Hostname: page-component-5f745c7db-6bmsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-06T22:38:47.193Z Has data issue: true hasContentIssue false

Continuous inclusion of other in the self

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

Benjamin Beranek*
Affiliation:
Department of Finance, Economics and Risk Management, Missouri State University, Springfield, MO, USA
Geoffrey Castillo*
Affiliation:
Department of Economics, Nottingham Business School, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, UK Vienna Centre for Experimental Economics, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The Inclusion of Other in the Self (IOS) scale is a popular tool to measure interpersonal closeness that is increasingly being used in economics. We develop and validate a continuous version of the IOS scale. This Continuous IOS scale gives a finer measure and addresses the reluctance of subjects to report low scores on the standard IOS scale. We also propose a version of the standard IOS scale that meets its original design features. Our IOS scales are easy-to-use, well-documented, standardised, and available at https://github.com/geoffreycastillo/ios-js.

Type
Original Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2024

1 Introduction

The Inclusion of Other in the Self (IOS) scale is one of the most popular measures of interpersonal closeness. It asks respondents to pick one of seven pairs of increasingly overlapping circles to indicate how close they feel toward another (Fig. 1a). Since its creation by Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Aron and Smollan1992), the IOS scale has been repeatedly validated (Gächter et al., Reference Gächter, Starmer and Tufano2015) and widely adopted, with over 5000 citations on Google Scholar (see Aron et al., Reference Aron, Lewandowski, Mashek and Aron2013; Branand et al., Reference Branand, Mashek and Aron2019, for reviews). In recent years it has spread to economics, for example to explain charitable donations (Goette & Tripodi, Reference Goette and Tripodi2021), dictator game allocations (Robson, Reference Robson2021), or team production (Gächter et al., Reference Gächter, Starmer and Tufano2023).

Fig. 1 Four different IOS scales

We offer a continuous version of the IOS scale (Fig. 1b) that allows a finer measurement of interpersonal closeness. In the Continuous IOS scale, respondents click the left circle (representing self) and drag-and-drop it on the right circle (representing the other) at the point of overlap that best represents their relationship with the other. Additionally, it allows a greater degree of overlap than the original IOS scale, and would thus be able to represent a higher perceived closeness.

Our Continuous IOS scale has the design features set out by Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Aron and Smollan1992) in their seminal paper. We find, however, that most existing implementations of the standard IOS scale do not. We thus offer, in addition to the Continuous IOS scale, a re-implementation of the standard IOS scale that does (Fig. 1c). As a middle-ground, we also offer the Step-Choice IOS scale (Fig. 1d) which displays only one pair of circles with arrows on the left and right. Respondents are instructed to find the pair of circles which best represents their connection with the other by clicking on the arrows to move back and forth between the different pairs. The Step-Choice IOS scale thus retains the discreteness of the standard IOS scale but allows the use of more pairs of circles.

In an online experiment, we validate the Continuous IOS scale and the Step-Choice IOS scale against our implementation of the standard IOS scale. We find that the standard IOS scale elicits higher IOS scores than the Continuous IOS scale. This mainly comes from subjects who select a minimal overlap on both scales. Subjects must select the second pair of circles in the standard IOS scale to report minimal overlap, while they can report minimal overlap by choosing an overlap very close to zero in the Continuous IOS scale. Our results thus suggest that subjects might be reluctant to report the lowest IOS score on the standard IOS scale. The Continuous IOS scale addresses this by offering subjects the opportunity to report a low level of overlap without having to select an overlap as large as the one that corresponds to the second pair of circles in the standard IOS scale.

Our IOS scales can be implemented in any web-based experimental software, such as Qualtrics, oTree (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Schonger and Wickens2016), or LIONESS (Giamattei et al., Reference Giamattei, Yahosseini, Gächter and Molleman2020). They are available as ios.js at https://github.com/geoffreycastillo/ios-js with full documentation. We also offer detailed instructions for Qualtrics and an example app for oTree.

Our scales offer various options. For all scales, the size of the circles can be customised. For the Step-Choice and standard IOS scales, users can select the number of circles. We also offer unbalanced versions of these scales with twice as many pairs of overlapping circles in the first half of the scale. This option would be useful, as we will see below, in cases where a majority of IOS scores are concentrated at the lower end of the scale.

Users can also customise the labels of the circles—‘You’ and ‘Other’ in Fig. 1. By doing so, our IOS scales can be directly translated into the Inclusion of Ingroup in the Self (IIS) scale (Tropp & Wright, Reference Tropp and Wright2001). The IIS scale is an extension of the IOS scale to groups obtained by replacing the ‘Other’ label attached to the right circle by the ingroup. The IIS allows one to measure the distance between an individual and a group or even between two groups.

In the next section following our discussion of the related literature, we describe how we construct our IOS scales to have the Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Aron and Smollan1992) features. In Sect. 3 we present the experiment we designed to validate our Continuous and Step-Choice IOS scales. Section 4 presents the results and Sect. 5 concludes.

Related literature. Other continuous implementations of the IOS scale exist. However, they use circles with a fixed diameter and thus do not have the features highlighted by Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Aron and Smollan1992). Further, they do not validate their continuous versions of the IOS scale in an experiment. For example, Le et al. (Reference Le, Moss and Mashek2007) offer a version that no longer works on modern browsers because the technology they relied on—Java applets—is deprecated. Kamphorst et al. (Reference Kamphorst, Nauts and Blouin-Hudon2017) ported this version to JavaScript, the technology we also rely on. More recently, Kinley and van Vugt (Reference Kinley and van Vugt2023) offer a version for jsPsych (de Leeuw et al., Reference de Leeuw, Gilbert and Luchterhandt2023).

