Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T18:38:55.388Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Racism in Singapore: A Review and Recommendations for Future Research

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 January 2018

Peter K.H. Chew*
Affiliation:
James Cook University, Singapore
*
Address for correspondence: Peter K.H. Chew, James Cook University, 149 Sims Drive, Singapore387380. Email: peter.chew@jcu.edu.au

Abstract

Racism refers to ‘the differential treatment enacted by an individual, group, or organization on individuals based on assumptions of a group's phenotypic, linguistic, or cultural differences’ (Gamst, Liang, & Der-Karabetian, 2011, p. 251). Currently, there is a lack of psychological research investigating racism in Singapore. The available research, narrative reviews, qualitative studies, and quantitative studies have yielded limited and inconclusive results. These researches are critically evaluated to provide four recommendations for a psychological research agenda: (a) develop a reliable and valid instrument to assess racism, (b) conduct experimental research to examine racism perpetuated by the majority or institutions, (c) examine the negative effects of racism, and (d) develop and evaluate interventions for racism.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2018

There is a lack of psychological research investigating racism in Singapore. This lack is intriguing, given psychology's long-standing interest in the topic since Gordon Allport's (Reference Allport1954) seminal book, The Nature of Prejudice. Instead, racism in Singapore has been examined by researchers from diverse fields such as history (e.g., Barr, Reference Barr2006), education (e.g., Khoo & Lim, Reference Khoo, Lim and Lai2004), sociology (e.g., Velayutham, Reference Velayutham2017), and even language and literature (e.g., Teo, Reference Teo2005). However, researchers in these fields tend to use narrative reviews and qualitative methods. An objective investigation of racism is challenging, if not impossible, without the use of psychometric instruments and experimental methods, tools that are often used in psychology. This article aims to (a) briefly review psychological research on racism, (b) consider problems associated with the reluctance of Singaporeans to discuss racial issues and their endorsement of meritocracy, (c) critically evaluate the limited and inconclusive research on racism in Singapore, and (d) provide recommendations for a psychological research agenda.

Psychological Research on Racism

Racism refers to ‘the differential treatment enacted by an individual, group, or organization on individuals based on assumptions of a group's phenotypic, linguistic, or cultural differences’ (Gamst et al., Reference Gamst, Liang and Der-Karabetian2011, p. 251). Due to the Civil Rights Movement in the 1960s, researchers argued that covert forms of racism have replaced overt racism in the United States. Covert forms of racism include ambivalent racism (Katz, Reference Katz1981), aversive racism (Gaertner & Dovidio, Reference Gaertner, Dovidio, Dovidio and Gaertner1986), modern racism (McConahay, Reference McConahay, Dovidio and Gaertner1986), and symbolic racism (McConahay & Hough, Reference McConahay and Hough1976). A similar phenomenon has been observed in other countries such as France, Germany, Britain, and the Netherlands (Pettigrew & Meertens, Reference Pettigrew and Meertens1995). Differences notwithstanding, these forms of racism share a common theme: individuals are more likely to identify themselves as non-racists while expressing prejudice or discriminating against others in subtle, socially justifiable ways. However, a literature review argued that overt and covert racism have always been expressed concurrently throughout history and that a distinction between the two is redundant (Leach, Reference Leach2005). Indeed, at least one study in Australia found that overt racism is still prevalent and should not be discounted (Mellor, Reference Mellor2003). Nonetheless, as this article will illustrate, a distinction is useful because it informs the development of instruments and targeted interventions.

The negative effects of racism have been well documented. For example, the relationships between self-reported perceived racism and a range of negative physical and mental health outcomes have been documented in several literature reviews (Brondolo, Gallo, & Myers, Reference Brondolo, Gallo and Myers2009; Paradies, Reference Paradies2006; Williams & Mohammed, Reference Williams and Mohammed2009) and at least two meta-analyses (Paradies et al., Reference Paradies, Ben, Denson, Elias, Priest, Pieterse and Gee2015; Pieterse, Todd, Neville, & Carter, Reference Pieterse, Todd, Neville and Carter2012). Racism has also been linked to reduced employment opportunities. Field experiments have been conducted where fictitious resumes, containing either a White name or a Black name, were sent in response to job advertisements (Bertrand & Mullainathan, Reference Bertrand and Mullainathan2004; Pager, Western, & Bonikowski, Reference Pager, Western and Bonikowski2009). The results indicated that resumes with a Black name are less likely to receive a call for an interview from employers. Overall, a review of field experiments concluded that a Black job seeker is ‘50 to 500 percent less likely to be considered by employers as an equally qualified White job applicant’ (Pager, Reference Pager2007, p. 114). Given these negative effects, interventions have been developed to reduce racism.

