Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T16:06:17.417Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Context-Dependent Responses to the Spread of COVID-19 Among National and International Students During the First Lockdown: An Online Survey

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 September 2023

Marta Andreatta*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Education and Child Studies, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Netherlands
Joran Jongerling
Affiliation:
Department of Methodology and Statistics, Tilburg School of Social and Behavioral Science, Tilburg, Netherlands
Matthias J. Wieser
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Education and Child Studies, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Netherlands
*
Corresponding author: Marta Andreatta; Email: andreatta@essb.eur.nl.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Restrictions to minimize social contact was necessary to prevent the spread of the severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) virus but may have impacted individuals’ mental well-being. Emotional responses are modulated by contextual information. Living abroad during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic may have boosted the feeling of isolation as the context is unfamiliar.

Objectives:

This study compared the psychological impact of social distancing in national students (living in a familiar context) versus international students (living in an unfamiliar context).

Methods:

During March/April 2020 (first lockdown in the Netherlands), 850 university students completed an online survey. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was conducted to compare how students’ responses to the virus were predicted by health anxiety, emotional distress, and personal traits.

Results:

Compared with national students, international students showed higher levels in 4 identified factors (COVID-19-related worry, perceived risk of infection, distance from possibly contaminated objects, distance from social situations). The factors were mainly predicted by health anxiety across international students, while emotional distress and individual traits (eg, intolerance of uncertainty) played a role across national students.

Conclusions:

In the familiar context, individual characteristics (traits) predicted the responses to the virus, while the unfamiliar context drove individuals’ health-focused responses. Living in a foreign country is associated with psychological burdens and this should be considered by universities for more pronounced social support and clear references to health-related institutions.

Type
Original Research
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Society for Disaster Medicine and Public Health

In December 2019, a new virus started spreading from a Chinese province and, within 4 mo reached almost all the countries of the world. The virus in question is called severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) and causes the coronavirus disease coronavirus disease 019 (COVID-19). Its health consequences could be devastating and potentially deadly for many individuals, especially when no vaccination was available. To prevent the further spread of the virus, many governments worldwide decided to restrict the liberties of their citizens greatly. Citizens were asked to stay home; they were only allowed to leave their houses to buy food or other necessities, while all other non-essential shops were closed, as were all places where individuals would meet and socialize. The permission to visit shops for essential items created a dilemma: while people were being urged to stay home, the availability of shops made it necessary to venture out. This dilemma resulted in both avoidance and hoarding behaviors, where individuals would avoid going to shops as much as possible, but when they did, they would stock up on items in case the restrictions became even more severe. The restrictions taken by the various European countries slightly varied, but in all countries, individuals were asked to keep physical distance and reduce social contacts, which suggested spending more time at home (home-office, remote studying) and being more socially isolated.Footnote 1 Most countries even restricted freedom to move almost entirely and ordered temporary school and business closures (lockdown). However, individuals showed a large variability in their compliance with these restrictions. Reference Clark, Davila and Regis2,Reference Hein, Gamer and Gall3 Unclear remains whether the individuals’ compliance with the restrictions as well as their emotional responses to the virus might be influenced by the degree of familiarity about the rules of the country in which they live.

Social isolation and social contact are crucially implicated in the etiology and resiliency of mental disorders, respectively. Reference Brooks, Webster and Smith4Reference Kim, Sherman and Taylor6 Social connections have been found to buffer against mental health pathologies as they may attenuate the adversity of stressful situations. Reference Kim, Sherman and Taylor6 In contrast, having too few social contacts seems to facilitate the onset of mental health issues or worsen the prognosis after treatments. Reference Cacioppo and Hawkley5,Reference Kim, Sherman and Taylor6 Previous pandemics (Ebola virus, SARS) have been demonstrated to have a deteriorating impact on mental health, especially when associated with strict containment measures. Reference Brooks, Webster and Smith4 Likewise, the outbreak of the SARS-CoV-2 virus was perceived by many as a very stressful situation, which may have been linked to being isolated, next to the anxiety of contracting the virus Reference Cacioppo and Hawkley5 Additionally, individuals also greatly differed in their fear of contamination, which was considered a risk factor for intense health and social anxiety. Reference Rachman7 For instance, obsessive-compulsive disorders (OCD), particularly in combination with fear of contamination before the COVID-19 pandemic, were predictive of increased contamination symptomatology during the pandemic.

Mental illness can result from the interaction between biological (eg, genetic) or psychological (eg, personality traits) factors and the aversiveness of the environment. Reference Ressler8 For instance, trait anxiety has been related to a higher risk of developing anxiety disorders or depression. Reference Sandi and Richter-Levin9,Reference Weger and Sandi10 In the case of the COVID-19 pandemic, recent studies showed that the first lockdown was associated with a higher prevalence of anxiety Reference Liu, Luo and Li11 and a reduction in mood Reference Taquet, Quoidbach and Fried12,Reference Concerto, Rodolico and La Rosa13 at the population level. However, it is still unclear how the interaction among biological, psychological, and environmental factors may lead to mental illness. There is evidence for certain personality traits’ role in mediating how individuals react to environmental changes. Anxious personalities are prone not only to respond to aversive situations more intensely Reference Childs, White and de Wit14 but also to avoid numerous situations which may (or may not) be aversive, and such avoidance is performed rigidly and conservatively. Reference Craske, Hermans and Vervliet15,Reference Penninx, Pine and Holmes16 The rigid use of avoidance has been implicated in the etiology and maintenance of anxiety disorders as the individual’s anxiety toward the situation is strengthened rather than diminished. Reference Craske, Hermans and Vervliet15 It is conceivable that such rigid avoidance is due to a chronic search for safety. Anxious individuals often exhibit a reduced ability to recognize safety and, therefore, can feel threatened in many situations. Reference Sep, Steenmeijer and Kennis17 The capacity to identify safety can be further reduced by aversive contexts or stressful situations Reference Klinke, Fiedler and Lange18 ; as such, these stressful situations elicit even greater avoidance. Reference von Dawans, Strojny and Domes19

