INTRODUCTION
Mature listeners are well attuned to the subtle relationships of acoustic dimensions that characterize native speech input. Yet, any given listening context can involve speech that deviates from these expected patterns. A talker may speak with an unusual accent or dialect, or environmental factors may create acoustic distortion. These adverse listening conditions relate to any situation in which the acoustic signal is distorted, degraded, or distinct from its typical form. This can arise from intrinsic (accented or dysfluent speech) as well as extrinsic (background noise or distortion over a channel) properties of speech. Such conditions can complicate the mapping of the acoustic speech signal onto meaningful native-language sounds and words and negatively impact comprehension (Cooper, Brouwer, & Bradlow, Reference Cooper, Brouwer and Bradlow2015; Kalikow, Stevens, & Elliott, Reference Kalikow, Stevens and Elliott1977). Even so, when listeners encounter speech under adverse listening conditions, adaptive perceptual adjustments can improve comprehension over time (Altmann & Young, Reference Altmann and Young1993; Francis, Baldwin, & Nusbaum, Reference Francis, Baldwin and Nusbaum2000; Greenspan, Nusbaum, & Pisoni, Reference Greenspan, Nusbaum and Pisoni1988; Liss, Spitzer, Caviness, & Adler, Reference Nicolson, Daum, Schugens, Fawcett and Schulz2002; Norris, McQueen, & Cutler, Reference Bertelson, Vroomen and De Gelder2003; Pallier, Sebastian-Gallés, Dupoux, Christophe, & Mehler, Reference Rae, Lee, Dixon, Blamire, Thompson, Styles and Stein1998; Schwab, Nusbaum, & Pisoni, Reference Schwab, Nusbaum and Pisoni1985). These improvements are thought of as adaptive plasticity in which exposure to distorted or degraded speech leads to perceptual adaptations that lead to more effectively subsequent mapping of the distorted speech. Adaptive plasticity has been observed across many different speech signal distortions, including foreign-accented, synthetic, time-compressed, noise-vocoded, and dysarthric speech (e.g., Altmann & Young, Reference Altmann and Young1993; Banai & Lavner, Reference Banai and Lavner2012, Reference Banai and Lavner2014; Bradlow & Bent, Reference Bradlow and Bent2008; Cainer, James, & Rajan, Reference Cainer, James and Rajan2008; Clarke & Garrett, Reference Clarke and Garrett2004; Davis, Johnsrude, Hervais-Adelman, Taylor, & McGettigan, Reference Davis, Johnsrude, Hervais-Adelman, Taylor and McGettigan2005; Gabay, Karni, & Banai, Reference Guediche2017; Liss et al., Reference Nicolson, Daum, Schugens, Fawcett and Schulz2002; Schwab et al., Reference Schwab, Nusbaum and Pisoni1985).
The mechanistic origins of adaptive plasticity are not yet well understood and there is debate about whether phenomena observed across different distortions and tasks arise from common mechanisms. However, the presence of information that disambiguates speech that deviates from the norm emerges as an important common factor. Speech recognition improves more when exposure to distorted speech is accompanied with disambiguating information like a written version of the speech (Francis, Nusbaum, & Fenn, Reference Francis, Nusbaum and Fenn2007; Loebach, Pisoni, & Svirsky, Reference Kast, Elmer, Jancke and Meyer2010; Schwab et al., Reference Schwab, Nusbaum and Pisoni1985), or a clear (undistorted) acoustic version of the speech (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Johnsrude, Hervais-Adelman, Taylor and McGettigan2005; Hervais-Adelman, Davis, Johnsrude, & Carlyon, Reference Hervais-Adelman, Davis, Johnsrude and Carlyon2008). Even so, adaptive plasticity is sometimes observed even when there are no external information sources available to disambiguate distorted speech (Altmann & Young, Reference Altmann and Young1993; Clarke & Garrett, Reference Clarke and Garrett2004; Liss et al., Reference Nicolson, Daum, Schugens, Fawcett and Schulz2002; Pallier et al., Reference Rae, Lee, Dixon, Blamire, Thompson, Styles and Stein1998). Guediche, Fiez, and Holt (Reference Fernandez, Juranek, Romanowska-Pawliczek, Stuebing, Williams and Fletcher2016) make the case that both external (e.g., written feedback) and internal (e.g., word knowledge) information sources can be used to generate predictions about the correct mapping of distorted speech input. Their results demonstrate that mere exposure is sufficient to drive adaptive plasticity for less-severely distorted speech input and that external information sources enhance adaptive plasticity above and beyond this only when input signals are so severely degraded that they cannot provide access to internal predictions to drive adaptive plasticity in speech perception via internal information.