Further, Baader et al. (Reference Baader, Starmer, Tufano and Gächter2024) propose expanding the original IOS scale to 11 circles—thus their extension is named IOS 11 . They show that it outperforms the original, seven-circle IOS scale. Their extension introduces more circles in the first and in the final third of the scale. We have added an option to ios.js inspired by IOS 11 that accomplishes this in the context of our scales.

As we hinted above, the IOS scale is becoming a popular tool in economics and has been used to show how social closeness impacts economic outcomes. For example, Goette and Tripodi (Reference Goette and Tripodi2021) find that social proximity as measured by the IOS scale drives social conformity, which influences how much money people give to an NGO. Hofmann et al. (Reference Hofmann, Fiagbenu, Özgümüs, Tahamtan and Regner2021) show that the presence of close others, as measured by the IOS scale, increases voluntary payments in a Pay-What-You-Want context. Dimant (Reference Dimant2024) studies political polarisation and distinguishes between ingroup love and outgroup hate thanks in part to the IOS scale. Gächter et al. (Reference Gächter, Starmer and Tufano2023) show that group cohesion, measured with an oneness scale that includes the IOS scale, increases team production as captured by a weak-link coordination game. We ourselves have already used the Continuous IOS scale and ios.js in a series of large online experiments to study social discounting (Beranek & Castillo, Reference Beranek and Castillo2024).

Further, in a lab-in-the-field experiment in Uganda, Robson (Reference Robson2021) uses the IOS scale to show that social connectedness enters the utility function and affects decisions in a modified dictator game. Castillo (Reference Castillo2021) uses the IOS scale to show how preferences defined over social distance depend on the task used to elicit them—a translation of the classical preference reversal phenomenon to the social domain. In both Robson (Reference Robson2021) and Castillo (Reference Castillo2021), about half of the respondents report the same IOS scores for the two recipients they face. The finer measurement brought by the Continuous IOS scale would help in those cases where the different others evaluated by respondents are too similar to be discriminated by the original IOS scale.

2 Constructing the IOS scale

When creating the figures for the IOS scale, the two design features highlighted by Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Aron and Smollan1992) were that: “(a) the total area of each figure is constant (thus as the overlap of the circles increases, so does the diameter), and (b) the degree of overlap progresses linearly, creating a seven-step, interval-level scale”. Feature (a) means that circle diameters increase as the distance between the circles decreases to keep constant the total area of self plus other. This feature ensures that no sense of self or other is lost when overlap increases, which aligns with Aron and Aron's (Reference Aron and Aron1986) conception of relationships. Feature (b) ensures there are no jumps between any two pairs of circles, a necessary condition to generate a valid, linear, one-to-seven measure. Otherwise, some increases in overlap would be larger than others which might give rise to threshold effects.

Table 1 Total area and change in the overlap of IOS scales

Pair

Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Aron and Smollan1992)

Ours

Total area

Change % overlap

Total area

Change % overlap

1

157.08

157.08

2

152.75

7.32

157.03

14.32

3

151.65

3.53

157.12

14.24

4

153.31

11.00

157.02

14.30

5

150.00

13.75

157.22

14.25

6

148.76

9.55

157.03

14.30

7

149.93

19.69

157.08

14.37

Starting diameter normalised to 10

‘Change % overlap’ does not sum to 100% because the last pair of circles does not represent full overlap (see Appendix A)

We show on Table 1, however, that the original figures in Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Aron and Smollan1992) do not have these features. That is the case for most, if not all, of the literature that followed which used either directly the Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Aron and Smollan1992) figures, their own figures, or a series of overlapping circles with non-increasing diameters.

Finding a series of overlapping circles that have both of these features is a non-trivial problem without a closed-form solution. We rely on simulations (see Appendix A for details) to find, for every proportion of overlap, a corresponding circle diameter. These calculations allow us to create an IOS scale that has the features highlighted by Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Aron and Smollan1992). We use these calculations to construct our version of the standard IOS scale as well as our new Step-Choice and Continuous IOS scales, which we test in the following experiment.

3 Experimental design

In our experiment, we test whether respondents choose the same overlap regardless of the version of our IOS scales they use. More specifically, within-subjects respondents indicate how close they feel toward another using our standard IOS scale and either our Continuous or our Step-Choice IOS scales. The order of the IOS scales is randomised between-subjects. We add a filler task between the IOS scales: we ask subjects to solve 10 mathematics problems involving the addition and subtraction of three-digit numbers under time pressure.

As highlighted by Kinley and van Vugt (Reference Kinley and van Vugt2023), a difference between the standard IOS scale and IOS scales that show only one pair of circles at a time—such as our Continuous and Step-Choice IOS scales—is that in the latter subjects are presented with an initial “default” degree of overlap as opposed to seeing all possible degrees of overlap. For this reason, we require subjects to manipulate the IOS scale from no overlap to full overlap before they can proceed to the task itself (see Figs. 11 and 13 of the instructions in Appendix B). This also ensures our JavaScript code works as intended on subjects’ browsers; if it did not, subjects could not proceed beyond this intentional point of failure.

In contrast to most of the literature, we use real people as the target of the IOS scales as opposed to hypothetical ‘others’. To do so, we first surveyed members of the US general public on MTurk forming a diverse pool of potential targets. We asked them a number of questions, mostly standard demographic questions as well as some questions about their opinions on various social issues (see Supplementary material for a list of questions used). Thereafter, we invited the subjects for the experiment reported here and asked them the same questions. We paired them with a target drawn at random from the pool of the originally surveyed participants.

We present the target—the ‘other’ participant in the IOS scale—using the card display shown in Fig. 2. The card suit is assigned randomly to the target and used to refer to them throughout the experiment. To make sure subjects engage with their assigned target, we ask them to write the first things that come to their mind when they read the card in at least 25 characters.