A large number of researches have been conducted on prejudice reduction (see Paluck & Green, Reference Paluck and Green2009, for a comprehensive review). Among these, two theories have received considerable empirical support. Contact theory states that prejudice will be reduced if intergroup contact takes place under four conditions: (a) equal status of groups, (b) common goals, (c) intergroup cooperation, and (d) support of authorities (Allport, Reference Allport1954). A meta-analysis of 515 studies found a negative relationship between intergroup contact and prejudice (mean r = −.22) and indicated that the four conditions are optimal, but not necessary, in the reduction of prejudice (Pettigrew & Tropp, Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006; Pettigrew, Tropp, Wagner, & Christ, Reference Pettigrew, Tropp, Wagner and Christ2011). Social identity theory states that we categorise people into groups, and this process results in the formation of in-groups (us) and out-groups (them) (Tajfel & Turner, Reference Tajfel, Turner, Austin and Austin1986). Because we derive our social identity and self-esteem via group membership, there is a tendency for in-group favouritism and outgroup derogation. Accordingly, an elimination or reorganisation of group boundaries should reduce prejudice (Bettencourt, Brewer, Croak, & Miller, Reference Bettencourt, Brewer, Croak and Miller1992; Crisp & Hewstone, Reference Crisp and Hewstone1999, Reference Crisp and Hewstone2007; Gaertner & Dovidio, Reference Gaertner and Dovidio2005). For example, the Common Ingroup Identity Model (CIIM) proposed that the adoption of a superordinate identity (e.g., university students), in addition to a pre-existing identity (e.g., Whites or Blacks), reduces prejudices by encouraging individuals to view themselves as a single group (Gaertner & Dovidio, Reference Gaertner and Dovidio2005). This model has been supported by a series of experimental studies (reviewed in Gaertner & Dovidio, Reference Gaertner and Dovidio2005).

Although a large number of studies have provided insights into the nature of, effects of, and interventions for racism, the majority of the research has been conducted among American samples in the United States. It is erroneous to assume that these researches are generalisable since replication attempts of psychological research across cultures have achieved only moderate success (Smith, Fischer, Vignoles, & Bond, Reference Smith, Fischer, Vignoles and Bond2013). For example, the concept of perfectionism (e.g., nature, consequences), developed using European American samples, has been shown to be different for a Taiwanese sample (Trotter, Reference Trotter2011), an African-American sample (Herman, Trotter, Reinke, & Ialongo, Reference Herman, Trotter, Reinke and Ialongo2011), and a Hong Kong sample (Suh, Yuen, Wang, Fu, & Trotter, Reference Suh, Yuen, Wang, Fu and Trotter2014). Accordingly, there is an urgent need to examine the generalisability of racism research to other cultures.

Racial Relations in Singapore

Singapore is a multiracial society consisting of four races: 74.1% of the population are Chinese, 13.4% Malays, 9.2% Indians, and 3.3% Others (Singapore Department of Statistics, 2010). Given the racial riots in the 1900s, the government implemented three initiatives to promote social integration: (a) public housing, (b) education, and (c) national service (Judd, Reference Judd, Pinxten and Preckler2005). Currently, over 80% of Singapore's population live in public housing (Housing & Development Board, 2015), and racial quotas are enforced to prevent the formation of racial enclaves (Sim, Yu, & Han, Reference Sim, Yu and Han2003). In 1997, the National Education program was launched in all public schools and universities to foster a common Singaporean identity and to educate students on Singapore's tumultuous past due to racial riots (Ministry of Education, Singapore, 1997). Lastly, under the National Service Act, all Singaporean males are conscripted for two years to serve in either the military (for the majority), police, or civil defence service, where they gain the opportunity to interact with males of all races (Judd, Reference Judd, Pinxten and Preckler2005).

Theoretically, the three government initiatives are consistent with intervention research on racism (Gaertner & Dovidio, Reference Gaertner and Dovidio2005; Pettigrew & Tropp, Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006; Pettigrew et al., Reference Pettigrew, Tropp, Wagner and Christ2011). For example, the racial quotas on public housing and the participation in national service promotes intergroup contact, facilitating the reduction of prejudice (Pettigrew & Tropp, Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006; Pettigrew et al., Reference Pettigrew, Tropp, Wagner and Christ2011). Also, the National Education program facilitates the adoption of a superordinate identity (i.e., Singaporeans) in addition to a pre-existing racial identity (e.g., Chinese or Malays). According to the CIIM, this identification with a single group should reduce prejudice (Gaertner & Dovidio, Reference Gaertner and Dovidio2005). Indeed, these initiatives appear to be successful; there have been no racial riots since the 1969 race riots of Singapore. However, it is unclear if the absence of racial riots is due to an effective reduction of prejudice or to the penalties imposed by the laws of Singapore.

Singapore has two laws that prohibit racism: The Sedition Act of Singapore (The Statutes of the Republic of Singapore, 1948) and Penal Code Chapter 224 (The Statutes of the Republic of Singapore, 1871). The Sedition Act makes it an offence for anyone to ‘to promote feelings of ill-will and hostility between different races or classes of the population of Singapore’ (The Statutes of the Republic of Singapore, 1948, p. 2). The Penal Code provides more details on the issue. Specifically, the code makes it an offence for anyone to intentionally wound ‘. . . the religious or racial feelings of any person, utters any word or makes any sound in the hearing of that person, or makes any gesture in the sight of that person, or places any object in the sight of that person, or causes any matter however represented to be seen or heard by that person’ (The Statutes of the Republic of Singapore, 1871, p. 135). Offenders will either be fined, imprisoned for up to three years, or both. These are not archaic laws that are no longer enforced; the Sedition Act has been used to arrest individuals for racist comments in recent years (e.g., AsiaOne News, 2012; The New Paper, 2008). However, while the government initiatives and these laws appear useful in reducing overt displays of racism, they are unable to address the protracted racial disparities in education and economic status.