Several groups have argued that results like these are consistent with generation of an error signal when there is a discrepancy between the sensory input predicted by disambiguated signal and the actual (distorted) speech experienced (Guediche, Holt, Laurent, Lim, & Fiez, Reference Guediche, Holt, Laurent, Lim and Fiez2015; Vroomen, Baart, Murray, & Wallace, Reference Banai and Lavner2012; Vroomen, van Linden, De Gelder, & Bertelson, Reference Brookes, Nicolson and Fawcett2007). This signal, in turn, may drive adaptive plasticity through supervised error-driven learning that impacts the mapping of acoustics to speech representations (see Guediche et al., Reference Guediche, Holt, Laurent, Lim and Fiez2015). Consistent with the possibility that error-driven learning may play a role in adaptive plasticity in speech perception, Guediche et al. (Reference Guediche, Holt, Laurent, Lim and Fiez2015) present evidence that a neurobiological system implicated in supervised error-driven learning in other domains (Wolpert, Diedrichsen, & Flanagan, Reference Idemaru and Holt2011; Wolpert & Kawato, Reference Pallier, Sebastian-Gallés, Dupoux, Christophe and Mehler1998; Wolpert, Miall, & Kawato, Reference Pallier, Sebastian-Gallés, Dupoux, Christophe and Mehler1998), namely the cerebellum, is involved in adaptive plasticity in speech perception.
This situates adaptive plasticity in speech perception in an interesting position with regard to developmental dyslexia, one of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders (Peterson & Pennington, Reference Peterson and Pennington2015). Although the common view suggests that developmental dyslexia arises from a phonological impairment (Ramus & Szenkovits, Reference Bradlow and Bent2008; Snowling, Reference Pallier, Sebastian-Gallés, Dupoux, Christophe and Mehler1998), this stance has been criticized for its inability to explain broader deficits not restricted to phonological information such as motor impairments (Orban, Lungu, & Doyon, Reference Bradlow and Bent2008; Stoodley, Harrison, & Stein, Reference Howard, Howard, Japikse and Eden2006) visual and auditory deficits (Farmer & Klein, Reference Farmer and Klein1995), and procedural learning impairments (Gabay, Schiff, & Vakil, Reference Gabay, Schiff and Vakil2012a, Reference Gabay, Schiff and Vakil2012c; Gabay, Vakil, Schiff, & Holt, Reference Gabay, Vakil, Schiff and Holt2015; Hedenius et al., Reference Hedenius, Persson, Alm, Ullman, Howard, Howard and Jennische2013; Howard, Howard, Japikse, & Eden, Reference Menghini, Hagberg, Caltagirone, Petrosini and Vicari2006; Lum, Ullman, & Conti-Ramsden, Reference Lum, Ullman and Conti-Ramsden2013; Pavlidou & Williams, Reference Pavlidou and Williams2014; Stoodley, Ray, Jack, & Stein, Reference Bradlow and Bent2008; Stoodley et al., Reference Howard, Howard, Japikse and Eden2006; Vicari et al., Reference Davis, Johnsrude, Hervais-Adelman, Taylor and McGettigan2005). It is possible that phonological impairments are a symptom, rather than a cause, of developmental dyslexia.
A recent explanatory framework posits that developmental dyslexia may be related to impairments to the procedural learning system (Nicolson & Fawcett, Reference Nicolson and Fawcett2010; Nicolson & Fawcett, Reference Nicolson and Fawcett2011; Nicolson, Fawcett, & Dean, Reference Nicolson, Fawcett and Dean2001; Ullman, Reference Ullman2004; Ullman, Earle, Walenski, & Janacsek, Reference Ullman, Earle, Walenski and Janacsek2020) related to knowledge honed through automatization of various skills (“learning how”) and contrasted with declarative learning (“learning that”) which represents memory for events and facts about the world (Knowlton, Siegel, & Moody, Reference Knowlton, Siegel, Moody and Byrne2017). Procedural learning impairments in developmental dyslexia are hypothesized to affect the automatization of various skills and subskills ultimately required for successful reading.
