Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T04:27:31.901Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Clinical significance of psychotic-like experiences across U.S. ethnoracial groups

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2023

Roberto Lewis-Fernández*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University and New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA
Chih-nan Chen
Affiliation:
Department of Economics, National Taipei University, Taipei, Taiwan, Republic of China
Mark Olfson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University and New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA
Alejandro Interian
Affiliation:
Mental Health and Behavioral Sciences, VA New Jersey Healthcare System, Lyons, NJ, USA
Margarita Alegría
Affiliation:
Disparities Research Unit, Department of Medicine, Massachusetts General Hospital/Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Roberto Lewis-Fernández; Email: Roberto.lewis@nyspi.columbia.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Prevalence of psychotic-like experiences (PLEs) – reports of hallucinations and delusional thinking not meeting criteria for psychotic disorder – varies substantially across ethnoracial groups. What explains this range of PLE prevalence? Despite extensive research, the clinical significance of PLEs remains unclear. Are PLE prevalence and clinical severity differentially associated across ethnoracial groups?

Methods

We examined the lifetime prevalence and clinical significance of PLEs across ethnoracial groups in the Collaborative Psychiatric Epidemiology Surveys (N = 11 139) using the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) psychosis symptom screener. Outcomes included mental healthcare use (inpatient, outpatient), mental health morbidity (self-perceived poor/fair mental health, suicidal ideation or attempts), and impairment (role interference). Individuals with outcome onsets prior to PLE onset were excluded. We also examined associations of PLEs with CIDI diagnoses. Cox proportional-hazards regression and logistic regression modeling identified associations of interest.

Results

Contrary to previous reports, only Asian Americans differed significantly from other U.S. ethnoracial groups, reporting lower lifetime prevalence (6.7% v. 8.0–11.9%) and mean number (0.09 v. 0.11–0.18) of PLEs. In multivariate analyses, PLE clinical significance showed limited ethnoracial variation among Asian Americans, non-Caribbean Latinos, and Afro-Caribbeans. In other groups, mental health outcomes showed significant ethnoracial clustering by outcome (e.g. hospitalization and role interference with Caribbean-Latino origin), possibly due to underlying differences in psychiatric disorder chronicity or treatment barriers.

Conclusions

While there is limited ethnoracial variation in U.S. PLE prevalence, PLE clinical significance varies across U.S. ethnoracial groups. Clinicians should consider this variation when assessing PLEs to avoid exaggerating their clinical significance, contributing to mental healthcare disparities.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

The prevalence of psychotic-like experiences (PLEs) – reports of hallucinations and delusional thinking not meeting criteria for psychotic disorder (Linscott & van Os, Reference Linscott and van Os2013) – varies by ethnoracial background and country of origin (Leaune et al., Reference Leaune, Dealberto, Luck, Grot, Zeroug-Vial, Poulet and Brunelin2018; Nuevo et al., Reference Nuevo, Chatterji, Verdes, Naidoo, Arango and Ayuso-Mateos2012; Oh, Yang, Anglin, & DeVylder, Reference Oh, Yang, Anglin and DeVylder2014). ‘Ethnoracial background’ denotes a person's identification as the intersection of two U.S. Census constructs – ‘race’ (e.g. Black, White) and ‘ethnicity’ (Latino, non-Latino). In nationally representative samples from 18 countries, lifetime prevalence of ≥1 PLEs was 1.2–14.9% and significantly higher in middle- and high-income countries than low-income ones (McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Saha, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Bromet, Bruffaerts and Kessler2015). In high-income countries, PLE elevations are most consistent among Black and/or Caribbean populations (Anglin et al., Reference Anglin, Ereshefsky, Klaunig, Bridgwater, Niendam, Ellman and van der Ven2021; Johns, Nazroo, Bebbington, & Kuijpers, Reference Johns, Nazroo, Bebbington and Kuijpers2002; Leaune et al., Reference Leaune, Dealberto, Luck, Grot, Zeroug-Vial, Poulet and Brunelin2018). In the USA, PLE prevalence is reported as significantly higher among Latinos and Blacks, and lower among Asian Americans, compared to non-Latino Whites (Cohen & Marino, Reference Cohen and Marino2013; DeVylder, Oh, Corcoran, & Lukens, Reference DeVylder, Oh, Corcoran and Lukens2014a).

What explains this broad range of PLE prevalence? Despite extensive research, the clinical significance of PLEs remains unclear (Powers, Reference Powers2019). PLEs are considerably more common than psychotic disorders, with a median prevalence of 5–9% among adults and youth (Kelleher et al., Reference Kelleher, Keeley, Corcoran, Lynch, Fitzpatrick, Devlin and Cannon2012; Linscott & van Os, Reference Linscott and van Os2013; McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Saha, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Bromet, Bruffaerts and Kessler2015), and are often transient and non-distressing (Hanssen, Bak, Bijl, Vollebergh, & van Os, Reference Hanssen, Bak, Bijl, Vollebergh and van Os2005; McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Saha, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Bromet, Bruffaerts and Kessler2015). Only 7% of individuals with PLEs develop a psychotic disorder (Linscott & van Os, Reference Linscott and van Os2013). Still, the relative risk of conversion rises in a dose–response relationship with PLE severity (Kaymaz et al., Reference Kaymaz, Drukker, Lieb, Wittchen, Werbeloff, Weiser and van Os2012) and PLEs share many risk factors with schizophrenia (Kelleher & Cannon, Reference Kelleher and Cannon2011). However, PLEs also predict non-psychotic common mental disorders (CMDs) (Kaymaz et al., Reference Kaymaz, Drukker, Lieb, Wittchen, Werbeloff, Weiser and van Os2012) and people with PLEs share genetic liability with a broad range of mental disorders (Legge et al., Reference Legge, Jones, Kendall, Pardiñas, Menzies, Bracher-Smith and Walters2019).

At the population level, does the clinical significance of PLEs vary across ethnoracial groups, complicating their etiological and predictive meaning? For example, are PLEs more prevalent in ethnoracial groups that consider them minimally impairing, familiar cultural idioms of distress (Johns et al., Reference Johns, Nazroo, Bebbington and Kuijpers2002; Lewis-Fernández et al., Reference Lewis-Fernández, Horvitz-Lennon, Blanco, Guarnaccia, Cao and Alegría2009)? Or is the clinical significance of PLEs equivalent across ethnoracial groups, making PLE prevalence variation a reliable indicator of cross-ethnoracial/cultural variation in psychopathology?

PLEs are associated with mental health morbidity, impairment, treatment-seeking, and mental disorder severity and comorbidity (DeVylder, Burnette, & Yang, Reference DeVylder, Burnette and Yang2014b), suicidal ideation and behaviors (Honings, Drukker, Groen, & van Os, Reference Honings, Drukker, Groen and van Os2016), and impaired social functioning (Oh, Koyanagi, Kelleher, & DeVylder, Reference Oh, Koyanagi, Kelleher and DeVylder2018).

When examined by ethnoracial background, the clinical significance of PLEs displays a complex pattern. Partly, this is because ethnoracial groupings are social proxies for structural conditions, cultural norms, and contextual factors that may contribute specific risks for PLEs. Although the few relevant studies emphasize ethnoracial similarities in clinical significance (e.g. DeVylder et al., Reference DeVylder, Burnette and Yang2014b; King et al., Reference King, Nazroo, Weich, McKenzie, Bhui, Karlson and Erens2005), unexplained differences often remain across ethnoracial groups in the number and strength of clinical associations (DeVylder et al., Reference DeVylder, Burnette and Yang2014b; Oh et al., Reference Oh, Koyanagi, Kelleher and DeVylder2018; Vanheusden et al., Reference Vanheusden, Mulder, van der Ende, Selten, van Lenthe, Verhuist and Mackenbach2008). Several limitations have hindered ethnoracial comparisons in the strength of association between PLEs and adverse outcomes: ignoring the timing of PLE and morbidity outcome onsets, confounding their temporal association (e.g. DeVylder et al., Reference DeVylder, Burnette and Yang2014b); examining few adverse outcomes (e.g. King et al., Reference King, Nazroo, Weich, McKenzie, Bhui, Karlson and Erens2005); analyzing small samples (e.g. Vanheusden et al., Reference Vanheusden, Mulder, van der Ende, Selten, van Lenthe, Verhuist and Mackenbach2008); and using country, not ethnoracial background, as unit of analysis (e.g. Nuevo et al., Reference Nuevo, Chatterji, Verdes, Naidoo, Arango and Ayuso-Mateos2012). The range of inter-ethnoracial differences is often unexamined, usually because studies collapsed ethnoracial groups – e.g. Caribbean and non-Caribbean-origin populations (e.g. DeVylder et al., Reference DeVylder, Burnette and Yang2014b) or Asian Americans and Latinos (e.g. Oh et al., Reference Oh, Koyanagi, Kelleher and DeVylder2018) – or only compared to non-Latino Whites (e.g. Vanheusden et al., Reference Vanheusden, Mulder, van der Ende, Selten, van Lenthe, Verhuist and Mackenbach2008).

This study examines whether the social-identity construct of ethnoracial background is associated with differences in prevalence and clinical significance of PLEs. Clinical significance was assessed as mental healthcare use (inpatient, outpatient), morbidity (self-perceived poor/fair mental health, suicidal ideation or attempts), and impairment (role interference). We also assessed whether respondents with lifetime PLEs had higher lifetime prevalence of CMDs. A priori, we hypothesized that ethnoracial groups with lower PLE prevalence would report stronger correlations of PLEs with mental health morbidity and impairment. Such an inverse correlation would suggest that ethnoracial groups with stronger associations of PLEs with psychopathology may be less likely to report PLEs without substantial impairment.

In contrast, groups that express PLEs within a broader range of situations, including subclinical distress and spiritual experiences, may more readily report them. Estimating ethnoracial variation in PLE clinical significance may clarify their wide prevalence range and suggest structural-contextual factors underlying variation by ethnoracial background. It may also enhance the cross-ethnoracial reliability of clinicians' use of PLEs as severity markers of psychopathology (Kelleher & Cannon, Reference Kelleher and Cannon2011).

Methods

Collaborative psychiatric epidemiological surveys (CPES)

Three national surveys of the non-institutionalized adult U.S. population comprise the CPES: the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R; Kessler & Merikangas, Reference Kessler and Merikangas2004), the National Survey of American Life (NSAL; Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Torres, Caldwell, Neighbors, Nesse, Taylor and Williams2004), and the National Latino and Asian American Study (NLAAS; Alegría et al., Reference Alegría, Takeuchi, Canino, Duan, Shrout, Meng and Gong2004); similar methods allow for their combination into a single, nationally representative dataset (Heeringa et al., Reference Heeringa, Wagner, Torres, Duan, Adams and Berglund2004). Only English-speaking respondents participated in the NCS-R and NSAL; the NLAAS included participants who spoke English, Spanish, or one of four Asian languages (Alegría et al., Reference Alegría, Takeuchi, Canino, Duan, Shrout, Meng and Gong2004). Interviewers were matched to participants by racialized (NSAL) or linguistic/cultural (NLAAS) background.

Our sample includes CPES respondents who completed the World Mental Health-Composite International Diagnostic Interview (WMH-CIDI) psychosis-symptom screener: NSAL African Americans (AfrAms; N = 3414) and Black Americans of Afro-Caribbean origin (AfrCaribs; N = 1400); NLAAS Asian Americans (AsAms; N = 2092), Caribbean Latinos (CaribLats; N = 1152), and non-Caribbean Latinos (NCLats; n = 1399; 68.7% of Mexican origin); and a random subsample of the NCS-R non-Latino Whites (NLWs; N = 1705) who completed the WMH-CIDI ‘long-form’. The Internal Review Boards of the principal investigators' institutions approved study methods and protocols; all participants provided informed consent.

Measures

Endorsement and age of onset of self-reported PLEs were assessed using the WMH-CIDI psychosis-symptom screener (Kessler & Üstün, Reference Kessler and Üstün2004), which assesses visual hallucinations, auditory hallucinations, thought insertion/withdrawal, delusions of control, delusions of reference, and persecutory delusions. Only experiences that occurred when not ‘dreaming or half-asleep or under the influence of alcohol or drugs’ were included. Respondents were classified as having PLEs if they endorsed ≥1 PLEs over their lifetime. Because of ethnoracial variation in access to clinical services and in misdiagnosis of mood conditions as psychotic disorders (Gara et al., Reference Gara, Vega, Arndt, Escamilla, Fleck, Lawson and Strakowski2012), we did not exclude individuals with a clinical diagnosis of psychotic disorder. Excluding such individuals would potentially exert a larger confounding effect across ethnoracial groups than including them.

Six measures obtained in all CPES samples comprised the mental health outcomes: ever having an overnight hospital admission for problems with emotions, nerves, mental health, or use of alcohol or drugs (psychiatric hospitalization); ever having an outpatient contact with specialty-mental-health or general-health professionals for mental health problems (outpatient mental healthcare); current self-perceived mental health (fair/poor v. excellent/very good/good); ever having self-reported suicidal ideation or attempts (yes/no for each, asked separately); and role interference, assessed as inability to fulfill social roles due to mental health problems on any day over the last month (v. no days) (Rehm et al., Reference Rehm, Ustun, Saxena, Nelson, Chatterji, Ivis and Adlaf1999).

Socio-demographic correlates included age at interview (18–34, 35–49, ≥50 years), gender (male/female), education (0–11, 12, 13–15, ≥16 years), annual household income (<$15 000, $15 000–34 999, $35 000–74 999, ≥$75 000), and marital status (married/cohabiting v. never married/separated/widowed/divorced). WMH-CIDI lifetime DSM-IV psychiatric diagnoses were grouped as any depressive disorder (major depressive episode, dysthymia), any anxiety or trauma-related disorder (agoraphobia without panic disorder, panic disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, social phobia, posttraumatic stress disorder), any substance use disorder (alcohol abuse/dependence, drug abuse/dependence), and any mental disorder (any of the 11 diagnoses). The sum of individual disorders yielded the number of lifetime disorders per respondent.

Statistical analysis

Categorical variables were compared with the Rao–Scott statistic (Rao & Scott, Reference Rao and Scott1984) and continuous variables using adjusted Wald tests. Tests for outliers and multicollinearity were negative. Respondents with missing data on PLE items were excluded from the analytic sample. All analyses were conducted applying sampling weights (Heeringa et al., Reference Heeringa, Wagner, Torres, Duan, Adams and Berglund2004) to generalize results to the U.S. population. For NLW respondents, we utilized the weights for the NCS-R sample administered additional measures (‘long-form’), including a random subsample completing the psychosis symptom screener; the ‘long-form’ survey oversampled NCS-R respondents with psychopathology plus a probability subsample of others (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005). Cross-tabulations display age-and-gender-adjusted ethnoracial differences in lifetime endorsement of PLEs for the full sample (N = 11 139). Significant omnibus tests (p < 0.05) were followed by post-hoc pairwise comparisons across individual ethnoracial groups, applying Benjamini–Hochberg correction for multiple comparisons (Thissen, Steinberg, & Kuang, Reference Thissen, Steinberg and Kuang2002). Among respondents with lifetime PLEs (n = 1138), the distribution of lifetime CMDs and mental health outcomes after PLE onset were compared across ethnoracial groups. Supplementary analyses examined ethnoracial variation of specific PLEs, other PLE-related characteristics, and socio-demographic correlates (online Supplementary Tables S1 and S2).

In the sample with lifetime PLEs, NLWs were compared to other ethnoracial groups using Cox proportional-hazards regression models to examine the strength of association between PLEs and four lifetime mental health outcomes after PLE onset (psychiatric hospitalization, outpatient mental healthcare, suicidal ideation, suicide attempts). Survival models, which include time as a covariate, tend to outperform logistic models that ignore time variation (Ngwa et al., Reference Ngwa, Cabral, Chen, Pencina, Gagnon, LaValley and Cupples2016). Post-hoc pairwise comparisons were limited to those in which the hazards estimates from one group's full model fell outside the comparator group's confidence interval (CI) range. We excluded participants who first experienced an outcome before PLE onset by comparing the date of first onset of each outcome to the date of first PLE onset. Possible explanatory variables included age at PLE onset, gender, education, household income, marital status, and number of lifetime CMDs. In sensitivity analyses, we removed the number of CMDs in backward regressions to test its role in multivariate models. Given the marked variation of nativity status across groups, we included it as a potential explanatory variable in separate supplementary analyses of the PLE-positive sample, combining AfrAms and AfrCaribs into one group and CaribLats and NCLats into another group due to sample size limitations (online Supplementary Table S3).

In the full sample (N = 11 139), we also examined associations between lifetime PLEs (n = 1138) and two outcomes current at time of interview – current self-perceived mental health status and role interference in the last month – using separate logistic regressions for each ethnoracial group, adjusting for PLE duration and other explanatory variables. The interaction between ethnoracial background and PLE status on each outcome was assessed separately, with NLWs as the reference group. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons of other ethnoracial groups and nativity analyses were conducted as above.

STATA 15.1 Survey Analysis procedures accounted for the complex sample design (Stata Corp, 2017).

Results

Lifetime PLE prevalence

Overall lifetime PLE endorsement was 9.1%, with significant variation by ethnoracial background (omnibus p < 0.0001) driven mainly by lower PLE prevalence among AsAm respondents (Table 1). NLWs (8.0%), CaribLats (10.8%), and AfrAms (11.9%) were significantly more likely than AsAms (6.7%) to report lifetime PLEs, after adjusting for multiple comparisons. The mean (s.d.) number of lifetime PLEs showed the same endorsement pattern (Table 1). The prevalence of visual hallucinations ranged from 4.6% (AsAms) to 9.5% (AfrCaribs) (omnibus p < 0.0001) and was significantly higher in AfrAms than NLWs and AsAms. Auditory hallucinations ranged from 2.5% (AfrCaribs) to 6.2% (CaribLats) (omnibus p < 0.0001); they were significantly more frequent among AfrAms and CaribLats than NLWs, AsAms, or AfrCaribs and among NCLats than AsAms. After adjustment for multiple comparisons, delusional thinking did not show significant ethnoracial variation.

Table 1. Lifetime PLEs among CPES respondents, adjusted by age and gender (N = 11 139)

s.e., standard error.

aOnly the NCS-R random subsample that completed the psychosis symptom screener was included.

bRespondents with missing PLE data were dropped from the sample.

cCaribbean Latinos include respondents who identify as Puerto Rican, Cuban, or Dominican.

dNon-Caribbean Latinos include all other respondents who identify as Latinos (68.7% of Mexican origin).

eAfro-Caribbean respondents identified as Black and reported West Indian or Caribbean descent in their own, their parents', or previous generations.

Note: All analyses were conducted applying sampling weights to generalize results to the U.S. population.

In the PLE-positive group, AfrCaribs were significantly younger at PLE onset than NLWs, CaribLats, or AfrAms (online Supplementary Table S1). PLE frequency varied significantly by ethnoracial background in omnibus tests, but not after Benjamini–Hochberg correction (online Supplementary Table S1). Significant socio-demographic variation among respondents with PLEs followed population-level ethnoracial differences in the CPES sample (online Supplementary Table S2; Alegria, Woo, Takeuchi, & Jackson, Reference Alegria, Woo, Takeuchi, Jackson, Ruiz and Primm2009), suggesting the PLE-group variation resulted from overall population differences.

Bivariate mental health outcomes by ethnoracial background

Lifetime prevalence of CMDs

In analyses of the age-and-gender-adjusted sample with PLEs (n = 1138), the prevalence of most lifetime CMDs varied significantly by ethnoracial background (omnibus p's < 0.02–0.001) (Table 2). NLWs had the highest lifetime prevalence of any CMD, including separately for depression and anxiety, and the largest proportion of respondents with multiple mental disorders. By contrast, AsAms had the lowest lifetime prevalence and proportion of respondents with multiple disorders.

Table 2. Lifetime psychiatric disorders and mental health outcomes of CPES respondents with lifetime PLEs, adjusted by age and gender (N = 1138)

PLE, psychotic-like experience.

aOnly the NCS-R random subsample that completed the psychosis symptom screener was included.

bRespondents with missing PLE data were dropped from the sample.

cCaribbean Latinos include respondents who identify as Puerto Rican, Cuban, or Dominican.

dNon-Caribbean Latinos include all other respondents who identify as Latinos (68.7% of Mexican origin).

eAfro-Caribbean respondents identified as Black and reported West Indian or Caribbean descent in their own, their parents', or previous generations.