Since the 1980s, Malays have been underperforming on educational achievement and economic indicators compared to Chinese and Indians (Mutalib, Reference Mutalib2011). These racial disparities have not been resolved. According to the latest Population Census, only 5.1% of Malays obtained a university qualification compared to 22.6% of Chinese and 35% of Indians (Singapore Department of Statistics, 2010). In addition, the median monthly household income for Malays was $3,844 compared to $5,100 for Chinese and $5,370 for Indians. Lastly, only 2.8% of Malays live in private housing (i.e., condominiums and landed properties) compared to 18% of Chinese and 16.3% of Indians. Unfortunately, due to the reluctance of Singaporeans to discuss racial issues, it is unlikely that these disparities will be resolved in the near future.

Reluctance to Discuss Racial Issues

In general, Singaporeans seem motivated to maintain the belief that Singapore is an egalitarian and a meritocratic society. A recent survey of 2,000 Singaporeans provided some evidence for this proposition (Mathews, Reference Mathews2016). For example, about half (53%) believe that racism is no longer an important problem currently. This finding could be interpreted as an indicator of covert racism (i.e., modern racism; McConahay, Hardee, & Batts, Reference McConahay, Hardee and Batts1981). However, this belief might be justified in the absence of experimental evidence of racism in Singapore. The survey also suggested that Singaporeans are reluctant to discuss racial issues. For example, about two thirds (64% to 66%) think that discussions about racial issues cause unnecessary tension. More important, about half to two thirds (46% to 70%) were not supportive of race-based information (about crime, educational performance, social problems, and so on) that could potentially reveal racial disparities.

The reluctance to discuss racial issues is problematic since the identification of racial disparities is the first step towards resolution. For example, the Council of Australian Governments identified disparities between non-Indigenous and Indigenous Australians and developed a comprehensive report to resolve those disparities (Council of Australian Governments, 2007). The report contains six specific measurable goals and a list of actions to be taken in order to achieve those goals. While there have been similar efforts in Singapore (e.g., tuition subsidies to close the income gap; Lian, Reference Lian2013), the effectiveness of those efforts are unclear. Also, in the absence of a comprehensive plan with specific goals, it seems likely that the racial disparities that have existed since the 1980s will continue, or even worsen, in the next Population Census in 2020 (Mutalib, Reference Mutalib2011). The situation is exacerbated by Singaporeans’ endorsement of meritocracy, providing them with a plausible, albeit flawed, explanation for racial disparities.

Endorsement of Meritocracy

In the same survey, a majority of Singaporeans endorsed meritocracy (Mathews, Reference Mathews2016). Specifically, 73% think that race is not an important determinant of success and 89% think that everyone, regardless of race, has an equal chance to be rich as long as they are hardworking. These findings are surprising given the problems associated with meritocracy (see Barr & Low, Reference Barr and Low2005; Koh, Reference Koh2014; Moore, Reference Moore2000; Tan, Reference Tan2008, for a comprehensive critique of meritocracy in Singapore). In particular, meritocracy assumes that everyone competes (or starts) with an equal background (e.g., regardless of race, socioeconomic status). Such an assumption ignores the potential benefits provided by race and wealth (Moore, Reference Moore2000). For example, trainee teachers in Singapore tended to ascribe positive stereotypes to Chinese students (e.g., industrious) and negative stereotypes to Malay students (e.g., lazy; Khoo & Lim, Reference Khoo, Lim and Lai2004). While the effect of such stereotypes on academic achievement in Singapore is currently unknown, four meta-analyses in the United States suggested that teachers’ expectations vary depending on the race of the student (Tenenbaum & Ruck, Reference Tenenbaum and Ruck2007) and that these expectations have a negative effect on racial minorities’ academic achievement (McKown & Weinstein, Reference McKown and Weinstein2008; Rubie-Davies, Hattie, & Hamilton, Reference Rubie-Davies, Hattie and Hamilton2006).

Further, Chinese households earned an average of $1,256 more per month than Malay households (Singapore Department of Statistics, 2010). The higher income allows Chinese to afford high-quality private tuition for their children and to send them to prestigious overseas universities (Moore, Reference Moore2000). In turn, such opportunities allow Chinese children to obtain higher grades in schools and degrees in high-paying fields (e.g., medicine, law). This argument is partially supported by research conducted in the United States. A meta-analysis involving 101,157 students from 6,871 schools found a moderate to strong positive relationship between socioeconomic status (SES) and academic achievement (Sirin, Reference Sirin2005). In other words, children from high SES families tend to have higher academic achievement. Taken together, it seems that Chinese have an unfair advantage over the other races due to their race and wealth.

