-014-0546-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langer, K., Jentsch, V. L., & Wolf, O. T. (2022). Cortisol promotes the cognitive regulation of high intensive emotions independent of timing. European Journal of Neuroscience, 55(9–10), 26842698. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.15182CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mareckova, K., Holsen, L. M., Admon, R., Makris, N., Seidman, L., Buka, S.,…Goldstein, J. M. (2016). Brain activity and connectivity in response to negative affective stimuli: Impact of dysphoric mood and sex across diagnoses. Human Brain Mapping, 37(11), 37333744. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23271CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mareckova, K., Miles, A., Andryskova, L., Brazdil, M., & Nikolova, Y. S. (2020). Temporally and sex-specific effects of maternal perinatal stress on offspring cortical gyrification and mood in young adulthood. Human Brain Mapping, 41(17), 48664875. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25163CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maruyama, J. M., Valente, J. Y., Tovo-Rodrigues, L., Santos, I. S., Barros, A. J. D., Munhoz, T. N.,…Matijasevich, A. (2023). Maternal depression trajectories in childhood, subsequent maltreatment, and adolescent emotion regulation and self-esteem: The 2004 Pelotas birth cohort. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(10), 19351945. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-022-02022-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazefsky, C. A., Herrington, J., Siegel, M., Scarpa, A., Maddox, B. B., Scahill, L., & White, S. W. (2013). The role of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(7), 679688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McRae, K., & Gross, J. J. (2020). Emotion regulation. Emotion, 20(1), 19. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000703CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McRae, K., Ochsner, K. N., Mauss, I. B., Gabrieli, J. J. D., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Gender differences in emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 11(2), 143162. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430207088035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monk, C., Lugo-Candelas, C., & Trumpff, C. (2019). Prenatal developmental origins of future psychopathology: Mechanisms and pathways. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 15, 317344. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095539CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morawetz, C., Bode, S., Derntl, B., & Heekeren, H. R. (2017). The effect of strategies, goals and stimulus material on the neural mechanisms of emotion regulation: A meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 72, 111128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Napadow, V., Dhond, R., Conti, G., Makris, N., Brown, E. N., & Barbieri, R. (2008). Brain correlates of autonomic modulation: Combining heart rate variability with fMRI. Neuroimage, 42(1), 169177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.238CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nasseri, M., Nurse, E., Glasstetter, M., Böttcher, S., Gregg, N. M., Laks Nandakumar, A.,…Brinkmann, B. H. (2020). Signal quality and patient experience with wearable devices for epilepsy management. Epilepsia, 61 Suppl 1, S25s35. https://doi.org/10.1111/epi.16527CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nickel, P., & Nachreiner, F. (2003). Sensitivity and diagnosticity of the 0.1-Hz component of heart rate variability as an indicator of mental workload. Human Factors, 45(4), 575590. https://doi.org/10.1518/hfes.45.4.575.27094CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2012). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: The role of gender. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 161187. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Donnell, K. J., Glover, V., Barker, E. D., & O’Connor, T. G. (2014). The persisting effect of maternal mood in pregnancy on childhood psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 26(2), 393403. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579414000029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Donnell, K. J., & Meaney, M. J. (2017). Fetal origins of mental health: The developmental origins of health and disease hypothesis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 174(4), 319328. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16020138CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., & Schoffelen, J. M. (2011). FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, 2011, 156869. https://doi.org/10.1155/2011/156869CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, C., Rosenblat, J. D., Lee, Y., Pan, Z., Cao, B., Iacobucci, M., & McIntyre, R. S. (2019). The neural systems of emotion regulation and abnormalities in major depressive disorder. Behavioural Brain Research, 367, 181188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2019.04.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, G., & Thayer, J. F. (2014). From the heart to the mind: Cardiac vagal tone modulates top-down and bottom-up visual perception and attention to emotional stimuli. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 278. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00278CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piler, P., Kandrnal, V., Kukla, L., Andryskova, L., Svancara, J., Jarkovsky, J.,…Klanova, J. (2017). Cohort profile: The European longitudinal study of pregnancy and childhood (elspac) in the czech republic. International Journal of Epidemiology, 46(5), 13791379f. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyw091Google ScholarPubMed