Lifetime prevalence of mental health outcomes after PLE onset

Several lifetime mental health outcomes also varied significantly across ethnoracial groups (omnibus p's < 0.015–0.0001) (Table 2). After Benjamini–Hochberg adjustment, psychiatric hospitalization after PLE onset was significantly higher among CaribLats than NCLats or AsAms. Outpatient mental healthcare after PLE onset was significantly more common among NLWs and AfrAms than NCLats. NLWs were significantly more likely than AsAms to report suicidal ideation following the onset of PLEs. There were no statistically significant ethnoracial differences in the prevalence of suicide attempts after PLE onset.

Current mental health outcomes

The sample with lifetime PLEs showed no significant ethnoracial variation in fair/poor self-rated mental health at the interview. After Benjamini–Hochberg correction, AfrAms had significantly higher role interference than NCLats and AsAms.

Multivariate analyses of mental health outcomes by ethnoracial background

Lifetime mental health outcomes

Among individuals with lifetime PLEs without hospitalizations before PLE onset, the adjusted hazards of first psychiatric hospitalization following PLE onset were not significantly different for any minoritized ethnoracial group compared to NLWs (Table 3). Four post-hoc pairwise comparisons of hazard ratios (HRs) among other ethnoracial groups were selected by inspection (see ‘Methods’). Lower hazards of hospitalization after PLE onset were found for NCLats (HR = 0.45; 95% CI 0.21–0.98) and AfrAms (HR = 0.52; 95% CI 0.30–0.92) compared to CaribLats.

Table 3. HRs of lifetime mental health outcomes by lifetime PLEs, adjusted by socio-demographic and clinical variables in CPES (Ns = 917–1074)

PLE, psychotic-like experience.

aOnly the NCS-R random subsample that completed the psychosis symptom screener was included.

bRespondents with missing PLE data were dropped from the sample.

cCaribbean Latinos include respondents who identify as Puerto Rican, Cuban, or Dominican.

dNon-Caribbean Latinos include all other respondents who identify as Latinos (68.7% of Mexican origin).

eAfro-Caribbean respondents identified as Black and reported West Indian or Caribbean descent in their own, their parents', or previous generations.

Note: Bolded values are statistically significant: **p < 0.01. Cox regressions adjusting for age of PLE onset, education, household annual income, marital status, and number of lifetime psychiatric disorders.

Significantly lower hazards of outpatient mental healthcare associated with PLEs were observed relative to NLWs among AfrAms (HR = 0.65; 95% CI 0.43–0.99) who had not received outpatient care before their PLE onset. Two post-hoc pairwise comparisons of HRs among other ethnoracial groups were selected by inspection; neither was statistically significant.

The minoritized ethnoracial groups did not differ from NLWs in their hazards of suicidal ideation or attempts after PLE onset. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons of HRs selected by inspection for suicidal ideation or attempts (one each) among other ethnoracial groups also yielded non-significant differences.

Current mental health outcomes

After controlling for background socio-demographic characteristics, number of lifetime CMDs, and duration of PLEs in the full sample (N = 11 139), lifetime PLEs were significantly associated with current poor/fair mental health among NLWs [odds ratio (OR) = 5.09; 95% CI 1.26–20.50] and AfrAms (OR = 1.98; 95% CI 1.20–3.24) (Table 4). As compared to NLWs, however, there were no significant pairwise ethnoracial differences in the strength of association between lifetime PLEs and current poor/fair mental health.

Table 4. Comparative odds of current mental health outcomes by lifetime PLEs across ethnoracial groups in the CPES (N = 11 139)

PLE: psychotic-like experience.

aOnly the NCS-R random subsample that completed the psychosis symptom screener was included.

bRespondents with missing PLE data were dropped from the sample.

cCaribbean Latinos include respondents who identify as Puerto Rican, Cuban, or Dominican.

dNon-Caribbean Latinos include all other respondents who identify as Latinos (68.7% of Mexican origin).

eAfro-Caribbean respondents identified as Black and reported West Indian or Caribbean descent in their own, their parents', or previous generations.

fp values are for the interaction between ethnoracial group membership (non-Latino Whites, reference) and lifetime PLEs with current fair or poor mental health.

gp values are for the interaction between ethnoracial group membership (non-Latino Whites, reference) and lifetime PLEs with days out of role in the last 30 days.

Note: Bolded values are statistically significant: *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01. Logistic regressions adjusting for age of PLE onset, education, household annual income, marital status, number of lifetime psychiatric disorders, and duration of PLEs.

In the fully adjusted model, PLEs were significantly associated with role interference for NLWs (OR = 3.72; 95% CI 1.57–8.85). A significant interaction between PLEs and higher odds of role interference was found for AfrAms relative to NLWs (p = 0.02). Of the four post-hoc pairwise comparisons examined among other ethnoracial groups, the only significant comparison was lower odds of role interference among AfrCaribs compared to CaribLats (p = 0.02).

Study hypothesis

Table 5 summarizes the significant bivariate and multivariate findings on ethnoracial variation in clinical outcomes associated with PLEs and juxtaposes them to PLE prevalences. These results contradict our hypothesis of an inverse correlation across ethnoracial groups between PLE prevalence and clinical significance: AsAms, for example, had both the lowest PLE prevalence and the lowest PLE clinical significance.

Table 5. Prevalence of PLEs and PLE-related functional outcomes across ethnoracial groups

PLE, psychotic-like experience.

Note: ‘>’ indicates that PLEs experienced by the ethnoracial group in the first column are associated with significantly greater clinical and functional interference than PLEs experienced by the ethnoracial groups following the ‘>’ sign.

Discussion

We examined the prevalence and clinical significance of self-reported PLEs in a representative U.S. sample, assessing variations across the social construct of ethnoracial background. Although the CPES was fielded in the early 2000s, it remains the best data source on PLEs in U.S. adults, since it includes a weighted nationally representative sample with sufficient ethnoracial diversity, uses the same PLE and mental health outcome measures across sub-studies, and includes ethnically or linguistically matched assessments.

While international epidemiological research reveals higher PLE prevalence among Black or Caribbean-descent individuals and lower prevalence among those of Asian origin, these findings were only partly confirmed. AsAms had significantly lower PLE prevalence and number of PLEs than NLWs, CaribLats, and AfrAms and were less likely to report auditory or visual hallucinations than several ethnoracial groups. But prevalence according to Black or Caribbean descent was inconsistent: AfrAms were significantly more likely to report auditory and visual hallucinations than NLWs but also more so than AfrCaribs, who did not differ statistically from NLWs. We observed a similar mixed pattern for auditory hallucinations for Caribbean origin for CaribLats, AfrCaribs, and NLWs; and the age of PLE onset showed significant intra-Black, intra-Caribbean variation.

The clinical significance of PLEs also showed ethnoracial variation. In bivariate analyses, AsAms with PLE's had lower risk of mental healthcare use, morbidity, and impairment after PLE onset than other groups; PLEs in nearly all ethnoracial groups were more likely to be associated with CMDs than AsAms. Among non-AsAm respondents with PLEs, the type of mental health outcome clustered by ethnoracial group. Relative to other groups with PLEs: NLWs with PLEs were significantly more likely to report CMDs, suicidal ideation, and outpatient treatment; AfrAms with PLEs reported more role interference and outpatient care; and CaribLats with PLEs, more hospitalization and role interference. Covariate adjustment eliminated significance differences relative to AsAms but not across several other groups. NCLats and AfrCaribs with PLEs were not associated with any specific type of mental health outcome.

Ethnoracial variation in PLE prevalence

Our prevalence findings show similarities and differences with previous research. Mean U.S. lifetime PLE prevalence (9.1%) is within the international range (1.2–14.9%), though higher than the cross-national median lifetime prevalence estimates among adults of 5–6% in meta-analyses (Linscott & van Os, Reference Linscott and van Os2013; McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Saha, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Bromet, Bruffaerts and Kessler2015). However, we found less ethnoracial variation than most studies, which reveal lower prevalence among Asian-origin respondents and higher PLE prevalence among African- or Caribbean-descent individuals (DeVylder et al., Reference DeVylder, Oh, Corcoran and Lukens2014a; Johns et al., Reference Johns, Nazroo, Bebbington and Kuijpers2002; Leaune et al., Reference Leaune, Dealberto, Luck, Grot, Zeroug-Vial, Poulet and Brunelin2018). Possible reasons for this discrepancy include differences in sampling, weighting, ethnoracial-group inclusion, prevalence period, PLE measurement, language of assessment, covariate selection, and analytical scheme (e.g. we included respondents with clinical psychotic-disorder diagnoses). Some prevalence reports have also raised methodological critiques (DeVylder, Reference DeVylder2014).

The lower prevalence of PLEs among AsAms than other groups confirms past reports from the CPES (DeVylder et al., Reference DeVylder, Oh, Corcoran and Lukens2014a; Oh et al., Reference Oh, Yang, Anglin and DeVylder2014) and is consistent with low community rates of CMDs in U.S. Asian-origin populations (Xu et al., Reference Xu, Okuda, Hser, Hasin, Liu, Grant and Blanco2011). Potential explanations for both findings include higher proportions of foreign-born respondents than in other ethnoracial groups, who tend to report lower prevalence of mental health problems (Breslau & Chang, Reference Breslau and Chang2006), possibly related to lower duration of exposure to U.S.-based racial discrimination than native-born individuals (Gee, Spencer, Chen, Yip, & Takeuchi, Reference Gee, Spencer, Chen, Yip and Takeuchi2007). However, including nativity in the model (online Supplementary Table S3) did not alter our findings. Other explanations include lower rates of substance use disorders, driving down risk for other psychiatric disorders (Xu et al., Reference Xu, Okuda, Hser, Hasin, Liu, Grant and Blanco2011); response bias due to higher stigmatization of mental illness among AsAms (Gee et al., Reference Gee, Spencer, Chen, Yip and Takeuchi2007); or culture-specific expressions of emotional distress, leading to under-reporting (Breslau & Chang, Reference Breslau and Chang2006).

Ethnoracial variation in clinical significance of PLEs

Variation in PLE clinical significance was notable among AsAms in bivariate analyses. This variation was attenuated after covariate adjustment, suggesting that demographic or clinical correlates account for a portion of the variance in PLE clinical significance among AsAms, relative to other ethnoracial groups. In backward regression analyses, removing the CMD covariate revealed significant differences between AsAms and other groups for suicidal ideation and hospitalization and a trend-level finding (p = 0.056) for role interference that persisted after demographic adjustments, suggesting that PLEs in AsAms are closely associated with CMDs.

Among non-AsAms with PLEs, ethnoracial background was associated either with non-significant variation in clinical significance (NCLats, AfrCaribs) or with a specific type of outcome (NLWs, CaribLats, AfrAms). Multivariate modeling suggests this variation was not due solely to demographic or clinical differences. The ethnoracial labels may indicate underlying socio-structural characteristics that pattern clinical significance outcomes. The association between NLWs and outpatient care (v. NCLats and AfrAms) may be due to longstanding disparities in access to ambulatory mental-health services among minoritized groups relative to NLWs (Chow, Jaffee, & Snowden, Reference Chow, Jaffee and Snowden2003). The fact that this association persisted in multivariate analyses suggests that, in NLWs, PLEs may be associated with using outpatient care independent of other covariates. The associations between AfrAms and role interference and between CaribLats, hospitalization, and role interference in multivariate models suggest that PLEs may be connected to these groups' greater chronicity (Vilsaint et al., Reference Vilsaint, NeMoyer, Fillbrunn, Sadikova, Kessler, Sampson and Alegría2019; Williams et al., Reference Williams, González, Neighbors, Nesse, Abelson, Sweetman and Jackson2007) and involuntary inpatient admissions (Bhalla et al., Reference Bhalla, Siegel, Chaudhry, Li, Torbati, Nuckols and Danovitch2022).

But why do NCLats and AfrCaribs show no specific association relative to other ethnoracial groups between PLEs and mental health outcomes in bivariate or multivariate analyses? These groups also face delays in seeking mental health care (Wells, Klap, Koike, & Sherbourne, Reference Wells, Klap, Koike and Sherbourne2001), often reporting persistent and severe mental disorders (Williams et al., Reference Williams, González, Neighbors, Nesse, Abelson, Sweetman and Jackson2007). In addition to socio-structural barriers, ethnoracial labels can signal cultural variation in the association of individual symptoms with adverse outcomes. Distinct cultural traditions of meaning and social practice may be impacting the acceptability and functional impact of PLEs (Kirmayer, Gómez-Carrillo, & Veissière, Reference Kirmayer, Gómez-Carrillo and Veissière2017) by influencing differential concern, social supports, stigmatization, and help-seeking processes (Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Haroz, Kohrt, Bolton, Bass and Hinton2015; Lewis-Fernández & Kirmayer, Reference Lewis-Fernández and Kirmayer2019). The anthropological term idiom of distress indicates the function of a symptom as an expression of suffering intended to communicate a specific message (Nichter, Reference Nichter2010). Superficially similar symptoms may signal distinct messages – including marital discord, social-status dissatisfaction, demoralization, or severe psychopathology – depending on the meaning constellations encoded in cultural traditions and the circumstances of the individual sufferer. For example, the symptom–disorder relationship between endorsing a threshold number of drinks over 1 year and alcohol use disorder was significantly less specific for NLWs than other ethnoracial groups. NLWs were likelier to report this symptom despite not meeting the disorder criteria (Alegría & McGuire, Reference Alegría and McGuire2003). NCLats and AfrCaribs may tend not to associate PLEs with special clinical significance due to these cultural interpretations.

Study hypothesis

The clinical significance of PLEs varies complexly across ethnoracial groups, probably depending on the constellations of socio-structural and cultural factors that underlie the ethnoracial label. To clarify the resulting associations between ethnoracial labels and PLE clinical significance outcomes, we would need to unpack the relationship of PLEs to these underlying mechanisms. Our study hypothesis was rejected because it was overly simple, ignoring the multiplicity of factors that likely affect PLE prevalence and clinical significance.

Clinical and nosological implications

Providers unfamiliar with cross-ethnoracial variation in PLE clinical significance may assign greater-than-warranted significance to PLEs in some ethnoracial groups (notably AsAms), potentially resulting in inappropriate diagnoses and interventions. Clinicians may also miss associations with specific mental health outcomes (e.g. CaribLats and hospitalization). In sum, using PLEs as markers of clinical severity (e.g. Kelleher et al., Reference Kelleher, Wigman, Harley, O'Hanlon, Coughlan, Rawdon and Cannon2015) should be re-examined within ethnoracial groups, including their use to predict conversion to psychotic disorders (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Xu, Tang, Li, Tang, Cui and Wang2019). To better calibrate diagnoses and treatment options, clinicians should obtain a thorough history of PLE characteristics, associated morbidities and impairments, barriers to care, patients' understandings of illness (e.g. spiritual causation), and current help-seeking expectations with instruments like the Cultural Formulation Interview (Lewis-Fernández et al., Reference Lewis-Fernández, Aggarwal, Lam, Galfalvy, Weiss, Kirmayer and Vega-Dienstmaier2017).

Without biomarkers, psychiatric nosologies define diagnoses by identifying cutpoints in symptom dimensions that distinguish psychopathology from non-clinical distress (Maser et al., Reference Maser, Norman, Zisook, Everall, Stein, Schettler and Judd2009). This process is made more complicated if the association of symptoms with morbidity and impairment can be affected by socio-structural factors, patients' patterns of expression, and clinicians' traditions of interpretation. This suggests the relationship between symptoms and disorders may not be constant across social groups (Alegría & McGuire, Reference Alegría and McGuire2003; Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Haroz, Kohrt, Bolton, Bass and Hinton2015).

Our findings challenge the conclusion that PLEs have uniform clinical significance across ethnoracial groups (e.g. DeVylder et al., Reference DeVylder, Burnette and Yang2014b). However, few studies have examined this issue directly, and the same limitations that impact the prevalence findings apply. Past studies deemphasized results suggestive of ethnoracial variation [e.g. in unemployment (DeVylder et al., Reference DeVylder, Burnette and Yang2014b), self-care (Oh et al., Reference Oh, Koyanagi, Kelleher and DeVylder2018)]; interpreted cross-sectional associations as indicating causation by PLEs rather than ethnoracial variation in PLE clinical significance (Vanheusden et al., Reference Vanheusden, Mulder, van der Ende, Selten, van Lenthe, Verhuist and Mackenbach2008); utilized small sample sizes (Vanheusden et al., Reference Vanheusden, Mulder, van der Ende, Selten, van Lenthe, Verhuist and Mackenbach2008); examined limited clinical significance outcomes (King et al., Reference King, Nazroo, Weich, McKenzie, Bhui, Karlson and Erens2005); focused on subjective need for care (DeVylder et al., Reference DeVylder, Oh, Corcoran and Lukens2014a); or bundled diverse outcomes (Vanheusden et al., Reference Vanheusden, Mulder, van der Ende, Selten, van Lenthe, Verhuist and Mackenbach2008). Instead, our study separately assessed mental healthcare use, morbidity, and impairment; excluded individuals whose clinical outcomes preceded PLE onset; used Cox regressions and adjustments for PLE duration to account for varying timeframes since PLE onset; stratified respondents by ethnoracial and Caribbean background; and compared across the major ethnoracial groups.

Limitations

Our study has several limitations. First, the cross-sectional design prevented prospective examination of the temporal relationship between PLEs and adverse outcomes; we tackled this by excluding from the analysis outcomes that preceded PLE onset. However, we lack information on frequency of mental healthcare use, which may vary after PLE onset across ethnoracial groups, affecting associations with adverse outcomes. Second, we cannot examine the temporal sequence of CMD v. PLE first-onsets due to sample size limitations. Third, the associations between ethnoracial group and PLE clinical significance could be affected by our inclusion of individuals with clinician diagnoses of psychotic disorders, which varied across ethnoracial groups (6.2–17.5%). In NLAAS Latinos, this proportion (12.5%) was higher than the SCID-obtained clinical reappraisal of PLE-screener data, which showed that 7.0% of respondents with PLEs met DSM-IV criteria for psychotic disorder (Lewis-Fernández et al., Reference Lewis-Fernández, Horvitz-Lennon, Blanco, Guarnaccia, Cao and Alegría2009). Excluding the group with self-reported clinician diagnoses of psychotic disorder could have resulted in greater sampling bias than their inclusion. Fourth, the survey aimed to exclude via self-report PLEs related to substance use; however, this determination can be complex and vulnerable to misclassification. Fifth, survey non-response and selection bias, especially if differentially distributed across ethnoracial groups, may influence the strength of reported associations, despite weighting for non-response. Sixth, retrospective reports of lifetime experiences are subject to recall biases, especially concerning their temporal order. Seventh, due to the small sample size of respondents with PLEs, we could not further disaggregate the AsAm or Latino samples; we also had reduced statistical power to detect differences in less-common outcomes, such as suicide attempts. Finally, the age of the data may limit its applicability to the changing ethnoracial composition of the USA.

Conclusions

Our analyses reveal less ethnoracial variation in PLE prevalence than previously described (Cohen & Marino, Reference Cohen and Marino2013; DeVylder et al., Reference DeVylder, Oh, Corcoran and Lukens2014a). AsAms reported significantly lower PLE prevalence than other U.S. ethnoracial groups, but the variation among Black and Caribbean-descent groups was inconsistent. Lower prevalence was not correlated with higher clinical significance. The clinical significance of PLEs varied across ethnoracial groups, being especially less salient among AsAms. Lack of familiarity with ethnoracial variation in PLE clinical significance may lead to the over-valuation of PLEs as markers of clinical severity in some ethnoracial groups. Future research could examine the association between PLEs, clinical severity, and conversion to psychotic disorder within ethnoracial groups, including the intersection of ethnoracial background with other social characteristics such as treatment barriers. The ethnoracial specificity of the relationships between other mental health symptoms and disorders could also be examined, as these relationships may vary across social groups.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723001496.

Acknowledgements

The authors appreciate the contributions of Jennifer Humensky, Ph.D., while she worked at Columbia University. The NLAAS data used in this analysis were provided by Massachusetts General Hospital.

Financial support

The project was supported by NIH Research Grant no. U01 MH06220-06A2 funded by the National Institute of Mental Health. Drs. Lewis-Fernández and Olfson receive institutional support from the New York State Psychiatric Institute.

Conflict of interest

All authors state they do not have any competing interests.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Alegría, M., & McGuire, T. (2003). Rethinking a universal framework in the psychiatric symptom–disorder relationship. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 44(9), 257274.10.2307/1519778CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alegría, M., Takeuchi, D., Canino, G., Duan, N., Shrout, P., Meng, X. L., … Gong, F. (2004). Considering context, place and culture: The National Latino and Asian American Study. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 13(4), 208220.10.1002/mpr.178CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alegria, M., Woo, M., Takeuchi, D., & Jackson, J. (2009). Ethnic and racial group differences in mental health and service use: Findings from the collaborative psychiatric epidemiological surveys. In Ruiz, P. & Primm, A. (Eds.), Disparities in psychiatric care: Clinical and cross-cultural perspectives (pp. 306318). Baltimore: Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins.Google Scholar
Anglin, D. M., Ereshefsky, S., Klaunig, M. J., Bridgwater, M. A., Niendam, T. A., Ellman, L. M., … van der Ven, E. (2021). From womb to neighborhood: A racial analysis of social determinants of psychosis in the United States. American Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 599610.10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20071091CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhalla, I. P., Siegel, K., Chaudhry, M., Li, N., Torbati, S., Nuckols, T., & Danovitch, I. (2022). Involuntary psychiatric hospitalization: How patient characteristics affect decision-making. Psychiatric Quarterly, 93, 297310.10.1007/s11126-021-09939-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, J., & Chang, D. F. (2006). Psychiatric disorders among foreign-born and US-born Asian-Americans in a U.S. national survey. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 41, 943950.10.1007/s00127-006-0119-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chow, J. C. C., Jaffee, K., & Snowden, L. (2003). Racial/ethnic disparities in the use of mental health services in poverty areas. American Journal of Public Health, 93, 792797.10.2105/AJPH.93.5.792CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, C. I., & Marino, L. (2013). Racial and ethnic differences in the prevalence of psychotic symptoms in the general population. Psychiatric Services, 64, 11031109.10.1176/appi.ps.201200348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeVylder, J. E. (2014). Prevalence of psychotic symptoms. Psychiatric Services, 65, 270.10.1176/appi.ps.650203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeVylder, J. E., Burnette, D., & Yang, L. H. (2014b). Co-occurrence of psychotic experiences and common mental conditions across four racially and ethnically diverse populations samples. Psychological Medicine, 44, 35033513.10.1017/S0033291714000944CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeVylder, J. E., Oh, H. Y., Corcoran, C. M., & Lukens, E. P. (2014a). Treatment seeking and unmet need for care among persons reporting psychosis-like experiences. Psychiatric Services, 65, 774780.10.1176/appi.ps.201300254CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gara, M. A., Vega, W. A., Arndt, S., Escamilla, M., Fleck, D. E., Lawson, W. R., … Strakowski, S. M. (2012). Influence of patient race and ethnicity on clinical assessment in patients with affective disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(6), 593600.10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gee, G. C., Spencer, M., Chen, J., Yip, T., & Takeuchi, D. T. (2007). The association between self-reported racial discrimination and 12-month DSM-IV mental disorders among Asian Americans nationwide. Social Science and Medicine, 64, 19841996.10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.02.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanssen, M., Bak, M., Bijl, R., Vollebergh, W., & van Os, J. (2005). The incidence and outcome of subclinical psychotic experiences in the general population. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 181191.10.1348/014466505X29611CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heeringa, S., Wagner, J., Torres, M., Duan, N., Adams, T., & Berglund, P. (2004). Sample designs and sampling methods for the Collaborative Psychiatric Epidemiology Studies (CPES). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 13(4), 221240.10.1002/mpr.179CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Honings, S., Drukker, M., Groen, R., & van Os, J. (2016). Psychotic experiences and risk of self-injurious behavior in the general population: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 46, 237251.10.1017/S0033291715001841CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, J., Torres, M., Caldwell, C. H., Neighbors, H. W., Nesse, R., Taylor, R. J., … Williams, D. R. (2004). The National Survey of American Life: A study of racial, ethnic and cultural influences on mental disorders and mental health. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 13(4), 196207.10.1002/mpr.177CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johns, L. C., Nazroo, J. Y., Bebbington, P., & Kuijpers, E. (2002). Occurrence of hallucinatory experiences in a community sample and ethnic variations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180(2), 174178.10.1192/bjp.180.2.174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaiser, B. N., Haroz, E. E., Kohrt, B. A., Bolton, P. A., Bass, J. K., & Hinton, D. E. (2015). ‘Thinking too much’: A systematic review of a common idiom of distress. Social Science and Medicine, 147, 170183.10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.10.044CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaymaz, N., Drukker, M., Lieb, R., Wittchen, H. U., Werbeloff, N., Weiser, M., … van Os, J. (2012). Do subthreshold psychotic experiences predict clinical outcomes in unselected non-help-seeking population-based samples? A systematic review and meta-analysis, enriched with new results. Psychological Medicine, 42, 22392253.10.1017/S0033291711002911CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelleher, I., & Cannon, M. (2011). Psychotic-like experiences in the general population: Characterizing a high-risk group for psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 41, 16.10.1017/S0033291710001005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelleher, I., Keeley, P., Corcoran, F., Lynch, C., Fitzpatrick, N., Devlin, C., … Cannon, M. (2012). Clinicopathological significance of psychotic experiences in non-psychotic young people: Evidence from four population-based studies. British Journal of Psychiatry, 201, 2632.10.1192/bjp.bp.111.101543CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelleher, I., Wigman, J. T. W., Harley, M., O'Hanlon, E., Coughlan, H., Rawdon, C., … Cannon, M. (2015). Psychotic experiences in the population: Association with functioning and mental distress. Schizophrenia Research, 165, 914.10.1016/j.schres.2015.03.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R., & Merikangas, K. (2004). The National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 13, 6068.10.1002/mpr.166CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 593602.10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., & Üstün, T. B. (2004). The World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 13, 93121.10.1002/mpr.168CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, M., Nazroo, J., Weich, S., McKenzie, K., Bhui, K., Karlson, S., … Erens, B. (2005). Psychotic symptoms in the general population of England: A comparison of ethnic groups (The EMPIRIC study). Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 40, 375381.10.1007/s00127-005-0900-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirmayer, L. J., Gómez-Carrillo, A., & Veissière, S. (2017). Culture and depression in global mental health: An ecosocial approach to the phenomenology of psychiatric disorders. Social Science and Medicine, 183, 163168.10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.04.034CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leaune, E., Dealberto, M. J., Luck, D., Grot, S., Zeroug-Vial, H., Poulet, E., & Brunelin, J. (2018). Ethnic minority position and migrant status as risk factors for psychotic symptoms in the general population: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 49(4), 545558.10.1017/S0033291718002271CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legge, S. E., Jones, H. J., Kendall, K. M., Pardiñas, A. F., Menzies, G., Bracher-Smith, M., … Walters, J. T. R. (2019). Association of genetic liability to psychotic experiences with neuropsychotic disorders and traits. JAMA Psychiatry, 76, 12561265.10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2508CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis-Fernández, R., Aggarwal, N. K., Lam, P., Galfalvy, H., Weiss, M. G., Kirmayer, L. J., … Vega-Dienstmaier, J. (2017). Feasibility, acceptability, and clinical utility of the Cultural Formulation Interview: Mixed-methods results from the DSM-5 international field trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 210, 290297.10.1192/bjp.bp.116.193862CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis-Fernández, R., Horvitz-Lennon, M., Blanco, C., Guarnaccia, P. J., Cao, Z., & Alegría, M. (2009). Significance of endorsement of psychotic symptoms by U.S. Latinos. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 197(5), 337347.10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181a2087eCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis-Fernández, R., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2019). Cultural concepts of distress and psychiatric disorders: Understanding symptom experience and expression in context. Transcultural Psychiatry, 56, 786803.10.1177/1363461519861795CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linscott, R. J., & van Os, J. (2013). An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: On the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychological Medicine, 43, 11331149.10.1017/S0033291712001626CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maser, J. D., Norman, S. B., Zisook, S., Everall, I. P., Stein, M. B., Schettler, P. J., & Judd, L. L. (2009). Psychiatric nosology is ready for a paradigm shift in DSM-V. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 16, 2440.Google Scholar
McGrath, J. J., Saha, S., Al-Hamzawi, A., Alonso, J., Bromet, E. J., Bruffaerts, R., … Kessler, R. J. (2015). Psychotic experiences in the general population: A cross-national analysis based on 31261 respondents from 18 countries. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(7), 697705.10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0575CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ngwa, J. S., Cabral, H. J., Chen, D. M., Pencina, M. J., Gagnon, D. R., LaValley, M. P., & Cupples, L. A. (2016). A comparison of time dependent Cox regression, pooled logistic regression, and cross-sectional pooling with simulations and an application to the Framingham heart study. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 16, 148.10.1186/s12874-016-0248-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nichter, M. (2010). Idioms of distress revisited. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 34, 401416.10.1007/s11013-010-9179-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nuevo, R., Chatterji, S., Verdes, E., Naidoo, N., Arango, C., & Ayuso-Mateos, J. L. (2012). The continuum of psychotic symptoms in the general population: A cross-national study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(3), 475485.10.1093/schbul/sbq099CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oh, H., Koyanagi, A., Kelleher, I., & DeVylder, J. (2018). Psychotic experiences and disability: Findings from the Collaborative Psychiatric Epidemiological Survey. Schizophrenia Research, 193(3), 343347.10.1016/j.schres.2017.07.049CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oh, H., Yang, L. H., Anglin, D. M., & DeVylder, J. (2014). Perceived discrimination and psychotic experiences across multiple ethnic groups in the United States. Schizophrenia Research, 157, 259265.10.1016/j.schres.2014.04.036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powers, A. R. (2019). Psychotic experiences in the general population: Symptom specificity and the role of distress and dysfunction. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(12), 12281229.10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2391CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rao, J. N. K., & Scott, A. J. (1984). On chi-squared tests for multiway contingency tables with cell proportions estimated from survey data. Annals of Statistics, 12, 4660.10.1214/aos/1176346391CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rehm, J., Ustun, T. B., Saxena, S., Nelson, C. B., Chatterji, S., Ivis, F., & Adlaf, E. (1999). On the development and psychometric testing of the WHO screening instrument to assess disablement in the general population. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 8, 110123.10.1002/mpr.61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stata Corp (2017). Stata Statistical Software: Release 15.1. College Station, TX: StataCorp, LLC.Google Scholar
Thissen, D., Steinberg, L., & Kuang, D. (2002). Quick and easy implementation of the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure for controlling the false-positive rate in multiple comparisons. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 27, 7783.10.3102/10769986027001077CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanheusden, K., Mulder, C. L., van der Ende, J., Selten, J. P., van Lenthe, J., Verhuist, F. C., & Mackenbach, J. P. (2008). Associations between ethnicity and self-reported hallucinations in a population sample of young adults in The Netherlands. Psychological Medicine, 38, 10951102.10.1017/S0033291707002401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vilsaint, C. L., NeMoyer, A., Fillbrunn, M., Sadikova, E., Kessler, R. C., Sampson, N. A., … Alegría, M. (2019). Ethnoracial differences in 12-month prevalence and persistence of mood, anxiety, and substance use disorders: Variation by nativity and socioeconomic status. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 89, 5260.10.1016/j.comppsych.2018.12.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, K., Klap, R., Koike, A., & Sherbourne, C. (2001). Ethnic disparities in unmet need for alcoholism, drug abuse, and mental health care. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 20272032.10.1176/appi.ajp.158.12.2027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, D. R., González, H. M., Neighbors, H., Nesse, R., Abelson, J. M., Sweetman, J., & Jackson, J. S. (2007). Prevalence and distribution of major depressive disorder in African Americans, Caribbean Blacks, and non-Hispanic Whites. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 305315.10.1001/archpsyc.64.3.305CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, Y., Okuda, M., Hser, Y. I., Hasin, D., Liu, S. M., Grant, B. F., … Blanco, C. (2011). Twelve-month prevalence of psychiatric disorders and treatment-seeking among Asian Americans/Pacific Islanders in the United States: Results from the National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45, 910918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, T., Xu, L., Tang, Y., Li, H., Tang, X., Cui, H., … Wang, J. (2019). Prediction of psychosis in prodrome: Development and validation of a simple, personalized risk calculator. Psychological Medicine, 49, 19901998.10.1017/S0033291718002738CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Lifetime PLEs among CPES respondents, adjusted by age and gender (N = 11 139)

Figure 1

Table 2. Lifetime psychiatric disorders and mental health outcomes of CPES respondents with lifetime PLEs, adjusted by age and gender (N = 1138)

Figure 2

Table 3. HRs of lifetime mental health outcomes by lifetime PLEs, adjusted by socio-demographic and clinical variables in CPES (Ns = 917–1074)

Figure 3

Table 4. Comparative odds of current mental health outcomes by lifetime PLEs across ethnoracial groups in the CPES (N = 11 139)

Figure 4

Table 5. Prevalence of PLEs and PLE-related functional outcomes across ethnoracial groups

Supplementary material: File

Lewis-Fernández et al. supplementary material

Lewis-Fernández et al. supplementary material
Download Lewis-Fernández et al. supplementary material(File)
File 42.2 KB