More important, the endorsement of meritocracy encourages Singaporeans to explain racial disparities using racial stereotypes. As mentioned, a majority of Singaporeans endorsed meritocracy, and about half of them believed that racism is no longer an important problem (Mathews, Reference Mathews2016). The combination of these attitudes encourages Singaporeans to attribute the cause of any educational or income disparities to the characteristics of the racial group (e.g., Malays have lower household income because they are lazy), effectively reinforcing negative stereotypes of the group. Indeed, research has found positive relationships between beliefs in meritocracy and racism (Bobocel, Son Hing, Davey, Stanley, & Zanna, Reference Bobocel, Son Hing, Davey, Stanley and Zanna1998; Poteat & Spanierman, Reference Poteat and Spanierman2012). Unfortunately, the limited and inconclusive research on racism in Singapore precluded the consideration of racism as an alternative explanation for racial disparities.

Racism Research in Singapore

A comprehensive search was conducted on Google Scholar to identify published studies on racism in Singapore (up till March 2017). Key search terms included ‘prejudice’, ‘discrimination’, ‘racism’, ‘racial relations’, and ‘Singapore’. Subsequently, reference lists of relevant publications were scrutinised for other relevant studies that were not found in the database. A total of 13 studies were found and cited in this section. Among these studies, seven (53.85%) are narrative reviews, two (15.38%) are qualitative studies, and four (30.77%) are quantitative studies.

Narrative Reviews

The limited research on racism in Singapore has been dominated by narrative reviews (Barr, Reference Barr2006; Barr & Low, Reference Barr and Low2005; Chua, Reference Chua2003; Moore, Reference Moore2000; Mutalib, Reference Mutalib2011; Teo, Reference Teo2005; Walsh, Reference Walsh2007). These reviews tended to adopt a historical narrative, tracking policies from the 1960s to explain how Malays might be marginalised by the Singapore government (i.e., institutional racism). Some of these policies include the Speak Mandarin Campaign for the Chinese but there is an absence of similar campaigns for the respective languages of the other races (Teo, Reference Teo2005). Also, primary school English textbooks in the 1980s tended to depict Malays in menial jobs (e.g., a street-sweeper) but Chinese in skilled jobs (e.g., a doctor; Barr, Reference Barr2006). Furthermore, it is an open secret that Malays are excluded from serving in sensitive vocations in the military (Walsh, Reference Walsh2007). Lastly, the recurring pattern of Malays requesting equal treatment and the government's resistance to such requests has been documented (Mutalib, Reference Mutalib2011). These reviews provided a compelling account of institutional racism and a strong rationale to study racism in Singapore.

There are two limitations associated with those reviews. First, there is little empirical evidence for some of their assertions. For example, anecdotal evidence aside, there is no empirical evidence to support the assertion that Malays are discriminated against in the military. In fact, the defence minister recently declared that the military does not discriminate against any individuals (Chow, Reference Chow2015). Researchers need to show that the proportion of Malays in some military vocations are significantly lower than the expected proportion in the population in order to provide support for their assertion. Second, although this was not an aim of those reviews, it is difficult to demonstrate a causal link between racism in the past and racism in the present. For example, while the education system might have been racialised in the 1980s (Barr, Reference Barr2006), its effects on Singaporeans’ stereotypes of Malays in the present are unknown. The absence of a causal link enables Singaporeans to dismiss those findings as history: important but not necessarily relevant in today's society. Researchers need to provide current evidence of racism to persuade Singaporeans that racism is a contemporary issue.

Qualitative Studies

There are at least two qualitative studies that have examined perceived racism in everyday situations toward Indians (Velayutham, Reference Velayutham2006) and Malays (Velayutham, 2016). The methodology of the first study was not stated; it seems that semi-structured interviews were conducted with Indians in their early twenties to their thirties. Although Indians seem to be doing better than Chinese on educational achievement and economic indicators (Singapore Department of Statistics, 2010), the study reported that Indians experienced racism in the form of ‘name-calling’ associated with their body (e.g., smelly) and skin colour (e.g., dark) in various situations like schools, public transport, and swimming pools (Velayutham, Reference Velayutham2006). In the second study, semi-structured interviews were conducted with Malays about their working experience in Singapore. The study highlighted the privileges enjoyed by Chinese: they are more likely to obtain preferential treatment from their superiors (usually Chinese), salary increases, and promotion than the other races (Velayutham, Reference Velayutham2017). The results were supplemented by high-profile cases of racism in social media. These studies provided an in-depth understanding of minorities’ experience of racism in Singapore.

There are a few limitations associated with qualitative studies (Anderson, Reference Anderson2010). For example, findings from qualitative studies are vulnerable to researcher bias due to the nature of data collection (e.g., semi-structured interviews) and cannot be generalised to the population at large because of the small sample sizes. Also, the small sample sizes make it likely that the findings will be dismissed as an exception to the norm in egalitarian and meritocratic Singapore. Indeed, in one high-profile case of racism, the prime minister rebuked the offender but asserted that it was ‘an isolated case that does not reflect the strength of race relations in Singapore’ (Ho, Reference Ho2012, para. 6; Velayutham, Reference Velayutham2016). Despite the strengths associated with qualitative studies (Anderson, Reference Anderson2010), it appears that quantitative experimental research are needed to provide conclusive evidence of racism in Singapore. Unfortunately, most of the quantitative studies are descriptive in nature.

