Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T00:52:19.549Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Dissent, Legitimacy, and Public Support for Court Decisions: Evidence from a Survey-Based Experiment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2024

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Scholars often argue that whereas unanimous rulings should boost public support for court decisions, dissents should fuel public opposition. Previous studies on public responses to U.S. Supreme Court decisions suggest that unanimity does in fact bolster support. However, a recent study has also found that dissents may increase support among opponents of a court decision by suggesting evidence of procedural justice. By examining how individuals react to dissents from the Supreme Court of Norway, this article is the first study outside the U.S. context of the public's reaction to unanimity and dissent. Breaking with the common notion of the negative effects of dissent on public support, the article shows that when the Supreme Court handles cases of higher political salience, the formulation of dissenting opinions can be a meaningful way of securing greater support for its policy outputs by suggesting evidence of procedural justice. Contrary to recent studies, however, this positive influence of dissent is irrespective of individuals' ex ante policy views.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© 2019 Law and Society Association.

Armed with powers of constitutional review, European high courts have experienced increased political significance (Reference SweetSweet 2000, Reference Sweet1992, Reference Sweet2002, Reference Tate and VallinderTate and Vallinder 1995). The judicialization of European politics has involved an expansion of the power of courts and judges in determining public policy outcomes, mainly through judgments pertaining to individual rights' protection or the limits of legislative or executive powers (Reference Hirschl, Caldeira, Kelemen and WhittingtonHirschl 2009). Consequently, it is of increasing importance to understand the factors that influence public support or opposition to court decisions.

The extent to which justices agree or disagree on the solution of a case has often been hypothesized as influencing public support.Footnote 1 Justices and judicial scholars frequently argue that whereas unanimity should boost public support for court decisions, dissent should fuel public opposition (for an overview, see Reference SalamoneSalamone 2014, Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. 2009). This alleged negative relationship between dissent and the courts' standing with the public has been one of the reasons why some European constitutional courts used to (or still do) prohibit the publication of dissenting opinions (Reference KelemenKelemen 2018) and that, for those courts that publish dissents, norms of consensus have tended to suppress the formalization of conflicting opinions in an effort to bolster legitimacy (e.g., Reference Hendershot, Hurwitz, Lanier and PacelleHendershot et al. 2013, Reference Walker, Epstein and DixonWalker et al. 1988).

Despite the presumed harmful effect of judicial dissent on public support, only a handful of empirical studies from the United States exist on this relationship. The results of these studies are also inconsistent. Whereas some studies show no effect of unanimity or dissent on public opinion (Reference Gibson, Caldeira and SpenceGibson et al. 2005, Reference MarshallMarshall 1987, Reference PetersonPeterson 1981), other studies suggest that unanimity does in fact bolster support (Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. 2009). In a recent study, however, Reference SalamoneSalamone (2014) breaks with the accepted notions in the literature and argues that dissents may help increase support of issues of higher salience among the court's policy opponents by suggesting evidence of procedural justice. Hence, the dynamics of how dissent might influence public support appear contingent upon individuals' preexisting attitudes toward the issues at stake.

This article expands upon the existing research on the relationship between judicial dissent and popular support through a series of original survey-based experiments using a nationally representative sample of Norway's population. The Norwegian Supreme Court offers a relevant and testable European case on the relationship between courts and their citizenry. Unlike several of its European counterparts, the Supreme Court has made individual justice opinions and dissents open to the public since 1863 (Reference MestadMestad 2015, Reference ØstlidØstlid 1955). Today, one in five decisions from the court involves one or more dissenting opinions. In addition, during the last few decades, the Supreme Court has emerged as a more prominent and consequential policy maker in the Norwegian judicial and political system (e.g., Reference Grendstad, Shaffer and WaltenburgGrendstad et al. 2015). We should thus expect the Court to have a greater ability to shape public opinion on issues of national interest. Understanding the potential role dissents play in shaping public opinion and support is an important aspect in this regard.

This article examines the relationship between dissent and public support through a series of vignette experiments covering three fictitious Supreme Court decisions on issues that represent a range of legal and political salience among the Norwegian public: (1) a high-salience asylum decision concerning residence permits for children of illegal immigrants, (2) a medium-salience workplace-privacy decision concerning an employer's right to read email and text messages on phones provided by the company, and (3) a low-salience decision concerning whether neighbor conflicts should be resolved by means of mediation or through litigation in the court system. The vignettes that presented the outcomes of each of the fictitious decisions were read by the participants, and information about whether the decisions were unanimous or dissensual was randomized. In addition, the experiments were designed to capture how policy supporters and policy opponents evaluate unanimity and dissent, as well as to account for an individual's diffuse support for the Supreme Court.

Breaking with accepted notions in the literature, the results of the experimental vignettes show no significant influence of unanimous decisions on public acceptance of court decisions. Instead, the results demonstrate that dissent leads to greater acceptance of the high-salience asylum decision by suggesting evidence of procedural justice. This observed effect of dissent on levels of acceptance is significant across both ex ante supporters and ex ante opponents, which is contrary to Reference SalamoneSalamone (2014) who shows that dissent primarily has a positive influence on support among ex ante opponents. However, the finding that dissent has a more general effect on public acceptance is consistent with expectations in existing literature arguing that individuals' support for policy outputs is rooted in their commitments to democratic values and processes (e.g., Reference Caldeira and GibsonCaldeira and Gibson 1992, Reference GibsonGibson 2007, Reference Gibson and NelsonGibson and Nelson 2015). Finally, the article demonstrates a substantial influence of diffuse support on public acceptance across the three court decisions, which underscores the importance for Supreme Courts to maintain a “reservoir” of diffuse support or legitimacy if they are to gain acceptance for decisions that are unpopular among segments of the population.

In the following sections, the article first discusses the widespread concerns in the literature about the public policy consequences of dissents while also making a case for why we instead should expect dissents to have a positive influence on public support for court decisions. This article then presents the experimental design and data generation process. Finally, the results of the vignette experiments are discussed, and the conclusion summarizes the article's main contributions and offers suggestions for future studies.

Judicial Dissent and Public Support for Court Decisions

When high courts review laws, justices must sometimes confront disagreements inherent in the legislative context on issues over which legislators and society may be deeply divided. However, because justices are usually appointed and not elected, they confront these issues without being electorally accountable. Consequently, scholars often argue that courts, perhaps more than the elected branches of government, require a reservoir of legitimacy and public support to gain acceptance for unpopular decisions (see, e.g., Reference Gibson and NelsonGibson and Nelson 2014, Reference Gibson, Caldeira and SpenceGibson et al. 2005).