A growing body of studies supports this possibility through demonstrations of procedural learning deficits in nonphonological and even non-language domains in developmental dyslexia. For example, impairments in motor sequence learning (Gabay et al., Reference Gabay, Schiff and Vakil2012a; Lum et al., Reference Lum, Ullman and Conti-Ramsden2013), nonspeech/visual statistical learning (Gabay, Thiessen, & Holt, Reference Gabay and Holt2015; Singh, Walk, & Conway, Reference Alvarez and Fiez2018), and probabilistic learning in a visual task (Gabay, Vakil, et al., Reference Gabay, Thiessen and Holt2015) have been observed in developmental dyslexia. Furthermore, core neurobiological components of the procedural learning system, such as the cerebellum (Fernandez et al., Reference Guediche, Fiez and Holt2016; Nicolson et al., Reference Brunswick, McCrory, Price, Frith and Frith1999; Pernet, Poline, Demonet, & Rousselet, Reference Samuel and Kraljic2009; Rae et al., Reference Pallier, Sebastian-Gallés, Dupoux, Christophe and Mehler1998; Stanberry et al., Reference Howard, Howard, Japikse and Eden2006; Stoodley, Reference Stoodley2014), and the basal ganglia (Brunswick, McCrory, Price, Frith, & Frith, Reference Brunswick, McCrory, Price, Frith and Frith1999; Kita et al., Reference Hedenius, Persson, Alm, Ullman, Howard, Howard and Jennische2013; Paulesu et al., Reference Paulesu, Frith, Snowling, Gallagher, Morton, Frackowiak and Frith1996), have been found to be affected in dyslexia. In addition, people with developmental dyslexia exhibit impairments in tasks that tap cerebellar function, including eye blink conditioning (Nicolson, Daum, Schugens, Fawcett, & Schulz, Reference Liss, Spitzer, Caviness and Adler2002) and motor adaptation (Brookes, Nicolson, & Fawcett, Reference Brookes, Nicolson and Fawcett2007). However, most studies broadly examining procedural learning in developmental dyslexia, and cerebellar dysfunction specifically, have concentrated on motor learning (Brookes et al., Reference Brookes, Nicolson and Fawcett2007; Gabay et al., Reference Gabay, Schiff and Vakil2012a; Gabay, Schiff, & Vakil, Reference Gabay, Schiff and Vakil2012b; Hedenius et al., Reference Hedenius, Persson, Alm, Ullman, Howard, Howard and Jennische2013; Howard et al., Reference Menghini, Hagberg, Caltagirone, Petrosini and Vicari2006; Lum et al., Reference Lum, Ullman and Conti-Ramsden2013; Menghini, Hagberg, Caltagirone, Petrosini, & Vicari, Reference Howard, Howard, Japikse and Eden2006; Needle, Nicolson, & Fawcett, Reference Needle, Nicolson and Fawcett2015; Stoodley et al., Reference Bradlow and Bent2008; Stoodley et al., Reference Howard, Howard, Japikse and Eden2006; Vicari, Marotta, Menghini, Molinari, & Petrosini, Reference Bertelson, Vroomen and De Gelder2003; Yang & Hong-Yan, Reference Idemaru and Holt2011). Given increasing evidence that implicates the cerebellum in purely auditory tasks that do not involve motor processing (Petacchi, Laird, Fox, & Bower, Reference Davis, Johnsrude, Hervais-Adelman, Taylor and McGettigan2005), prediction error signals in speech perception (Rothermich & Kotz, Reference Hedenius, Persson, Alm, Ullman, Howard, Howard and Jennische2013) and the adaptive plasticity of speech perception phenomena described above (Guediche et al., Reference Guediche, Holt, Laurent, Lim and Fiez2015), there is the potential that disrupted cerebellar processing in developmental dyslexia (Alvarez & Fiez, Reference Gabay, Karni and Banai2018; Fawcett & Nicolson, Reference Fawcett, Nicolson, Reid, Fawcett, Manis and Siegel2008; Pernet et al., Reference Samuel and Kraljic2009) could impact perception, as well. On this hypothesis, we conducted a behavioral study of adaptive plasticity in speech perception following the methodological approach of Guediche and colleagues (Guediche, Blumstein, Fiez, & Holt, Reference Guediche, Blumstein, Fiez and Holt2014; Guediche et al., Reference Guediche, Holt, Laurent, Lim and Fiez2015, Reference Guediche, Fiez and Holt2016) across adults with developmental dyslexia, and matched control participants. This approach has the advantage of introducing distortion to spectral acoustic information, leaving temporal information largely intact to protect against the possibility of the distortion manipulation interacting with reported temporal auditory processing impairments in dyslexia (Cohen-Mimran & Sapir, Reference Cohen-Mimran and Sapir2007; Farmer & Klein, Reference Farmer and Klein1995; Gabay, Najjar, & Reinisch, Reference Gabay, Najjar and Reinisch2019; Van Ingelghem et al., Reference Van Ingelghem, Van Wieringen, Wouters, Vandenbussche, Onghena and Ghesquière2001).