Power, J. D., Mitra, A., Laumann, T. O., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2014). Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage, 84, 320341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, J. D., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2015). Recent progress and outstanding issues in motion correction in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage, 105, 536551. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.044CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(4), 717731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pupikova, M., Simko, P., Lamos, M., Gajdos, M., & Rektorova, I. (2022). Inter-individual differences in baseline dynamic functional connectivity are linked to cognitive aftereffects of tDCS. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 20754. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-25016-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwarze, C. E., von der Heiden, S., Wallwiener, S., & Pauen, S. (2024). The role of perinatal maternal symptoms of depression, anxiety and pregnancy-specific anxiety for infant’s self-regulation: A prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 346, 144153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2023.10.035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seedat, S., Scott, K. M., Angermeyer, M. C., Berglund, P., Bromet, E. J., Brugha, T. S.,…Kessler, R. C. (2009). Cross-national associations between gender and mental disorders in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(7), 785795. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.36CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, P., Stringaris, A., Nigg, J., & Leibenluft, E. (2014). Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(3), 276293. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070966CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheppes, G., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 11, 379405. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112739CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Śliwerski, A., Kossakowska, K., Jarecka, K., Świtalska, J., & Bielawska-Batorowicz, E. (2020). The effect of maternal depression on infant attachment: A systematic review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(8). https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17082675CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H., Tomoda, A., & Andersen, S. L. (2006). Neurobiological consequences of early stress and childhood maltreatment: Are results from human and animal studies comparable? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1071, 313323. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1364.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ward, N. G., Doerr, H. O., & Storrie, M. C. (1983). Skin conductance: A potentially sensitive test for depression. Psychiatry Research, 10(4), 295302. https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-1781(83)90076-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webb, T. L., Miles, E., & Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 775808. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027600CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaehringer, J., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., Schmahl, C., Ende, G., & Paret, C. (2020). Psychophysiological effects of downregulating negative emotions: Insights from a meta-analysis of healthy adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 470. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00470CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Maternal mental health during the perinatal period and brain response during emotion regulation task in young adulthood. Greater maternal depression during pregnancy was associated with lower brain response in the left occipital cluster in women (blue) during the regulate vs observe negative contrast (1A). Greater maternal depression after birth was associated with greater brain response in the right frontal cluster in women (blue) during the regulate vs observe negative contrast (1B). The box represents the standard error, the whiskers represent the standard deviation, and the square represents the mean activity.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Brain response in the right frontal cluster mediated the relationship between greater maternal depression after birth and greater suppression of emotions in young adult women (ab = 0.11, SE = 0.05, 95% CI [0.016; 0.226]). The a path coefficient refers to the effect of maternal depression after birth on brain response in the right frontal cluster during emotion regulation in young adulthood. The b path coefficient refers to the effect of brain response in the right frontal cluster during emotion regulation on emotion suppression in young adulthood. The indirect effect of maternal depression after birth on emotion suppression in young adulthood through the brain response in the right frontal cluster during emotion regulation in young adulthood is obtained by multiplying a and b path coefficients. The c’ path coefficient refers to the direct effect of the maternal depression after birth on the emotion suppression in young adulthood. Unstandardized regression weights are provided and followed by standard errors (in brackets). n.s. means not significant.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Skin conductance and emotion regulation in young adulthood. Greater nSCR (3A; R2 = 0.16, FDRp = 0.01) and greater skin conductance latency (3B; R2 = 0.13, FDRp = 0.04) during emotion regulation in women was associated with greater brain response in the right frontal cluster associated with maternal depression after birth. Further, greater nSCR during emotion regulation in women (3C; R2 = 0.10, p = 0.02) and greater skin conductance latency during emotion regulation irrespective of sex (3D; AdjR2 = 0.04, FDRp = 0.02) predicted more emotion reappraisal. Women are depicted in blue, men are depicted in red.

Figure 3

Figure 4. Greater HFn during emotion regulation was associated with more emotion suppression in women (blue; R2 = 0.08, beta = 0.29, p = 0.002). Women are depicted in blue, men are depicted in red.

Supplementary material: File

Mareckova et al. supplementary material

Mareckova et al. supplementary material
Download Mareckova et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.7 MB