Several studies have demonstrated the number of ways in which courts may influence public opinion and support (see, e.g., Reference Clawson, Kegler and WaltenburgClawson et al. 2001, Reference Franklin and KosakiFranklin and Kosaki 1989, Reference Gibson and CaldeiraGibson and Caldeira 2003, Reference Grosskopf and MondakGrosskopf and Mondak 1998, Reference Hoekstra and SegalHoekstra and Segal 1996, Reference Johnson and MartinJohnson and Martin 1998, Reference MondakMondak, 1990, Reference Mondak1991, Reference Mondak1992, Reference Stoutenborough, Haider-Markel and AllenStoutenborough et al. 2006). According to this literature, court decisions, particularly on issues of national interest, may have an impact on public opinion, which can manifest itself either as increased support or increased opposition to the position taken by the court. However, there has been little research on the role of judicial dissent in influencing mass opinion. This is puzzling, given that justices and scholars in the United States and elsewhere have frequently debated whether dissents are good or bad for public support.

On the one hand, judicial scholars frequently express concern about the public opinion consequences of handing down dissensual decisions (see, e.g., Reference Hettinger, Lindquist and MartinekHettinger et al. 2006, Reference SalamoneSalamone 2014, Reference Walker, Epstein and DixonWahlbeck et al. 1988, Reference Wahlbeck, Spriggs and Maltzman1999, Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. 2009). A common argument is that whereas the absence of dissent may promote the perception that the law is applied in a uniform and impartial manner, the occurrence of dissent may “shake public confidence in the judiciary by bringing into question the certainty of the law” (Reference Walker, Epstein and DixonWalker et al. 1988, 387). This alleged negative relationship between dissent and public confidence echoes the widely held view that the legitimacy of high courts is contingent upon the extent to which the public perceives them to be apolitical and neutral adjudicators of the law (see, e.g., Reference Gibson and NelsonGibson and Nelson 2017, Reference Scheb and LyonsScheb and Lyons 2001). According to these views, dissent should have a generally harmful effect on public support, and this should be true regardless of whether individuals hold a favorable or unfavorable attitude toward the courts' policy output.

On the other hand, dissent may help democratize a court by bringing different viewpoints to the deliberative process (see, e.g., Reference BrennanBrennan 1985, Reference DubéDubé 2000, Reference PetersonPeterson 1981). Contrary to conventional views on the harm of dissent, such a democratizing effect of dissents might lead to greater support for dissensual decisions. This argument rests on studies that have demonstrated that legitimacy and support for courts are rooted in commitment to democratic values and processes (Reference Caldeira and GibsonCaldeira and Gibson 1992, Reference GibsonGibson 2007, Reference Gibson and NelsonGibson and Nelson 2015). In addition, studies in the social psychology of law and legal compliance suggest that perceptions of procedural justice and the fairness of the resolution process are central to our evaluation of judicial outcomes (see, e.g., Reference TylerTyler 1988, Reference Tyler2006, Reference Tyler and RasinskiTyler and Rasinski 1991). Accordingly, when individuals see evidence of debate in court—as shown by dissenting opinions—they might “interpret it as the result of a fair, democratic decision-making process in which both sides were heard” (Reference SalamoneSalamone 2014, 322). This positive influence of dissent on acceptance might be particularly prevalent among the “losers” of a court decision. Whereas “winners” will ordinarily accept a court decision with which they agree (Reference Gibson and NelsonGibson and Nelson 2014), losers should be more inclined to make evaluations of procedural fairness when they decide whether to support a decision or not (Reference Lind and TylerLind and Tyler 1988, 111).

Of course, whether unanimity or dissent can influence public support is not expected to be uniform across all issues. The extent to which courts can influence individuals' acceptance of a decision by means of unanimity and dissent should depend on the salience of the issue at hand (see Reference SalamoneSalamone 2014, Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. 2009). For example, studies of public evaluations of U.S. Supreme Court decisions demonstrate that the Court is less able to influence opinion when a policy is of high ideological salience (Reference Brickman and PetersonBrickman and Peterson 2006, Reference Grosskopf and MondakGrosskopf and Mondak 1998, Reference Hoekstra and SegalHoekstra and Segal 1996, Reference Johnson and MartinJohnson and Martin 1998, Reference MondakMondak 1990, Reference Mondak1991, Reference Mondak1992). This finding is supported by studies in social psychology (e.g., Reference Petty, Cacioppo and BerkowitzPetty and Cacioppo 1986) and political behavior (e.g., Reference ZallerZaller 1992), demonstrating that individuals are more difficult to persuade when they hold strong (or crystallized) opinions about a topic but are persuadable when they are moderately (or to a lesser degree) engaged with an issue.

On this basis, unanimity and dissent should have little influence on individuals' evaluations of court outputs on highly salient (“hot-button”) issues on which they hold strong and crystallized opinions. However, on moderately salient issues, unanimity and dissent might invoke a reaction consistent with any of the expectations discussed above. Because of the moderate levels of salience, opinions on these issues should not be so crystallized in the minds of the public that they cannot be moved. However, at the same time, people should feel enough attachment to these issues so that it might matter for them if the decision is made unanimously in their (dis)favor or whether a dissenting minority is seen to represent their policy views. Finally, courts should quite easily be able to move opinion on issues of very low salience, as individuals should be quite willing to defer to perceived experts on matters in which they have not invested much thought. However, as people will be less politically invested in these issues, we should not expect dissent to invoke any strong feelings of procedural justice (see Reference SalamoneSalamone 2014, Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. 2009).

Previous Studies of the Public Opinion Consequences of Dissent

Despite widespread concerns about the public policy consequences of dissent, the first few studies on this topic found little evidence of this effect. In a review of the literature on the causes and consequences of judicial dissent in the Unites States, Reference PetersonPeterson (1981) noted that dissent might weaken a court's authority, but he argued that this was unlikely because of the limited public awareness of the workings of the courts. A few years later, Reference MarshallMarshall (1987) conducted a study of opinion polls before and after 18 U.S. Supreme Court decisions. Among several factors believed to affect public reactions to court decisions, Marshall included a measure of whether decisions were unanimous or not. His results showed no significant effect of unanimity on public opinion. However, it should be noted that only two of the decisions in Marshall's sample were unanimous.