METHODS
Participants
Thirty-four participants took part in the study (N = 17 adults with developmental dyslexia; N = 17 typical readers). All were university students in Pittsburgh, PA. The dyslexia group was recruited from the office of disability resources at Carnegie Mellon University. A documented diagnosis of a comorbid learning disability, such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder or specific language impairment, or any sensory or neurological impairment was an exclusion criterion. All participants with dyslexia reported having substantial difficulties with reading and writing skills during school entry. The inclusion criteria for the dyslexia group were (a) a formal diagnosis of dyslexia by a qualified psychologist, as indicated by previous and recent test records of the participant and (b) a score of below the 50th percentile on at least two word-level-reading tests (untimed and timed tests of word reading and decoding skills, similar to the study of Thompson et al. [Reference Alvarez and Fiez2018]). The control group consisted of individuals with no reading impairments (i.e., performance above the inclusion criteria of the dyslexia group in both timed and untimed tests of word reading and decoding skills), or history of developmental dyslexia who were matched to the dyslexia group on age and cognitive ability, as measured by the Raven Matrices tests (Raven, Court, & Raven, Reference Raven, Court and Raven1992). Based on these criteria, one participant with dyslexia was excluded from the analysis. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants and they were compensated for their participation. The Institutional Review Board of Carnegie Mellon University approved the study and it was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
All participants performed a battery of linguistic and cognitive tests to evaluate general cognitive ability, verbal working memory, rapid automatized naming reading skills, and phonological awareness. Details about these tasks are presented in Table 1. These tests were administered to confirm that groups differed significantly in phonological and reading skills, with similar levels of intelligence.
Indeed, the groups did not differ according to age or intelligence (Table 2). However, compared with the normal reading group, the dyslexia group showed a clear profile of reading disability, conforming to the symptomatology of dyslexia. The group differed significantly from the control group on word reading and decoding skills, in both rate and accuracy measures. In addition, the dyslexia group exhibited characteristic deficits in the three major phonological domains: phonological awareness (Spoonerisms), verbal short-term memory (digit span), and rapid naming (rapid automatized naming).
a Standard scores (whereby smaller numbers are expected for dyslexia group), other scores are raw scores. RAN = Rapid Naming, WI = Word Identification, WA = Word Attack, SA = Sight Word, PD = Phonological Decoding.
Notably, the dyslexia group was comprised of high-functioning university students with dyslexia. Such high-functioning adults typically exhibit average performance on standardized reading tests (including performance on reading low frequency words, as in the Woodcock Reading Mastery Test-Revised (Wilson & Lesaux, Reference Nicolson, Fawcett and Dean2001). Nevertheless, the demands of complex phonological tests such as the Spoonerism test can identify highly compensated adults with a history of developmental dyslexia (Wilson & Lesaux, Reference Nicolson, Fawcett and Dean2001). Our dyslexic participants match this profile. All participants in the dyslexia group had a history of developmental dyslexia identified in childhood development by a qualified psychologist. Furthermore, all were university students receiving testing accommodations. Despite performance with in the normal range on standardized reading tests, these participants performed significantly more poorly than matched controls on all literacy measures and exhibited phonological processing impairments under high phonological demands (as in the Spoonerism test). This profile is clearly indicative of a sample of dyslexic adults.
Stimuli
The procedure and stimulus design followed Guediche et al. (Reference Fernandez, Juranek, Romanowska-Pawliczek, Stuebing, Williams and Fletcher2016, Experiment 3). Stimuli were drawn from seven 50-word phonetically balanced English monosyllabic word lists (Egan, Reference Egan1948; list 1–7). Words were monosyllabic and high frequency, and therefore relatively easy to read. Words were spoken by a female monolingual English talker (LLH), recorded using a digital Marantz PMC670 recorder (16-bit, 22050 Hz), matched in root-mean-square amplitude and saved as a digital file.