Quantitative Studies

Currently, only a few studies have examined racism quantitatively. Of these, one is a journal article (Ooi, Reference Ooi2005) and the rest are institutional reports (i.e., not likely to be peer-reviewed; Chin & Vasu, Reference Chin and Vasu2012; Mathews, Reference Mathews2013, Reference Mathews2016). In general, the conclusions from these studies are inconsistent with those from the narrative reviews and qualitative studies. For example, a descriptive survey found that racial relations in Singapore have improved from 1990 to 2001; there was an increase in the percentage of Chinese who reported inviting friends of other races to celebrate special occasions (Ooi, Reference Ooi2005). Also, across all races, 80% of the participants indicated support for having multiple races in Singapore. Another study conducted face-to-face interviews using a set of survey questions (e.g., ‘Would you mind if your next-door neighbour were a Malay?’) and found evidence for strong multiracial ties in Singapore (Chin & Vasu, Reference Chin and Vasu2012). The last two studies administered questionnaires using a set of survey questions (e.g., ‘People from all races should be treated equally’) in the homes of Singaporeans, to examine racial harmony (Mathews, Reference Mathews2013, Reference Mathews2016). Overall, although there is some evidence of racism, the results from both studies provided support for racial harmony in Singapore. Taken together, these studies suggested that racism might not be an issue in Singapore.

The predominantly descriptive nature of the studies is a limitation. For example, most of the questionnaires have not been subjected to psychometric investigations. Consequently, it is debatable whether those studies were assessing multicultural resilience (Chin & Vasu, Reference Chin and Vasu2012), racial harmony (Mathews, Reference Mathews2013), and race relations (Mathews, Reference Mathews2016). Also, asking participants to accurately report on their own attitudes and experiences is difficult for four reasons. First, participants might respond in a manner that presents a positive image of themselves (i.e., the social desirability bias; Paulhus, Reference Paulhus, Robinson, Shaver and Wrightsman1991). For example, 96% of the participants indicated agreement with the item ‘People from all races should be treated equally’ (Mathews, Reference Mathews2016). In this instance, they might have provided a socially desirable response in order to appear non-racist. Second, participants might not provide truthful responses due to the lack of anonymity, as their home address is identified (Chin & Vasu, Reference Chin and Vasu2012; Mathews, Reference Mathews2013, Reference Mathews2016). The fear of being arrested under the Sedition Act could have inhibited the expression of racism.

Third, consistent with how racism has evolved in other developed countries (e.g., Pettigrew & Meertens, Reference Pettigrew and Meertens1995), racism in contemporary Singapore is likely to be covert, instead of overt, in nature. For example, participants might identify as a non-racist but discriminate in subtle, socially justifiable manners (Dovidio & Gaertner, Reference Dovidio and Gaertner2000). However, most of the studies appeared to be assessing overt racism only (Chin & Vasu, Reference Chin and Vasu2012; Mathews, Reference Mathews2013, Reference Mathews2016). Lastly, variables that could not be controlled for in descriptive studies might have affected the accuracy of participants’ responses. For example, one study found that only 33% of Malays and 36% of Indians reported that they experienced racism (Mathews, Reference Mathews2016). Such a finding is probably an underestimation since minorities with meritocratic beliefs tended to under-report instances of racism (Major et al., Reference Major, Gramzow, McCoy, Levin, Schmader and Sidanius2002). Also, minorities might be unable to detect subtle covert racism, resulting in lower levels of perceived racism (Barreto & Ellemers, Reference Barreto and Ellemers2015). Taken together, it would be premature to conclude that racism might not be an issue in Singapore.

In summary, the limited research on racism has yielded inconsistent results. Also, the limitations associated with these studies precluded an accurate evaluation of racial relations in Singapore. In turn, the absence of such an evaluation prevented an identification of the actual causes of racial disparities. Given the importance of resolving racial disparities, this article makes four sequential recommendations for future research on racism in Singapore.

Recommendations for a Psychological Research Agenda

First, a reliable and valid instrument should be developed to assess both overt and covert racism in Singapore. Existing instruments are inappropriate because they were developed for minorities in other countries and might not be applicable to the Singaporean context. For example, the item ‘Generally speaking, I favour full racial integration’ on the Modern Racism Scale (McConahay et al., Reference McConahay, Hardee and Batts1981) is not applicable because there has never been racial segregation in Singapore. The instrument developed to assess racism in Singapore should be administered online with a measure of social desirability (Paulhus, Reference Paulhus, Robinson, Shaver and Wrightsman1991) to preserve anonymity and to control for the social desirability bias. This procedure allows researchers to examine the prevalence of racism in the population. More important, the availability of such an instrument enables researchers to evaluate the effectiveness of interventions designed to reduce racism.