Two decades after Marshall's study, Reference Gibson, Caldeira and SpenceGibson et al. (2005) included dissent among several other factors in an experimental vignette study about a fictional case dealing with the handling of the Florida ballots in the wake of the 2000 presidential election. Reference MarshallUnlike Marshall (1987), the authors focused on factors affecting public acquiescence to court decisions. For the specific manipulation of dissent, the participants were given vignettes characterized either as “deeply divided along partisan lines” or as “consensual and therefore not divided by party.” Again, however, the results showed little evidence for the hypothesis that sharp divisions lead to lower acceptance of the outcomes of court decisions.

Two subsequent experimental studies would yield different (though conflicting) results. In the first experiment, Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. (2009) presented individuals with mock newspaper articles reporting on Supreme Court decisions in which they systematically varied unanimity and dissent and the court's upholding or overturning precedents. Reference Gibson, Caldeira and SpenceUnlike Gibson et al. (2005), the authors included a manipulation for dissent independent of partisanship (i.e., “unanimous” or “minimum winning coalition”). In addition, they conducted experiments on a range of cases on different political issues of varying levels of salience. Contrasting previous studies, they found that unanimous rulings (as opposed to dissenting ones) boost support for Supreme Court decisions at all levels of salience and among all participants, including those who disagreed with the court's policy output.

In a second experiment, Reference SalamoneSalamone (2014) sought to improve on the two former studies by examining the influence of unanimity and dissent in isolation from other factors. In addition, Salamone considered the distinction between large and small dissenting majorities by separating out decisions that were unanimous, divided eight-to-one, or divided five-to-four. The results of the study showed no effect of unanimity or dissent among those who stated that they agreed with the policy outcome of the court's decisions. However, among those who disagreed with the court's policy outcome, Salamone found that unanimity and large majorities led to higher acceptance of the low-salience contract dispute resolution and, conversely, that dissensual decisions (including both five-to-four and eight-to-one decisions) led to higher acceptance among the court's opponents in a moderately salient workplace-privacy decision by suggesting evidence of procedural justice.

Overall, recent experimental studies have demonstrated that unanimity (as opposed to dissent) might foster higher levels of acceptance of Supreme Court decisions (in particular, Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. 2009), though this appears to be the case primarily with issues of low salience (Reference SalamoneSalamone 2014). In addition, Reference SalamoneSalamone's (2014) study suggests that we should include the possibility that dissent may foster greater levels of support for moderately salient issues by suggesting evidence of procedural justice.

The Supreme Court of Norway and the Public

Little research exists on the relationship between dissent and public opinion and support outside the U.S. context. In this regard, the Supreme Court of Norway constitutes a relevant and testable European case. The Supreme Court began to publish individual justice opinions, including those supporting dissents, in 1863 (Reference MestadMestad 2015). The decision to publish individual opinions did not come about without debate. For over four decades, the Supreme Court fought against proposals in parliament to make opinions public. A central reason for the court's resistance was that it had serious concerns about its reputation with the public (Reference ØstlidØstlid 1955).

The number of dissents in the Supreme Court has varied over time. In the decades following World War II, the annual percentage of dissensual decisions fluctuated between 10 and 20% before reaching its lowest level in the late 1980s (Reference Grendstad, Shaffer and WaltenburgGrendstad et al. 2015, 69). Then, during the last two decades, the number of dissents increased in the 1990s, reaching a record high of 29% in 1999, before stabilizing at a rate of 20% annually in the twenty-first century. The rise in dissents in the 1990s was mainly catalyzed by a reform of the criminal procedures in 1995, which secured the Court near-complete discretionary jurisdiction over its criminal caseload (Reference BentsenBentsen 2017). Since then, the court has only accepted cases for review that are expected to be instrumental to the development of Norwegian law (Reference BentsenBentsen 2017, Reference Grendstad, Shaffer and WaltenburgGrendstad et al. 2015, Reference Skiple, Grendstad, Shaffer and WaltenburgSkiple et al. 2016, Reference SundeSunde 2015).

Citizens usually rely on the media for information about court cases (Reference Clawson, Kegler and WaltenburgClawson et al. 2001, Reference Stoutenborough, Haider-Markel and AllenStoutenborough et al. 2006, Reference ZilisZilis 2015). Historically, the Norwegian media's coverage of the Supreme Court has been irregular and sporadic (Reference Grendstad, Shaffer, Waltenburg, Davis and TarasGrendstad et al. 2017). The depoliticized nature of judicial appointments to the court has been identified as a central reason for this.Footnote 2 In addition, because the Norwegian legal system is highly trusted, the Supreme Court justices have seen little need to use the media to maintain or increase the Court's standing among its citizens (Reference Grendstad, Shaffer, Waltenburg, Davis and TarasGrendstad et al. 2017). Hence, it is reasonable to assume that the public has been unaware of the daily workings and decisions of the Supreme Court.

However, in the last few decades, the Supreme Court has emerged as a more prominent and consequential policy maker in the Norwegian judicial and political system (e.g., Reference Grendstad, Shaffer and WaltenburgGrendstad et al. 2015). Consequently, it has come under greater scrutiny by the media. In particular, in their reporting on the Supreme Court, the media has given greater attention to individual justices, and this appears especially true when referencing those justices authoring or joining dissents (Reference Grendstad, Shaffer, Waltenburg, Davis and TarasGrendstad et al. 2017, 249). Therefore, we can assume that today's Supreme Court will have a greater potential to shape public opinion on issues of national interest. Understanding the role dissents play in shaping public opinion and support is an important aspect in this regard.

Experimental Design

To examine the relationship between Supreme Court dissent and public support, I use a series of survey-based vignette experiments included in Wave 7 of the Norwegian Citizen Panel (NCP) conducted from November 1, 2016, to December 2, 2016. The NCP is a research-purpose web-based panel owned by the University of Bergen. The participants in the NCP have been recruited via random sampling from the official national population registry and are representative of both the online and offline population older than 18 years of age in Norway. In total, the data for the present experiment comprises 1085 registered participants.Footnote 3

The experimental survey covered three issue areas that represent a range of legal and political salience among the Norwegian public: a high-salience asylum decision concerning the issuing of residence permits for children of illegal immigrants that are being deported, a medium-salience workplace-privacy decision concerning an employer's right to read the emails and text messages on a phone provided by the company, and a low-salience decision concerning whether neighbor conflicts should be resolved by means of mediation or through litigation in the court system. Whereas asylum issues—particularly those involving the rights of children—have been of high salience in Norwegian public debate in the aftermath of the European “refugee crisis” of 2015 (see Reference Skiple, Gudbrandsen and GrendstadSkiple et al. 2013), the other issue areas were selected based on their relative degree of public salience. This difference in issue area and salience is important in order to address the differential influence of unanimity and dissent on public opinion discussed above.