We used a signal processing technique from the cochlear implant literature to introduce an incremental signal distortion that systematically influenced speech intelligibility (Shannon, Zeng, Kamath, Wygonski, & Ekelid, Reference Farmer and Klein1995; Zeng, Reference Clarke and Garrett2004). The resulting vocoded, spectrally shifted speech introduced distortion to the signal by vocoding natural speech into 20 bands, limiting the spectral resolution of available acoustic speech information. We also introduced a spectral shift between the filters applied to analysis of the speech input and the output filters to which input is passed to create a spectral shift in the final distorted speech signal. With increasingly large disparities in the spectral shift between input and output filters, speech intelligibility decreases (Guediche et al., Reference Fernandez, Juranek, Romanowska-Pawliczek, Stuebing, Williams and Fletcher2016).
Distortions to the natural speech stimuli were created using Tiger Speech software (see Fu & Galvin, Reference Bertelson, Vroomen and De Gelder2003). Each spoken word was band-pass filtered into 20 frequency bands using eighth-order Butterworth filters with 24 dB/octave falloff. The frequency band cut-offs were chosen on the basis of the Nucleus-24M cochlear implant (covering frequencies from 116 to 7871 Hz). The temporal envelope of each band was removed by half-wave rectification and low-pass filtering at 160 Hz. This envelope was created in order to modulate a carrier band. We used the following equation in order to compute the frequency range of the carrier band. Here, P0 is the “insertion depth” (in millimeters) of the theoretical implant model, with i corresponding to the 20 frequency bands:
The carrier band frequencies were computed with the assumption of a 35-mm cochlea (Greenwood, Reference Greenwood1990) and were determined by:
Together these two equations defined the corner frequencies of the carrier bands, calculated to generate the spectrally shifted speech tokens across increasing “insertion depths” corresponding to greater spectral shifts. The most apical carrier band location (P0) ranged between 9.25 and 15.25 mm from the apex of the cochlea. This created a range of 9.25 mm to 15.25 mm insertion depths and shifted the vocoded speech spectra incrementally upward through the frequency dimension, as illustrated in Figure 1. Carrier bands incrementing in steps of 0.25 mm insertion depth were generated between these endpoint values. For example, no spectral energy below 448 Hz was present for an insertion depth of 9.25 mm speech in which speech was shifted upward in frequency (lowest distortion). For the highest distortion (15.25 mm), there was no spectral energy below 1214 Hz. Crossover reduction between neighboring bands was −3 dB.
Procedure
Participants were seated in a sound-attenuated booth with a computer monitor mounted at eye level. Stimulus presentation and the recording of response time and accuracy were controlled by a computer program (E-prime; Schneider, Eschman, & Zuccolotto, Reference Liss, Spitzer, Caviness and Adler2002). On each trial, participants heard a distorted spoken word diotically over headphones (Beyer DT-150, approximately 70 dB) and typed what they heard using a keyboard within a 4-s time window. Participants were encouraged to guess if they were unsure of the word. The typed response appeared on the screen and participants were allowed to use backspace to correct typing errors. Participants indicated that their response was complete with a key press. Immediately thereafter, they heard the distorted word repeated with concurrent presentation of the visual orthographic representation of the word as feedback. The visual orthographic feedback was presented for a length of 1000 ms thereby assuring that even participants with slow reading skills would receive and comprehend the orthographic feedback.
The task involved a pretest, training, and a posttest which required open-set speech recognition (i.e., participants freely typed each word they heard, with no response options provided). Pre- and posttests each involved 50 unique words that were not previously encountered in the task, presented at the highest level of distortion (15.25 mm insertion depth) that, in the Guediche et al. (Reference Fernandez, Juranek, Romanowska-Pawliczek, Stuebing, Williams and Fletcher2016) study, resulted in open-set speech recognition of about 10% correct. No orthographic feedback was provided in the pre- and posttests. The pretest established baseline word recognition performance for a severe distortion and posttest performance provided a measure of the improvement in speech recognition relative to baseline, as a function of the adaptive plasticity evoked in the training block.
