Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T06:50:04.719Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Polygenic risk for ADHD and ASD and their relation with cognitive measures in school children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 September 2020

Sofía Aguilar-Lacasaña
Affiliation:
University of Vic – Central University of Catalonia (UVIC-UCC), Vic, Spain Barcelona Research Institute for Global Health (ISGlobal), Barcelona, Spain
Natàlia Vilor-Tejedor
Affiliation:
Centre for Genomic Regulation (CRG), The Barcelona Institute of Science and Technology, Barcelona, Spain Barcelona Beta Brain Research Center (BBBRC), Pasqual Maragall Foundation, Barcelona, Spain Department of Clinical Genetics, ERASMUS MC, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Philip R. Jansen
Affiliation:
Department of Complex Trait Genetics, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research, Amsterdam Neuroscience, VU University and Department of Clinical Genetics, Amsterdam UMC - location VUmc, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry/Psychology, Erasmus University Medical Center, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Mònica López-Vicente
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry/Psychology, Erasmus University Medical Center, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Mariona Bustamante
Affiliation:
Barcelona Research Institute for Global Health (ISGlobal), Barcelona, Spain Centre for Genomic Regulation (CRG), The Barcelona Institute of Science and Technology, Barcelona, Spain Universitat Pompeu Fabra (UPF), Barcelona, Spain CIBER Epidemiología y Salud Pública (CIBERESP), Spain
Miguel Burgaleta
Affiliation:
Department of Technology, Center for Brain and Cognition, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Roc Boronat 138, 08018 Barcelona, Spain
Jordi Sunyer
Affiliation:
Barcelona Research Institute for Global Health (ISGlobal), Barcelona, Spain Universitat Pompeu Fabra (UPF), Barcelona, Spain CIBER Epidemiología y Salud Pública (CIBERESP), Spain IMIM (Hospital del Mar Medical Research Institute), Barcelona, Spain
Silvia Alemany*
Affiliation:
Barcelona Research Institute for Global Health (ISGlobal), Barcelona, Spain Universitat Pompeu Fabra (UPF), Barcelona, Spain CIBER Epidemiología y Salud Pública (CIBERESP), Spain
*
Author for correspondence: Silvia Alemany, E-mail: silvia.alemany1983@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are child-onset neurodevelopmental disorders frequently accompanied by cognitive difficulties. In the current study, we aim to examine the genetic overlap between ADHD and ASD and cognitive measures of working memory (WM) and attention performance among schoolchildren using a polygenic risk approach.

Methods

A total of 1667 children from a population-based cohort aged 7–11 years with data available on genetics and cognition were included in the analyses. Polygenic risk scores (PRS) were calculated for ADHD and ASD using results from the largest GWAS to date (N = 55 374 and N = 46 351, respectively). The cognitive outcomes included verbal and numerical WM and the standard error of hit reaction time (HRTSE) as a measure of attention performance. These outcomes were repeatedly assessed over 1-year period using computerized version of the Attention Network Test and n-back task. Associations were estimated using linear mixed-effects models.

Results

Higher polygenic risk for ADHD was associated with lower WM performance at baseline time but not over time. These findings remained significant after adjusting by multiple testing and excluding individuals with an ADHD diagnosis but were limited to boys. PRS for ASD was only nominally associated with an increased improvement on verbal WM over time, although this association did not survive multiple testing correction. No associations were observed for HRTSE.

Conclusions

Common genetic variants related to ADHD may contribute to worse WM performance among schoolchildren from the general population but not to the subsequent cognitive-developmental trajectory assessed over 1-year period.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are complex neurodevelopmental disorders that emerge during childhood (Lord, Elsabbagh, Baird, & Veenstra-Vanderweele, Reference Lord, Elsabbagh, Baird and Veenstra-Vanderweele2018; Thapar & Cooper, Reference Thapar and Cooper2016). Several aspects are common to both conditions including early onset, delays and alterations in brain development, a male preponderance and cognitive difficulties (Visser, Rommelse, Greven, & Buitelaar, Reference Visser, Rommelse, Greven and Buitelaar2016).

In this regard, cognitive performance of individuals with ADHD has been found to be consistently lower compared to controls for several domains such as working memory (WM) or variability in reaction time (RT), which reflects attentional lapses (Karalunas, Geurts, Konrad, Bender, & Nigg, Reference Karalunas, Geurts, Konrad, Bender and Nigg2014; Pievsky & McGrath, Reference Pievsky and McGrath2018). Remarkably, cognitive performance has been related to later symptom severity and functional outcomes among ADHD patients in a longitudinal study (van Lieshout et al., Reference van Lieshout, Luman, Twisk, Faraone, Heslenfeld, Hartman and Oosterlaan2017). Furthermore, cognitive deficits are related to ADHD symptoms beyond clinical status. In a population-based sample study, inattention symptoms were related to deficits in WM (Tillman, Eninger, Forssman, & Bohlin, Reference Tillman, Eninger, Forssman and Bohlin2011). In the case of ASD, although some individuals exhibit cognitive strengths in certain domains (Iuculano et al., Reference Iuculano, Rosenberg-Lee, Supekar, Lynch, Khouzam, Phillips and Menon2014), impairments have been reported in functions such as WM or attention (Boxhoorn et al., Reference Boxhoorn, Lopez, Schmidt, Schulze, Hänig and Freitag2018; Karalunas et al., Reference Karalunas, Hawkey, Gustafsson, Miller, Langhorst, Cordova and Nigg2018; Velikonja, Fett, & Velthorst, Reference Velikonja, Fett and Velthorst2019; Willcutt, Sonuga-Barke, Nigg, & Sergeant, Reference Willcutt, Sonuga-Barke, Nigg and Sergeant2008).

Family and twin studies indicate that common familial/genetic factors are underlying these cognitive functions, ADHD and possibly ASD (Cheung, Fazier-Wood, Asherson, Rijsdijk, & Kuntsi, Reference Cheung, Fazier-Wood, Asherson, Rijsdijk and Kuntsi2014; Frazier-Wood et al., Reference Frazier-Wood, Bralten, Arias-Vasquez, Luman, Ooterlaan, Sergeant and Rommelse2012; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Cheung, Kitsune, Brandeis, Banaschewski, McLoughlin and Kuntsi2018). In line with these findings, it has been suggested that cognitive difficulties, social-communication behavioral traits and ADHD symptoms are underpinned at a biological level, which may involve genetic variants related to these disorders (Martin, Hamshere, Stergiakouli, O'Donovan, & Thapar, Reference Martin, Hamshere, Stergiakouli, O'Donovan and Thapar2015). This can be examined by computing polygenic risk scores which index genetic susceptibility for a given disorder (Wray et al., Reference Wray, Lee, Mehta, Vinkhuyzen, Dudbridge and Middeldorp2014). Polygenic risk scores can then be used to assess shared genetic influences across different traits (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Mak and O’Reilly2020).

So far, a few studies have used polygenic risk scores to investigate the potential genetic overlap between ADHD and ASD and cognitive deficits (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Lupton, Fernandez-Pujals, Starr, Davies, Cox and McIntosh2016; Du Rietz et al., Reference Du Rietz, Coleman, Glanville, Choi, O'Reilly and Kuntsi2018; Schork et al., Reference Schork, Brown, Hagler, Thompson, Chen, Dale and Akshoomoff2019; Stergiakouli et al., Reference Stergiakouli, Martin, Hamshere, Heron, Pourcain, Timpson and Smith2017). A study investigating polygenic risk for ASD and ADHD in a large population-based sample found that ASD-related variants may confer cognitive advantages, while the polygenic risk for ADHD did not show a consistent pattern of results for the cognitive domains analyzed (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Lupton, Fernandez-Pujals, Starr, Davies, Cox and McIntosh2016). Another study also found evidence suggesting that polygenic risk for autism is related with better performance in executive function (N = 417) but these findings were not replicated in an independent sample (N = 3681) (Schork et al., Reference Schork, Brown, Hagler, Thompson, Chen, Dale and Akshoomoff2019). Other studies in children, adolescents and adults showed negative associations between polygenic risk for ADHD and cognitive and school performance (Du Rietz et al., Reference Du Rietz, Coleman, Glanville, Choi, O'Reilly and Kuntsi2018; Martin et al., Reference Martin, Hamshere, Stergiakouli, O'Donovan and Thapar2015; Stergiakouli et al., Reference Stergiakouli, Martin, Hamshere, Heron, Pourcain, Timpson and Smith2017). Thus, it is still not clear whether the manifestation of polygenic risk for these neurodevelopmental conditions involves cognitive deficits in population-based samples. Furthermore, none of these studies investigated attention performance, a core aspect of ADHD (Barkley, Reference Barkley1997).

In the current study, we examined whether polygenic risk for ADHD and ASD was associated with baseline and 1-year cognitive-developmental trajectories of measures of WM and attention performance among school-aged children from the general population.

Material and methods

Study population

Participants were drawn from the BREATHE project (European Commission: FP7-ERC-2010-AdG, ID 268479), a population-based cohort of primary school-aged children designed to analyze associations between air pollution and behavior, cognitive abilities and brain structure and function. A full description of the project is available elsewhere (Sunyer et al., Reference Sunyer, Esnaola, Alvarez-Pedrerol, Forns, Rivas, L??pez-Vicente and Querol2015). The project was conducted in 39 schools in or near Barcelona (Spain). All families of children without special needs who were enrolled in second, third and fourth grades at the selected schools were invited to participate in the study. A total of 2897 children participated in the project for which genotype data was available in 1667 children of European ancestry.

All parents or legal guardians gave written informed consent, and the study was approved by the IMIM-Parc de Salut Mar Research Ethics Committee (No. 2010/41,221/I), Barcelona, Spain; and the FP7-ERC-2010-AdG Ethics Review Committee (268,479–22,022,011).

Cognitive measures

The outcomes of the study were WM and attention performance. WM was assessed using the n-back task (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Monk, Lin, Carver, Thomas and Truwit2000). A full description of the task can be found in Methods S2. Briefly, in the n-back task, a series of stimuli are presented individually at the center of the screen. Participants were required to monitor those items and indicate whenever the stimulus matched the one presented 1, 2 or 3 stimuli back, also known as back loads. Here, we used numbers and words as stimuli in the 3-back level and we calculated d prime (d′) as an indicator of WM accuracy. These measures, 3-back numbers d′ and 3-back words d′ were treated in the analysis as indicators of verbal and numerical WM performance when using words and numbers, respectively, as stimuli. These measures were analyzed as continuous variables. Higher d′ scores indicated more accuracy and thus, better performance.

Attention performance was assessed using the computerized version of the Attention Network Test [ANT, (Rueda et al., Reference Rueda, Fan, McCandliss, Halparin, Gruber, Lercari and Posner2004)]. A full description of the test can be found in Methods S1. Briefly, reaction time measures (i.e. the time between the introduction of a stimulus and the reaction on the subject to that stimulus) were used to calculate the different outcomes that can be obtained with the ANT. The outcome analyzed herein corresponds to the hit reaction time standard error (HRTSE) (standard error of reaction time for correct responses), a measure of intraindividual variability reflecting response speed and consistency throughout the test. HRTSE was analyzed as a continuous variable with lower scores indicating better performance.

The participants completed these tests repeatedly through four different sessions (every 3 months) over 1 year. We used these four repeated measures to model the 1-year developmental trajectories of verbal and numerical WM and HRTSE. The modeled 1-year trajectory may include 1–4 repeated measures of the cognitive measures based on available data.

Behavioral measures

To validate the PRS in our cohort, behavioral data were used to test the association between the PRS and the phenotype. Behavioral measures were obtained at the beginning of the cognitive data collection (visit 1, baseline). ADHD symptoms were assessed using a questionnaire based on the ADHD diagnostic criteria described in Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th edition [DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association., 2002)], and was completed by teachers. The ADHD-DSM-IV questionnaire consists of a list of 18 symptoms, assessing two separate symptom groups: inattention (nine symptoms) and hyperactivity/impulsivity (nine symptoms). Each ADHD symptom is rated on a 4-point frequency scale from never or rarely (0) to very often (3). The ADHD symptom score ranges from 0 to 54, with higher scores indicating more symptoms. ASD symptoms were not collected in this project.

Genotyping

DNA samples were obtained from saliva and genome-wide genotyping was performed using the HumanCore BeadChipWG-330-1,101 (Illumina). A full description of the genotyping and quality control procedures is available elsewhere (Alemany et al., Reference Alemany, Vilor-Tejedor, Bustamante, Pujol, Macià, Martínez-Vilavella and Sunyer2016) and summarized in Methods S3.

Polygenic scoring

Genotyped data that passed quality control were used to compute PRS using PRSice (Euesden, Lewis, & O'Reilly, Reference Euesden, Lewis and O'Reilly2015). PRSice calculates individual polygenic scores by summing up all the SNP alleles carried by the participants weighted by the SNP allele effect size estimated in a previous GWAS. PRS based on five different p value thresholds (PT) for SNP inclusion PT = {0.01, 0.05, 0.1, 0.5, 1} were calculated for each disorder. Polygenic scoring was performed in clumped variants (representative genetic variants per linkage disequilibrium block) using r 2 > 0.1 as a cut-off for LD independent markers in a 250-kb window. This means that SNPs with the smallest p value in each 250 kb window is retained and all those in LD (r 2 > 0.1) with these SNPs are removed. All PRS were standardized to a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1. The number of variants included in the PRS for each PT can be found in online Supplementary Table S1.

PRS were based on the most recent ASD and ADHD GWAS. The ASD GWAS included 46 351 individuals (18 382 cases) of European ancestry and 9 112 387 SNPs (Grove et al., Reference Grove, Ripke, Als, Mattheisen, Walters, Won and Børglum2019). The ADHD GWAS included 53 293 individuals (19 099 cases) of European ancestry and 8 047 421 SNPs (Demontis et al., Reference Demontis, Walters, Martin, Mattheisen, Als, Agerbo and Neale2019).

Statistical analysis

The final data included 1667 children with complete data on PRS, verbal WM (N = 1504, representing 6130 observations), numerical WM (N = 1490, representing 6100 observations) and HRTSE (N = 1487, representing 6177 observations) (Fig. 1). Of these, 80% had data on at least three of the four repeated assessments. To validate the PRS, we tested the association between the PRS and the behavioral manifestation of ADHD. We estimated the associations between PRS for ADHD across all significance thresholds and the behavioral measure using linear mixed-effects models with the school as nested random effect. A total of 1555 individuals were included in these analyses.

Fig. 1. Flow chart depicting the final sample size for the outcomes analyzed including hit reaction time (HRT-SE) of ANT as a measure of HRTSE and d′ values from the 3-back task with words and numbers as stimuli as measures of WM. Solid lines and boxes represent individuals remaining in the study, dashed lines and boxes represent individuals excluded. Reason and number of individuals excluded are indicated in dashed boxes.

We tested the cross-sectional association between polygenic risk for ADHD (PRS-ADHD) and ASD (PRS-ASD) and cognitive measures using linear mixed-effects models with the school as a nested random effect. These associations were tested using the cognitive outcomes assessed at baseline. The baseline measures correspond to those obtained in visit 1 when participants completed the cognitive tasks for the first time.

In the validation and cross-sectional analysis, we included schools as nested random effects to account for the multilevel nature of the data (i.e. children within schools).

We analyzed the association between PRS-ADHD and PRS-ASD and changes in cognitive measures repeatedly assessed over 1-year period (1-year trajectories) using linear mixed-effects models with individual children nested within schools as random effects. An interaction between age (centered at visit 1) at each visit and the PRS was included to capture changes in 1-year trajectory associated with polygenic risk for ADHD or ASD as shown below.

$$ \hskip-40pt\eqalign{ {Y_{sit} = & \;\beta _{0\;} + \beta _1PRS + \beta _2( {Age_t-Age_1} ) + \beta _3( {Age_t-Age_1} ) \ast PRS} \hskip-17pc \cr & \hskip-4pc\; {+ \beta _4Sex + \beta _5PC1 + \beta _6PC2 + \beta _7PC3 + \beta _8PC4 + u_s} \cr \quad+ v_{i( s ) } + \varepsilon _{sit}}$$

where Ysit is the test score of the cognitive measure for the individual i in school s at visit t, t = {1,2,3,4}; us is random effects at the school level, vi (s) is random effects associated with individual i in school s and εsit is the model residuals. Only participants with data on at least two visits (representing around 85% of the sample) were included in the analyses of 1-year trajectories. Thus, the modeled 1-year trajectory may include two–four repeated measures of the cognitive outcomes per individual. Of note, around 73% of the participants have data for the four visits. Previous research in the BREATHE entire sample (n = 2897) have shown age-related growth patterns on both WM and ANT measures over the 1-year period (López-Vicente et al., Reference López-Vicente, Forns, Suades-González, Esnaola, García-Esteban, Álvarez-Pedrerol and Sunyer2016; Suades-González et al., Reference Suades-González, Forns, García-Esteban, López-Vicente, Esnaola, Álvarez-Pedrerol and Sunyer2017). These findings suggest that the repeated measures through the year were capturing developmental trajectories where the accuracy of the tasks improved with age. These trajectories are likely to reflect cognitive maturation processes rather than practice effects because no important differences were observed in the median scores of children at same age assessed in different sessions (López-Vicente et al., Reference López-Vicente, Forns, Suades-González, Esnaola, García-Esteban, Álvarez-Pedrerol and Sunyer2016; Mollica, Maruff, Collie, & Vance, Reference Mollica, Maruff, Collie and Vance2005).

Because ADHD and ASD present higher prevalence among boys (Lord et al., Reference Lord, Elsabbagh, Baird and Veenstra-Vanderweele2018; Polanczyk, De Lima, Horta, Biederman, & Rohde, Reference Polanczyk, De Lima, Horta, Biederman and Rohde2007), we explored PRS interaction by sex by fitting an interaction term in each model. Stratified models by sex are presented when significant interactions between PRS and sex were detected.

In addition, we performed sensitivity analyses, in which we excluded individuals with ADHD diagnosis made by a medical doctor. Clinical status of other psychiatric disorders was not collected in the study.

In all models, PRS were the determinant and cognitive measures were the outcome. Cognitive measures included verbal and numerical WM and HRTSE. All models were adjusted by sex, age and the first four genetic principal components. One model for each PT was constructed.

Effect sizes were reported as beta coefficients throughout the results. Baseline and trajectory analysis were corrected for multiple testing across all PRS, generated at five different significance thresholds, tested for association with three cognitive outcomes using the false discovery rate (FDR) method (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995). Results at p uncorrected <0.05 were considered nominally significant and results at p FDR<0.05 were considered statistically significant.

All statistical analyses were conducted using the R statistical software (version 3.6.0).

Results

Descriptive results

Descriptive characteristics of the subjects included in the analyses are presented in Table 1. The mean age of the sample was 8.5 ± 0.9 years old, 46.9% were female and 61.4% of the mothers have higher education (Table 1). In comparison with the participants of the BREATHE project for whom genetic data were not available (n = 1230), children who were included were significantly more likely to be male and presented better verbal and numerical WM performance as well as less HRTSE. Regarding behavioral measures, children included had lower inattention and total ADHD symptoms. There were no significant differences in terms of age or hyperactivity symptoms (online Supplementary Table S2). Boys performed better for numerical WM and HRTSE measures and presented higher total ADHD, inattention and hyperactivity symptoms (online Supplementary Table S3).

Table 1. Characteristics of the sample

NOTE: ADHD total symptoms score, teacher-reported attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms where higher scores indicate more symptoms. HRTSE, standard error of the hit reaction time obtained from the Attentional Network Test, higher scores indicate worse attention performance. WM, working memory performance (d’ values) from the 3-back task of the n-back test with words (Verbal) and numbers (Numerical) as stimuli, higher values indicate better WM.

a Baseline (visit 1).

Online Supplementary Fig. S1 shows the correlations between cognitive measures in each visit and online Supplementary Fig. S2 summarizes the correlations between the PRS for ADHD and ASD. Measures within each domain (i.e. WM and HRTSE) were positively correlated across the four visits. WM and HRTSE measures were negatively correlated, which indicates that better performance in the first domain (high d′) is related to better performance in the second (low HRT-SE).

Validation using behavioral measures

Polygenic risk for ADHD was positively associated with ADHD symptoms total score and hyperactivity at all significance thresholds. Positive associations between polygenic risk for ADHD and inattention were observed at all significance thresholds but were not significant at PT <0.01 (online Supplementary Table S4).

Cross-sectional association between PRS and cognitive performance

PRS for ADHD were negatively associated with verbal WM across all significance thresholds with the strongest association observed at PT = 1 (β = −0.086, 95% CI = −0.139, −0.033; p-uncorrec. = 0.002, p-FDR = 0.026) (Table 2). All the associations remained significant after FDR-correction except for the association with PT = 0.05. Associations between PRS for ADHD and numerical WM were also negative and nominally significant at all significance thresholds except at PT <0.01. Only the association between PRS for ADHD at PT <0.05 survived FDR-correction (β = −0.078, 95% CI = −0.131, −0.025; p-uncorrec. = 0.004, p-FDR = 0.028).

Table 2. Association results between polygenic risk scores (PRS) for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) with cognitive measures at baseline adjusting by age, sex and the first four genetic principal components

NOTE: PT, significance threshold for inclusion of variants in the polygenic score; CI, confidence interval; p-uncorr., uncorrected p value; p-FDR, false discovery rate adjusted p value; ΔR 2, difference between the R 2 of the full model (PRS + covariates) compared to the R 2 of the model including only covariates.

Polygenic risk for ASD did not show significant associations with WM measures. Although not significant, a consistent pattern of results was observed suggesting positive associations between PRS for ASD and verbal and numerical WM performance (Table 2).

No significant associations were found between PRS for ADHD or ASD and HRTSE (Table 2).

Significant interactions between PRS for ADHD and sex were observed on verbal (p value for interaction for PT <0.5 and PT <1 were 0.028 and 0.039, respectively) and numerical WM (p value for interaction for PT <0.01 and PT <0.05 were 0.038 and 0.028, respectively) indicating that negative associations between PRS for ADHD and WM measures were stronger among boys (online Supplementary Table S5). Stratified results revealed that significant associations were limited to boys (online Supplementary Table S5).

Association between PRS and cognitive performance over 1-year period

No significant associations at p-FDR<0.05 were observed between PRS for ADHD or ASD and WM or HTRSE measures modeled as 1-year trajectories (Table 3).

Table 3. Association results between polygenic risk scores (PRS) for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) with cognitive 1-year trajectories adjusting by age, sex and the first four genetic principal components

NOTE: PT, significance threshold for inclusion of variants in the polygenic score; CI, confidence interval; p-uncorr., uncorrected p value; p-FDR, false discovery rate adjusted p value; the R 2 of the full model (PRS + covariates) compared to the R 2 of the model including only covariates.

A nominally significant association was observed between PRS for ASD at PT < 1 and verbal WM (β = 0.041, 95% CI = 0.004, 0.077; p-uncorrec. = 0.032, p-FDR = 0.497) suggesting that higher polygenic load for ASD is related to increased cognitive growth over the 1-year period (online Supplementary Fig. S3). Individuals with higher polygenic risk for ASD would improve faster than individuals with lower loads.

No significant interactions by sex were detected (online Supplementary Table S6).

Sensitivity analysis

Similar results were observed after excluding 174 children with an ADHD diagnosis (online Supplementary Table S6). Specifically, PRS for ADHD were negatively nominally associated with verbal WM performance (d′) assessed at baseline across all significance thresholds. At all significance thresholds, these associations remained significant following FDR-correction except at PT  < 0.05. The strongest association was observed at PT = 0.05 (β = −0.086, 95% CI = −0.139, −0.033; p-uncorrec. = 0.002, p-FDR = 0.026). Nominally significant associations were also observed with numerical WM performance (d′) for all significance thresholds except PT < 0.01. Of these, one association survived FDR-correction (β = −0.078, 95% CI = −0.131, −0.025; p-uncorrec. = 0.004, p-FDR = 0.028) (online Supplementary Table S7).

As previously observed in the sample including children with ADHD diagnosis, PRS for ASD did not show significant associations with WM measures and no significant associations were observed between PRS for ADHD or ASD and HRTSE (online Supplementary Table S7).

Discussion

In the current study, we examined the genetic overlap between ADHD and ASD and cognitive abilities among school-aged children from the general population. We found that polygenic risk for ADHD was associated with worse WM performance. These associations were observed at baseline assessments but not over time. In contrast, PRS for ASD did not show significant associations with any of the cognitive measures analyzed at baseline. Only a nominal association was observed between PRS for ASD and increased cognitive growth in verbal WM over time, although this association did not survive FDR-correction. The exclusion of individuals diagnosed with ADHD from the analyses did not change meaningfully these findings. No associations were observed for HRTSE.

As expected, the polygenic risk for ADHD was positively associated with ADHD symptoms total score, inattention and hyperactivity. The association between polygenic risk for ADHD and hyperactivity and inattention was very similar in strength and magnitude. These findings provide additional support for a dimensional conceptualization of ADHD with genetic factors operating through the distribution of the trait (Larsson, Anckarsater, Råstam, Chang, & Lichtenstein, Reference Larsson, Anckarsater, Råstam, Chang and Lichtenstein2012).

In our study, PRS for ADHD were associated with worse baseline WM performance which is in line with previous studies reporting negative associations between polygenic risk for ADHD and cognitive measures. In the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents And Children population-based birth cohort, negative associations were found between the PRS for ADHD and intelligence and WM (N = 6832) (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Hamshere, Stergiakouli, O'Donovan and Thapar2015). Also, PRS for ADHD were related with worse educational outcomes (lower scores obtained in national examinations) among children (N = 5748) and low intelligence quotient (IQ) scores among adolescents (N = 4958) from the general population (Stergiakouli et al., Reference Stergiakouli, Martin, Hamshere, Heron, Pourcain, Timpson and Smith2017). Another study conducted in three cohorts, the Generation Scotland Scottish Family Health Study (N = 9863), the Lothian Birth Cohorts 1936 and 1921 (N = 1522), and the Brisbane Adolescent Twin Sample (BATS) (N = 921), did not show consistent results regarding the associations between polygenic risk for ADHD and cognitive outcomes, but a negative association was observed with IQ at age 11 years in the Lothian Birth Cohorts (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Lupton, Fernandez-Pujals, Starr, Davies, Cox and McIntosh2016). A more recent study based on a large sample of adult individuals drawn from the UK Biobank Study (N = 135 726) found that verbal-numerical reasoning scores, a cognitive measurement related to IQ, decreased with increasing polygenic load for ADHD (Du Rietz et al., Reference Du Rietz, Coleman, Glanville, Choi, O'Reilly and Kuntsi2018). Remarkably, in this last study, summary statistics used to compute PRS were the same used in the current study. Therefore, our findings extend previous results suggesting a genetic overlap between ADHD and worse WM performance during childhood.

Interestingly, when stratifying by sex, we observed that the detrimental effects of ADHD polygenic risk on WM performance at baseline were limited to boys. This is in contrast with previous studies that did not find evidence for differential sex-effects (Du Rietz et al., Reference Du Rietz, Coleman, Glanville, Choi, O'Reilly and Kuntsi2018; Stergiakouli et al., Reference Stergiakouli, Martin, Hamshere, Heron, Pourcain, Timpson and Smith2017). Another study observed sex-differential effects for PRS for ADHD on WM assessed at age 8 years but limited to girls (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Hamshere, Stergiakouli, O'Donovan and Thapar2015). Our results may be explained by the higher prevalence of ADHD symptoms observed among boys in our sample, which is in agreement with the literature (Polanczyk et al., Reference Polanczyk, De Lima, Horta, Biederman and Rohde2007). Although the reasons underlying the higher rates of childhood ADHD among boys remain unclear, recent research indicates that the genetic variants related to ADHD largely overlap between boys and girls (Martin et al., Reference Wray, Lee, Mehta, Vinkhuyzen, Dudbridge and Middeldorp2018). Nevertheless, an alternative explanation for our findings regards the possibility that genetic susceptibility for ADHD indexed using PRS operates via different mechanisms in boys and girls.

Considering all the above findings, genetic risk variants for ADHD may have pleiotropic effects in closely related cognitive domains. These pleiotropic effects may arise from common genetic variants influencing both ADHD symptoms and cognition, reflecting horizontal or biological pleiotropy; or genetically influenced mechanisms may lead to one trait that influences the other, also known as vertical or mediated pleiotropy (Hemani, Bowden, & Davey Smith, Reference Hemani, Bowden and Davey Smith2019). Furthermore, detecting these associations in population-based samples, even after excluding clinical cases, suggest that the association between genetic risk for ADHD and worse WM performance is independent of clinical status.

To our knowledge, this study is the first study to examine the association between polygenic risk for ADHD and ASD on 1-year cognitive-developmental trajectories. The interest in studying 1-year trajectories was exploring whether polygenic risk for ADHD and ASD was related to cognitive changes over this period. While we have observed that overall all children improve their performance from the first to the last time that they complete the task, there is substantial interindividual variation in the speed (whether improvement occurs in the first or last visits) and magnitude (difference in the score from first to the last visit) of this improvement (López-Vicente et al., Reference López-Vicente, Forns, Suades-González, Esnaola, García-Esteban, Álvarez-Pedrerol and Sunyer2016; Suades-González et al., Reference Suades-González, Forns, García-Esteban, López-Vicente, Esnaola, Álvarez-Pedrerol and Sunyer2017). However, our significant findings were largely limited to baseline assessments of WM performance. This is largely in line with a previous study conducted in the entire BREATHE sample (N = 2897), reporting worse WM performance at baseline but similar cognitive growth trajectories for individuals with and without ADHD diagnosis for tasks with numbers as stimuli (López-Vicente et al., Reference López-Vicente, Forns, Suades-González, Esnaola, García-Esteban, Álvarez-Pedrerol and Sunyer2016). However, López-Vicente et al. observed differences in WM trajectories for verbal tasks between children with and without an ADHD diagnosis. Our results suggest that genetic risk variants for ADHD may be underlying the observed worse WM performance at baseline, but they are not related to variability in 1-year cognitive-developmental trajectories. In other words, in our study, children with a higher genetic risk for ADHD showed worse performance the first time they were tested, but these variants do not influence the subsequent improvement in WM development during 1-year follow-up. Although we cannot rule out practice effects (Mollica et al., Reference Mollica, Maruff, Collie and Vance2005), the improvement in WM tasks through childhood and adolescence is thought to be an age-dependent process underlined by brain maturation (Moriguchi et al., Reference Moriguchi, Miller, Mammarella, Pelegrina, Lechuga, García-Madruga and Bajo2015; Tamnes et al., Reference Tamnes, Walhovd, Grydeland, Holland, Østby, Dale and Fjell2013; Vuontela et al., Reference Vuontela, Steenari, Carlson, Koivisto, Fjallberg and Aronen2003).

Regarding HRTSE the lack of significant associations with this domain may be related to findings indicating that PRS for ADHD are more strongly related with hyperactivity than inattention symptoms (Brikell et al., Reference Brikell, Larsson, Lu, Pettersson, Chen, Kuja-Halkola and Martin2020; Martin, Hamshere, Stergiakouli, O'Donovan, & Thapar, Reference Martin, Hamshere, Stergiakouli, O'Donovan and Thapar2014; Sudre et al., Reference Sudre, Frederick, Sharp, Ishii-Takahashi, Mangalmurti, Choudhury and Shaw2019; Vuijk et al., Reference Vuijk, Martin, Braaten, Genovese, Capawana, O'Keefe and Doyle2019). However, this was not the case for our sample where we found similar associations between PRS for ADHD and hyperactivity and inattention symptoms. Indeed, as it has been previously stated (Sudre et al., Reference Sudre, Frederick, Sharp, Ishii-Takahashi, Mangalmurti, Choudhury and Shaw2019), WM and other cognitive abilities such as intelligence have been shown to be genetically correlated with ADHD (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Hamshere, Stergiakouli, O'Donovan and Thapar2014; Savage et al., Reference Savage, Jansen, Stringer, Watanabe, Bryois, de Leeuw and Posthuma2018). However, up to our knowledge, genetic correlation or overlap between ADHD and attentional measures at a molecular level remains to be established.

In our study, the polygenic risk of ASD was not significantly associated with any of the cognitive measures analyzed. Only a nominal association was observed between polygenic for ASD and increased cognitive growth in verbal WM over time suggesting that common genetic variants related to ASD may be related to better cognitive performance on this domain. This agrees with previous findings from the abovementioned studies by Clarke and colleagues in three cohorts and findings in one of the samples analyzed in Schork and colleagues (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Lupton, Fernandez-Pujals, Starr, Davies, Cox and McIntosh2016; Schork et al., Reference Schork, Brown, Hagler, Thompson, Chen, Dale and Akshoomoff2019). In these studies, a higher polygenic risk for ASD was associated with better cognitive functioning. Thus, although not significant, our results at baseline and over time show a direction consistent with previous findings. The fact that they were not significant may not be only related to sample size since this association was significant in a smaller sample in the study by Schork et al. (Reference Schork, Brown, Hagler, Thompson, Chen, Dale and Akshoomoff2019). Further research is needed to elucidate which are the effects of genetic variants related to ASD on cognition.

Our results should be interpreted in the context of several strengths and limitations. One of the strengths of the study regards the obtention of cognitive measures using computerized tests which reduces the examiner bias. Also, we used well-powered GWAS to compute PRS, a key aspect to increase the accuracy of the PRS (Choi et al. Reference Choi, Mak and O’Reilly2020). Among the limitations, first, our study has a relatively small sample (N = 1667). It has been pointed out that a target sample of around 2000 subjects provides sufficient power to detect a significant proportion of variance explained, which is slightly larger than our sample (Marees, Stringer, Claire, & Derks, Reference Marees, de Kluiver, Stringer, Vorspan, Curis, Marie-Claire and Derks2018). Second, ASD symptoms were not available, thus we were not able to confirm that PRS for ASD were capturing this phenotype in our sample. Third, children included in the study presented better cognitive performance compared to excluded children which might have resulted in underestimated associations between PRS for ADHD and cognition. Finally, polygenic scores typically explain only a small proportion of the total phenotypic variance of complex traits (Wray et al., Reference Wray, Lee, Mehta, Vinkhuyzen, Dudbridge and Middeldorp2014).

To conclude, common genetic variants related to ADHD may be, at least partially, underlying cognitive deficits among school-aged children regarding WM performance. These associations were limited to baseline assessment suggesting that polygenic risk for ADHD is not associated with 1-year trajectories of cognitive development. Our findings suggest that genetic risk variants for ADHD may have pleiotropic effects in WM, a closely related neurodevelopmental domain in the context of ADHD. Further research is needed to characterize the neurobiological mechanisms underlying the genetic overlap between ADHD and WM.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720003189.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all of the children, families and schools who participated in this study. We also thank Cecilia Persavento, Judit González, Laura Bouso and Pere Figueras for their contribution to the fieldwork. This work was supported by the European Research Council under the Grant Agreement number 268479 – the BREATHE project. ISGlobal acknowledges support from the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation through the ‘Centro de Excelencia Severo Ochoa 2019–2023’ Program (CEX2018-000806-S), and support from the Generalitat de Catalunya through the CERCA Program. S.Alemany and N.Vilor-Tejedor are funded by Juan de la Cierva Programme (Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities - Spanish State Research Agency, ref. IJCI-2017-34068, and ref. FJC-2018-038085-I).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Alemany, S., Vilor-Tejedor, N., Bustamante, M., Pujol, J., Macià, D., Martínez-Vilavella, G., … Sunyer, J. (2016). A genome-wide association study of attention function in a population-based sample of children. PLoS One, 11(9), e0163048. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163048.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2002). Manual diagnóstico y estadístico de los trastornos mentales [in Spanish]. Barcelona, Spain: Masson.Google Scholar
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 6594. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 57(1), 289300. doi:10.2307/2346101.Google Scholar
Boxhoorn, S., Lopez, E., Schmidt, C., Schulze, D., Hänig, S., & Freitag, C. M. (2018). Attention profiles in autism spectrum disorder and subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27(11), 14331447. doi:10.1007/s00787-018-1138-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brikell, I., Larsson, H., Lu, Y., Pettersson, E., Chen, Q., Kuja-Halkola, R., … Martin, J. (2020). The contribution of common genetic risk variants for ADHD to a general factor of childhood psychopathology. Molecular Psychiatry, 25(8), 18091821. doi:10.1038/s41380-018-0109-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheung, C. H. M., Fazier-Wood, A. C., Asherson, P., Rijsdijk, F., & Kuntsi, J. (2014). Shared cognitive impairments and aetiology in ADHD symptoms and reading difficulties. PLoS One, 9(6), e98590. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0098590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, S. W., Mak, T. S. -H, & O’Reilly, P. F. (2020). Tutorial: a guide to performing polygenic risk score analyses A guide to performing polygenic risk score analyses. Nature Protocols, 15(9), 27592772. doi: 10.1101/416545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, T.-K., Lupton, M. K., Fernandez-Pujals, A. M., Starr, J., Davies, G., Cox, S., … McIntosh, A. M. (2016). Common polygenic risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is associated with cognitive ability in the general population. Molecular Psychiatry, 21(3), 419425. doi:10.1038/mp.2015.12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demontis, D., Walters, R. K., Martin, J., Mattheisen, M., Als, T. D., Agerbo, E., … Neale, B. M. (2019). Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Genetics, 51(1), 6375. doi:10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Du Rietz, E., Coleman, J., Glanville, K., Choi, S. W., O'Reilly, P. F., & Kuntsi, J. (2018). Association of polygenic risk for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with co-occurring traits and disorders. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(7), 635643. doi:10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.11.013.Google ScholarPubMed
Euesden, J., Lewis, C. M., & O'Reilly, P. F. (2015). PRSice: Polygenic risk score software. Bioinformatics (Oxford, England), 31(9), 14661468. doi:10.1093/bioinformatics/btu848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frazier-Wood, A. C., Bralten, J., Arias-Vasquez, A., Luman, M., Ooterlaan, J., Sergeant, J., … Rommelse, N. N. J. (2012). Neuropsychological intra-individual variability explains unique genetic variance of ADHD and shows suggestive linkage to chromosomes 12, 13, and 17. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 159B(2), 131140. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.32018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grove, J., Ripke, S., Als, T. D., Mattheisen, M., Walters, R. K., Won, H., … Børglum, A. D. (2019). Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nature Genetics, 51(3), 431444. doi:10.1038/s41588-019-0344-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hemani, G., Bowden, J., & Davey Smith, G. (2019). Evaluating the potential role of pleiotropy in Mendelian randomization studies. Human Molecular Genetics. NLM (Medline), 27(R2), R195R208. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddy163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iuculano, T., Rosenberg-Lee, M., Supekar, K., Lynch, C. J., Khouzam, A., Phillips, J., … Menon, V. (2014). Brain organization underlying superior mathematical abilities in children with autism. Biological Psychiatry, 75(3), 223230. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.06.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karalunas, S. L., Geurts, H. M., Konrad, K., Bender, S., & Nigg, J. T. (2014). Annual research review: Reaction time variability in ADHD and autism spectrum disorders: Measurement and mechanisms of a proposed trans-diagnostic phenotype. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 55(6), 685710. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karalunas, S. L., Hawkey, E., Gustafsson, H., Miller, M., Langhorst, M., Cordova, M., … Nigg, J. T. (2018). Overlapping and distinct cognitive impairments in attention-deficit/hyperactivity and Autism Spectrum disorder without intellectual disability. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(8), 17051716. doi:10.1007/s10802-017-0394-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, H., Anckarsater, H., Råstam, M., Chang, Z., & Lichtenstein, P. (2012). Childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as an extreme of a continuous trait: A quantitative genetic study of 8500 twin pairs. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(1), 7380. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02467.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
López-Vicente, M., Forns, J., Suades-González, E., Esnaola, M., García-Esteban, R., Álvarez-Pedrerol, M., … Sunyer, J. (2016). Developmental trajectories in primary schoolchildren using n-back task. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(MAY), 111. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Elsabbagh, M., Baird, G., & Veenstra-Vanderweele, J. (2018). Autism spectrum disorder. Lancet (London, England), 392(10146), 508520. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(18)31129-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marees, A. T., de Kluiver, H., Stringer, S., Vorspan, F., Curis, E., Marie-Claire, C., & Derks, E. M. (2018). A tutorial on conducting genome-wide association studies: Quality control and statistical analysis. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 27(2), 110. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, J., Hamshere, M. L., Stergiakouli, E., O'Donovan, M. C., & Thapar, A. (2014). Genetic risk for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder contributes to neurodevelopmental traits in the general population. Biological Psychiatry, 76(8), 664671. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.02.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, J., Hamshere, M. L., Stergiakouli, E., O'Donovan, M. C., & Thapar, A. (2015). Neurocognitive abilities in the general population and composite genetic risk scores for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 56(6), 648656. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, J., Walters, R. K., Demontis, D., Mattheisen, M., Lee, S. H., & Robinson, E., et al. (2018). A Genetic Investigation of Sex Bias in the Prevalence of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Biol Psychiatry, 83(12), 10441053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michelini, G., Cheung, C. H. M., Kitsune, V., Brandeis, D., Banaschewski, T., McLoughlin, G., … Kuntsi, J. (2018). The etiological structure of cognitive-neurophysiological impairments in ADHD in adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Attention Disorders, 1, doi: 1087054718771191. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054718771191.Google Scholar
Mollica, C. M., Maruff, P., Collie, A., & Vance, A. (2005). Repeated assessment of cognition in children and the measurement of performance change. Child Neuropsychology, 11(3), 303310. doi:10.1080/092970490911306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moriguchi, Y., Miller, M. R., Mammarella, I. C., Pelegrina, S., Lechuga, M. T., García-Madruga, J. A., … Bajo, M. T. (2015). Normative data on the n-back task for children and young adolescents. Frontiers Psychology, 6, 1544. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01544.Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A., Monk, C. S., Lin, J., Carver, L. J., Thomas, K. M., & Truwit, C. L. (2000). Functional neuroanatomy of spatial working memory in children. Developmental Psychology, 36(1), 109116. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10645748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pievsky, M. A., & McGrath, R. E. (2018). The neurocognitive profile of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A review of meta-analyses. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 33(2), 143157. doi:10.1093/arclin/acx055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polanczyk, G., De Lima, M. S., Horta, B. L., Biederman, J., & Rohde, L. A. (2007). The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: A systematic review and metaregression analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(6), 942948. doi:10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42(8), 10291040. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, J. E., Jansen, P. R., Stringer, S., Watanabe, K., Bryois, J., de Leeuw, C. A., … Posthuma, D. (2018). Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nature Genetics, 50(7), 912919. doi:10.1038/s41588-018-0152-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schork, A. J., Brown, T. T., Hagler, D. J., Thompson, W. K., Chen, C-H, Dale, A. M., … Akshoomoff, N. (2019). Polygenic risk for psychiatric disorders correlates with executive function in typical development. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 18(4), e12480. doi:10.1111/gbb.12480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stergiakouli, E., Martin, J., Hamshere, M. L., Heron, J., Pourcain, B. S., Timpson, N. J., … Smith, G. D. (2017). Association between polygenic risk scores for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and educational and cognitive outcomes in the general population. International Journal of Epidemiology, 46(2), 421428. doi:10.1093/ije/dyw216.Google ScholarPubMed
Suades-González, E., Forns, J., García-Esteban, R., López-Vicente, M., Esnaola, M., Álvarez-Pedrerol, M., … Sunyer, J. (2017). A longitudinal study on attention development in primary school children with and without teacher-reported symptoms of ADHD. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(MAY), 655. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00655.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sudre, G., Frederick, J., Sharp, W., Ishii-Takahashi, A., Mangalmurti, A., Choudhury, S., & Shaw, P. (2019). Mapping associations between polygenic risks for childhood neuropsychiatric disorders, symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, cognition, and the brain. Molecular Psychiatry. doi:10.1038/s41380-019-0350-3.Google ScholarPubMed
Sunyer, J., Esnaola, M., Alvarez-Pedrerol, M., Forns, J., Rivas, I., L??pez-Vicente, M., … Querol, X. (2015). Association between traffic-related Air pollution in schools and cognitive development in primary school children: A prospective cohort study. PLoS Medicine, 12(3), e1001792. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1001792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamnes, C. K., Walhovd, K. B., Grydeland, H., Holland, D., Østby, Y., Dale, A. M., & Fjell, A. M. (2013). Longitudinal working memory development is related to structural maturation of frontal and parietal cortices. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(10), 16111623. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thapar, A., & Cooper, M. (2016). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Lancet, 387(10024), 12401250. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00238-X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tillman, C., Eninger, L., Forssman, L., & Bohlin, G. (2011). The relation between working memory components and ADHD symptoms from a developmental perspective. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(2), 181198. doi:10.1080/87565641.2010.549981.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Lieshout, M., Luman, M., Twisk, J. W. R., Faraone, S. V., Heslenfeld, D. J., Hartman, C. A., … Oosterlaan, J. (2017). Neurocognitive predictors of ADHD outcome: A 6-year follow-up study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(2), 261272. doi:10.1007/s10802-016-0175-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velikonja, T., Fett, A.-K., & Velthorst, E. (2019). Patterns of nonsocial and social cognitive functioning in adults with Autism Spectrum disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(2), 135151. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.3645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Visser, J. C., Rommelse, N. N. J., Greven, C. U., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2016). Autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in early childhood: A review of unique and shared characteristics and developmental antecedents. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 65, 229263. doi:10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2016.03.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuijk, P. J., Martin, J., Braaten, E. B., Genovese, G., Capawana, M. R., O'Keefe, S. M., … Doyle, A. E. (2019). Translating discoveries in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder genomics to an outpatient child and adolescent psychiatric cohort. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 59(8), 964977. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.08.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vuontela, V., Steenari, M. R., Carlson, S., Koivisto, J., Fjallberg, M., & Aronen, E. T. (2003). Audiospatial and visuospatial working memory in 6–13 year old school children. Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.), 10(1), 7481. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=12551966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willcutt, E. G., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Nigg, J. T., & Sergeant, J. A. (2008). Recent developments in neuropsychological models of childhood psychiatric disorders. Advances in Biological Psychiatry, 24, 195226. doi:10.1159/000118526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wray, N. R., Lee, S. H., Mehta, D., Vinkhuyzen, A. A. E., Dudbridge, F., & Middeldorp, C. M. (2014). Research review: Polygenic methods and their application to psychiatric traits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(10), 10681087. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Flow chart depicting the final sample size for the outcomes analyzed including hit reaction time (HRT-SE) of ANT as a measure of HRTSE and d′ values from the 3-back task with words and numbers as stimuli as measures of WM. Solid lines and boxes represent individuals remaining in the study, dashed lines and boxes represent individuals excluded. Reason and number of individuals excluded are indicated in dashed boxes.

Figure 1

Table 1. Characteristics of the sample

Figure 2

Table 2. Association results between polygenic risk scores (PRS) for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) with cognitive measures at baseline adjusting by age, sex and the first four genetic principal components

Figure 3

Table 3. Association results between polygenic risk scores (PRS) for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) with cognitive 1-year trajectories adjusting by age, sex and the first four genetic principal components

Supplementary material: File

Aguilar-Lacasaña et al. supplementary material

Aguilar-Lacasaña et al. supplementary material

Download Aguilar-Lacasaña et al. supplementary material(File)
File 192.3 KB