Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T09:03:57.666Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Do nutritional warnings encourage healthier choices on food ordering websites? An exploratory experimental study in Uruguay

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 March 2021

Vanessa Gugliucci
Affiliation:
Espacio Interdisciplinario, Universidad de la República, José Enrique Rodó 1843, CP 11200Montevideo, Uruguay
Leandro Machín
Affiliation:
Centro de Investigación Básica en Psicología, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de la República, Tristán Narvaja 1674, CP 11200Montevideo, Uruguay
María Rosa Curutchet
Affiliation:
Instituto Nacional de Alimentacion, Piedras 165, CP 11000Montevideo, Uruguay
Gastón Ares*
Affiliation:
Centro de Investigación Básica en Psicología, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de la República, Tristán Narvaja 1674, CP 11200Montevideo, Uruguay Sensometrics & Consumer Science, Instituto Polo Tecnológico de Pando, Facultad de Química, Universidad de la República, By Pass de Rutas 8 y 101 s/n, CP 91000 Pando, Canelones, Uruguay
*
*Corresponding author: Email gares@fq.edu.uy
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

To evaluate if the inclusion of nutritional warnings in food ordering websites can discourage consumers from purchasing foods with excessive content of nutrients associated with non-communicable diseases (NCD).

Design:

Participants were randomly assigned to one of the two experimental conditions: control (n 225) or nutritional warnings (n 222). Nutritional warnings corresponded to separate black octagonal signs containing the word ‘Excess’ followed by the corresponding nutrient: total fat, saturated fat, sugars and sodium. Participants were asked to purchase a lunch for themselves using a simulated food ordering website.

Setting:

Online study in Uruguay.

Participants:

Convenience sample of 447 Uruguayan participants, recruited using social media.

Results:

In the control condition, 76 % of the participants selected a dish or a beverage with excessive content of at least one nutrient in the simulated food ordering website. When nutritional warnings were included, this percentage significantly decreased to 62 % (P = 0·002). In addition, nutritional warnings caused a significant reduction in the percentage of participants who selected dishes with excessive content of total fat: 50 % v. 62 % (P = 0·012).

Conclusions:

Results from the present work provide preliminary evidence that the inclusion of nutritional warnings in food ordering websites could discourage consumers from selecting dishes and beverages with excessive content of nutrients associated with NCD.

Type
Short Communication
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Nutritional warnings are a front-of-package nutrition labelling scheme that is gaining increased interest among policy-makers, particularly in the region of the Americas(Reference Khandpur, Swinburn and Monteiro1,2) . They consist of text-based logos that highlight products with excessive amounts of nutrients associated with non-communicable diseases (NCD)(2). Nutritional warnings make negative aspects of the nutritional composition of products more salient in consumers’ mind, encouraging them to re-assess their food purchase decisions(Reference Ares, Antúnez and Curutchet3,Reference Ares, Antúnez and Curutchet4) . A growing body of evidence shows that nutritional warnings are efficient at discouraging consumption of products with excessive content of nutrients associated with NCD(Reference Ares, Antúnez and Curutchet3Reference Taillie, Reyes and Colchero7).

To date, the implementation of nutritional warnings has been mostly restricted to packaged foods(2,Reference Ares, Antúnez and Curutchet4Reference Deliza, de Alcantara and Pereira8) . However, their extension to food establishments selling unpackaged foods deserves further attention given the growing contribution of prepared meals and meals consumed outside the home to the overall diet(9,Reference Farfán, Genoni and Vakis10) . Compared to home-made meals, consumption of meals prepared away from home has been associated with a higher intake of total calories, total fat, saturated fat, trans-fatty acids, and sodium, and a lower intake of fruit, vegetables and fibre(Reference Binkley and Liu11). In recent years, food delivery services have risen worldwide as consumers increasingly order food online through websites or apps(Reference Eagle12,Reference Yeo, Goh and Rezaei13) . This trend is expected to be exacerbated in the context of the social distancing measures implemented to contain the outbreak of COVID-19(Reference Sheth14).

The implementation of nutritional information to unpackaged foods has received increasing attention worldwide to encourage healthier food choices(9). However, it has been mainly focused on calorie labelling on restaurant menus, which has been reported to have a limited effect on consumer behaviour(Reference Fernandes, Oliveira and Proenca15Reference Rubin18). Considering that simple and visible summaries of the nutritional quality of foods have been shown to be more impactful than numeric information(2,Reference Taillie, Reyes and Colchero7) , the implementation of nutritional warnings in food retail establishments selling unpackaged foods deserves further consideration. Based on research conducted with packaged foods(Reference Ares, Antúnez and Curutchet4Reference Taillie, Reyes and Colchero7), it could be hypothesised that nutritional warnings could potentially discourage selection of foods and beverages with excessive content of nutrients associated with NCD. In this sense, the limitation of nutritional warnings to pre-packaged foods has been a concern during the public consultation held before the implementation of this public policy in Chile and Uruguay(19,Reference Ares, Bove and Díaz20) . The Chilean government identified the extension of nutritional warnings to retail food establishments as a relevant topic for the medium-term policy agenda(19).

In this context, the aim of the present work was to evaluate if the inclusion of nutritional warnings in food ordering websites can discourage consumers from purchasing foods with excessive content of nutrients associated with NCD.

Methods

Participants

A total of 447 participants were recruited using an advertisement on Facebook and Instagram targeted at Uruguayan adult users between 12 June and 27 June 2020. The advertisement included the following text ‘Click on the link, choose a lunch as you do in a food ordering website, and enter a raffle for a gift card’, accompanied by an image containing a cellphone and food delivery motorcycle. The advertisement was delivered to 149 050 social media users, randomly selected by Facebook software, from which 1640 clicked on the advertisement and 447 completed the shopping task. All the participants who completed the task used food ordering websites, at least occasionally. Participants (64 % female) were aged between 18 and 71 years (M = 39·9, sd = 14·5) and were diverse in terms of their educational level (Table 1). The study was approved by the ethics committee of the School of Chemistry of Universidad de la República (Uruguay).

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics of the participants (n 447)

Stimuli

The study was conducted using a website that simulated an online food ordering website. A total of 224 dishes and beverages were included. Dishes and beverages were grouped into 31 categories: Beverages (mineral water – still and sparkling, flavoured water – regular and sugar-free versions, soft drinks – regular and sugar-free versions), Coffee, Calzone, Meat, Chivito (a typical Uruguayan meat sandwich), Arabic, Chinese, Indian, Empanadas (baked or fried mould of pastry filled with a variety of ingredients), Salads, Revuelto gramajo (typical dish composed of French fries, scrambled egg, ham and other ingredients), Burgers, Ice cream, Juices and Smoothies, Lahmacun (thin piece of dough topped with minced meat, minced vegetables and herbs), Milanesas (slice of meat with breading, typically fried), Hot dogs, French fries, Parrilla (grilled meats), Pasta, Fish and seafood, Appetizers, Pizza, Poke (cubes of fish served in a bowl along with rice, dressing, vegetables and seasonings), Chicken, Desserts, Sandwiches, Sushi, Pies, Tortilla (omelette made with eggs and potatoes), and Wraps. The categories were selected based on the options available on the most popular Uruguayan food ordering website. Within each category, 3–18 options were selected, based on the recommended dishes and beverages available in the food ordering website. Dishes and beverages were presented on the website using a written description of the dish or beverage, a picture and the price. A screen capture of the website is shown in the Appendix. Prices were selected based on the information available in the most popular Uruguayan food ordering website. Only beverages included brand information (shown in the product name and the picture).

Two experimental conditions were considered: (i) a control condition with no nutritional information and (ii) the Uruguayan nutritional warning system. This system uses separate black stop signs, containing the word ‘Excess’ followed by the corresponding nutrient, to highlight products with excessive content of total fat, saturated fat, sugars and sodium. According to the Uruguayan regulation, nutritional warnings were included on dishes and beverages added with fat, sugar and/or salt if their content exceeded pre-established limits, based on a flexibilisation of the nutrient profile model of the Pan American Health Organization(21). From all the available options, 50 % contained excessive content of total fat, 61 % excessive content of saturated fat, 10 % excessive content of sodium and 14 % excessive content of sugars. Nutritional warnings were included next to the picture of the dish or beverage, as exemplified in the Appendix.

Experimental procedure

Participants were randomly assigned to one of the two experimental conditions: control (n 225) or nutritional warnings (n 222). Randomisation was automatically generated by the software and was blinded to the researchers during the study. No significant differences between the groups were found in their gender, age, educational status, household composition and responsibility for food purchasing (all P-values higher than 0·23).