Fig. 2 Card display used in the experiment

The advantage of this procedure is that we can assess whether the IOS score decreases with the demographic dissimilarity between subjects and their target. We can also assess how subjects’ characteristics influence the IOS scores they report.

The experiment took place in November 2020. A session lasted about 13 min and the average payment was $1.43. The payment was composed of a fixed $0.50 participation fee and $0.10 for each mathematics problem correctly solved. We analyse below the choices of 644 participants: 328 who evaluate their target using the standard and the Continuous IOS scales and 316 who use the standard and the Step-Choice IOS scales.Footnote 1 Full instructions for the tasks used in the experiment can be found in Appendix B.

4 Results

4.1 Comparing the Continuous and Step-Choice IOS scales to the standard IOS scale

We consider our new IOS scales—Step-Choice IOS scale, Continuous IOS scale—to be validated against our implementation of the standard IOS scale if there is no difference between the scores reported by subjects when they use the new IOS scale or the standard IOS scale. The scores generated by the standard IOS scale and the Step-Choice IOS scale are discrete variables ranging from one (representing no overlap) to seven (representing substantial overlap). In contrast, the score generated by the Continuous IOS scale is a continuous variable ranging between zero and one. To allow comparison, we first convert the overlap given by the Continuous IOS scale into a one-to-seven measure.Footnote 2 We then subtract the score obtained with the new IOS scale from the score obtained with the standard IOS scale. A positive difference means that subjects reported a higher IOS score when using the standard IOS scale.

In Fig. 3, we plot the average of this difference. We start by comparing the Continuous IOS scale to the standard IOS scale in the top part of the Figure. We see that the standard IOS scale leads to higher IOS scores than the Continuous IOS scale: the difference is small but positive and significant (Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test, z = 4.943 and p < 0.001 ).Footnote 3 Therefore, we reject the null hypothesis that subjects report the same IOS score on both scales. Hence our first result:

Fig. 3 Average difference between the IOS scores reported with the standard IOS scale and the Continuous or the Step-Choice IOS scale (with bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals)

Result 1

The standard IOS scale elicits higher scores than the Continuous IOS scale.

The bottom part of Fig. 3 compares the standard IOS scale to the Step-Choice IOS scale. As can be seen, there is no difference between these two scales: the difference is not statistically different from 0 (Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test, z = - 0.810 and p = 0.4135 ).Footnote 4 Therefore, we cannot reject the null hypothesis that subjects report the same IOS score on both scales. Hence our second result:

Result 2

We do not detect a difference between the scores elicited from the standard IOS scale and the Step-Choice IOS scale.

4.2 Why does the standard IOS scale result in higher IOS scores than the Continuous IOS scale?

To better understand why the standard IOS scale yields higher IOS scores compared to the Continuous IOS scale, Fig. 4 displays the IOS score reported with the Continuous IOS scale as a function of the IOS score reported with the standard IOS scale. Each dot represents a subject. There are two notables features. First, Continuous IOS scores are dispersed around their corresponding standard IOS scores. Second, when considering a specific standard IOS score, the Continuous IOS scores tend to skew towards lower values.Footnote 5

Fig. 4 Relation between the IOS scores reported with the standard IOS scale and those reported with the Continuous IOS scale

The dispersion can be explained by random errors resulting from asking subjects to report the IOS score twice in a row. Continuous IOS scores are mostly the same as the corresponding standard IOS scores or in the adjacent categories. For example, 25 of the 66 subjects (37.88%) who report a standard IOS score of 3 report the exact same score on the Continuous IOS scale. An additional 36 subjects report scores in the categories adjacent, with 25 subjects (37.88%) reporting a score of 2, and 11 subjects (16.67%), a score of 4. Only 5 subjects (7.58%) report scores beyond the adjacent categories. (See Appendix C for the detailed frequency tables.)Footnote 6

The skew of the Continuous IOS scores towards lower values is the reason the average difference in Fig. 3—standard IOS score minus Continuous IOS score—is positive. It is particularly apparent in Fig. 4 by focusing on subjects who report a Continuous IOS score of 1: many of these subjects report a standard IOS score of 2. In fact, when we remove these 54 subjects, the average difference between standard and Continuous IOS scale is no longer significant (Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test, z = 1.018 and p = 0.3095 ).

This pattern could be explained by the reluctance of subjects to report the lowest IOS score with the standard IOS scale. In the standard IOS scale, subjects who want to report some connection with their match, at least more connection than no overlap would imply, are forced to pick the second pair of circles. On the other hand, the Continuous IOS scale allows them to report at least some overlap; while at the same time, this overlap falls short of an overlap corresponding to an IOS score of 2. As would be predicted by this explanation, we observe some bunching below the threshold that corresponds to an IOS score of 2.

The fact that subjects avoid reporting an IOS score of 1 with the standard IOS scale could also be a manifestation of the compromise effect (see, for example, Beauchamp et al., Reference Beauchamp, Benjamin, Laibson and Chabris2020, in the context of risk preference elicitation). According to the compromise effect, people want to avoid extremes and prefer to choose options that are more in the middle. In the standard IOS scale, avoiding the low extreme means reporting an IOS score of 2. In the Continuous IOS scale, avoiding the low extreme means reporting a small degree of overlap that might not correspond to an IOS score of 2.

To mitigate this issue in the standard IOS scale, we propose adding intermediate pairs of overlapping circles in-between the first half of the circles to create an unbalanced, standard or Step-Choice IOS scale. This addition would give subjects more opportunity to report small levels of overlap. For example, the unbalanced version of the standard IOS scale now has 10 pairs of overlapping circles: the original 7 with extra pairs of overlapping circles between 1 and 2, between 2 and 3, and between 3 and 4. We have implemented this option in ios.js.