Second, experimental research should be conducted to examine racism perpetuated by the majority or institutions. For example, field experiments could be conducted by sending resumes containing either a Chinese name or a Malay name in response to job advertisements in Singapore (Bertrand & Mullainathan, Reference Bertrand and Mullainathan2004; Pager et al., Reference Pager, Western and Bonikowski2009). Evidence of racism would be obtained if Malay candidates received significantly fewer calls for interviews than equally qualified Chinese candidates. The results would provide an alternative explanation for racial disparities. Specifically, the observed lower monthly household income for Malays (Singapore Department of Statistics, 2010) could be due to discrimination during the hiring process, resulting in reduced employment opportunities for them. In addition, experimental researches tend to have large sample sizes (cf. qualitative studies), preventing the findings from being dismissed as an exception to the norm.

Third, the negative effects of racism on important variables should be examined. For example, positive relationships have been found between perceived racism and a range of negative physical and mental health outcomes (Brondolo et al., Reference Brondolo, Gallo and Myers2009; Paradies, Reference Paradies2006; Paradies et al., Reference Paradies, Ben, Denson, Elias, Priest, Pieterse and Gee2015; Pieterse et al., Reference Pieterse, Todd, Neville and Carter2012; Williams & Mohammed, Reference Williams and Mohammed2009). Hence, researchers could examine whether there are racial disparities in health outcomes and determine the extent to which racism is a contributor to those disparities. Also, given that Singapore uses the education system as a meritocratic identification system for talents (Moore, Reference Moore2000), it is important that researchers examine the effects of racism on educational achievement of minorities. For example, since teachers hold negative stereotypes of Malay students (Khoo & Lim, Reference Khoo, Lim and Lai2004), researchers could examine its effects on the educational achievement of Malays. Evidence of a negative effect would cast doubts on the egalitarian nature of the education system. Given the positive relationships between beliefs in meritocracy and racism (Bobocel et al., Reference Bobocel, Son Hing, Davey, Stanley and Zanna1998; Poteat & Spanierman, Reference Poteat and Spanierman2012), an awareness of the problems associated with meritocracy could serve as a potential intervention for racism.

Fourth, interventions for racism should be developed and evaluated. Specifically, researchers could evaluate the generalisability of known interventions to the Singaporean context. For example, researchers could examine whether intergroup contact would lead to a reduction of prejudice (Pettigrew & Tropp, Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006; Pettigrew et al., Reference Pettigrew, Tropp, Wagner and Christ2011). Also, researchers could use the CIIM (Gaertner & Dovidio, Reference Gaertner and Dovidio2005) and examine whether the adoption of a superordinate identity (e.g., Singaporeans) would reduce prejudice. While laws like the Sedition Act (The Statutes of the Republic of Singapore, 1948) might be useful in discouraging overt racism, such interventions have the potential to reduce both overt and covert racism.

In conclusion, this article makes four recommendations for a psychological research agenda in Singapore. These recommendations should improve the quality and quantity of racism research. In particular, the use of experimental research provides strong evidence of racism whereas an examination of the negative effects of racism could highlight problems associated with meritocracy. In turn, the empirical evidence could address Singaporeans’ reluctance to discuss racial issues and their endorsement of meritocracy. With these barriers removed, the interventions for racism have the potential to finally resolve the protracted racial disparities between Malays and the other races in Singapore.