In total, the experiment comprised two parts: (1) a pretreatment questionnaire asking the participants about their policy positions on the above three issues and about their diffuse support (or legitimacy) of the Supreme Court and (2) a series of vignettes describing fictitious Supreme Court decisions, including a posttreatment question about the participants' acceptance of the decisions. Prior to the experiment, the participants were asked to read the following short text about the Supreme Court: You will now be asked a few questions about the Norwegian Supreme Court. The Supreme Court is the highest court in the country and last court of appeal. Ordinary cases are usually decided by a panel of five justices.” This information was provided because we may assume that several of the participants in the survey were unaware of the fact that most Supreme Court decisions are made by five-justice panels. (A complete and translated overview of the pretreatment questionnaire and of the experimental vignettes is available in the Appendix).

Pretreatment Questionnaire

Prior to treatment, all participants were first asked questions about their policy positions on the three separate issues (these questions can be found in the Appendix, Section A). To avoid priming the participants, these questions were asked together with a range of other unrelated questions in the survey (this procedure is similar to Reference SalamoneSalamone 2014). That said, we are unable to fully eliminate the possibility that individuals, by virtue of already having declared their attitudes on the issues earlier in the survey, have been put in a position of reconciling their attitudes with the position of the Supreme Court and thus chose their own position over the position of the Supreme Court. However, this type of priming should not negatively influence treatment effects, given that in a situation in which individuals are fully primed to follow their ex ante positions, we should not expect unanimity or dissent to have any substantial influence on their opinions. In other words, if individuals are primed to follow their initial policy position, the experiment should constitute a stricter test of the influence of unanimity and dissent.

The answers to the ex ante policy questions were later used to create measures indicating whether the position of a participant prior to treatment was the same as that described in the vignette randomly assigned to her (or not). Accordingly, if a participant was given a vignette of a Supreme Court decision reflecting her policy positions on the issue, she is considered an ex ante supporter of the decision. If a participant was given a vignette with a Supreme Court decision opposing her policy position, she is considered an ex ante opponent.

Table 1 shows the distributions of the participants' responses to the three policy statements. As expected, we can see that the participants are highly polarized in their position on the high-salience asylum issue, while the medium-salience workplace-privacy issue and the low-salience neighbor dispute issue produce less polarization. Looking at the proportions of participants agreeing or disagreeing, 39% agree that children of illegal immigrants should be allowed to stay in Norway if their parents are evicted, while 44% disagree with this statement. On the workplace-privacy issue, 21% agree that employers should have a right to read texts and emails on company cell phones, while 72% disagree. Finally, as many as 91% of the participants agree that neighbor conflicts ought to be resolved by mediation and not in the courts, while only 4% disagree.

Table 1. Individuals' Ex Ante Policy Positions

Note: The variables were coded on a seven-point scale: 1 = strongly agree, 2 = agree, 3 = somewhat agree, 4 = neither agree nor disagree, 5 = somewhat disagree, 6 = disagree, and 7 = strongly disagree. Data: Norwegian Citizen Panel Wave 7, 2016. N = 1085.

In addition to the questions about their policy preferences, the participants were given four questions about the Supreme Court's institutional legitimacy (the questions can be found in the Appendix, Section A). These questions are designed to capture the participant's diffuse support for the institution (Reference Caldeira and GibsonCaldeira and Gibson 1992, Reference Gibson, Caldeira and SpenceGibson et al. 2003, Reference Gibson, Caldeira and Spence2005) and are commonly used in the literature on judicial support and legitimacy (see, e.g., Reference Bartels and JohnstonBartels and Johnston 2013, Reference Gibson and CaldeiraGibson and Caldeira 1995, Reference Gibson and NelsonGibson and Nelson 2015, Reference SalamoneSalamone 2014, Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. 2009).Footnote 4 Whereas specific support refers to the court's decisions (i.e., what they are asked to evaluate in this experiment), diffuse support refers to a reservoir of good will or favorable attitudes that help individuals tolerate outputs that they oppose (Reference EastonEaston, 1965, Reference Easton1975, Reference Gibson, Caldeira and SpenceGibson et al. 2003, Reference Gibson and NelsonGibson and Nelson 2014). Reference Caldeira and GibsonCaldeira and Gibson (1992) conceptualize diffuse support for judicial institutions as synonymous with legitimacy. This is an important control in the analysis because we should assume that those with low faith in the institution would generally have a lower likelihood of accepting its decisions (e.g., Reference Gibson, Caldeira and SpenceGibson et al. 2005) and that this should be true regardless of whether the decision is unanimous or dissensual (Reference SalamoneSalamone 2014).

Experimental Vignettes

After completing the pretreatment questionnaire, the participants were exposed to a series of vignettes about three fictitious Supreme Court decisions on the above issues (the vignettes can be found in the Appendix, Section B).Footnote 5 The participants were randomly assigned to three groups: a baseline group in which participants were given no information about unanimity or dissents; a treatment group presented with a unanimous decision; and a treatment group presented with a dissensual three-two decision (i.e., the experiment is a 2x2 with a baseline group). In addition, the experiment randomly varied the outcome of the decision in order to control for the possibility that the results are driven by the participants' policy preferences for or against the decision.

For each vignette, the participants were asked about the degree to which they find the decision acceptable (coded 1 to 5, with larger scores indicating greater acceptance). This is in line with a common research design in the literature on courts and public opinion, which usually involves presenting individuals with a court decision with which they either agree or disagree and then ascertaining whether the characteristics of the court ruling increase the probability of an individual accepting the decision or not (e.g., Reference Gibson, Caldeira and SpenceGibson et al. 2005, Reference SalamoneSalamone 2014, Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. 2009).Footnote 6 The wording of the experimental vignettes was as follows (the following example is for the asylum decision):

Several immigrant children have parents who are residing illegally in Norway. Imagine that the Supreme Court [no information; unanimously; by a bare majority of three against two judges] decided that these children [should be; should not be] entitled to stay in Norway if the parents are deported. In general, to what extent do you think this would be an acceptable decision by the Supreme Court?

Analysis and Results

To examine the relationship between dissent and public support, I ran two sets of logistic regression analyses of the extent to which individuals accept or reject each of the three fictitious court decisions. Whereas the first set of analyses examines the effects of the main treatment (unanimity/dissent), the second set of analyses tests whether unanimity and dissent have a differential influence on acceptance rates among ex ante supporters and ex ante opponents.

Positive Influence of Dissent on Acceptance of High-Salience Issues

The first set of analyses examine the general influence of the treatments (unanimous/dissent) when controlling for diffuse support. For each decision, the analyses compare the effect of each treatment category to the baseline referent category for which the participants received no information regarding majority size. The results of the regressions are shown in Table 2. In addition, Figure 1 presents the change in terms of the probability of acceptance of each of the court decisions for the various treatment groups compared with the untreated baseline group.

Table 2. Dissent Has a Positive Effect on Support for Asylum Decision

* p < .05 (two tailed).

Note: Effects of unanimity and dissent on support for court decisions. Effects compared to the baseline group. Data: Norwegian Citizen Panel Wave 7, 2016.

Figure 1. Change in the Probability of Acceptance Across Treatment Groups for Each of the Three Court Decisions (95% Confidence Intervals). Data: Norwegian Citizen Panel Wave 7, 2016.

Looking at the treatment effects of unanimity and dissent, there are no negative effects of dissent on levels of acceptance for any of the three decisions. Instead, there is a positive and statistically significant effect of dissent on participants' acceptance of the high-salience asylum decision. More specifically, there is a 9% increase in the levels of acceptance of the dissensual decision when compared to the levels of support in the untreated baseline group. This finding supports the alternative theoretical expectation that dissensual decisions might lead to greater levels of acceptance by suggesting evidence of procedural justice (e.g., Reference SalamoneSalamone 2014).

The effects of the treatments on the two other issues indicate further support for this finding. There is a 4% increase in acceptance of the dissensual privacy decision and a close to 4% increase in acceptance of the dissensual neighbor conflict decision. However, the differences in acceptance rates across these treatment groups fail standard statistical significance tests. Thus, it is only on the high-salience asylum issue that dissent has any positive and generalizable influence on citizens' acceptance of the court decision.

The results show some support for the hypothesis that unanimity might foster greater acceptance of the asylum decision and the neighbor conflict decision. There is a 5% increase in acceptance of the unanimous asylum decision and a 5% increase in support for the neighbor conflict decision (when compared to the untreated baseline group). However, these differences fail statistical significance tests, and we are unable to conclude whether this is in fact the case for the public at large. Nevertheless, these findings are important insofar as they suggest that individuals' evaluations of the court's policy output do not appear to increase significantly because of unanimity.

Finally, there is a positive and statistically significant influence of diffuse support on acceptance rates across the three court decisions. Hence, when citizens are asked to evaluate Supreme Court decisions, they will have a greater propensity to accept the decision if they have a high regard for the Supreme Court as an institution from the outset. This is an important finding insofar as it confirms the theoretical assumptions and findings in the previous empirical literature that high courts rely on diffuse support to gain acceptance of their decisions (e.g., Reference Gibson, Caldeira and SpenceGibson et al. 2005, Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. 2009).

Ex Ante Policy Views

In the second set of analyses, I include interaction terms between the treatments and the variable capturing whether individuals are either ex ante supporters or ex ante opponents of the decision. These interactions are included to test whether unanimity or dissent have a different influence on acceptance rates among ex ante supporters and ex ante opponents. It is noted, however, that individuals' support or opposition to the decisions is not randomly assigned. While we are able to randomize whether individuals are given a favorable or unfavorable decision based on their preexisting attitudes, we are naturally unable to randomize individuals' preexisting policy positions on these issues. Consequently, a number of covariates might predict the ex ante policy views that form the basis for the agreement determination. The following analysis thus includes a number of control variables related to partisanship (i.e., whether they would vote for either a left-of-center or a right-of-center party in the next election) and sociodemographic factors (i.e., the participants' gender, education, and age), which might be related to the participants' preexisting policy positions on the three issues.

The results of the second sets of regressions are presented in Table 3. First, we see that there is a statistically significant and substantial influence of ex ante policy positions on acceptance across all three decisions. Because of the added interaction term, the effects of the ex ante opponent variable are interpreted as the difference in acceptance between ex ante opponents and ex ante supporters in the baseline group: for this treatment group, ex ante opponents have a 53% lower level of acceptance for the asylum decision when compared to ex ante supporters. For the privacy and neighbor conflict decisions, the difference in acceptance between supporters and opponents is 61% and 68%.

Table 3. No Conditional Effects of Ex Ante Policy Views

* p < .05 (two tailed).

Note: Effects of unanimity and dissent on support for court decisions conditional on ex ante policy views. Effects compared to the baseline group. Data: Norwegian Citizen Panel Wave 7, 2016.

However, although there is a large difference between ex ante supporters and ex ante opponents in their posttreatment evaluations of all three court decisions, there are no statistically significant effects of the added interaction terms between the treatments (unanimous/dissent) and the participants' ex ante policy views. Hence, the extent to which unanimity or dissent influences acceptance rates across the three issues is not conditional on whether individuals are predisposed to either support or oppose the decisions they were asked to evaluate.

Again, there is statistically significant effect of diffuse support on acceptance rates across all issues. Figure 2 shows the levels of acceptance for each decision conditional on the participants' pretreatment policy views. Among both ex ante supporters and ex ante opponents, the levels of acceptance of each court decision increases as the levels of diffuse support increases. If we look specifically at ex ante opponents, the influence of high levels of diffuse support is the strongest among those who were given the high-salience asylum decision. For this decision, the acceptance levels are almost 50% higher among those who hold the Court in the highest regard when compared to those who hold it in very low regard. Consequently, when citizens are asked to evaluate unfavorable and politically salient Supreme Court decisions, they will have a greater propensity to accept the decision if from the onset they have a high regard for the Supreme Court as an institution.

Figure 2. Effects of Diffuse Support on Ex Ante supporters' and Ex Ante opponents' Acceptance of the Asylum Decision (Upper Left Graph), the Privacy Decision (Upper Right Graph), and the Neighbor Conflict Decision (Lower Left Graph) (95% Confidence Intervals). Data: Norwegian Citizen Panel Wave 7, 2016.

Discussions and Conclusion

As European courts are experiencing growing political power (e.g., Reference SweetHirschl 2004, Sweet 1992, Reference Sweet2000, Reference Sweet2002, Reference Tate and VallinderTate and Vallinder 1995), it is of increasing importance to understand what factors influence popular support or opposition to court decisions. One important aspect in this regard concerns the public's evaluations of unanimity and dissent. However, despite widespread concerns about the public policy consequences of dissents, the literature on the relationship between dissent and public opinion is sparse and limited to the U.S. public's evaluations of Supreme Court decisions.

This article offers the first study of public reactions to judicial dissent outside the U.S. context by conducting a series of survey-based vignette experiments on a nationally representative sample of Norway's population. The vignettes covered three fictitious Supreme Court decisions on issues that represent a range of legal and political salience among the Norwegian public. In addition, the experiment was designed to capture how policy supporters and policy opponents evaluate dissensual decisions in order to test the alternative expectation that policy opponents might show greater support for a dissensual decision because they see their policy views represented in the outcome.

Breaking with accepted notions in the literature, the experimental results show no significant influence of unanimous decisions on public acceptance of court decisions. In addition, there is no evidence in the experimental data suggesting that dissent has a negative influence on people's support for court decisions. Instead, the results demonstrate that dissent leads to greater acceptance of the high-salience asylum decision by suggesting evidence of procedural justice. This observed effect of dissent on the levels of acceptance is significant across both ex ante supporters and ex ante opponents, which is contrary to the expectation that opponents (or “losers”) of a court decision should be more inclined than supporters (“winners”) to make evaluations of procedural justices when they decide to oppose a decision or not. This is also contrary to the experimental results by Reference SalamoneSalamone (2014), which shows that dissent primarily has a positive influence on support among ex ante opponents. However, that dissent has a more general effect on public acceptance is consistent with expectations that individuals' support for policy outputs is rooted in their commitments to democratic values and processes (e.g., Reference Caldeira and GibsonCaldeira and Gibson 1992, Reference GibsonGibson 2007, Reference Gibson and NelsonGibson and Nelson 2015).

Dissent only moves opinion on the high-salience asylum issue, which is contrary to the theoretical expectation that courts should have a difficult time influencing public opinion on “hot-button” issues on which individuals hold strong and crystallized opinions. There are two potential explanations for this result. First, we might assume that when the public is highly polarized on an issue, which is arguably the case concerning the rights of immigrant children in Norway, they might expect justices to be polarized on the issue as well. Evidence that both sides of the issue have been heard—as shown by the dissenting opinion—should therefore matter more when public opinion is polarized. To put it more succinctly, procedural justice might play a greater role in the evaluations of court decisions on issues on which the public is polarized, which might explain why dissent leads to greater levels of acceptance of the high-salience asylum decision. Future studies could address the relationship between issue polarization and procedural justice more directly.

The second explanation regards the relative salience of the asylum issue. Although questions about the rights of immigrant children have been of high salience in Norwegian public debate in the wake of the 2015 European “immigration crisis,” it might be considered less salient that those “hot-button” issues included in existing experimental studies in the United States (e.g., gay marriage, Florida ballots, abortion). This is especially true if we consider the decisive role the U.S. Supreme Court plays in deciding these issues. Hence, the observed effect of dissent on individuals' acceptance of the asylum issue might instead be regarded as evidence that courts should have an easier time moving public opinion on moderately salient issues on which opinions are not fully crystallized in the minds of the public.

In addition to the effects of dissent on acceptance rates, there is a substantial influence of diffuse support on public acceptance across the three court decisions. Most importantly, there is a relatively strong influence of high levels of diffuse support on acceptance rates among ex ante opponents of the high-salience asylum decisions. This is an important finding insofar as it confirms the theoretical assumptions and findings in the previous empirical literature that high courts rely on diffuse support to gain acceptance of unpopular decisions (e.g., Reference Gibson, Caldeira and SpenceGibson et al. 2005, Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. 2009). Moreover, this result underscores the importance for Supreme Courts to maintain a “reservoir” of diffuse support or legitimacy if they are to gain acceptance for decisions that are unpopular among segments of the population.

Finally, as this is the first empirical study outside the U.S. context to suggest that dissent may bolster public acceptance of court decisions, the constraints of this one-time experiment still leave unanswered questions about the long-term effects of unanimity and dissents. While one possible implication of this study is that courts in the long term might bolster their legitimacy by means of publishing dissents, more research on such a long-term view would need to be considered if we are to reach this conclusion.

Appendix A. Pretreatment Questionnaire

For each question, the participants were asked to what extent they agree or disagree with the statement. Their answers were measured on a scale from 1 (strongly agree) to 7 (strongly disagree).

Ex Ante Policy Positions

In general, to what extent do you disagree or agree with the following statements?

  1. 1. Immigrant children whose parents are illegal residents in Norway should be entitled to stay in the country if their parents are deported.

  2. 2. If an employer gives you a mobile phone to be used for work, the employer should be allowed to read all text messages and emails sent and received on this phone.

  3. 3. It should not be possible to bring neighbor conflicts (e.g., about the height of trees and fences) to the court without first having tried to resolve the conflict through mediation.

Diffuse Support for the Supreme Court

In general, to what extent do you disagree or agree with the following statements?

  1. 1. The Supreme Court of Norway can usually be trusted to make the right decisions.

  2. 2. The right of the Supreme Court of Norway to decide certain types of controversial issues should be reduced.

  3. 3. The decisions of the Supreme Court of Norway consistently favor some groups more than others.

  4. 4. If the Supreme Court of Norway started making many decisions most people disagreed with, it might be better to do away with the court altogether.

Appendix B. Experimental Vignettes

For each vignette, the participants were asked about their acceptance of the fictitious court decision. This was measured on a scale from 1 to 5, where higher values indicate greater levels of acceptance.

Vignette for High-Salience Decision

Several immigrant children have parents who are residing illegally in Norway. Imagine that the Supreme Court [no information, unanimously, or by a bare majority of three against two judges] decided that these children [should be or should not be] entitled to stay in Norway if their parents are deported. In general, to what extent do you think this would be an acceptable decision by the Supreme Court?

Vignette for Medium-Salience Decision

Several employers give their employees cell phones to use for work. Imagine that the Supreme Court [no information, unanimously, or by a bare majority of three against two judges] decided that employers [should be or should not be] allowed to read all the text messages and emails contained on the telephones issued to the employees by the company. In general, to what extent do you think this would be an acceptable decision by the Supreme Court?

Vignette for Low-Salience Decision

Thousands of neighbor conflicts arise in Norway every year. Imagine that the Supreme Court [no information, unanimously, or by a bare majority of three against two judges] decided that people involved in neighbor conflicts (e.g., about the height of trees and fences) [should be or should not be] allowed to go directly to the courts without first having tried to resolve the conflict through mediation. In general, to what extent do you think this would be an acceptable decision by the Supreme Court?