4.3 The effect of demographic dissimilarity and of demographic characteristics on IOS scores

We conclude with a number of exploratory, non-pre-registered analyses. We focus on the standard IOS scale since we have the most observations for this scale. We first look at how demographic dissimilarity between a subject and their target influence the IOS score they report. For us, demographic dissimilarity measures the proportion of discordant responses given to the questions displayed on the card shown in Fig. 2. A dissimilarity of 0 refers to a subject who answers exactly as their target to those questions, and a dissimilarity of 1, to a subject who answers as differently as possible.

Model (1) in Table 2 reports the result of the corresponding ordered logistic regression. We find that, as dissimilarity increases, interpersonal closeness as indicated by the IOS score decreases. Therefore, the IOS scale captures something tangible and measurable.

Then, in models (2) and (3) in Table 2, we look at how a subject's own demographic characteristics influence the IOS score they report. We find that those who do part-time work, who have only completed 12th grade without a degree or less, or who support a political party different from the main parties, report lower IOS scores. This finding suggests that one needs a minimal level of stability to start feeling connected to others. Further, people who think others are trustworthy and people who belong to a labour union report higher IOS scores. The results are unchanged if we leave dissimilarity out (in model 2) or include it (in model 3).Footnote 7

Some associations are more difficult to interpret: for example, people who approve of sex before marriage report lower IOS scores. Others should be interpreted with care: for example, people who report their race as Chinese or Korean report lower IOS scores. However, there are not many people in our sample who reported their race as Chinese or Korean and, consequently, they are less likely to be matched with someone who reported the same race.

Table 2 How dissimilarity and respondents’ characteristics explain the reported IOS score, ordered logistic regression

Dependent variable: IOS score

Coef. (SE)

(1)

(2)

(3)

Dissimilarity

– 1.838*** (0.521)

– 2.264*** (0.595)

Age

– 0.004 (0.008)

0.000 (0.008)

Gender (ref.: Female)

Male

0.333** (0.165)

0.319* (0.165)

Race (ref.: White)

Asian Indian

0.137 (0.525)

0.388 (0.536)

Black

0.309 (0.362)

0.510 (0.361)

Chinese

– 1.016*** (0.390)

– 0.981** (0.381)

Korean

– 1.645** (0.768)

– 1.466* (0.769)

Other

0.091 (0.364)

0.355 (0.362)

Ethnicity (ref.: None)

Mexican

– 0.204 (0.393)

0.149 (0.429)

Other

– 0.779 (0.556)

– 0.408 (0.572)

Religion (ref.: None)

Jewish

0.256 (0.566)

0.356 (0.578)

Other

– 0.585 (0.383)

– 0.409 (0.382)

Protestant

– 0.583** (0.264)

– 0.487* (0.263)

Roman Catholic

0.390* (0.226)

0.493** (0.230)

Political party (ref.: Democrat)

Independent

0.070 (0.194)

0.025 (0.194)

No preference

– 0.479 (0.419)

– 0.356 (0.448)

Other party

– 0.975** (0.487)

– 1.001* (0.522)

Republican

0.382 (0.254)

0.367 (0.252)

Marital status (ref.: Married)

Divorced

– 0.554** (0.319)

– 0.528 (0.323)

Never

– 0.283 (0.205)

– 0.244 (0.205)

Separated or widowed

0.870* (0.446)

0.879* (0.451)

Social class (ref.: Middle class)

Lower class

0.001 (0.301)

– 0.095 (0.295)

Upper class

0.317 (0.469)

0.237 (0.473)

Working class

0.079 (0.184)

0.029 (0.186)

Work last week (ref.: Full time work)

Housework

– 0.237 (0.209)

– 0.250 (0.208)

Part time work

– 0.565** (0.254)

– 0.547** (0.258)

School

– 0.127 (0.426)

– 0.038 (0.458)

Place growing up (ref.: Small town)

Farm

0.172 (0.434)

0.099 (0.422)

Large city

0.071 (0.262)

0.099 (0.261)

Medium city

0.142 (0.212)

0.096 (0.215)

Open country

– 0.225 (0.343)

– 0.313 (0.349)

Suburb

0.039 (0.224)

– 0.027 (0.228)

Highest degree (ref.:College or some college)

12th grade no degree and less

– 1.099** (0.540)

– 1.024* (0.556)

Beyond bachelors

0.207 (0.214)

0.183 (0.212)

High school graduate

– 0.052 (0.271)

– 0.060 (0.266)

Number of children

0.063 (0.047)

0.066 (0.046)

Household income

– 0.616 (0.451)

– 0.610 (0.449)

People are helpful

0.154 (0.230)

0.154 (0.228)

People try to take advantage of you

0.255 (0.249)

0.154 (0.228)

People are trustworthy

0.717*** (0.229)

0.716*** (0.224)

Belong to labour union

0.789*** (0.304)

0.797** (0.311)

Unemployed in the past 10 years

– 0.112 (0.171)

– 0.084 (0.173)

Support affirmative action

0.516 (0.292)

0.465 (0.296)

Approve sex before marriage

– 0.957*** (0.360)

– 1.000*** (0.358)

Approve same-sex relations

0.011 (0.347)

0.046 (0.348)

Approve death penalty

0.278 (0.172)

0.225 (0.174)

cut1

– 1.845*** (0.204)

– 1.733** (0.701)

– 2.384*** (0.711)

cut2

– 0.097 (0.190)

0.286 (0.698)

– 0.331 (0.706)

cut3

0.774*** (0.190)

1.310* (0.702)

0.705 (0.708)

cut4

1.505*** (0.198)

2.161*** (0.702)

1.565** (0.707)

cut5

2.100*** (0.214)

2.834*** (0.707)

2.241*** (0.712)

cut6

2.992*** (0.281)

3.808*** (0.728)

3.213*** (0.731)

Observations

644

644

644

Pseudo R 2

0.006

0.081

0.088

Wald χ 2

12.437

160.775

162.343

Prob > χ 2

0.000

0.000

0.000

Robust standard errors. * p < 0.1 , ** p < 0.05 , *** p < 0.01

5 Conclusion

In summary, we offer a new implementation of the standard IOS scale that has the features highlighted by Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Aron and Smollan1992). We also offer a new version of the IOS scale—the Continuous IOS scale—that allows for the selection of any degree of overlap. As an intermediary, we propose the Step-Choice IOS scale. We validate our new IOS scales in an experiment, where we find that the standard IOS scale results in higher IOS scores than the Continuous IOS scale. We interpret this difference as a reluctance of some subjects to select the lowest IOS score corresponding to no overlap in the standard IOS scale. We also find that IOS scores decrease with demographic dissimilarity between subject and target.

Acknowledgements

We thank Gary W. Lewandowski, Matthew Robson, and Bart Kamphorst, for comments on earlier versions.

Funding

Open access funding provided by University of Vienna. We thank the AWS Cloud Credit for Research program, the RF Johnson Faculty Development Fund of Connecticut College, and the Heinrich Graf Hardegǵsche Stiftung, for financial support.

Data availability

Our pre-registration is available at https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/Z6Y7D. Data and code to reproduce our results are available at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.11913643. Our oTree code is available at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.11913676.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

We have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Ethical approval

The Connecticut College Institutional Review Board has approved the experiment reported in this paper.

Appendices

Appendix A: Formulas to generate the IOS scale

Following Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Aron and Smollan1992), the IOS has two requirements:

  1. 1. The total area of each pair of overlapping circles should stay constant; and

  2. 2. The degree of overlap should increase linearly between each pair.

Let r be the radius of the circles and d the distance between them measured centre-to-centre. Before any overlap occurs, the total area is the area of the two circles:

A C ( r ) = 2 π r 2 .

As the circles start to overlap, however, using this formula would result in counting the overlap area twice. We thus need to compute the overlapping areaFootnote 8

A O ( r , d ) = 2 r 2 cos - 1 d 2 r - 1 2 d 4 r 2 - d 2

and remove it once from the total area, giving us the area

A T ( r , d ) = A C ( r ) - A O ( r , d ) = 2 π r 2 - 2 r 2 cos - 1 d 2 r + 1 2 d 4 r 2 - d 2 .

Denote by r 0 the starting radius, when there is no overlap. Using these formulas, we can translate the first requirement as: for all r and d, it must be that

A T ( r , d ) = A C ( r 0 ) ;

and the second requirement as: the ratio

A O ( r , d ) A T ( r , d )

should increase linearly as the distance d decreases.

To find a solution to the problem,Footnote 9 we applied the following algorithm:

  • Fix the initial radius r 0 , d = 2 r 0 , and compute the initial area A C ( r 0 )

  • Generate all (r, d) such that r 0 < r < 2 r 0 and 0 < d < 2 r 0

  • For each (r, d), compute A O ( r , d ) , A T ( r , d ) and A O ( r , d ) A T ( r , d )

  • Remove any (r, d) such that A T ( r , d ) is not within 0.1% of A C ( r 0 )

  • If we want n pairs of overlapping circles, for each target proportion of overlap τ 0 , 1 n , 2 n , , 1 , pick the (r, d) that result in the closest A O ( r , d ) A T ( r , d ) to τ .

Therefore, we find suitable (r, d) for any number of circles n. Note that, in the IOS scale, the last pair of circles does not represent full overlap. Therefore, to find an IOS scale with n pairs of circles, we do it as if we were generating n + 1 pairs of circles and then ignore the last one. For this reason, the overlaps we target are 0 , 1 n , 2 n , , 1 and not 0 , 1 n - 1 , 2 n - 1 , , 1 . Table 1 in the main text compares our values to Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Aron and Smollan1992) for r 0 = 5 and n = 7 .

To create the Continuous IOS scale, we repeat the above steps with n = 100 , then fit a polynomial

2 r ^ = i = 0 m β i d i + ϵ

on the generated data. We stop at m = 4 since a greater m does not improve fit. Table 3 shows the estimates. In our JavaScript widget, we use all the decimals we get from the estimation.

Table 3 Estimates from the polynomial fitted to the simulated values

β 0

1.41450 × 103*** (1.39585 × 10–1)

β 1

− 6.42812 × 10–1*** (2.14726 × 10–3)

β 2

1.78871 × 10–4*** (9.48586 × 10–6)

β 3

2.05904 × 10–8 (1.52208 × –8)

β 4

6.93823 × 10–11*** (7.96705 × 10–12)

R 2

9.99991 × 10–1

Observations

101

*** p < 0.001 ; ** p < 0.01 ; * p < 0.05

The code of the simulation is available at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.11913700.

Appendix B: Tasks used in the experiment

First, subjects complete a forty question survey. Appendix B contains a full list of the questions from the survey.

Second is the presentation of the target. As described in Sect. 3 of the main text, the survey that subjects in this experiment completed was the same survey that a pool of previously surveyed participants completed. We randomly pair each subject in this experiment with a target from the previous subject pool. We introduce the target to the subject by revealing the target's answers to several of the survey questions. Figure 5 shows how we present the target's answers to the selected survey questions on a card.

Third, subjects complete a target familiarization task where they write the first things that come to mind about the target in at least 50 characters. This functions, to some extent, as an attention check for the subjects to ensure subjects read closely the information on the card. Our evaluation of their responses is one means by which we identify bots. The instructions for this target familiarisation task are shown in Fig. 6.

Fourth, subjects complete our version of the standard IOS scale task. The instructions for the standard IOS scale task are shown in Fig. 7. The standard IOS scale task itself is shown in Fig. 8.

Fifth, subjects complete a filler task where they add or subtract two three-digit numbers. This is the only experimental task that is financially incentivised; subjects receive $0.10 for each correctly solved problem. We implement this filler task so that subjects do not complete the two IOS tasks back to back. The instructions for the filler task are shown in Fig. 9. The filler task itself is shown in Fig. 10.

Sixth, subjects complete either the Step-Choice IOS task or the Continuous IOS task. To advance from the instruction screen to the task itself, we require subjects to manipulate each scale such that the circles overlap as much as possible. This functions as an attention check for the subjects and forces them to recognise the full extent of possible overlap. The instructions for the Step-Choice IOS scale task are shown in Fig. 11. The Step-Choice IOS scale task itself is shown in Fig. 12. The instructions for the Continuous IOS scale task are shown in Fig. 13. The Continuous IOS scale task itself is shown in Fig. 14.

All subjects completed two IOS tasks: the standard IOS scale task and one of either the Continuous IOS scale task or the Step-Choice IOS scale task. The order in which they participate the IOS tasks is randomised between subjects.

Demo versions of our IOS scales are available at https://geoffreycastillo.com/ios-js-demo/.

Fig. 5 Presentation of the target

Fig. 6 Target familiarisation task

Fig. 7 Standard IOS scale task instructions

Fig. 8 Standard IOS scale task

Fig. 9 Filler task instructions

Fig. 10 Filler task

Fig. 11 Step-Choice IOS task instructions

Fig. 12 Step-Choice IOS task

Fig. 13 Continuous IOS scale task instructions

Fig. 14 Continuous IOS task

Appendix C: Frequency tables

Table 4 shows the number of subjects who reported each Continuous IOS score as a function of their chosen standard IOS score. It also shows:

  • The row percentage: the percentage of subjects who chose a Continuous IOS score for a given standard IOS score;

  • The column percentage: the percentage of subjects who chose a standard IOS score for a given Continuous IOS score; and finally,

  • The cell percentage: the percentage of subjects overall who chose a particular combination of standard and Continuous IOS score.

Table 5 provides the same information for the Step-Choice IOS score.

Table 4 Number of subjects, row %, column % and cell % for all combinations of standard IOS score and Continuous IOS score (converted from the proportion of overlap)

Standard

Continuous

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Total

1

71

1

0

0

0

1

1

74

95.95

1.35

0.00

0.00

0.00

1.35

1.35

100.00

52.59

1.10

0.00

0.00

0.00

20.00

4.17

22.56

21.65

0.30

0.00

0.00

0.00

0.30

0.30

22.56

2

54

61

5

0

1

0

5

126

42.86

48.41

3.97

0.00

0.79

0.00

3.97

100.00

40.00

67.03

13.16

0.00

9.09

0.00

20.83

38.41

16.46

18.60

1.52

0.00

0.30

0.00

1.52

38.41

3

3

25

25

11

1

0

1

66

4.55

37.88

37.88

16.67

1.52

0.00

1.52

100.00

2.22

27.47

65.79

45.83

9.09

0.00

4.17

20.12

0.91

7.62

7.62

3.35

0.30

0.00

0.30

20.12

4

4

3

5

11

2

1

4

30

13.33

10.00

16.67

36.67

6.67

3.33

13.33

100.00

2.96

3.30

13.16

45.83

18.18

20.00

16.67

9.15

1.22

0.91

1.52

3.35

0.61

0.30

1.22

9.15

5

2

1

1

2

4

0

2

12

16.67

8.33

8.33

16.67

33.33

0.00

16.67

100.00

1.48

1.10

2.63

8.33

36.36

0.00

8.33

3.66

0.61

0.30

0.30

0.61

1.22

0.00

0.61

3.66

6

0

0

2

0

2

3

5

12

0.00

0.00

16.67

0.00

16.67

25.00

41.67

100.00

0.00

0.00

5.26

0.00

18.18

60.00

20.83

3.66

0.00

0.00

0.61

0.00

0.61

0.91

1.52

3.66

7

1

0

0

0

1

0

6

8

12.50

0.00

0.00

0.00

12.50

0.00

75.00

100.00

0.74

0.00

0.00

0.00

9.09

0.00

25.00

2.44

0.30

0.00

0.00

0.00

0.30

0.00

1.83

2.44

Total

135

91

38

24

11

5

24

328

41.16

27.74

11.59

7.32

3.35

1.52

7.32

100.00

100.00

100.00

100.00

100.00

100.00

100.00

100.00

100.00

41.16

27.74

11.59

7.32

3.35

1.52

7.32

100.00

Example: Looking at the first cell, 71 subjects reported a standard IOS score of 1 and a Continuous IOS score of 1. They represent 95.95% of the 74 subjects who reported a standard IOS score of 1 and 52.59% of the 135 subjects who reported a Continuous IOS score of 1. They also represent 21.65% of the 328 overall subjects

Bold indicates the diagonal where standard and Continuous IOS scores are equal one to another

Table 5 Number of subjects, row %, column % and cell % for all combinations of standard IOS score and Step-Choice IOS score