References

Allport, G.W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Anderson, C. (2010). Presenting and evaluating qualitative research. American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education, 74, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
AsiaOne News. (2012, July 1). 2 arrested for making racist remarks online. Retrieved from http://news.asiaone.com/News/AsiaOne+News/Crime/Story/A1Story20120701-356542.html Google Scholar
Barr, M.D. (2006). Racialised education in Singapore. Educational Research for Policy and Practice, 5, 1531.Google Scholar
Barr, M.D., & Low, J. (2005). Assimilation as multiracialism: The case of Singapore's Malays. Asian Ethnicity, 6, 161182. https://doi.org/10.1080/14631360500226606 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barreto, M., & Ellemers, N. (2015). Chapter three — Detecting and experiencing prejudice: New answers to old questions. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 139219. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2015.02.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertrand, M., & Mullainathan, S. (2004). Are Emily and Greg more employable than Lakisha and Jamal? A field experiment on labor market discrimination. American Economic Review, 94, 9911013. https://doi.org/10.1257/0002828042002561 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bettencourt, B.A., Brewer, M.B., Croak, M.R., & Miller, N. (1992). Cooperation and the reduction of intergroup bias: The role of reward structure and social orientation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 301319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bobocel, D.R., Son Hing, L.S., Davey, L.M., Stanley, D.J., & Zanna, M.P. (1998). Justice-based opposition to social policies: Is it genuine? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 653669. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.653 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brondolo, E., Gallo, L.C., & Myers, H.F. (2009). Race, racism and health: Disparities, mechanisms, and interventions. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 32, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chin, Y., & Vasu, N. (2012). The ties that bind & blind: A report on inter-racial and inter-religious relations in Singapore. Singapore: Centre of Excellence for National Security, S. Rajaratnam School of International Studies, Nanyang Technological University. Retrieved from https://www.rsis.edu.sg/wp-content/uploads/2014/10/PB121231_The_Ties_that_Bind__Blind.pdf Google Scholar
Chow, J. (2015, February 16). Malays deployed in the SAF as sailors: Ng Eng Hen. The Straits Times. Retrieved from http://www.straitstimes.com/singapore/malays-deployed-in-the-saf-as-sailors-ng-eng-hen Google Scholar
Chua, B.H. (2003). Multiculturalism in Singapore: An instrument of social control. Race & Class, 44, 5877. https://doi.org/10.1177/0306396803044003025 Google Scholar
Council of Australian Governments. (2007). National Indigenous Reform Agreement (Closing the Gap). Retrieved from http://www.federalfinancialrelations.gov.au/content/npa/health/_archive/indigenous-reform/national-agreement_sept_12.pdf Google Scholar
Crisp, R.J., & Hewstone, M. (1999). Differential evaluation of crossed category groups: Patterns, processes, and reducing intergroup bias. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 2, 307333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crisp, R.J., & Hewstone, M. (2007). Multiple social categorization. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 163254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, J.F., & Gaertner, S.L. (2000). Aversive racism and selection decisions: 1989 and 1999. Psychological Science, 11, 315319.Google ScholarPubMed
Gaertner, S.L., & Dovidio, J.F. (1986). The aversive form of racism. In Dovidio, J.F. & Gaertner, S.L. (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 6189). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gaertner, S.L., & Dovidio, J.F. (2005). Understanding and addressing contemporary racism: From aversive racism to the common ingroup identity model. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 615639. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00424.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gamst, G.C., Liang, C.T.H., & Der-Karabetian, A. (2011). Handbook of multicultural measures. Los Angeles, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herman, K.C., Trotter, R., Reinke, W.M., & Ialongo, N. (2011). Developmental origins of perfectionism among African American youth. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 58, 321334. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023108 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, D. (2012, October 8). PM Lee, ministers condemn racist remarks on Facebook. The Straits Times. Retrieved from http://www.straitstimes.com/singapore/pm-lee-ministers-condemn-racist-remarks-on-facebook Google Scholar
Housing & Development Board. (2015). Public housing – A Singapore icon. Retrieved December 21, 2016, from http://www.hdb.gov.sg/cs/infoweb/about-us/our-role/public-housing-a-singapore-icon Google Scholar
Judd, M. (2005). Is harmony possible in a multiracial society? The case of Singapore. In Pinxten, R. & Preckler, E. (Eds.), Racism in metropolitan areas (pp. 107111). New York, NY: Berghahn Books.Google Scholar
Katz, I. (1981). Stigma: A social psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Khoo, A., & Lim, K.M. (2004). Trainee teachers’ stereotypes of ethnic groups in Singapore. In Lai, A.E. (Ed.), Beyond rituals and riots: Ethnic pluralism and social cohesion in Singapore (pp. 197227). Singapore: Eastern Universities Press.Google Scholar
Koh, A. (2014). Doing class analysis in Singapore's elite education: Unravelling the smokescreen of ‘meritocratic talk.’ Globalisation, Societies and Education, 12, 196210. https://doi.org/10.1080/14767724.2014.888308 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leach, C.W. (2005). Against the notion of a ‘new racism’. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 15, 432445. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.841 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lian, G.C. (2013, August 24). Two new steps to help Malay-Muslim community shrink income gap, increase social mobility. The Straits Times. Retrieved from http://www.straitstimes.com/singapore/two-new-steps-to-help-malay-muslim-community-shrink-income-gap-increase-social-mobility Google Scholar
Major, B., Gramzow, R.H., McCoy, S.K., Levin, S., Schmader, T., & Sidanius, J. (2002). Perceiving personal discrimination: The role of group status and legitimizing ideology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 269282. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.3.269 Google ScholarPubMed
Mathews, M. (2013). Indicators of racial and religious harmony. Singapore: Institute of Policy Studies, National University of Singapore.Google Scholar
Mathews, M. (2016). Channel NewsAsia-Institute of Policy Studies (CNA-IPS) survey on race relations. Singapore: Institute of Policy Studies, National University of Singapore.Google Scholar
McConahay, J.B. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the Modern Racism Scale. In Dovidio, J.F. & Gaertner, S.L. (Eds.), Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism (pp. 91125). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