Footnotes

1 Terms like “support,” “acquiescence,” “confidence,” and “legitimacy” are used interchangeably throughout the article depending on the terms used in the relevant literature.

2 Upon the appointment of a new justice, an independent Judicial Appointments Board, consisting of five representatives from the legal community and two lay members, will evaluate applicants and send a ranked list to the Ministry of Justice. The ministry will then transmit its preferred nominee to the King in Council for a formal (and symbolic) appointment. Although the ministry is not obliged to follow the board's ranking, it has made no changes to its ordering.

3 For more information about the Norwegian Citizen Panel, see http://digsscore.uib.no/panel. Documentation of the field methods used, response rates, and representativeness is reported in the online methodology report, while the data and codebook are available gratis from the Norwegian Centre for Research Data (NSD). Data and documentation can be downloaded from http://digsscore.uib.no/data-and-documentation. A Stata script for the replication of the study is also available.

4 I made a change to the wording of one of the statements used to capture diffuse support in previous studies of the U.S. Supreme Court. It is common to ask individuals to evaluate the following statement: The United States Supreme Court can usually be trusted to make decisions that are right for the country as a whole. When designing the experiment, concerns were raised about the extent to which Norwegian citizens would ever expect the Supreme Court to make decisions for the country as a whole (the Norwegian Supreme Court does not usually make decisions as far-reaching as those of the U.S. Supreme Court). Thus, a simplified version of the question was included, stating that The Supreme Court of Norway can usually be trusted to make the right decisions.

5 The use of fictitious cases might lead to a lower external validity of the results. Hence, in an effort to minimize issues of external validity, the selected issues were loosely based on actual and potential court cases.

6 Unlike similar studies by Reference SalamoneSalamone (2014) and Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. (2009), I do not ask individuals whether they agree (or not) with the decision before asking about the extent to which they accept the decision. Instead, I ask individuals directly about their acceptance of the decision, assuming that those who receive a decision that is contrary to their previously stated policy position on the issue are in fact in disagreement with the outcome of the decision. Another difference from previous studies concerns how I ask individuals about acceptance. The most common question in the literature asks, Do you accept the decision? That is, do you think that the decision ought to be accepted and considered the final word on the matter or that there ought to be an effort to challenge the decision and get it changed? Again, in the preparations for the experiment, concerns were raised about the extent to which Norwegian citizens would ever feel that there are ways to challenge a Supreme Court decision. Following this discussion, a simplified version of this question was developed, asking only Generally speaking, to what extent do you mean this would be an acceptable decision by the Supreme Court?