First, participants provided informed consent using an online form. They completed a series of sociodemographic questions and indicated their frequency of use of food ordering websites to confirm eligibility criteria. Participants were also asked to indicate their willingness to participate in a raffle for a voucher worth 70 US dollars by clicking on the corresponding option and providing their contact details. Then, they were given the following instructions: ‘The study consists of making a purchase of a lunch for yourself, as if you were using a food ordering website’. They were explained that products were grouped into categories and that they could go through the products included in each category by making a mouse click on the category name. They were asked to select all the products they would buy by clicking on the ‘Add’ button of each product. Once they had finished their purchase, they were instructed to click on the shopping cart symbol at the top of the website to review the purchase and submit their response.

Data analysis

The primary outcome of the study was the percentage of participants who selected a dish or beverage with excessive content of any nutrient (total fat, saturated fat, sodium and sugars). The secondary outcomes were the percentage of participants who selected a dish or beverage with excessive content of each separate nutrient (total fat, saturated fat, sodium and sugars). The minimum number of participants to be included in the study was calculated a priori. A minimum of 342 participants (171 per group) was needed to detect a significant difference of 15 % in the percentage of participants who selected dishes or beverages with nutritional warnings, from 71 % in the control condition. This last percentage was estimated based on the percentage of the dishes and beverages in the food ordering website which featured warnings, whereas the 15 % change was estimated based on results from a previous study with packaged products(Reference Machin, Curutchet and Giménez5).

A set of binary variables were created to identify whether a participant selected a dish or beverage with excessive content of any nutrient and excessive content of total fat, saturated fat, sodium and sugars (1: participant selected a dish or beverage with excessive content of the target nutrient and 0: participant did not select a dish or beverage with excessive content of the target nutrient). The percentage of participants who selected dishes or beverages with excessive content of total fat, saturated fat, sodium and sugars were calculated. A generalised linear model was used to evaluate the effect of the experimental condition on the likelihood of participants’ selecting a product with excessive content of each of the four nutrients. A logit link function and a 5 % significance level were considered. Hypotheses and analytic plan were not publicly registered prior to data collection.

Results

Participants selected dishes and beverages within an average of 2·6 categories. Fifty-six per cent of the participants selected at least one beverage (24 % selected mineral water, 26 % flavoured water, 15 % soft drinks and 10 % juices or smoothies), whereas 44 % selected a dessert. The most frequently selected dishes corresponded to the categories: Meat (selected by 20 % of the participants), Salads (17 %), Pies (9 %), Sandwiches and Bocattas (8 %), Chivito (8 %), Empanadas (7 %), and Milanesas (7 %) (data not shown).

In the control condition, 76 % of the participants selected a dish or a beverage with excessive content of at least one nutrient in the simulated food ordering website. When nutritional warnings were included, this percentage significantly decreased to 62 % (P = 0·002). As shown in Table 2, when data were analysed at the level of individual nutrients, it can be concluded that nutritional warnings caused a significant reduction in the percentage of participants who selected dishes with excessive content of total fat: 50 % v. 62 % (P = 0·012). In addition, a marginal reduction in the percentage of participants who selected dishes or beverages with excessive content of saturated fat (P = 0·079) and sugars (P = 0·074) was observed (Table 2).

Table 2 Percentage of participants who selected a dish or beverage with excessive content of nutrients for participants in the control group (without nutritional information) and the nutritional warnings group

Percentages highlighted in bold are significantly different between the two experimental conditions according to the generalised linear model.

Discussion

Although nutritional warnings have been compulsorily implemented on packaged foods in several countries worldwide, their application to unpackaged foods has not gained much attention yet(Reference Khandpur, Swinburn and Monteiro1,2,19,Reference Ares, Bove and Díaz20) . The only application of this policy to food establishments is restricted to the inclusion of sodium warnings on the menus of chain restaurants in New York(Reference Anekwe, Lent and Farley22). However, empirical evidence of the efficacy of warnings to discourage selection of dishes with excessive content of nutrients associated with NCD is still limited(Reference Byrd, Almanza and Ghiselli23Reference Musicus, Moran and Lawman25). In this context, the present work intended to contribute to the literature by assessing the effect of including nutritional warnings on food ordering websites.

Results showed that the inclusion of nutritional warnings in a simulated food ordering website led to a reduction in the percentage of participants who selected dishes or beverages with excessive content of at least one nutrient associated with NCD. Warnings discouraged participants from selecting dishes with excessive content of total fat and beverages with excessive content of sugars. These results are in agreement with empirical evidence on the impact of the inclusion of nutritional warnings on packaged foods in experimental studies(Reference Machin, Curutchet and Giménez5,Reference Grummon and Hall6) , as well as after their compulsorily implementation in Chile and Uruguay(Reference Ares, Antúnez and Curutchet4,Reference Taillie, Reyes and Colchero7) .

The inclusion of nutritional warnings did not significantly modify the percentage of participants choosing meals with excessive sodium content. One of the possible explanations for this result is that dishes with excessive content of sodium only accounted for 10 % of all the available options. Similar results have been reported by Byrd et al. when assessing the impact of sodium warnings on restaurant menus(Reference Byrd, Almanza and Ghiselli23). According to these authors, the inclusion of sodium warnings on restaurant menus was associated with meal selections with lower sodium content only for participants that regarded foods with lower sodium content and healthy foods as healthy(Reference Byrd, Almanza and Ghiselli23). Similarly, Burton et al. reported that provision of numeric information about sodium content only influenced the choice of consumers with heart disease risk(Reference Burton, Tangari and Howlett24). However, another recent study reported that the inclusion of red octagonal sodium warnings on restaurant menus led to a significant reduction in the average sodium content of the selected items(Reference Musicus, Moran and Lawman25). Further research is needed to explore the variables that mediate the effect of warnings on consumers’ food choices in food ordering websites and other establishments selling unpackaged foods.

Limitations of the study

To the authors’ knowledge, the present study is the first to explore the influence of nutritional warnings on consumer food choices in food ordering websites. Despite its novelty, the study is not free of limitations.

First of all, the hypotheses and analytic plan were not publicly registered prior to data collection. Secondly, the study involved hypothetical choices as participants did not consume the selected items nor paid for them. In addition, although the simulated food ordering website included a wide range of dishes and beverages, no information about food retail establishments was provided. Further research should be conducted to evaluate the influence of nutritional warnings on real purchase behaviour in food ordering websites and other establishments selling unpackaged foods.

A relatively small sample of participants recruited using social media was considered, which may be the cause of some of the marginal effects reported in the present work. In addition, the small sample size did not enable to evaluate the effect of nutritional warnings on specific segments of the population (e.g. age groups). In this sense, the effect of nutritional warnings on participants with NCD deserves special attention(Reference Burton, Tangari and Howlett24).

Finally, results were not analysed considering specific categories of dishes and beverages. Research on packaged products suggests that nutritional warnings are expected to be more efficient in food categories wrongfully regarded as healthy compared to categories that are clearly perceived as unhealthy (e.g. French fries)(Reference Ares, Antúnez and Curutchet3,Reference Ares, Antúnez and Curutchet4,Reference Ares, Aschemann-Witzel and Curutchet26) . Future studies should look at category effects to provide an in-depth understanding of how nutritional warnings influence consumer choices of unpackaged foods and prepared meals.

Conclusions

Results from the present work provide preliminary evidence that the inclusion of nutritional warnings in food ordering websites has the potential to discourage consumers from selecting dishes and beverages with excessive content of nutrients associated with NCD. Further research is needed to confirm and extend the findings of the present work to other food establishments selling unpackaged foods and support their implementation.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980021001026