McConahay, J.B., Hardee, B.B., & Batts, V. (1981). Has racism declined in America? It depends on who is asking and what is asked. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 25, 563579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McConahay, J.B., & Hough, J.C. (1976). Symbolic racism. Journal of Social Issues, 32, 2345. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1976.tb02493.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKown, C., & Weinstein, R.S. (2008). Teacher expectations, classroom context, and the achievement gap. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 235261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2007.05.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellor, D. (2003). Contemporary racism in Australia: The experiences of Aborigines. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 474486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ministry of Education, Singapore. (1997). National Education. Retrieved December 15, 2016, from https://www.moe.gov.sg/media/press/1997/pr01797.htm Google Scholar
Moore, R.Q. (2000). Multiracialism and meritocracy: Singapore's approach to race and inequality. Review of Social Economy, 58, 339360. https://doi.org/10.1080/00346760050132364 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mutalib, H. (2011). The Singapore minority dilemma. Asian Survey, 51, 11561171. https://doi.org/10.1525/as.2011.51.6.1156 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ooi, G.L. (2005). The role of the developmental state and interethnic relations in Singapore. Asian Ethnicity, 6, 109120. https://doi.org/10.1080/14631360500135336 Google Scholar
Pager, D. (2007). The use of field experiments for studies of employment discrimination: Contributions, critiques, and directions for the future. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 609, 104133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pager, D., Western, B., & Bonikowski, B. (2009). Discrimination in a low-wage labor market a field experiment. American Sociological Review, 74, 777799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paluck, E.L., & Green, D.P. (2009). Prejudice reduction: What works? A review and assessment of research and practice. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 339367. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163607 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradies, Y. (2006). A systematic review of empirical research on self-reported racism and health. International Journal of Epidemiology, 35, 888901. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyl056 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradies, Y., Ben, J., Denson, N., Elias, A., Priest, N., Pieterse, A., . . . Gee, G. (2015). Racism as a determinant of health: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One, 10, e0138511. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138511 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paulhus, D.L. (1991). Measurement and control of response bias. In Robinson, J.P., Shaver, P.R., & Wrightsman, L.S. (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 1759). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, T.F., & Meertens, R.W. (1995). Subtle and blatant prejudice in Western Europe. European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 5775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, T.F., & Tropp, L.R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 751783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettigrew, T.F., Tropp, L.R., Wagner, U., & Christ, O. (2011). Recent advances in intergroup contact theory. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 35 (3), 271280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.03.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pieterse, A.L., Todd, N.R., Neville, H.A., & Carter, R.T. (2012). Perceived racism and mental health among Black American adults: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59, 19. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026208 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poteat, V.P., & Spanierman, L.B. (2012). Modern racism attitudes among white students: The role of dominance and authoritarianism and the mediating effects of racial color-blindness. The Journal of Social Psychology, 152 (6), 758774. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2012.700966 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubie-Davies, C., Hattie, J., & Hamilton, R. (2006). Expecting the best for students: Teacher expectations and academic outcomes. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 429444. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709905X53589 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sim, L.L., Yu, S.M., & Han, S.S. (2003). Public housing and ethnic integration in Singapore. Habitat International, 27, 293307. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0197-3975(02)00050-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singapore Department of Statistics. (2010). Census of population 2010 Statistical release 2: Households and housing. Singapore: Author.Google Scholar
Sirin, S.R. (2005). Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Review of Educational Research, 75, 417453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P.B., Fischer, R., Vignoles, V.L., & Bond, M.H. (2013). Understanding social psychology across cultures: Engaging with others in a changing world. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Suh, H.N., Yuen, M., Wang, K.T., Fu, C.-C., & Trotter, R.H. (2014). Comparing perfectionist types on family environment and well-being among Hong Kong adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 111116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.023 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J.C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In Austin, S. & Austin, W.G. (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 724). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.Google Scholar
Tan, K.P. (2008). Meritocracy and elitism in a global city: Ideological shifts in Singapore. International Political Science Review / Revue Internationale de Science Politique, 29, 727.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tenenbaum, H.R., & Ruck, M.D. (2007). Are teachers’ expectations different for racial minority than for European American students? A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 253273. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.2.253 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teo, P. (2005). Mandarinising Singapore: A critical analysis of slogans in Singapore's ‘Speak Mandarin’ campaign. Critical Discourse Studies, 2 (2), 121142. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405900500283565 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The New Paper. (2008, May 21). Blogger arrested for posting racist online content. Retrieved from http://news.asiaone.com/News/AsiaOne+News/Singapore/Story/A1Story20080521-66273.html Google Scholar
The Statutes of the Republic of Singapore. Penal Code, 298 Chapter 224 § (1871).Google Scholar
The Statutes of the Republic of Singapore. Sedition Act, Chapter 290 § (1948).Google Scholar
Trotter, R. (2011). Perfectionism, coping, adjustment and achievement in Taiwanese culture (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Missouri-Columbia, United States.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velayutham, S. (2006, September). Everyday racism in Singapore. Paper presented at the Everyday Multiculturalism Conference Proceedings, Sydney, Australia.Google Scholar
Velayutham, S. (2016). Races without racism?: Everyday race relations in Singapore. Identities, 24, 455473. https://doi.org/10.1080/1070289X.2016.1200050 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walsh, S.P. (2007). The roar of the lion city ethnicity, gender, and culture in the Singapore armed forces. Armed Forces & Society, 33, 265285. https://doi.org/10.1177/0095327X06291854 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, D.R., & Mohammed, S.A. (2009). Discrimination and racial disparities in health: Evidence and needed research. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 32, 2047. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-008-9185-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed