Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T09:53:28.005Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Echinococcus multilocularis infection affects risk-taking behaviour in Microtus arvalis: adaptive manipulation?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 May 2024

Matilde Martini
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, Ethology Unit, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy Department of Biological, BIOME Unit, Geological and Environmental Sciences, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
Teila Cioli
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, Ethology Unit, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
Thomas Romig
Affiliation:
Parasitology Unit, Institute of Biology, University of Hohenheim, Stuttgart, Germany
Anna Gagliardo
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, Ethology Unit, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
Dimitri Giunchi
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, Ethology Unit, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
Marco Zaccaroni
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
Alessandro Massolo*
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, Ethology Unit, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada UMR CNRS 6249 Chrono-environnement, Université Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
*
Corresponding author: Alessandro Massolo; Email: alessandro.massolo@unipi.it

Abstract

Manipulation of host behaviour by parasites to enhance transmission to the next host is a fascinating phenomenon that has interested scientists since the 1970s. It has been proposed that infection with the cestode Echinococcus multilocularis produces an impairment of the antipredatory behaviour in the rodent intermediate host common vole, Microtus arvalis, which may facilitate transmission of the tapeworm to the canid final host. In this study, we observed the behaviour of infected common voles at 12 weeks post-infection, when protoscoleces production and maturation commonly occurs, in order to assess behavioural changes compared to uninfected controls, that might ease predation in the wild. Infected and uninfected voles were monitored for 24 h to observe their spontaneous activity. In addition, the next day, both infected and uninfected voles were subjected to 4 different behavioural tests: open field test, barrier test, platform test and air-puff test in a running wheel. No significant difference between uninfected and infected voles emerged during the behavioural tests. However, observation of spontaneous activity revealed that infected voles increased their feeding frequency and spent significantly more time above bedding even when not eating, compared to the uninfected controls. In the wild, these behavioural changes increase the animals exposure to predators, raising their chance of becoming prey. These findings are the first direct evidence consistent with behavioural manipulation by E. multilocularis on common voles.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Understanding the transmission dynamics of zoonotic parasites is essential to design efficient public health control strategies. The relationship between a parasite and its host(s) has been the subject of many investigations. For some parasites completion of the life cycle occurs within a single host (simple life-cycle parasites; SLCs), whereas others need different host species (complex life-cycle parasites; CLCs) (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Renaud, Guégan and Thomas2001). For the latter, 1 or more hosts acts as Intermediate Host (IH), in which the parasite reproduces asexually, and one host becomes the Definitive Host (DH), in which the parasite reaches sexual maturity (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Renaud, Guégan and Thomas2001). For a CLC parasite, the most common form of host-to-host transmission is trophic, in which the prey plays the role of IH and the predator DH (Bethel and Holmes, Reference Bethel and Holmes1973). In the 20th century, the so-called ‘manipulation hypothesis’ was proposed to explain the evolution of the parasite–host relationship in some CLC parasites. This hypothesis predicts that the ability of a parasite to induce phenotypic alteration in the IH produces increased parasite transmission rates to the DH final host (Dobson, Reference Dobson1988; Freedman, Reference Freedman1990; Moore, Reference Moore2002). The ‘manipulation hypothesis’ is supported by several studies showing an advantage for the parasite presenting an infection-induced phenotypic alteration (e.g. behaviour, morphology and/or physiology) in the IH (Bethel and Holmes, Reference Bethel and Holmes1973; Lafferty and Morris, Reference Lafferty and Morris1996; Lefèvre et al., Reference Lefèvre, Lebarbenchon, Gauthier-Clerc, Missé, Poulin and Thomas2009).

Echinococcus multilocularis (Leuckart, 1863), the third most relevant human food-borne parasite in the world (FAO/WHO, Reference FAO/WHO2014), is a cestode presenting a CLC with IH-DH transmission through predator-prey relationship and could represent a model to study parasite transmission strategies. E. multilocularis is a gastrointestinal parasite whose adult develops in the small intestine of wild and domestic canids (e.g. coyote Canis latrans, red fox Vulpes vulpes, raccoon dog Nyctereutes procyonoides, golden jackal Canis aureus, wolf Canis lupus, domestic dog Canis lupus familiaris) and, to a lesser extent, in cats (Felis catus) (Romig et al., Reference Romig, Deplazes, Jenkins, Giraudoux, Massolo, Craig, Wassermann, Takahashi and De La Rue2017). In the intestinal lumen of the DH, the adult worm produces fertilized eggs which are shed in the environment with the host feces (Romig et al., Reference Romig, Deplazes, Jenkins, Giraudoux, Massolo, Craig, Wassermann, Takahashi and De La Rue2017). If accidentally ingested by a competent IH (mostly rodent), the eggs hatch in the host's gastrointestinal tract, releasing an oncosphere which migrates to the target organs, most frequently the liver, through the bloodstream of the portal vein (Eckert et al., Reference Eckert, Deplazes, Kern, Palmer, Soulsby, Torgerson and Brown2011). Once in the liver, the oncosphere develops into the metacestode (Heath and Lawrence, Reference Heath and Lawrence1976) which starts asexual multiplication, causing multilocular cyst-like lesions (Kamiya, Reference Kamiya2008) and produces the infectious larval stage: the protoscolex. The cycle is completed only when a competent predator consumes an infectious prey (i.e. with fertile lesions containing mature protoscoleces).

Although it has been hypothesized that E. multilocularis infected rodents might have a diminished ability to escape predation, so far there are no data supporting such a hypothesis (Vervaeke et al., Reference Vervaeke, Davis, Leirs and Verhagen2006). Therefore, clarifying the effects of E. multilocularis infection on IH anti-predatory behaviour may be a further key feature to consider in the epidemiology and transmission studies of this parasite. The overall aim of this study was to preliminarily investigate possible behavioural alterations caused by E. multilocularis in the common vole Microtus arvalis, a key host species for the maintenance of the parasitic cycle in Europe (see Oksanen et al., Reference Oksanen, Siles-Lucas, Karamon, Possenti, Conraths, Romig, Wysocki, Mannocci, Mipatrini and La Torre2016 and references therein). With this pilot study, we aimed to obtain the proof of concept to support the hypothesis of parasitic manipulation of IH behaviour in E. multilocularis so as to facilitate its transmission. Specifically, that if any behavioural alteration is detected, this should be in the direction of an increased risk to predation of infected animals compared to controls; this in the natural cycle would translate into increased transmission rate. Our working hypotheses were: (i) the parasite induces alteration of specific behavioural patterns of the IHs, facilitating its predation (behavioural alteration); (ii) this alteration occurs when the IH is infective (i.e. with viable protoscoleces larvae).

Materials and methods

Experimental animals

The experimental groups included 16 M. arvalis aged between 9 and 14 weeks, bred and kept in captivity at the Unit of Parasitology of the University of Hohenheim, Stuttgart, Germany. The animals were randomly assigned to 1 of 2 experimental groups: one (treated, T) consisted of 10 animals (5 males and 5 females) infected with 500 viable embryonated eggs of E. multilocularis, whereas the other (control, C) was made of 6 sham-treated animals (3 males and 3 females). One infected and 1 control animal died or had to be euthanized by CO2, according to legal requirements, before the experiment at 12 weeks post infection (w.p.i.). Therefore, the experiment was done with 14 animals: 9 infected (4 male, 5 female) and 5 control animals (2 male, 3 female). Animals were individually housed, under standard conditions of 12 hours of daylight and 12 hours of darkness (12-12 DL); Light (L): 7 a.m.-7 p.m.; 22°C; humidity 40–60%, in standard polycarbonate cages (Type III: 42.0 × 26.5 × 15.5 cm). Cages were provided with bedding (wood shavings and hay) that was changed once a week. Water and food pellets (pellet food for rats and mice; Altromin®, Germany, Lage) were available ad libitum.

Experimental infection

E. multilocularis eggs were obtained from adults collected from the intestines of naturally infected foxes hunted in Switzerland. Before inoculation, the viability of the eggs was tested using the ‘Sodium Hypochlorite Resistance’ test (Joekel and Deplazes, Reference Joekel and Deplazes2017), resulting in a viability of 12.5%. Each treated vole was infected with about 500 eggs by oral gavage, using a metal feeding needle, with the viable eggs suspended in 0.5 ml of PBS. The control animals were sham-inoculated with 0.5 ml of distilled water. After the infection, the voles were housed under BIO III security level for 3 days due to the risk of regurgitation of parasite eggs. After this period, the animals were brought back to the Animal Facility and monitored daily for post-inocula trauma or illness.

Behavioural observation and tests

All behavioural observation and tests were performed at 12 w.p.i., to allow for protoscolex (infectious larva stage) production and maturation (Woolsey et al., Reference Woolsey, Jensen, Deplazes and Kapel2015). Initially, we planned to carry out behavioural observations also at 18 w.p.i., but due to progressing disease several animals had to be euthanized and the remaining group size did not allow for adequate replication.

Prior to the experimental phase, the voles were observed for 24 hours to assess spontaneous behaviour in the cage. Afterwards, the animals were subjected to 4 behavioural tests: open field, barrier test, platform test and air puff test in a running wheel (Fig. 1). All tests targeted behaviours potentially associated with anti-predatory strategies. Both open field and barrier tests were conducted to observe exploratory and risk-taking behaviours. The platform test aimed at assessing an anti-predatory response. The observation in a running wheel system aimed at assessing the physical performance, whereas the air puff test, conducted when the animal was stationary in the running wheel, aimed to assess the vole reaction time to a physical stimulus. Many behavioural variables were chosen as recommended by a previous study of Herde and Eccard (Reference Herde and Eccard2013). All behavioural tests were carried out in succession, in a different room from the one in which the animals had been kept till then, but under the same conditions (12-12 DL, L: 7 a.m.-7 p.m.; 22 C° and 40–60% humidity). The animals were allowed to rest in a cage for 2 h between each trial. All tests were video-recorded, and operators left the test room during the trials.

Figure 1. Schematic drawing of the behavioural observation and tests conducted on 9–14 weeks old males and females Microtus arvalis to test behavioural manipulation by Echinococcus multilocularis infection. Structure of (A) Behavioural observation (24-h monitoring) apparatus; (B) Open field test; (C) Barrier test; (D) Platform test; (E) Running wheel and air puff test (APH, air puff holes). Treated group was subjected to oral injection of 500 E. multilocularis eggs (estimated viability of 12.5%), whereas control group was sham-inoculated with distilled water. Figure was created with BioRender.com.

Behavioural observation (24-h monitoring)

The spontaneous behaviour of treated and control voles was recorded for 24 h at 12 w.p.i. starting from 6 a.m. To increase the view of the animal in the home cage during the observations, the hay, but not the bedding, was removed from each cage 3 days before the start of monitoring, to accustom the animal to its absence. The 24-h observation was carried out using a night-vision surveillance video camera (Lorex, Flir 8-Channel 1080p DVR) per cage (Fig. 1A). The setup allowed monitoring a maximum of 8 home cages simultaneously, so 2 sessions were needed, 1 day apart. The monitoring was carried out under controlled conditions (12-12 DL, L: 7 a.m. –7 p.m.; 22°C, and 40–60% humidity). No operators entered the room during the 24 h of monitoring. The time spent above the bedding and the frequency and duration of eating and drinking events were recorded (see Table 1).

Table 1. List of tested behavioural variables for the in-cage monitoring and behavioural trials performed in the study conducted on 9–14 weeks old males and females M. arvalis to test behavioural alteration by E. multilocularis infection

Treated group was injected with 500 E. multilocularis eggs (estimated viability of 12.5%), whereas control group was sham-inoculated with distilled water. The variables reflect mainly anti-predatory behaviours and activity of the tested animals.

Open field test

According to Herde and Eccard (Reference Herde and Eccard2013), the open field test apparatus was built as an arena consisting of a round aluminium box (100 cm diameter) with an open top. The grey wall of the arena was 35 cm high, preventing any animals from escaping. A circle of 80 cm in diameter was drawn around the centre of the arena on the white floor of the arena so as to allow the identification of 2 areas: a safe peripheral zone near the outer wall (a 20 cm wide ring) and an unsafe zone (the centre of the arena; Figure 1B). Areas far from walls are known to be perceived as ‘unsafe’ for small mammals (Prior and Sachser, Reference Prior and Sachser1995). Each animal was transported in the test room in a white polycarbonate plastic tube (5.08 × 10.16 cm) and placed in the centre of the arena. The test lasted 10 min and started when the animal reached the arena wall for the first time. The time spent by the vole in each of the 2 areas was recorded (see Table 1).

Barrier test

The barrier test consisted of a semi-transparent plastic box (36 × 26 × 21 cm) with sterilized sand as bedding. The plastic box was divided into 2 equal compartments by a white, 4.5-cm high wood barrier (Fig. 1C). Each animal was carried to the test room in a white polycarbonate plastic tube (5.08 × 10.16 cm). The animal was randomly placed in 1 of the 2 compartments and the latency to cross the barrier the first time was measured. The duration of the test was 5 min. The other measured variables are reported in Table 1.

Platform test

The platform test was conducted in a Type IV cage (59 × 380 × 200 cm) with a small white platform (an overturned box of 30 × 17.5 × 6 cm) put in the centre of the home-cage filled with 3 cm of bedding layer (Fig. 1D). Each animal was transported in the test room in a white polycarbonate plastic tube (5.08 × 10.16 cm). The test started when the animal was placed in the centre of the platform and the latency to hide in the bedding was measured. The test ended after 3 min.

Running wheel and Air puff test

The apparatus used in this test consisted of a voluntary running wheel (11.5 cm diameter). The vole was forced to stay in the running wheel by 3 wooden walls (1 large wall measuring 17 × 14.5 cm, and 2 small walls measuring 11 × 14.5 cm) covered with black cardboard and 1 transparent plastic wall allowing video-monitoring during the test. The air puff was delivered through 2 holes (3 mm diameter) in the largest wooden wall located at 4.5 cm from the bottom of the wheel, so that the air puff stimulus (delivered from compressed air bottle, Air Duster 125 ml, Vivanco®, Germany, Ahrensburg) reached the right side of the vole (Fig. 1E). The number of rotations was recorded by a Liquid Crystal Display (LCD) activity counter (STARR®, life science group; United States, Oakmont) connected to the running wheel. In the described apparatus, the animal was forced to stay inside the wheel, but unlike the standard forced running system, it was free to stay still or move. Each animal was transported in the test room in a white polycarbonate plastic tube (5.08 × 10.16 cm). The test started when the animal was placed in the running wheel. The air puff was delivered when the vole was stationary. The animal received an air-puff stimulus of 1 sec after at least 5 min on the running wheel. The vole was video-monitored from the beginning of the test till 1 min after the stimulus application. The spontaneous activity in the running wheel before the stimulus application and the freezing time after the air puff were recorded. The variables measured are listed in Table 1.

Post-mortem analyses

All the experimental animals were sacrificed after 18 weeks by CO2, according to legal requirements, and subjected to a post-mortem visual inspection of the lesions. For estimating the number of mature protoscoleces in each subject the portion of the liver presenting lesions was cut off and homogenized using a pestle in 1 ml of distilled water. The homogenate was filtered through a nylon mesh of 1 mm pore size (the filter was washed with 1 ml of distilled water) onto a squared petri dish (12 × 12 cm) with a 5 × 5 mm squared grid on the bottom for a total of 576 cells. The protoscoleces inside the cells of the grid laying on both diagonals of the petri dish (48 cells) were manually counted under a microscope. An estimate of the number of protoscoleces was then computed by multiplying the average number of protoscoleces calculated for the inspected cells by the total number of cells.

Behaviour quantitative analysis

The videos were exported in .asf format and imported using Boris version 6.3.5 software (Friard and Gamba, Reference Friard and Gamba2016). Each video was checked at normal speed, and the total duration in seconds (state behaviour) and the occurrence of an event(s) of each variable (event behaviour) was considered. Software R 4.2.2 (R Core Team, 2022) was used for all data analyses. Each variable reported in Table 1 was compared between infected and control groups using a 2 sample bootstrap t-test (Efron and Tibshirani, Reference Efron, Tibshirani, Efron and Tibshirani1994) by means of the boot.t.test function in the package MKinfer (Kohl, Reference Kohl2019), setting the number of bootstrap samples = 10 000. This resampling approach was chosen as it is more robust and powerful with small and unbalanced samples, as well as for hypothesis testing for groups with different patterns of non-normality but equal variance (Dwivedi et al., Reference Dwivedi, Mallawaarachchi and Alvarado2017). Sex was excluded from the analysis because we did not expect a differential effect of infection on different sexes (Woolsey et al., Reference Woolsey, Jensen, Deplazes and Kapel2015). Furthermore, a Fisher exact test was performed to compare the frequency of infected and uninfected animals that showed spontaneous activity in the wheel, before the application of the air puff stimulus. The same test was also applied to compare the frequency of infected and uninfected vole behavioural reaction within 3 sec after the air puff stimulus application. We used an alpha level of 0.05 for all statistical tests.

Results

Behavioural observation (24-h monitoring)

During the 24-h in-cage monitoring period, infected voles spent, on average, 5.6 h more above bedding compared to intact voles (32.4% increase, T-voles = 22.93 ± 0.87 vs C-voles = 17.29 ± 2.61, P = 0.03; Table 2, Fig. 2B). Furthermore, infected voles showed a mean increase of 62.5 eating events per 24 h compared to control animals (41.37% increase, T-voles = 151.11 ± 15.52 vs C-voles = 88.60 ± 5.05; P = 0.002; Table 2, Fig. 2A).

Table 2. Results of the bootstrap t-test calculated on the variables measured in the behavioural tests on 9–14 weeks old males and females M. arvalis aimed at testing behavioural manipulation by E. multilocularis infection

Treated group was subjected to oral injection of 500 E. multilocularis eggs (estimated viability of 12.5%), whereas control group was sham-inoculated with distilled water. The bootstrap P value is based on 10 000 bootstrap replications. Significant P values (P < 0.05) are in bold. Difference of means = control group mean – treated group mean.

Figure 2. Results of 24-h behavioural observation conducted on 9–14 weeks old males and females Microtus arvalis to test behavioural manipulation by Echinococcus multilocularis infection. Boxplots of: (A) Eating events; (B) Time spent above bedding within 24 h (duration). Treated group (white colour) was injected with 500 E. multilocularis eggs (estimated viability of 12.5%), whereas control group (grey colour) was treated only with distilled water.

Behavioural tests

No significant differences between control and treated voles were found for any of the behavioural tests (open field, platform, barrier, running wheel) (Table 2). Moreover, in spontaneous running activity in the wheel, infected and uninfected voles did not show any significant difference (Fisher exact test, P > 0.05). In fact, all control voles displayed spontaneous running activity in the running wheel before the air puff test, as did 8 out of 9 infected voles. As regards the voles that engaged in some spontaneous running, we did not detect any significant difference between control and treated voles in the time they spent doing this activity and the running events (Table 2). The most common reaction to the air puff observed within 3 sec from the stimulus was a freezing reaction (3 control and 7 infected voles froze after the stimulus; Fisher exact test, P > 0.05). In addition, the time spent freezing in the 60 seconds after stimulus delivery did not differ in the 2 experimental groups (Table 2).

Post-mortem analyses

Seven of the infected voles were euthanized and necropsied at 18 w.p.i., 2 had to be euthanized and necropsied earlier due to disease progress in line with animals welfare requirements (13 w.p.i.). All infected voles had E. multilocularis lesions in the liver and mature protoscoleces. One animal necropsied at 13 w.p.i. presented 5 discrete lesions with 44 904 protoscoleces, 1 necropsied at 13 w.p.i. had 3 discrete lesions with 10 836 protoscoleces, while animals necropsied at 18 w.p.i. presented an average ± s.e. of 2.7 ± 0.9 lesions each with a mean ± s.e. of 113 773 ± 59 431 (min = 1342, max = 452 687) mature protoscoleces, N = 7. Lesions were present in all the liver lobes, with 9 lesions found in the Left Lateral Lobe, 4 in the Left Medial Lobe, 6 in the Right Medial, 7 in the Right Lateral Lobes and 1 in the Caudate Lobe. All control voles were euthanized 15–18 w.p.i., no lesions and protoscoleces were present in any of these animals.

Discussion

Our study aimed to assess whether and which behavioural alterations might occur following E. multilocularis infections in one of its IHs, M. arvalis. Specifically, we compared the behaviour of voles experimentally infected with E. multilocularis eggs to the behaviour of uninfected ones at 12 weeks post-infection.

Both infected and uninfected voles displayed comparable behaviours when tested in arenas, experimental cages, or running wheels. However, despite the small sample size, a clear difference emerged between the 2 groups in spontaneous behaviours in their home cage. In fact, we observed a significant increase in the time spent above bedding by infected animals compared to intact voles (Fig. 2B). This noteworthy result highlights that the condition of being infected may have affected the animals’ hiding behaviour, which is their spontaneous anti-predator response (Sundell and Ylönen, Reference Sundell and Ylönen2004), resulting in a significant increase in time spent in the open space of the cage. Moreover, the infected voles, when above bedding, fed more frequently compared to the controls (Fig. 2A). In the wild this behavioural alteration would translate to longer time spent in the open, outside the nest, and more frequent feeding with an increased likelihood of predation by a DH. This is consistent with the hypothesis that the parasite can induce behavioural modification, making the IH more susceptible to predation. This conclusion is in line with previous studies reporting that cestodes alter IH's activity by decreasing or increasing the host activity level depending on the species involved (Lafferty and Shaw, Reference Lafferty and Shaw2013). For instance, Echinococcus granulosus (sensu lato) infection may cause a moose physical impairment due to lung lesions induced by the parasite larval stage (Messier et al., Reference Messier, Rau and McNeill1989). This case can be explained by the side effect of being infected (Dantzer et al., Reference Dantzer, O'connor, Freund, Johnson and Kelley2008; Adamo, Reference Adamo2013) rather than a manipulation of the parasite on the IH. Differently, fishes infected by Schistocephalus solidus larval stages display increased surfacing activity, consequently increasing the probability of being detected by a predator (Øverli et al., Reference Øverli, Pall, Borg, Jobling and Winberg2001). In this case the effect of the parasite can be more plausibly explained by a specific manipulation aimed at increasing the transmission probability. This seems to be the same effect we observed in our voles infected by E. multilocularis. As a matter of fact, spontaneous activity in the running wheel (before the air puff test) did not differ between infected and uninfected voles, suggesting that the parasite did not seem to cause (at least up to 12 weeks from infection onset) a general debilitation of the voles. Furthermore, the freezing reaction, an anti-predatory strategy displayed by rodents, is triggered by traumatic or stressful experiences (Roelofs, Reference Roelofs2017). Both control and treated animals comparably showed freezing reaction to the air puff stimulus, suggesting that the parasite infection did not significantly alter fear elicited behavioural responses.

Although we did not measure the amount of food eaten by intact and infected animals when above bedding, it would not be surprising if the more frequent feeding of the infected voles resulted in greater food consumption. However, the observed increase in frequency of feeding might be consistent with 1 or 2 of two different mechanisms linked to an increase in energy demand in infected animals: the parasite's direct draw of energy from the host or the host's immune response to the infection (Schmid-Hempel, Reference Schmid-Hempel2005). Referring to the former mechanism, an alteration of the insulin pathway and, possibly, of the glucose levels might induce a nutritional needs impairment and an alteration of food intake requirements. It is known that E. multilocularis has receptors for insulin to target the liver as an optimal environment for the settlement and establishment of metacestodes (Hemer et al., Reference Hemer, Konrad, Spiliotis, Koziol, Schaack, Förster, Gelmedin, Stadelmann, Dandekar and Hemphill2014). However, it is unknown whether the parasite might have nutritional dependence on host glucose, as is the case of the trematode Schistosoma mansoni (Ahier et al., Reference Ahier, Khayath, Vicogne and Dissous2008) or what the role of insulin receptors in modulating the glucose parasite level could be (Brehm et al., Reference Brehm, Spiliotis, Zavala-Góngora, Konrad and Frosch2006). In addition, the time spent above bedding might be altered via physiological systems other than the digestive system. For instance, the parasite might increase the host oxygenation demand, as studied in swine affected by pulmonary metastrongylosis, a parasitosis caused by nematode Metastrongylus species (Pavlović et al., Reference Pavlović, Bojkovski, Zdravković, Radanović, Dobrosavljević, Stokić-Nikolić, Spalević, Jovčevski and Jovčevski2021). Another example of pulmonary debilitation which involves the helminth-amphibian host system, regards infected toads (Bufo bufo) which might have decreased lung function and, consequently, an altered oxygenation demand (Goater and Ward, Reference Goater and Ward1992). In our case, the altered oxygen demand can directly induce the vole to stay above bedding almost all the time. In this case, the increased feeding frequency might be caused simply by the continuous availability of food close to the animal. Another possibility could be that liver tissue damage (in this case induced by larvae proliferation) might cause a host blood intoxication (Lockwood et al., Reference Lockwood, McDonald, Reiman, Gelbard, Laughlin, Duffy and Plum1979). Toxicants in the blood, such as ammonia, might have a neurotoxic effect, impairing the activity of the IH, its reactivity to stimuli and mobility, thus making it more susceptible to predation (Ong et al., Reference Ong, Aggarwal, Krieger, Easley, Karafa, Van Lente, Arroliga and Mullen2003; Lockwood, Reference Lockwood2004). Our findings are not consistent with this hypothesis as the infected voles do not seem to be less mobile nor less reactive than control voles.

Despite the small sample size and the unclear physiological mechanisms underlying the increased above-bedding activity in E. multilocularis-infected IHs, the pilot study provided the first direct evidence of IH behavioural alteration following E. multilocularis infection. In particular, the parasite infection increased both the time spent above bedding as well as the time the voles spent feeding compared to the intact controls, suggesting a specific effect of the parasite on the activity of the IH. The study is intended as a starting point for investigating the effect of E. multilocularis on the physiology of rodent IHs.

Data availability statement

All data and code used for the analyses are available at GitHub repository link: https://github.com/MatildeUNIBO/ParManPilot.git.

Acknowledgements

We thank Dr Jens Jacob of the Julius Kühn-Institut for valuable suggestions for the study protocol and technical support. We thank Prof. Dr Jana Anja Eccard of the University of Potsdam for help in the lab for behavioural apparatus design. We thank Giulia Ferrucci for help in the lab. We thank the reviewers for helping with useful comments that improved the manuscript.

Author contributions

MM conceived and designed the study, conducted data gathering, performed statistical analyses and wrote the article, TC conducted data gathering and wrote the article, TR conceived and designed the study, reviewed and edited the article and supervised the project, AG designed the study, reviewed and edited the article, DG designed the study, performed statistical analyses, reviewed and edited the article, MZ reviewed and edited the article, AM conceived and designed the study, reviewed and edited the article and supervised and coordinated the project, acquired funding.

Financial support

This work was partially funded by PhD grant from the Natural Sciences & Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC300), project number: 10013904, project title: ‘The ecology of parasites with complex lifecycles in multi-host systems: Echinococcus multilocularis transmission dynamics at multiple spatial scales’.

Competing interests

None.

Ethical standards

Our study was done with animals that had been infected with E. multilocularis for other purposes (in vivo propagation of Echinococcus, permit no. A391/14 PA, Regierungspräsidium Stuttgart, Germany). The behavioural tests were approved by the animal welfare commission of the Regierungspräsidium Stuttgart, Germany on 10.12.2018, permit no. V350/18 PA.

References

Adamo, SA (2013) Parasites: evolution's neurobiologists. Journal of Experimental Biology 216, 310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahier, A, Khayath, N, Vicogne, J and Dissous, C (2008) Insulin receptors and glucose uptake in the human parasite Schistosoma mansoni. Parasite 15, 573579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bethel, WM and Holmes, JC (1973) Altered evasive behavior and responses to light in amphipods harboring acanthocephalan cystacanths. The Journal of Parasitology 59, 945956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehm, K, Spiliotis, M, Zavala-Góngora, R, Konrad, C and Frosch, M (2006) The molecular mechanisms of larval cestode development: first steps into an unknown world. Parasitology International 55, S15S21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, S, Renaud, F, Guégan, JF and Thomas, F (2001) Evolution of trophic transmission in parasites: the need to reach a mating place? Journal of Evolutionary Biology 14, 815820.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dantzer, R, O'connor, JC, Freund, GG, Johnson, RW and Kelley, KW (2008) From inflammation to sickness and depression: when the immune system subjugates the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9, 4656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dobson, AP (1988) The population biology of parasite-induced changes in host behavior. The Quarterly Review of Biology 63, 139165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dwivedi, AK, Mallawaarachchi, I and Alvarado, LA (2017) Analysis of small sample size studies using nonparametric bootstrap test with pooled resampling method. Statistics in Medicine 36, 21872205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eckert, J, Deplazes, P and Kern, P (2011) Alveolar echinococcosis (Echinococcus multilocularis) and neotropical forms of echinococcosis (Echinococcus vogeli and Echinococcus oligarthrus). In Palmer, S, Soulsby, L, Torgerson, PR and Brown, D (eds), Oxford Textbook of Zoonoses Biology, Clinical Practice, and Public Health Control. United States of America: Oxford University Press, pp. 669699.Google Scholar
Efron, B and Tibshirani, RJ (1994) An introduction to the bootstrap. In Efron, B and Tibshirani, RJ (eds), 1st Edn. New York: Chapman and Hall/CRC, 456 pp.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FAO/WHO, (2014) Multicriteria-based Ranking for Risk Management of Food-Borne Parasites: Report of A Joint FAO. Rome, Italy: FAO, World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Freedman, H (1990) A model of predator-prey dynamics as modified by the action of a parasite. Mathematical Biosciences 99, 143155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friard, O and Gamba, M (2016) BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 7, 13251330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goater, C and Ward, P (1992) Negative effects of Rhabdias bufonis (Nematoda) on the growth and survival of toads (Bufo bufo). Oecologia 89, 161165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heath, D and Lawrence, S (1976) Echinococcus granulosus: development in vitro from oncosphere to immature hydatid cyst. Parasitology 73, 417423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hemer, S, Konrad, C, Spiliotis, M, Koziol, U, Schaack, D, Förster, S, Gelmedin, V, Stadelmann, B, Dandekar, T and Hemphill, A (2014) Host insulin stimulates Echinococcus multilocularis insulin signalling pathways and larval development. BMC Biology 12, 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herde, A and Eccard, JA (2013) Consistency in boldness, activity and exploration at different stages of life. BMC Ecology 13, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joekel, DE and Deplazes, P (2017) Optimized dexamethasone immunosuppression enables Echinococcus multilocularis liver establishment after oral egg inoculation in a rat model. Experimental Parasitology 180, 2732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamiya, M (2008) Echinococcosis/hydatidosis. In OIE (ed.), Manual of Diagnostic Tests and Vaccines for Terrestrial Animals: Mammals, Birds and Bees. 6th Edn. Paris: OIE, pp. 175189.Google Scholar
Kohl, M (2019) MKinfer: Inferential Statistics. R package version 0.9. Available at: https://www.stamats.de. 2020Google Scholar
Lafferty, KD and Morris, AK (1996) Altered behavior of parasitized killifish increases susceptibility to predation by bird final hosts. Ecology 77, 13901397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lafferty, KD and Shaw, JC (2013) Comparing mechanisms of host manipulation across host and parasite taxa. Journal of Experimental Biology 216, 5666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lefèvre, T, Lebarbenchon, C, Gauthier-Clerc, M, Missé, D, Poulin, R and Thomas, F (2009) The ecological significance of manipulative parasites. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 24, 4148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lockwood, AH (2004) Blood ammonia levels and hepatic encephalopathy. Metabolic Brain Disease 19, 345349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lockwood, AH, McDonald, JM, Reiman, RE, Gelbard, AS, Laughlin, J, Duffy, TE and Plum, F (1979) The dynamics of ammonia metabolism in man. Effects of liver disease and hyperammonemia. The Journal of Clinical Investigation 63, 449460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Messier, F, Rau, ME and McNeill, MA (1989) Echinococcus granulosus (Cestoda: Taeniidae) infections and moose–wolf population dynamics in southwestern Quebec. Canadian Journal of Zoology 67, 216219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, J (2002) Parasites and the Behavior of Animals. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oksanen, A, Siles-Lucas, M, Karamon, J, Possenti, A, Conraths, FJ, Romig, T, Wysocki, P, Mannocci, A, Mipatrini, D and La Torre, G (2016) The geographical distribution and prevalence of Echinococcus multilocularis in animals in the European Union and adjacent countries: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Parasites & Vectors 9, 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ong, JP, Aggarwal, A, Krieger, D, Easley, KA, Karafa, MT, Van Lente, F, Arroliga, AC and Mullen, KD (2003) Correlation between ammonia levels and the severity of hepatic encephalopathy. The American Journal of Medicine 114, 188193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Øverli, Ø, Pall, M, Borg, B, Jobling, M and Winberg, S (2001) Effects of Schistocephalus solidus infection on brain monoaminergic activity in female three-spined sticklebacks Gasterosteus aculeatus. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 268, 14111415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavlović, I, Bojkovski, J, Zdravković, N, Radanović, O, Dobrosavljević, I, Stokić-Nikolić, S, Spalević, L, Jovčevski, S and Jovčevski, S (2021) The role of parasitic infections in the development of respiratory diseases in swine. Scientific Papers Journal 64, 59.Google Scholar
Prior, H and Sachser, N (1995) Effects of enriched housing environment on the behaviour of young male and female mice in four exploratory tasks. Journal of Experimental Animal Science (Germany) 37, 5768.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2022) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Roelofs, K (2017) Freeze for action: neurobiological mechanisms in animal and human freezing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 372, 20160206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romig, T, Deplazes, P, Jenkins, D, Giraudoux, P, Massolo, A, Craig, PS, Wassermann, M, Takahashi, K and De La Rue, M (2017) Chapter five – Ecology and life cycle patterns of Echinococcus species. Advances in Parasitology 95, 213314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid-Hempel, P (2005) Evolutionary ecology of insect immune defenses. Annual Review of Entomology 50, 529551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sundell, J and Ylönen, H (2004) Behaviour and choice of refuge by voles under predation risk. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 56, 263269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vervaeke, M, Davis, S, Leirs, H and Verhagen, R (2006) Implications of increased susceptibility to predation for managing the sylvatic cycle of Echinococcus multilocularis. Parasitology 132, 893901.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woolsey, ID, Jensen, PM, Deplazes, P and Kapel, CMO (2015) Establishment and development of Echinococcus multilocularis metacestodes in the common vole (Microtus arvalis) after oral inoculation with parasite eggs. Parasitology International 64, 571575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Schematic drawing of the behavioural observation and tests conducted on 9–14 weeks old males and females Microtus arvalis to test behavioural manipulation by Echinococcus multilocularis infection. Structure of (A) Behavioural observation (24-h monitoring) apparatus; (B) Open field test; (C) Barrier test; (D) Platform test; (E) Running wheel and air puff test (APH, air puff holes). Treated group was subjected to oral injection of 500 E. multilocularis eggs (estimated viability of 12.5%), whereas control group was sham-inoculated with distilled water. Figure was created with BioRender.com.

Figure 1

Table 1. List of tested behavioural variables for the in-cage monitoring and behavioural trials performed in the study conducted on 9–14 weeks old males and females M. arvalis to test behavioural alteration by E. multilocularis infection

Figure 2

Table 2. Results of the bootstrap t-test calculated on the variables measured in the behavioural tests on 9–14 weeks old males and females M. arvalis aimed at testing behavioural manipulation by E. multilocularis infection

Figure 3

Figure 2. Results of 24-h behavioural observation conducted on 9–14 weeks old males and females Microtus arvalis to test behavioural manipulation by Echinococcus multilocularis infection. Boxplots of: (A) Eating events; (B) Time spent above bedding within 24 h (duration). Treated group (white colour) was injected with 500 E. multilocularis eggs (estimated viability of 12.5%), whereas control group (grey colour) was treated only with distilled water.