Standard

Step-Choice

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Total

1

67

9

0

0

1

0

0

77

87.01

11.69

0.00

0.00

1.30

0.00

0.00

100.00

89.33

6.87

0.00

0.00

6.67

0.00

0.00

24.37

21.20

2.85

0.00

0.00

0.32

0.00

0.00

24.37

2

4

115

6

2

0

1

1

129

3.10

89.15

4.65

1.55

0.00

0.78

0.78

100.00

5.33

87.79

13.04

8.00

0.00

9.09

7.69

40.82

1.27

36.39

1.90

0.63

0.00

0.32

0.32

40.82

3

0

6

29

6

1

0

2

44

0.00

13.64

65.91

13.64

2.27

0.00

4.55

100.00

0.00

4.58

63.04

24.00

6.67

0.00

15.38

13.92

0.00

1.90

9.18

1.90

0.32

0.00

0.63

13.92

4

0

0

9

11

6

1

2

29

0.00

0.00

31.03

37.93

20.69

3.45

6.90

100.00

0.00

0.00

19.57

44.00

40.00

9.09

15.38

9.18

0.00

0.00

2.85

3.48

1.90

0.32

0.63

9.18

5

1

0

2

6

4

3

1

17

5.88

0.00

11.76

35.29

23.53

17.65

5.88

100.00

1.33

0.00

4.35

24.00

26.67

27.27

7.69

5.38

0.32

0.00

0.63

1.90

1.27

0.95

0.32

5.38

6

1

1

0

0

3

3

3

11

9.09

9.09

0.00

0.00

27.27

27.27

27.27

100.00

1.33

0.76

0.00

0.00

20.00

27.27

23.08

3.48

0.32

0.32

0.00

0.00

0.95

0.95

0.95

3.48

7

2

0

0

0

0

3

4

9

22.22

0.00

0.00

0.00

0.00

33.33

44.44

100.00

2.67

0.00

0.00

0.00

0.00

27.27

30.77

2.85

0.63

0.00

0.00

0.00

0.00

0.95

1.27

2.85

Total

75

131

46

25

15

11

13

316

23.73

41.46

14.56

7.91

4.75

3.48

4.11

100.00

100.00

100.00

100.00

100.00

100.00

100.00

100.00

100.00

23.73

41.46

14.56

7.91

4.75

3.48

4.11

100.00

Example: Looking at the first cell, 67 subjects reported a standard IOS score of 1 and a Step-Choice IOS score of 1. They represent 87.01% of the 77 subjects who reported a standard IOS score of 1 and 89.33% of the 75 subjects who reported a Step-Choice IOS score of 1. They also represent 21.20% of the 316 overall subjects

Bold indicates the diagonal where standard and Step-Choice IOS scores are equal one to another

Footnotes

Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at https://doi.org/10.1007/s40881-024-00176-4.

1 In total 998 people participated in our experiment. We removed 96 obvious bots who for example copied-and-pasted the instruction text or random text found online. We also removed 257 participants who gave at least two suspicious answers; for example, stating a ZIP code too far from the location inferred from the IP address or reporting being less than 12 years old or greater than 75 years old when they had their first child. All of these exclusion criteria were pre-registered. We also excluded a subject who managed to report a proportion of overlap greater than 1 in the Continuous IOS scale. Our results are similar when we include all 998 participants regardless of whether they satisfy our exclusion criteria.

2 We convert the proportion of overlap x with the thresholds: 1: 0 ≤ x < 0.0711687543 ; 2: 0.0711687543 ≤ x < 0.2144646286 ; 3: 0.2144646286 ≤ x < 0.3594493146 ; 4: 0.3594493146 ≤ x < 0.5017821132 ; 5: 0.5017821132 ≤ x < 0.6441712323 ; 6: 0.6441712323 ≤ x < 0.7889021411 ; 7: 0.7889021411 ≤ x ≤ 1 . To generate the thresholds, we took the proportions of overlaps that correspond to each pair of circles in the standard IOS scale, then divided equally the space around them. The thresholds were pre-registered.

3 We confirm this with Somers’ D = 0.278 , with a 95% confidence interval [0.089, 0.466].

4 We also confirm this with Somers’ D = 0.063 , with a 95% confidence interval [ - 0.124 , 0.251 ] .

5 Also notable is the fact that a number of subjects, who reported a standard IOS score between 1 and 6, report a Continuous IOS score of 7 irrespectively. They represent, however, only 18 subjects out of the 328 in this treatment (about 5.49%). We observe the same phenomenon in the Step-Choice IOS scale, albeit less pronounced (9 out of 316 subjects, 2.85%). We attribute this to a combination of inattention to the IOS task at hand and to the text on the IOS scale task instructions page before where we asked subjects to change the circles so that they are overlapping as much as possible to go to the next page. It is possible that a few inattentive subjects mistook the actual task as an additional attention check and thought they had to report as much overlap as possible in the task itself to go to the next page.

6 Step-Choice IOS scores are also dispersed around their corresponding standard IOS scores, but less so than Continuous IOS scores. This is perhaps not surprising given that, with the standard and the Step-Choice IOS scales, subjects can simply count the number of circles starting from no-overlap to choose the same pair of circles twice.

7 Subjects’ own demographic characteristics enter the computation of demographic dissimilarity. Therefore, one might be worried about collinearity. As we show in Table 2, the results are the same regardless of whether we leave dissimilarity out (in model 2) or include it (in model 3). Further, the VIF also indicates the absence of collinearity with no value above 3.03.

8 https://mathworld.wolfram.com/Circle-CircleIntersection.html, accessed 17/02/2022.

9 We thank Christopher Hammond and Yan Zhuang from Connecticut College who helped us try finding a closed-form solution before we turned to simulations.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

Aron, A, Aron, EN. (1986). Love and the Expansion of Self: Understanding Attraction and Satisfaction, New York, NY, USHemisphere Publishing Corp/Harper & Row Publishers.Google Scholar
Aron, A, Aron, EN, Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 4, 596612. 10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aron, A, Lewandowski, GW, Mashek, D, Aron, EN. (2013). The self-expansion model of motivation and cognition in close relationships. The Oxford Handbook of Close Relationships, Oxford University Press 90115.Google Scholar
Baader, M, Starmer, C, Tufano, F, Gächter, S. (2024). Introducing IOS11 as an extended interactive version of the ‘Inclusion of Other in the Self’ scale to estimate relationship closeness. Scientific Reports, 14, 1, 8901. 10.1038/s41598-024-58042-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beauchamp, JP, Benjamin, DJ, Laibson, DI, Chabris, CF. (2020). Measuring and controlling for the compromise effect when estimating risk preference parameters. Experimental Economics, 23, 4, 10691099. 10.1007/s10683-019-09640-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beranek, B., & Castillo, G. (2024). Similarity and social discounting. HAL Archives Ouvertes hal-04627788, 2024. https://hal.science/hal-04627788Google Scholar
Branand, B, Mashek, D, Aron, A. (2019). Pair-bonding as inclusion of other in the self: A literature review. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2399. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castillo, G. (2021). Preference reversals with social distances. Journal of Economic Psychology, 86, 10.1016/j.joep.2021.102410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, DL, Schonger, M, Wickens, C. (2016). oTree–An open-source platform for laboratory, online, and field experiments. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 9, 8897. 10.1016/j.jbef.2015.12.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Leeuw, JR, Gilbert, RA, Luchterhandt, B. (2023). jsPsych: Enabling an open-source collaborative ecosystem of behavioral experiments. Journal of Open Source Software, 8, 85, 5351. 10.21105/joss.05351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dimant, E. (2024). Hate trumps love: The impact of political polarization on social preferences. Management Science, 70, 1, 131. 10.1287/mnsc.2023.4701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gächter, S, Starmer, C, Tufano, F. (2015). Measuring the closeness of relationships: A comprehensive evaluation of the ‘Inclusion of the Other in the Self’ scale. PLOS One, 10, 6, 10.1371/journal.pone.0129478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gächter, S, Starmer, C, Tufano, F. (2023). Measuring ’Group Cohesion’ to reveal the power of social relationships in team production. Review of Economics and Statistics, 10.1162/rest_a_01283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giamattei, M, Yahosseini, KS, Gächter, S, Molleman, L. (2020). LIONESS Lab: A free web-based platform for conducting interactive experiments online. Journal of the Economic Science Association, 6, 1, 95111. 10.1007/s40881-020-00087-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goette, L, Tripodi, E. (2021). Social influence in prosocial behavior: Evidence from a large-scale experiment. Journal of the European Economic Association, 19, 4, 23732398. 10.1093/jeea/jvaa054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, E, Fiagbenu, ME, Özgümüs, A, Tahamtan, AM, Regner, T. (2021). Who is watching me? Disentangling audience and interpersonal closeness effects in a Pay-What-You-Want context. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 90, 10.1016/j.socec.2020.101631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamphorst, BA, Nauts, S, Blouin-Hudon, E-M. (2017). Introducing a continuous measure of future self-continuity. Social Science Computer Review, 35, 3, 417421. 10.1177/0894439316653513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinley, I., van Vugt, F Tijmen. (2023). (. A jsPsych Plugin for the Inclusion of Other in the Self (IOS) Scale. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/u429fCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Le, B, Moss, WB, Mashek, D. (2007). Assessing relationship closeness online: Moving from an interval-scaled to continuous measure of including others in the self. Social Science Computer Review, 25, 3, 405409. 10.1177/0894439307297693.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robson, M. (2021). Inequality aversion, self-interest and social connectedness. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 183, 744772. 10.1016/j.jebo.2020.12.029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tropp, LR, Wright, SC. (2001). Ingroup identification as the Inclusion of Ingroup in the Self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 585600. 10.1177/0146167201275007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Four different IOS scales

Figure 1

Table 1 Total area and change in the overlap of IOS scales

Figure 2

Fig. 2 Card display used in the experiment

Figure 3

Fig. 3 Average difference between the IOS scores reported with the standard IOS scale and the Continuous or the Step-Choice IOS scale (with bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals)

Figure 4

Fig. 4 Relation between the IOS scores reported with the standard IOS scale and those reported with the Continuous IOS scale

Figure 5

Table 2 How dissimilarity and respondents’ characteristics explain the reported IOS score, ordered logistic regression

Figure 6

Table 3 Estimates from the polynomial fitted to the simulated values

Figure 7

Fig. 5 Presentation of the target

Figure 8

Fig. 6 Target familiarisation task

Figure 9

Fig. 7 Standard IOS scale task instructions

Figure 10

Fig. 8 Standard IOS scale task

Figure 11

Fig. 9 Filler task instructions

Figure 12

Fig. 10 Filler task

Figure 13

Fig. 11 Step-Choice IOS task instructions

Figure 14

Fig. 12 Step-Choice IOS task

Figure 15

Fig. 13 Continuous IOS scale task instructions

Figure 16

Fig. 14 Continuous IOS task

Figure 17

Table 4 Number of subjects, row %, column % and cell % for all combinations of standard IOS score and Continuous IOS score (converted from the proportion of overlap)

Figure 18

Table 5 Number of subjects, row %, column % and cell % for all combinations of standard IOS score and Step-Choice IOS score

Supplementary material: File

Beranek and Castillo supplementary material

Beranek and Castillo supplementary material
Download Beranek and Castillo supplementary material(File)
File 189.2 KB