References

Bartels, Brandon L. & Johnston, Christopher D. (2013) “On the Ideological Foundations of Supreme Court Legitimacy in the American Public,” 57 American J. of Political Science 184–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentsen, Henrik Litleré (2017) “Court Leadership, Agenda Transformation, and Judicial Dissent: A European Case of a 'European Demise of Consensual Norms',” 6 J. of Law and Courts 189213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brennan, William J. 1985. In Defense of Dissents. Available at: http://repository.uchastings.edu/tobriner/17 (accessed 6 September 2017).Google Scholar
Brickman, Danette & Peterson, David A. M. (2006) “Public Opinion Reactions to Repeated Events: Citizen Response to Multiple Supreme Court Abortion Decisions,” 28 Political Behavior 87112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caldeira, Gregory A. & Gibson, James L. (1992) “The Etiology of Public Support for the Supreme Court,” 36 American J. of Political Science 635–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clawson, Rosalee A., Kegler, Elisabeth R., & Waltenburg, Eric N. (2001) “The Legitimacy-Conferring Authority of the U.S. Supreme Court. An Experimental Design,” 26 American Politics Research 566–91.Google Scholar
Dubé, Claire L' Heureux (2000) “The Dissenting Opinion: Voice of the Future?38 Osgoode Hall Law J. 495518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Easton, David (1965) A Systems Analysis of Political Life. New York: John Wiley & Son, Inc.Google Scholar
Easton, David (1975) “A re-Assessment of the Concept of Political Support,” 5 British J. of Political Science 435–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, Charles H. & Kosaki, Liane C. (1989) “Republican Schoolmaster: The U.S. Supreme Court, Public Opinion, and Abortion,” 83 The American Political Science Rev. 751–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, James L. (2007) “The Legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court in a Polarized Polity,” 4 J. of Empirical Legal Studies 507–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L. & Caldeira, Gregory A. (1995) “The Legitimacy of Transnational Legal Institutions: Compliance, Support and the European Court of Justice,” 39 American J. of Political Science 459–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L. & Caldeira, Gregory A. (2003) “Defenders of Democracy? Legitimacy, Popular Acceptance, and the South African Constitutional Court,” 65 The J. of Politics 130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L., Caldeira, Gregory A., & Spence, Lester Kenyatta (2003) “Measuring Attitudes toward the United States Supreme Court,” 47 American J. of Political Science 354–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L., Caldeira, Gregory A., & Spence, Lester Kenyatta (2005) “Why Do People Accept Public Policies they Oppose? Testing Legitimacy Theory with a Survey Based Experiment,” 58 Political Research Quarterly 187201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L. & Nelson, Michael J. (2014) “The Legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court: Conventional Wisdoms and Recent Challenges Thereto,” 10 Annual Rev. of Law and Social Science 201–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L. & Nelson, Michael J. (2015) “Is the U.S. Supreme Court's Legitimacy Grounded in Performance Satisfaction and Ideology?59 American J. of Political Science 162–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L. & Nelson, Michael J. (2017) “Reconsidering Positivity Theory: What Roles Do Politicization, Ideological Disagreement, and Legal Realism Play in Shaping U.S. Supreme Court Legitimacy?14 J. of Empirical Legal Studies 592617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grendstad, Gunnar, Shaffer, William R., & Waltenburg, Eric N. (2015) Policy Making in an Independent Judiciary. The Norwegian Supreme Court. Colchester, UK: ECPR Press.Google Scholar
Grendstad, Gunnar, Shaffer, William R., & Waltenburg, Eric N. (2017) “Norway - Managed Openness and Transparency,” in Davis, R. & Taras, D., eds., Justices and Journalists: The Global Perspective. Cambridge: New York. 235–54.Google Scholar
Grosskopf, Anke & Mondak, Jeffrey J. (1998) “Do Attitudes toward Specific Supreme Court Decisions Matter? The Impact of Webster and Texasv.V Johnson on Public Confidence in the Supreme Court,” 51 Political Research Quarterly 633–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hendershot, Marcus E., Hurwitz, Mark S., Lanier, Drew Noble, & Pacelle, Richard L. Jr. (2013) “Dissensual Decision Making: Revisiting the Mysterious Demise of Consensual Norms within the U.S. Supreme Court,” 66 Political Research Quarterly 467–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hettinger, Virginia A., Lindquist, Stefanie A., & Martinek, Wendy L. (2006) Judging on a Collegial Court. Influences on Federal Appellate Decision Making. Charlottesville and London: Univ. of Virginia Press.Google Scholar
Hirschl, Ran (2009) “The Judicialization of Politics,” in Caldeira, Gregory A., Kelemen, Daniel R., & Whittington, Keith E., eds., The Oxford Handbook of Law and Politics. Oxford Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Hoekstra, Valerie J. & Segal, Jeffrey A. (1996) “The Shepherding of Local Public Opinion: The Supreme Court and Lamb's Chapel,” 58 The J. of Politics 1079–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, Timothy R. & Martin, Andrew D. (1998) “The Public's Conditional Response to Supreme Court Decisions,” 98 The American Political Science Rev. 299309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelemen, Katalin (2018) Judicial Dissent in European Constitutional Courts. A Comparative and Legal Perspective. London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lind, Edgar Allen & Tyler, Tom R. (1988) The Social Psychology of Procedural Justice. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, Thomas R. (1987) “The Supreme Court as an Opinion Leader. Court Decisions and the Mass Public,” 15 American Politics Quarterly 147–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mestad, Ola (2015). "Dissensens alvor. Om nyare dissensar i Høgsterett," in Schei, T., J. E. A. Skoghøy, & T. Øie, eds., Lov, Sannhet, Rett. Norges Høyesterett 200 år. Oslo: Universitetsforlaget.Google Scholar
Mondak, Jeffrey J. (1990) “Perceived Legitimacy of Supreme Court Decisions: Three Functions of Source Credibility,” 12 Political Behavior 363–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mondak, Jeffrey J. (1991) “Substantive and Procedural Aspects of Supreme Court Decisions as Determinants of Institutional Approval,” 19 American Politics Quarterly 174–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mondak, Jeffrey J. (1992) “Institutional Legitimacy, Policy Legitimacy, and the Supreme Court,” 20 American Politics Quarterly 457–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, Steven A. (1981) “Dissent in American Courts,” 43 The J. of Politics 412–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petty, Richard E. & Cacioppo, John T. (1986) “The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion,” in Berkowitz, Leonard, ed., Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. San Diego: Academic Press. 123205.Google Scholar
Salamone, Michael F. (2014) “Judicial Consensus and Public Opinion: Conditional Response to Supreme Court Majority Size,” 67 Political Research Quarterly 320–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheb, John M. & Lyons, William (2001) “Judicial Behavior and Public Opinion: Popular Expectations Regarding the Factors that Influence Supreme Court Decisions,” 23 Political Behavior 181–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skiple, Jon Kåre, Grendstad, Gunnar, Shaffer, William R., & Waltenburg, Eric N. (2016) “Supreme Court Justices' Economic Behavior: A Multilevel Model Analysis,” 39 Scandinavian Political Studies 7394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skiple, Jon Kåre, Gudbrandsen, Frøy, & Grendstad, Gunnar (2013) “«...som speiler det samfunnet vi har». Høyesterett, asylbarn og den alminnelige rettsoppfatning,” 52 Lov og Rett 609–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sunde, Jørn Øyrehagen (2015) Høgsteretts Historie 1965–2015. Bergen: Fagbokforlaget.Google Scholar
Sweet, Alec Stone (1992) The Birth of Judicial Politics in France. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Sweet, Alec Stone (2000) Governing with Judges. Constitutional Politics in Europe. New York: Oxford Univ. Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweet, Alec Stone (2002) “Constitutional Courts and Parliamentary Democracy,” 25 West European Politics 77100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoutenborough, James, Haider-Markel, Donald P., & Allen, Mahalley D. (2006) “Reassessing the Impact of Supreme Court Decisions on Public Opinion: Gay Civil Rights Cases,” 53 Political Research Quarterly 419–33.Google Scholar
Tate, Neal & Vallinder, Torbjörn (1995) The Global Expansion of Judicial Power. New York and London: New York Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Tyler, Tom R. (1988) “What Is Procedural Justice?: Criteria Used by Citizens to Assess the Fairness of Legal Procedures,” 22 Law & Society Rev. 103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, Tom R. (2006) Why People Obey the Law. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, Tom R. & Rasinski, Kenneth (1991) “Procedural Justice, Institutional Legitimacy, and the Acceptance of Unpopular U.S. Supreme Court Decisions: A Reply to Gibson,” 25 Law & Society Rev. 621–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wahlbeck, Paul J., Spriggs, James F., & Maltzman, Forrest (1999) “The Politics of Dissent and Concurrences on the U.S. Supreme Court,” 27 American Politics Research 488514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, Thomas G., Epstein, Lee, & Dixon, William J. (1988) “On the Mysterious Demise of Consensual Norms in the United States Supreme Court,” 50 J. of Politics 361–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaller, John R. (1992) The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zilis, Michael A. (2015) The Limits of Legitimacy. Dissenting Opinions, Media Coverage, and Public Responses to Supreme Court Decisions. Ann Arbor: Univ. of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zink, James R., Spriggs, James F. II, & Scott, John T. (2009) “Courting the Public: The Influence of Decision Attributes on individuals' Views of Court Opinions,” 71 The J. of Politics 909–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Østlid, Henry (1955) “Hvordan offentlig votering ble gjennomført i Norges Høyesterett,” 68 Tidsskrift for Rettsvitenskap 170207.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Individuals' Ex Ante Policy Positions

Figure 1

Table 2. Dissent Has a Positive Effect on Support for Asylum Decision

Figure 2

Figure 1. Change in the Probability of Acceptance Across Treatment Groups for Each of the Three Court Decisions (95% Confidence Intervals). Data: Norwegian Citizen Panel Wave 7, 2016.

Figure 3

Table 3. No Conditional Effects of Ex Ante Policy Views

Figure 4

Figure 2. Effects of Diffuse Support on Ex Ante supporters' and Ex Ante opponents' Acceptance of the Asylum Decision (Upper Left Graph), the Privacy Decision (Upper Right Graph), and the Neighbor Conflict Decision (Lower Left Graph) (95% Confidence Intervals). Data: Norwegian Citizen Panel Wave 7, 2016.