References

Khandpur, N, Swinburn, B & Monteiro, CA (2018) Nutrient-based warning labels may help in the pursuit of healthy diets. Obesity 26, 16701671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pan American Health Organization (2020) Front-of-Package Labeling as a Policy Tool for the Prevention of Noncommunicable Diseases in the Americas. Washington DC: Pan American Health Organization.Google Scholar
Ares, G, Antúnez, L, Curutchet, MR et al. (2020) Sick, salient and full of salt, sugar and fat: understanding the impact of nutritional warnings on consumers’ associations through the salience bias. Food Qual Prefer 86, 103991.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ares, G, Antúnez, L, Curutchet, MR et al. (2020) Immediate effects of the implementation of nutritional warnings in Uruguay: awareness, self-reported use and increased understanding. Public Health Nutr 24, 364375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Machin, L, Curutchet, MR, Giménez, A et al. (2019) Do nutritional warnings do their work? Results from a choice experiment involving snack products. Food Qual Prefer 77, 159165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grummon, AH & Hall, MG (2020) Sugary drink warnings: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. PLoS Med 17, e1003120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taillie, LS, Reyes, M, Colchero, MA et al. (2020) An evaluation of Chile’s Law of Food Labeling and Advertising on sugar sweetened beverage purchases from 2015 to 2017: a before-and-after study. PLoS Med 17, e1003015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deliza, R, de Alcantara, M, Pereira, R et al. (2020) How do different warning signs compare with the guideline daily amount and traffic-light system?. Food Qual Prefer 80, 103821.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
EUFIC (2017) Global Update on Nutrition Labelling. The 2017 Edition. Brussels: EUFIC.Google Scholar
Farfán, G, Genoni, ME & Vakis, R (2017) You are what (and where) you eat: capturing food away from home in welfare measures. Food Policy 72, 146156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binkley, JK & Liu, Y (2019) Food at home and away from home: commodity composition, nutrition differences, and differences in consumers. Agric Res EconRev 48, 221252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eagle, D (2017) Reasons to get on board the Uber Eats or Deliveroo thing everyone’s doing. https://www.kounta.com/10-reasons-get-board-ubereats-deliveroo/ (accessed November 2020).Google Scholar
Yeo, VCS, Goh, SW & Rezaei, S (2017) Consumer experiences, attitude and behavioral intention toward online food delivery (OFD) services. J Retailing Consum Serv 35, 150162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheth, J (2020) Impact of Covid-19 on consumer behavior: will the old habits return or die? J Bus Res 117, 280283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandes, AC, Oliveira, RC, Proenca, RPC et al. (2016) Influence of menu labeling on food choices in real-life settings: a systematic review. Nutr Rev 74, 534548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bleich, SN, Economos, CD, Spiker, ML et al. (2017) A systematic review of calorie labeling and modified calorie labeling interventions: impact on consumer and restaurant behavior. Obesity 25, 20182044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulos, E & Leng, K (2010) Evaluation of a voluntary menu-labeling program in full-service restaurants. Am J Public Health 100, 10351039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, R (2018) Will posting nutritional information on menus prod diners to make healthier choices. J Am Med Assoc 319, 19691971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ministerio de Salud (2015) Consolidado de respuestas a observaciones recibidas durante consulta pública nacional e internacional sobre propuesta de modificación del Decreto Supremo No 977/96, Reglamento sanitario de los alimentos, del Ministerio de Salud de Chile, para la ejecución de la Ley No 20.606, sobre composición nutricional de los alimentos y su publicidad [Consolidated responses to the comments received during national and international public consultation on the proposal to amend Supreme Decree No. 977/96, Food Sanitary Regulations, of the Ministry of Health of Chile, for the implementation of Law No. 20.606, on the nutritional composition of food and its advertising]. Santiago: Ministerio de Salud.Google Scholar
Ares, G, Bove, I, Díaz, R et al. (2020) Argumentos de la industria alimentaria en contra del etiquetado frontal de advertencias nutricionales en Uruguay [Arguments of the food industry against the front-of-package nutritional warnings in Uruguay]. Rev Panam Salud Publica 44, e20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ministerio de Salud Pública (2018) Decreto 272/018 [Decree 272/018]. Montevideo: Ministerio de Salud Pública.Google Scholar
Anekwe, AV, Lent, M, Farley, SM et al. (2019) New York city’s sodium warning regulation: from conception to enforcement. Am J Public Health 109, 11911192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrd, K, Almanza, B, Ghiselli, RF et al. (2018) Adding sodium information to casual dining restaurant menus: beneficial or detrimental for consumers? Appetite 125, 474485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burton, S, Tangari, Ah, Howlett, E et al. (2014) How the perceived healthfulness of restaurant menu items influences sodium and calorie misperceptions: implications for nutrition disclosures in chain restaurants. J Cons Affairs 48, 6295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Musicus, AA, Moran, AJ, Lawman, HG et al. (2019) Online randomized controlled trials of restaurant sodium warning labels. Am J Prev Med 57, e181e193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ares, G, Aschemann-Witzel, J, Curutchet, MR et al. (2018) Nutritional warnings and product substitution or abandonment: policy implications derived from a repeated purchase simulation. Food Qual Prefer 65, 4048.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics of the participants (n 447)

Figure 1

Table 2 Percentage of participants who selected a dish or beverage with excessive content of nutrients for participants in the control group (without nutritional information) and the nutritional warnings group

Supplementary material: PDF

Gugliucci et al. supplementary material

Figures S1-S2

Download Gugliucci et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 897.3 KB