Indeed, it has already been shown that individual risk factors such as intolerance of uncertainty Reference Mertens, Gerritsen and Duijndam20,Reference Saulnier, Koscinski and Volarov21 or concerns for physical symptoms, ie, anxiety sensitivity, Reference Paluszek, Asmundson and Landry22 were crucially involved in the safety responses to the COVID-19 pandemic. These studies found that concerns for physical health and health anxiety predicted safety responses to the virus (measured by means of questionnaires). In line with this, Taylor and colleagues Reference Taylor, Landry and Rachor23 demonstrated that worries about the dangerousness of the virus were the most important factor mediating the subjective stress associated with the current situation. Furthermore, Concerto and colleagues Reference Concerto, Rodolico and La Rosa13 observed that depressive temperament could worsen the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on the mental health of young adults. However, the role of intolerance of uncertainty remains unclear as this personality trait did not strongly predict subjective fear of COVID-19 (it was only a trend level, or only in combination with anxiety sensitivity Reference Saulnier, Koscinski and Volarov21 ). Together these studies Reference Mertens, Gerritsen and Duijndam20,Reference Paluszek, Asmundson and Landry22,Reference Taylor, Landry and Rachor23 suggest that fear or anxiety of the virus and symptoms related to obsessive-compulsive disorders were crucial in emotional responses to the SARS-CoV-2 virus.

During 2020, our lives were catapulted into an uncertain and unpredictably threatening situation. This state of uncertainty could have had a particularly profound impact on young people, given that they are naturally in a life phase that is rife with future uncertainties. Reference Leshner24Reference Commodari, La Rosa and Carnemolla26 The virus, or the threat it posed, was highly unpredictable due to the inability of humans to identify it with certainty. According to previous research, unpredictable threats lead to a continuous search for safety and a sustained feeling of fear, also defined as anxiety. Reference Davis, Walker and Miles27,Reference Lohr, Olatunji and Sawchuk28 However, a distinction between fear and anxiety does exist; fear has been defined as a response to an imminent and concrete threat, while anxiety is a future-oriented response toward possible but not concrete threats. Reference Davis, Walker and Miles27 Based on the unpredictability of the virus, its threat can be perceived as more aversive than an inherently identifiable or tangible threat. Reference Grillon, Baas and Lissek29,Reference Grillon, O’Connell and Lieberman30 The responses to such threats can be modulated by contextual information meaning that the context can be informative about the presence and the imminence of the threat. Reference Andreatta and Pauli31Reference Mobbs, Headley and Ding33 In the case of COVID-19 pandemic, national governments provided the necessary guidance to deal with the threat, but individuals demonstrated a great variance in their adherence to such recommendations. Reference Clark, Davila and Regis2,Reference Hein, Gamer and Gall3 Moreover, living abroad may be considered as living in an uncertain context as it suggests a reduced knowledge about the host nation’s government, greater isolation and less confidence in the situational characteristics. Reference Pantelidou and Craig34 In the stressful and uncertain situation of the pandemic, living aboard seemed to lead to reduced mental health. Reference Behisi, Altaweel and Gassas35

This study investigated the role of context, defined as living abroad vs in the country of birth, in the emotional and behavioral responses to the spread of the SARS-CoV-2 virus. It was assumed that individual levels of anxiety, intolerance of uncertainty, and fear of contamination would predict the emotional and behavioral responses to the COVID-19 pandemic and the related lockdown measures. We focused on young adults, differentiating between international and national students. Reasons for selecting young adults are as follows; first, undergraduate students may experience more uncertainty about the situation due to transitioning to a new academic environment and studies suggest that they exhibit a greater incidence of anxiety and depression than the general population. Reference Leshner24,Reference Kaparounaki, Patsali and Mousa25,Reference Green, van de Groep and Sweijen36 Second, the brain of young adults may be more vulnerable, especially under circumstances of constant threat and stress. Reference Abend, Gold and Britton37,Reference Santomauro, Mantilla Herrera and Shadid38 Third, international students may have experienced even more uncertainty in the situation as they resided in a foreign country with unfamiliar rules and potentially a different language. Moreover, international students may have experienced greater isolation than national students, given the distance from their families, and often dealing with communication difficulties due to differing time zones.

Methods

Participants

All participants were undergraduate students of the Erasmus University Rotterdam and recruited through an advertisement on an Internet platform (ie, which Internet platform). The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Psychology, Education and Child Studies Faculty of the Erasmus University Rotterdam in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki and followed the standards for conducting Web-based surveys (CHERRIES Reference Eysenbach39 ). All participants gave written informed consent. Psychology students received course credits for their participation.

One thousand fifty-six individuals filled out the online survey. For the analyses, 205 participants were excluded: 203 participants did not complete the survey, 1 participant took 3 sec to finish the survey (lowest quartile), and 1 participant took over 19 h (the highest quartile) to fill in the survey. One participant completed the survey twice, therefore, we considered only the first submission excluding the second one. For the analyses, we considered 80.49% of these participants meaning that the final sample consisted of 850 participants (658 females, and 12 nonspecified; mean age, 21.47; SD, = 3.89; range, 17-66 y). Approximately one-third of participants (N = 240) indicated a nationality other than Dutch and were, therefore, labeled as internationals (for descriptive statistics, see Table 1).

Table 1. Overview of sociodemographic variables and mental health questionnaires, separated for national and international students

Questionnaires

Emotional and Behavioral Responses to the Covid-Pandemic

COVID-Inventory

To create COVID-related items, we took the Swine Flu Anxiety Items Reference Wheaton, Abramowitz and Berman40 and the Ebola Fear Inventory, Reference Blakey, Reuman and Jacoby41 adapting the questions to the current situation. In addition, we developed 6 items on avoidance and hoarding behaviors. The items are shown in Table 2 and were rated from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much). The Cronbach’s alpha for these items is 0.85 (CI 95%: 0.83, 0.87), indicating a relatively good internal consistency.

Table 2. Items for the COVID-inventory

Individual Risk Factors (Predictors)

Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II Reference Beck, Steer and Ball42 )

The BDI-II is a diagnostic tool consisting of 21 items examining the strength of depressive symptoms experienced in the past 2 wk, with response options ranging from 0 (not experienced) to 3 (very strongly). For each individual, a sum score was calculated across the 21 items, the individual’s strength of depressive symptomatology. The Cronbach’s alpha of this questionnaire within the current sample is 0.90 (CI 95%: 0.89, 0.91).

Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale (IUS Reference Carleton, Fetzner and Hackl43 )

This questionnaire assesses how individuals perceive aversive uncertain situations, specifically, the behavior, or the feeling in response to an uncertain situation. Participants were asked to evaluate 27 items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all characteristic of me) to 5 (highly characteristics of me). The Cronbach’s alpha of the IUS in our sample is 0.95 (CI 95%: 0.95, 0.95).

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene and Scalarum44 )

This questionnaire includes 2 sub-scales, 1 measuring the anxiety level at the present moment (state), while the other measures anxiety as a stable characteristic of an individual (trait). Both scales consist of 20 items, rated on a 4-point Likert scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very much so). For our sample, Cronbach’s alpha for the STAI state is 0.95 (CI 95%: 0.94, 0.95), and 0.87 for the trait STAI (CI 95%: 0.86, 0.88).

Anxiety Sensitivity Inventory (ASI Reference Reiss, Peterson and Gursky45 )

Anxiety sensitivity is a well-established questionnaire and confirmed personality traits related to the risk for panic attacks. In addition, individuals suffering from panic attacks or at risk for panic disorder are highly sensitive to any physiological changes in their body (eg, increased heart rate or breathing). Therefore, these changes in the physiological responses are interpreted as signs of a possible panic attack or imminent threat. The questionnaire includes 16 items, rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (very little) to 4 (very much). The Cronbach’s alpha of this questionnaire in our sample is 0.92 (CI 95%: 0.91, 0.92).

Short Health Anxiety Inventory (SHAI Reference Salkovskis, Rimes and Warwick46 )

The SHAI is an 18-item self-report measure assessing health anxiety independently of physical health status. The items measure worry about health, awareness of bodily sensations or changes, and feared consequences of having an illness, using a multiple-choice response format ranging from 0 (no symptoms) to 3 (severe). The SHAI has demonstrated good reliability and validity as a measure of health anxiety in clinical and nonclinical samples. Reference Salkovskis, Rimes and Warwick46,Reference Abramowitz, Deacon and Valentiner47 The Cronbach’s alpha in the current sample is 0.88 (CI 95%: 0.87, 0.90).

Fear of Contamination (FoC Reference Burns, Keortge and Formea48 )

The FoC questionnaire reflects an adapted version of the Padua Inventory of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder Symptoms. Reference Sanavio49 This adapted version distinguishes between obsessive behaviors and worries. As the questionnaire is lengthy and we were not interested in obsessive-compulsive disorders but rather in behaviors mediated by the fear of contamination, only the contamination subscale was administered. This subscale consists of 10 items describing behaviors, and participants had to evaluate how well each item described their behavior on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very much). The Cronbach’s alpha of this questionnaire for this sample is 0.90 (CI 95%: 0.89, 0.91).

Body Vigilance Score (BVS Reference Schmidt, Lerew and Trakowski50 )

In the BVS, participants were asked to indicate the degree to which they agree with a particular statement regarding attentional focus on bodily sensations on an 11-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (none) to 10 (extreme). More, specifically, 3 items measure attentional focus, perceived sensitivity to changes in bodily sensations, and the average duration of time spent attending to bodily sensations. A fourth item involves having participants rate their attention to 15 bodily sensations, as defined by the DSM-IV physical symptoms for panic attacks. Responses to the fourth item are averaged to yield a single score and the 4 items are summed up to derive a total BVS score. The Cronbach’s alpha of this questionnaire in our sample is 0.93 (CI 95%: 0.92, 0.94).

Analysis Strategy

We first aimed at establishing the best indicators for measuring behavioral responses and emotions during the COVID-19 pandemic. As a first step of determining the factor-structure, Horn’s parallel analysis Reference Horn51 was calculated to determine how many factors need to be considered for the items of the COVID-Inventory. Then, we ran a factor analysis with the number of factors obtained by the parallel analysis to examine which factors fit well and made substantive sense. The R package “paran” was used for the analysis.

Second, Structural Equation Modeling (SEM Reference Fan, Chen and Shirkey52Reference Schreiber, Nora and Stage54 ) was used to investigate the structural relations across the factors and questionnaires. In the model, we considered the confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to define the latent variables for the factors and used these as dependent variables for the simultaneous linear regression. For the regression, the independent variables BDI, IUS, STAI, ASI, SHAI, FoC, and BVS were inserted. Additionally, we calculated the full information maximum likelihood (FILM Reference Kline55 ) to estimate the missing values of 28 participants for the STAI X2 and of 3 participants for the BVS.

The main goal of the study was to test whether the uncertainties related to the spread of the COVID-19 might have a stronger impact on international students, who are abroad, and, therefore, in a more unpredictable and unknown context, as compared with national students, who live in a more known and possibly predictable context. To this end, we tested the factor structure of our COVID-inventory for measurement-invariance between the 2 groups of students and conducted multi-group analysis based on the SEM model described above to investigate differences between the 2 groups of students. To this purpose, we tested for measurement invariance between the groups, testing for (in order) configural, metric, and scalar invariance. Differences between successive models were tested a using chi-squared (χ2) test for further details please consider. Reference Fan, Chen and Shirkey52,Reference Putnick and Bornstein56 To test the overall fit of the model, we also considered whether the model differed less than 0.010 in comparative fix index (CFI), less than 0.015 in root mean square error approximation (RMSEA), and less than 0.030 in standardized root mean residuals (SRMR). Reference Fan, Chen and Shirkey52,Reference Putnick and Bornstein56 For these analyses, the R package “lavaan” was used. The alpha level was set at 0.05 for all the analyses.

Results

Parallel Factor Analysis

The Horn’s parallel analysis identified 5 factors. We then ran a factor analysis to identify the underlying constructs and considered adjusted eigenvalues larger than 0 (Table 3). Specifically, Items 2, 5, and 6 of the COVID-Inventory were grouped as 1 factor, which we labeled as “COVID-19 pandemic.” Considering that these questions (eg, whether participants believe that the spread of the SARS-CoV-2 may become a pandemic) were asked at the very beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic, and the requested information was clarified during the survey, we decided to exclude this factor from further analyses. Items 1, 7, and 11 represent a second factor labeled as “COVID-19-related worry,” which included concerns about the virus and hoarding behavior. The third factor, labeled “perceived risk of infection” included Items 3 and 4. The fourth factor included Items 10 and 14 of the COVID-Inventory and was labeled “physical distancing,” which refers to strategies and behaviors to avoid potential contamination situations. All the remaining items were included within a final fifth factor labeled as “social distancing,” which refers to strategies and behaviors to reduce social contacts.

Table 3. Parallel factor analysis merged for the COVID-Inventory and COVID-Behavior

Structural Equation Model (SEM)

The model appeared to be a good fit for the observed data (χ2 (131) = 364.47; P < 0.001; Figure 1) as indicated by the CFI (CFI = 0.945), the RMSEA (RMSEA = 0.046) and the SRMR (SRMR = 0.032).Footnote 2

Figure 1. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). The boxes on the bottom depict the observed variables (ie, the items of the 2 COVID-related questionnaires) with the intercepts and the variance (in grey). The circles depict the latent variables (ie, the factors). The solid black lines indicate the betas for each item in relation to the factors. The dotted lines on the top represent the covariance between the factors.

The factor “perceived risk of infection” was not predicted by any questionnaire (all P values < 0.061). Anxiety sensitivity did not significantly predict any factors (all P values < 0.144). The variance of the other 3 factors were significantly predicted by a different set of questionnaires (see Figure 1b). Briefly, the factor “COVID-19-related worry” was significantly predicted by intolerance of uncertainty, trait and state anxiety, health anxiety, and fear of contamination; the factor “physical distancing” was significantly predicted by depression, trait anxiety, body vigilance, and fear of contamination; while the factor “social distancing” was predicted by depression, both trait and state anxiety, body vigilance, and fear of contamination.

Comparison of Dutch and International Students

Both the configural model (χ2 (262) = 524.89; P < 0.001; CFI: 0.935, RMSEA: 0.049, SRMR: 0.037) and the metric model (χ2 (271) = 539.30; P < 0.001; CFI: 0.934, RMSEA: 0.048, SRMR: 0.038) fitted the data well overall, and did not differ substantially in fit (χ 2 (9) = 14.42; P = 0.108).

A model in which the intercepts were constrained to be equal across groups in addition to the factor loadings (scalar invariance: χ 2 (280) = 648.12; P < 0.001; CFI: 0.910; RMSEA: 0.056; SRMR: 0.043), fitted worse than the metric model (χ 2 (9) = 108.81; P < 0.001). We, therefore, determined which intercepts differed the most between groups in the metric model and freed 1 intercept at a time between groups in the scalar model, starting with the item with the largest difference between groups and working in order of magnitude of difference. After freely estimating Items 14, 12, 11, 9, 10, and 1 between groups, the difference in fit between the metric model and the (partial) scalar invariance model (χ 2 (274) = 546.88; P < 0.001; CFI: 0.933; RMSEA: 0.048; SRMR: 0.038) were within our cutoffs again (χ2 (3) = 7.58; P = 0.056).

We then considered the (partial) scalar model and looked at whether the factors were predicted by different questionnaires separately for each group (Figures 2 and 3). Fear of contamination was the only questionnaire in both groups, which significantly predicted all the factors (except for the perceived risk of infection). The factor COVID-19-related worry was additionally predicted by trait (STAI X2) and anxiety in both groups as well as by state (STAI X1) anxiety and intolerance of uncertainty (IUS) across the national students, while by health anxiety (SHAI) across the international students. The factor physical distancing was additionally predicted by trait anxiety in both groups, and by depressive scores across the national students. Finally, factor social distancing was predicted by trait anxiety, depression, and body vigilance across national students.

Figure 2. Results of the structural equation model (SEM). The boxes on the bottom and the top depict the questionnaires (and their sum scores). Blue lines depict negative predictions, while blue lines positive predictions. The factor perceived risk of infection (infection) was predicted by any questionnaire, and ASI predicted any factor. COVID-19-related worry (worry), physical (physical) and social (social) distancing were predicted negatively by trait anxiety (STAI X2) but positively by fear of contamination (FoC). Both body vigilance (BVS) and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) positively predicted safety behaviors, while state anxiety (STAI X1) predicted COVID-19-related worry and social distancing. COVID-19-related worry was additionally predicted by intolerance for uncertainty (IUS) and health anxiety (SHAI).

Figure 3. Results of the structural equation model (SEM) separately for the groups. The boxes depict the questionnaires (and their sum scores). Blue lines depict negative predictions, while black lines positive predictions. The factors were predicted by different questionnaires between the two groups. Health anxiety (SHAI) predicted any factor across Dutch students, while across international students the intolerance of uncertainty (IUS), depression (BDI), and body vigilance (BVS) were not significant predictors. Across international students, social distancing were exclusively predicted by fear of contamination (FoC), while across Dutch students this factor was additionally predicted by trait anxiety (STAI X2), BDI, and BVS. Physical distancing was predicted by STAI X2 and FoC in both groups, but among Dutch students the BDI was additionally predictive. Lastly, COVID-19-related worry was predicted in both groups by FoC, trait and state (STAI X1) anxiety, but for Dutch students the IUS additionally predicted the preoccupations for the virus, while across internationals the SHAI.

Discussion

The primary aim of this study was to investigate the impact of the restrictions imposed during the first lockdown due to the spread of the SARS-CoV-2 virus. Specifically, we studied whether being abroad and, therefore, in a lesser-known context might have increased the experience of psychological burden. For this purpose, national and international students were compared. In general, we observed that young people, who lived abroad, experienced a greater negative impact on their well-being, replicating previous findings Reference Kaparounaki, Patsali and Mousa25,Reference Commodari, La Rosa and Carnemolla26,Reference Behisi, Altaweel and Gassas35

General Psychological Burdens

International students exhibited higher scores across all questionnaires (except for fear of contamination), suggesting that they were more anxious, displayed higher depression scores, were more intolerant of uncertainty, and reported stronger fear related to health and body symptomology. Although it is unclear whether differences between the 2 groups of students were present before the COVID-19 pandemic, it is quite striking that those individuals who live abroad and who, therefore, might feel less confident and safe about the context they are living in report higher burdens than those, who know the context (ie, the country) since birth. This finding aligns with the idea that context provides crucial information about the imminence of a threat or whether it will be (or will not be) encountered. Reference Andreatta and Pauli31 . Reference Mobbs, Headley and Ding33,Reference Fanselow57 The SARS-CoV-2 virus is, per se, an unpredictable threat. In other words, human beings do have the ability to detect viruses only through symptoms such as fever, cough, and fatigue. However, some infected individuals may not show any noticeable symptoms. This makes it difficult or incapable to identify SARS-CoV-2 virus. Such incapacity to identify the virus leads to a sustained fear response (also called anxiety response Reference Davis, Walker and Miles27 ) linked to the inability to identify safety. Reference Lohr, Olatunji and Sawchuk28,Reference Mobbs, Headley and Ding33 Because of its unpredictability, the virus can be perceived as quite threatening, Reference Grillon, Baas and Lissek29 and some individuals can be particularly sensitive to such unpredictable threats. Reference Grillon, O’Connell and Lieberman30 We, therefore, suggest that living in a well-known (ie, assuring) context may provide the necessary information to feel safe. In a familiar context, individuals have guidance on how to predict an “encounter” with the virus, and this in turn could reduce the individuals’ psychological burdens.

COVID-19 Related Factors of Emotional and Behavioral Responses

Despite the psychological burdens seeming higher among international students, this difference is not necessarily related to the spread of the virus. As such, we used the COVID-19-related items to investigate whether the students’ psychological burdens were linked to the virus’s spread. We identified 4 factors based on the 16 self-formulated questions on how participants perceived and felt about the spread of the virus. First, the factor labeled COVID-19-related worry grouped 3 items: concerns related to the spread of the coronavirus, the potential severity of the illness, and hoarding behavior. This group of items suggest that being concerned about the consequences of the virus is strongly associated with hoarding-like behaviors (eg, grocery shopping), which is in line with previous studies. Reference Vazirani and Bhattacharjee58,Reference Yoshino, Shimotsukasa and Hashimoto59 Second, the factor labeled perceived risk of infection included items where participants evaluated the risk of getting infected or that someone they know becomes infected. Third, the factor labeled physical distancing grouped questions focusing on how strongly objects and situations, which might be contaminated, are avoided and how strongly safety behaviors such as hand washing are undertaken. Finally, the fourth factor included items on avoiding social contacts, such as meeting someone, traveling, and maintaining distance (labeled as social distancing). These findings were replicated in the whole sample and for each group. Thus, we found that fear of contamination Reference Knowles and Olatunji60 and bodily preoccupations Reference Hein, Gamer and Gall3 predicts safety-like behaviors such as washing hands, maintaining physical distance, or wearing a mask and that fear of contamination is associated with more substantial anxiety of the virus. Reference Ojalehto, Abramowitz and Hellberg61

Additionally, our model suggests that fear of contamination is not only related to physical distancing but also predicts social distancing, such as avoiding crowded situations or social activities, as well as changing travel plans. Moreover, both of these safety behaviors were also predicted by body vigilance and level of depression, but not by health anxiety or intolerance of uncertainty. These findings support contemporary cognitive-behavioral models, Reference Asmundson, Abramowitz and Richter62,Reference Asmundson and Taylor63 which suggest that health anxiety plays a crucial role in the etiology of mental disorders due to a misinterpretation of the body sensations and catastrophic beliefs about the illness. It is, therefore, conceivable that individuals more vigilant to their body symptoms are more motivated to engage in safety behaviors (ie, physical and social distancing) to prevent the appearance of bodily symptoms. In contrast, individuals with stronger health anxiety or intolerance of uncertainty show more preoccupations because they tend to catastrophize the impact of the virus on health more pronouncedly or because they have fewer resources to endure aversive situations. Reference Mertens, Gerritsen and Duijndam20,Reference Saulnier, Koscinski and Volarov21,Reference Carleton, Fetzner and Hackl43

Comparison of Dutch and International Students

Of interest, our model outlined a distinct pattern of predictors for national and international students, respectively. Specifically, concerns or worries about the spread of the coronavirus were predicted by intolerance of uncertainty among national students but by health anxiety among international students. In contrast, social and physical distancing was predicted by depression and body vigilance among national students but not among international students. Our model suggests that the factors describing worries for and safety behaviors to the virus were influenced by health-specific characteristics (ie, fear of contamination and health anxiety) among international students, while among national students, the virus-related factors were more influenced by individual-specific characteristics such as depression, intolerance of uncertainty, and body vigilance. As mentioned above, a well-known context might provide the necessary information to feel safe compared with an unfamiliar one. It is, therefore, conceivable that the former is less anxiogenic and definable as a weak situation, while the latter is more anxiogenic and thus defined as a strong situation. Weak and strong situations can allow inter-individual differences to emerge or not, respectively. Reference Lissek, Pine and Grillon64,Reference Monson, Hesley and Chernick65 In light of our results, the inter-individual differences of national students could emerge and predict the response to the virus because these students are in a weak situation. Whereas being in a strong and anxiogenic situation such as the international students determined that contamination-related processes mainly predicted the virus-related responses.

In line with our assumptions, international students had more worries and concerns about the spread of the virus, and they initiated more intense safety-like behaviors (eg, avoidance of possibly contaminated areas or situations with possible social contact), suggesting that either feeling less safe or having higher psychological burdens might lead to more preoccupations and distancing. In contrast, international students did not report a higher perceived risk of infection. This was unexpected, but a possible explanation could be that this factor encompasses distinct mechanisms and is more closely associated with cognitive processes than the other factors. In other words, asking about the risk of contracting the virus may require a cognitive and “cold” evaluation of the situation, while avoidance and worries can be more strongly modulated by an emotional evaluation of the situation. Reference LeDoux and Pine66 Moreover, our model suggests that the perceived risk of infection is not predicted by any of the questionnaires, a finding consistent across both groups. Hence, it seems that affective mechanisms such as being anxious, experiencing fear, or feeling depressed do not influence how the risk is “coldly” estimated.

Our model revealed no effect for anxiety sensitivity, while trait anxiety negatively predicted COVID-19-related factors. At first glance, these results seem contra-intuitive and contrast with previous studies. Reference Warren, Zolfaghari and Fresnedo67 However, the relationship between anxiety sensitivity and fear of coronavirus appeared to be more prominent in the clinical population Reference Rogers, Bogiaizian and Salazar68 or when combined with intolerance of uncertainty. Reference Saulnier, Koscinski and Volarov21 At the same time, prepandemic trait anxiety predicted fear of COVID-19, a prediction qualified in the clinical population. Reference Brosch, Meller and Pfarr69 It is, therefore, conceivable that our sample does not present with “sufficient” clinical scores to detect the association between COVID-19-related factors and anxiety sensitivity or trait anxiety.

Moreover, we did not plan interaction effects between the questionnaires, which may have prevented the detection of different types of structures within the data. To date, few studies have focused on the relationship between trait anxiety and fear of coronavirus. The few known studies that have examined such relationships suggest that pre-pandemic trait anxiety predicts fear of COVID-19, Reference Brosch, Meller and Pfarr69 and increased trait anxiety in university students during the COVID-19 pandemic reduced motivation and increased boredom. Reference Alemany-Arrebola, Rojas-Ruiz and Granda-Vera70 However, both of these results do not apply to our study. Instead, we suggest that highly anxious university students may have found the cost of being isolated too high, leading them to decrease their level of social distancing. Such behavior is similar to what has been observed in patients with spider phobia, where they tend to exhibit less avoidance behavior toward the feared animal if doing so leads to an advantageous result. Reference Pittig, Brand and Pawlikowski71

Limitations

In the following, we acknowledge 2 limitations of the study. First, knowing where and with whom the students lived during the lockdown may have been an essential factor to consider. It is possible that some of the international students did not remain in the Netherlands during the first lockdown but returned home to their native countries and were within the well-known context. Moreover, returning to their native country might have increased their social contacts (for instance, by living with the family), which in turn may have been a buffer for psychological distress, as studies on social support suggest. Reference Kim, Sherman and Taylor6 Second, our virus-related factors might deviate from the numerous factors observed in the broader literature regarding the psychological burdens of the SAR-CoV-2’s spread. For example, some articles focus on physical distancing (eg, see Hein et al. Reference Hein, Gamer and Gall3 and Knowles et al. Reference Knowles and Olatunji60 ), while others, like us, differentiate between physical and social distancing. Future research should, therefore, precisely define what a safety behavior is, so that we can then better distinguish the underlying processes.

Conclusions

In summary, we found 4 factors describing the individuals’ reactions to the spread of the virus. These referred to concerns or preoccupations about the spread of the virus, the perceived risk of infection, and safety behaviors which were subdivided into physical (eg, washing hands, maintaining distance) and social (eg, avoiding social contact or crowded situations) distancing. Living abroad and thus in a less-known and unfamiliar context was linked to higher psychological burdens and increased health-related concerns. These results underscore the importance of prioritizing the mental well-being of international students in any risk mitigation or mental health plans developed by universities in the aftermath of the COVID-19 pandemic.

International students may benefit from a more structured support system within the university, which would facilitate finding social contacts, accessing information on locally relevant rules and regulations, and identifying appropriate health-related resources.

Acknowledgements

We sincerely thank H. Galvin for her contribution in the preparation of this manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by a grant of the Erasmus Trust Fund, awarded to M.J. Wieser and M. Andreatta.

Competing interests

No potential conflicting interests are declared.

Footnotes

1 Please note that we did not consider virtual contact in this study. However, virtual contact has not found as buffer for loneliness Reference Rumas, Shamblaw and Jagtap1 .

2 We calculated a post-hoc power analysis for the SEM. For the sample of 850 participants, we found a power larger than > 99.99% (beta: 6.513097e-14) to reject the wrong model with df = 131, RMSEA = 0.046, and alpha = 0.05.

References

Rumas, R, Shamblaw, AL, Jagtap, S, et al. Predictors and consequences of loneliness during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Psychiatry Res. 2021;300:113934. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2021.113934 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, C, Davila, A, Regis, M, et al. Predictors of COVID-19 voluntary compliance behaviors: an international investigation. Glob Transit. 2020;2:76-82. doi: 10.1016/j.glt.2020.06.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hein, G, Gamer, M, Gall, D, et al. Social cognitive factors outweigh negative emotionality in predicting COVID-19 related safety behaviors. Prev Med Rep. 2021;24:101559. doi: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2021.101559 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, SK, Webster, RK, Smith, LE, et al. The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet. 2020;395(10227):912-920. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, JT, Hawkley, LC. Social isolation and health, with an emphasis on underlying mechanisms. Perspect Biol Med. 2003;46 (93 Suppl):S39-S52. doi:doi: 10.1353/pbm.2003.0049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, HS, Sherman, DK, Taylor, SE. Culture and social support. Am Psychol. 2008;63(6):518-526. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachman, S. Fear of contamination. Behav Res Ther. 2004;42(11):1227-1255. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2003.10.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ressler, KJ. Translating across circuits and genetics toward progress in fear- and anxiety-related disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2020;177(3):214-222. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20010055 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandi, C, Richter-Levin, G. From high anxiety trait to depression: a neurocognitive hypothesis. Trends Neurosci. 2009;32(6):312-320. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.02.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weger, M, Sandi, C. High anxiety trait: a vulnerable phenotype for stress-induced depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;87:27-37. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.01.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, X, Luo, W-T, Li, Y, et al. Psychological status and behavior changes of the public during the COVID-19 epidemic in China. Infect Dis Poverty. 2020;9(1):58. doi: 10.1186/s40249-020-00678-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taquet, M, Quoidbach, J, Fried, EI, et al. Mood homeostasis before and during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) lockdown among students in the Netherlands. JAMA Psychiatry. 2021;78(1):110-112. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.2389 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Concerto, C, Rodolico, A, La Rosa, VL, et al. Flourishing or languishing? Predictors of positive mental health in medical students during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(23):15814. doi: 10.3390/ijerph192315814 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Childs, E, White, TL, de Wit, H. Personality traits modulate emotional and physiological responses to stress. Behav Pharmacol. 2014;25(5-6):493-502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craske, MG, Hermans, D, Vervliet, B. State-of-the-art and future directions for extinction as a translational model for fear and anxiety. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2018;373(1742):20170025. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penninx, BW, Pine, DS, Holmes, EA, et al. Anxiety disorders. Lancet. 2021;397(10227):914-927. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(21)00359-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sep, MSC, Steenmeijer, A, Kennis, M. The relation between anxious personality traits and fear generalization in healthy subjects: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;107:320-328. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klinke, CM, Fiedler, D, Lange, MD, et al. Evidence for impaired extinction learning in humans after distal stress exposure. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2020;167:107127. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2019.107127 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Dawans, B, Strojny, J, Domes, G. The effects of acute stress and stress hormones on social cognition and behavior: current state of research and future directions. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;121:75-88. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.11.026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mertens, G, Gerritsen, L, Duijndam, S, et al. Fear of the coronavirus (COVID-19): predictors in an online study conducted in March 2020. J Anxiety Disord. 2020;74:102258. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102258 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saulnier, KG, Koscinski, B, Volarov, M, et al. Anxiety sensitivity and intolerance of uncertainty are unique and interactive risk factors for COVID-19 safety behaviors and worries. Cogn Behav Ther. 2022;51(3):217-228. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2021.1976819 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paluszek, MM, Asmundson, AJN, Landry, CA, et al. Effects of anxiety sensitivity, disgust, and intolerance of uncertainty on the COVID stress syndrome: a longitudinal assessment of transdiagnostic constructs and the behavioural immune system. Cogn Behav Ther. 2021;50(3):179-184. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2021.1877339 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S, Landry, CA, Rachor, GS, et al. Fear and avoidance of healthcare workers: an important, under-recognized form of stigmatization during the COVID-19 pandemic. J Anxiety Disord. 2020;75:102289. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102289 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leshner, AI. Target student mental well-being. Science. 2021;371(6527):325. doi: 10.1126/science.abg5770 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaparounaki, CK, Patsali, ME, Mousa, D-PV, et al. University students’ mental health amidst the COVID-19 quarantine in Greece. Psychiatry Res. 2020;290:113111. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113111 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Commodari, E, La Rosa, VL, Carnemolla, G, et al. The psychological impact of the lockdown on Italian university students during the first wave of COVID-19 pandemic: psychological experiences, health risk perceptions, distance learning, and future perspectives. Mediterr J Clin Psychol. 2021. doi: 10.13129/2282-1619/mjcp-3009 Google Scholar
Davis, M, Walker, DL, Miles, L, et al. Phasic vs sustained fear in rats and humans: role of the extended amygdala in fear vs anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):105-135. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.109 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lohr, JM, Olatunji, BO, Sawchuk, CN. A functional analysis of danger and safety signals in anxiety disorders. Clin Psychol Rev. 2007;27(1):114-126. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.07.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grillon, C, Baas, JP, Lissek, S, et al. Anxious responses to predictable and unpredictable aversive events. Behav Neurosci. 2004;118(5):916-924. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.5.916 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grillon, C, O’Connell, K, Lieberman, L, et al. Distinct responses to predictable and unpredictable threat in anxiety pathologies: effect of panic attack. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2017;2(7):575-581. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.08.005 Google ScholarPubMed
Andreatta, M, Pauli, P. Contextual modulation of conditioned responses in humans: a review on virtual reality studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2021;90:102095. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102095 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Context, Bouton ME., ambiguity, and unlearning: Sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52(10):976-986.Google Scholar
Mobbs, D, Headley, DB, Ding, W, et al. Space, time, and fear: survival computations along defensive circuits. Trends Cogn Sci. 2020;24(3):228-241. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.12.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pantelidou, S, Craig, TKJ. Culture shock and social support: a survey in Greek migrant students. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2006;41(10):777-781. doi: 10.1007/s00127-006-0096-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behisi, MA, Altaweel, HM, Gassas, RF, et al. COVID-19 pandemic and mental health status of Saudi citizens living abroad. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(15):7857. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18157857 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, KH, van de Groep, S, Sweijen, SW, et al. Mood and emotional reactivity of adolescents during the COVID-19 pandemic: short-term and long-term effects and the impact of social and socioeconomic stressors. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):11563. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-90851-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abend, R, Gold, AL, Britton, JC, et al. Anticipatory threat responding: associations with anxiety, development, and brain structure. Biol Psychiatry. 2020;87(10):916-925. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.11.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santomauro, DF, Mantilla Herrera, AM, Shadid, J, et al. Global prevalence and burden of depressive and anxiety disorders in 204 countries and territories in 2020 due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Lancet. 2021;398(10312):1700-1712. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(21)02143-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenbach, G. Improving the quality of web surveys: the checklist for reporting results of internet E-surveys (CHERRIES). J Med Internet Res. 2004;6(3):e34. doi: 10.2196/jmir.6.3.e34 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheaton, MG, Abramowitz, JS, Berman, NC, et al. Psychological predictors of anxiety in response to the H1N1 (swine flu) pandemic. Cognit Ther Res. 2012;36:210-218. doi: 10.1007/s10608-011-9353-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blakey, SM, Reuman, L, Jacoby, RJ, et al. Tracing “Fearbola”: psychological predictors of anxious responding to the threat of Ebola. Cognit Ther Res. 2015;39(6):816-825. doi: 10.1007/s10608-015-9701-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, AT, Steer, RA, Ball, R, et al. Comparison of Beck Depression inventories-IA and-II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess. 1996;67(3):588-597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carleton, RN, Fetzner, MG, Hackl, JL, et al. Intolerance of uncertainty as a contributor to fear and avoidance symptoms of panic attacks. Cogn Behav Ther. 2013;42(4):328-341. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2013.792100 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spielberger, CD, Gorsuch, RL, Lushene, RE. STAI. In: Scalarum, CIP, ed. Internationale Skalen Für Psychiatrie. 4th ed. Beltz-Test; 1996.Google Scholar
Reiss, S, Peterson, RA, Gursky, DM, et al. Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety frequency and the prediction of fearfulness. Behav Res Ther. 1986;24(1):1-8. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(86)90143-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salkovskis, PM, Rimes, KA, Warwick, HMC, et al. The Health Anxiety Inventory: development and validation of scales for the measurement of health anxiety and hypochondriasis. Psychol Med. 2002;32(5):843-853. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702005822 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abramowitz, JS, Deacon, BJ, Valentiner, DP. The Short Health Anxiety Inventory: psychometric properties and construct validity in a non-clinical sample. Cognit Ther Res. 2007;31(6):871-883. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9058-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burns, GL, Keortge, SG, Formea, GM, et al. Revision of the Padua Inventory of obsessive compulsive disorder symptoms: distinctions between worry, obsessions, and compulsions. Behav Res Ther. 1996;34(2):163-173. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(95)00035-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanavio, E. Obsessions and compulsions: The Padua inventory. Behav Res Ther. 1988;26(2):169-177. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(88)90116-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, NB, Lerew, DR, Trakowski, JH. Body vigilance in panic disorder: evaluating attention to bodily perturbations. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1997;65(2):214-220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, JL. A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika. 1965;30:179-185. doi: 10.1007/BF02289447 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, Y, Chen, J, Shirkey, G, et al. Applications of structural equation modeling (SEM) in ecological studies: an updated review. Ecol Process. 2016;5:19. doi: 10.1186/s13717-016-0063-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, RP, Ho, M-HR. Principles and practice in reporting structural equation analyses. Psychol Methods. 2002;7(1):64-82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schreiber, JB, Nora, A, Stage, FK, et al. Reporting structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis results: a review. J Educ Res. 2006;99:323-338. doi: 10.3200/JOER.99.6.323-338 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, TJB. Psychological Testing: A Practical Approach to Design and Evaluation. Sage Publications Inc; 2005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnick, DL, Bornstein, MH. Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: the state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Dev Rev. 2016;41:71-90. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fanselow, MS. Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear. Psychon Bull Rev. 1994;1(4):429-438. doi: 10.3758/BF03210947 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vazirani, A, Bhattacharjee, T. COVID-19 lockdowns and a response model to health, economic and food anxiety. Curr Psychol. 2022;41(8):5734-5744. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-01210-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoshino, S, Shimotsukasa, T, Hashimoto, Y, et al. The association between personality traits and hoarding behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic in Japan. Pers Individ Dif. 2021;179:110927. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110927 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knowles, KA, Olatunji, BO. Anxiety and safety behavior usage during the COVID-19 pandemic: the prospective role of contamination fear. J Anxiety Disord. 2021;77:102323. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102323 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ojalehto, HJ, Abramowitz, JS, Hellberg, SN, et al. Predicting COVID-19-related anxiety: The role of obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions, anxiety sensitivity, and body vigilance. J Anxiety Disord. 2021;83:102460. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2021.102460 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asmundson, GJG, Abramowitz, JS, Richter, AA, et al. Health anxiety: current perspectives and future directions. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2010;12(4):306-312. doi: 10.1007/s11920-010-0123-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asmundson, GJG, Taylor, S. Coronaphobia: fear and the 2019-nCoV outbreak. J Anxiety Disord. 2020;70:102196. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102196 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lissek, S, Pine, DS, Grillon, C. The strong situation: a potential impediment to studying the psychobiology and pharmacology of anxiety disorders. Biol Psychol. 2006;72(3):265-270. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.11.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monson, TC, Hesley, JW, Chernick, L. Specifying when personality traits can and cannot predict behavior: an alternative to abandoning the attempt to predict single-act criteria. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1982;43(2):385-399. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.43.2.385 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeDoux, JE, Pine, DS. Using neuroscience to help understand fear and anxiety: a two-system framework. Am J Psychiatry. 2016;173(11):1083-1093. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16030353 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warren, AM, Zolfaghari, K, Fresnedo, M, et al. Anxiety sensitivity, COVID-19 fear, and mental health: results from a United States population sample. Cogn Behav Ther. 2021;50(3):204-216. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2021.1874505 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, AH, Bogiaizian, D, Salazar, PL, et al. COVID-19 and anxiety sensitivity across two studies in Argentina: associations with COVID-19 worry, symptom severity, anxiety, and functional impairment. Cogn Ther Res. 2021;45(4):697-707. doi: 10.1007/s10608-020-10194-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brosch, K, Meller, T, Pfarr, J-K, et al. Which traits predict elevated distress during the Covid-19 pandemic? Results from a large, longitudinal cohort study with psychiatric patients and healthy controls. J Affect Disord. 2022;297:18-25. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.10.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alemany-Arrebola, I, Rojas-Ruiz, G, Granda-Vera, J, et al. Influence of COVID-19 on the perception of academic self-efficacy, state anxiety, and trait anxiety in college students. Front Psychol. 2020;11:500017. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.570017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pittig, A, Brand, M, Pawlikowski, M, et al. The cost of fear: avoidant decision making in a spider gambling task. J Anxiety Disord. 2014;28(3):326-334. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.03.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Overview of sociodemographic variables and mental health questionnaires, separated for national and international students

Figure 1

Table 2. Items for the COVID-inventory

Figure 2

Table 3. Parallel factor analysis merged for the COVID-Inventory and COVID-Behavior

Figure 3

Figure 1. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). The boxes on the bottom depict the observed variables (ie, the items of the 2 COVID-related questionnaires) with the intercepts and the variance (in grey). The circles depict the latent variables (ie, the factors). The solid black lines indicate the betas for each item in relation to the factors. The dotted lines on the top represent the covariance between the factors.

Figure 4

Figure 2. Results of the structural equation model (SEM). The boxes on the bottom and the top depict the questionnaires (and their sum scores). Blue lines depict negative predictions, while blue lines positive predictions. The factor perceived risk of infection (infection) was predicted by any questionnaire, and ASI predicted any factor. COVID-19-related worry (worry), physical (physical) and social (social) distancing were predicted negatively by trait anxiety (STAI X2) but positively by fear of contamination (FoC). Both body vigilance (BVS) and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) positively predicted safety behaviors, while state anxiety (STAI X1) predicted COVID-19-related worry and social distancing. COVID-19-related worry was additionally predicted by intolerance for uncertainty (IUS) and health anxiety (SHAI).

Figure 5

Figure 3. Results of the structural equation model (SEM) separately for the groups. The boxes depict the questionnaires (and their sum scores). Blue lines depict negative predictions, while black lines positive predictions. The factors were predicted by different questionnaires between the two groups. Health anxiety (SHAI) predicted any factor across Dutch students, while across international students the intolerance of uncertainty (IUS), depression (BDI), and body vigilance (BVS) were not significant predictors. Across international students, social distancing were exclusively predicted by fear of contamination (FoC), while across Dutch students this factor was additionally predicted by trait anxiety (STAI X2), BDI, and BVS. Physical distancing was predicted by STAI X2 and FoC in both groups, but among Dutch students the BDI was additionally predictive. Lastly, COVID-19-related worry was predicted in both groups by FoC, trait and state (STAI X1) anxiety, but for Dutch students the IUS additionally predicted the preoccupations for the virus, while across internationals the SHAI.