Following the approach of Guediche et al. (Reference Fernandez, Juranek, Romanowska-Pawliczek, Stuebing, Williams and Fletcher2016, Experiment 3), the training block involved both incremental increases in speech distortion and orthographic feedback. At the commencement of training, words with modest distortion (9.25 mm insertion depth, approximately 55% correct in open-set speech recognition in Guediche et al., Reference Fernandez, Juranek, Romanowska-Pawliczek, Stuebing, Williams and Fletcher2016) were presented. After each 10 trials, regardless of performance, the speech signal distortion was increased by incrementing the spectral shift (+0.25 mm insertion depth). This resulted in 25 distortion increments across 250 trials of training. All words presented in training were unique and distinct from those presented at pre- and posttests.
Across pretest, training, and posttest phases of the study, responses were scored as correct if they phonologically matched the undistorted spoken word. Misspellings were not coded as mistakes. The first author and a research assistant checked each response several times to assure misspellings were not coded as error for both dyslexia and control groups. The percent of trials with observed misspellings was relatively small (3.3% in the group and 3% in the control group). The experiment was conducted across two sessions. Participants performed the cognitive and linguistics tests first in a 1-hr session. In a second 20-min session, participants completed the word recognition task across pretest, training, and posttest blocks.
RESULTS
Data Analysis
Participants’ performance during pretest, posttest, and training phases was calculated as the mean proportion of words correctly recognized across all words in a given phase.
We first calculated participants’ level of performance during the pretest phase as a measure of baseline word recognition performance for severely distorted speech. We next divided the training phase into 10-trial bins to assess word recognition across training. We measured generalization of training to novel words not experienced in training by comparing word recognition performance at posttest relative to pretest. Finally, we examined the correlation between word recognition performance in training and word recognition improvement from pretest to posttest as a means of investigating the extent to which internal predictions from accurate lexical mapping might relate to the degree of adaptive plasticity observed across groups.
Word Recognition at Pretest
As shown in Figure 2, word recognition across the 50 highly distorted speech signals (15.25 mm insertion depth) experienced at pretest was very poor. On average, the control group accurately recognized an average of 6.52 words out of 50 (SE = 8.2) for 13.05% average accuracy. The dyslexia group accurately recognized an average of 4.6 of the 50 severely distorted words (SD = 6.4) for 9.2% average accuracy. The two groups did not significantly differ in pretest word comprehension, as reflected by an independent samples t-test, t (1, 31) = −1.47, p = .15, Cohen’s d = .56.
Word Recognition in Training
Figure 3 plots average word recognition accuracy as a function of the severity of the speech distortion across training for each group. During training, the signal distortion began at a moderate level and incremented each 10 trials, with orthographic feedback provided on each of the 250 trials. We examined word recognition accuracy as a function of the speech signal distortion, which increased every 10 trials, with a mixed-model ANOVA with Group as a between-subjects factor and signal distortion as a within-subjects factor. This revealed a significant main effect of Group, F(1, 31) = 17.59, p = .001, η p 2 = .35, as well as a main effect of the increasing signal distortion across training, F(24, 744) =58.40, p = .001, η p ² = .65. There was no interaction between these factors, F(24, 744) = .71, p = .841; η p ² = .02. Across training, individuals in the dyslexia group recognized significantly fewer words (M = 63%, SE = 1%) than listeners in the control group (M = 74%, S.E. = 1%).
Word Recognition at Posttest
Word recognition at posttest provides a measure of the generalization of adaptive plasticity evoked in the training block because each word presented in the experiment is unique. Any pre to posttest improvement in word recognition for the most severely distorted speech must arise as a result of generalization (e.g., Schwab et al., Reference Schwab, Nusbaum and Pisoni1985; Guediche et al., Reference Fernandez, Juranek, Romanowska-Pawliczek, Stuebing, Williams and Fletcher2016). Given that there was no baseline group difference in performance at pretest, we evaluated adaptive plasticity with a 2 × 2 mixed-model ANOVA with testing phase (pretest and posttest), as a within-subjects factor and Group (dyslexia, control) as a between-subjects factor. There was a significant main effect of testing phase, F(1, 31) = 105.8, p = .00000, η p ² = .77, such that there was a general improvement in word recognition for severely distorted speech from pre- to posttest (M = 11%, S.E. = 1.29, M = 20%, S.E. = 2.24, for pre vs. posttest, respectively) consistent with adaptive plasticity. There also was a main effect of group, F(1, 31) = 7.4, p = .010, η p ² = .15. In general, across pre- and posttests, individuals with dyslexia were less accurate (M = 16%, S.E. = .21) in word recognition than control listeners (M = 24.20%, S.E. = .20). Crucially, these main effects must be understood in the context of a significant group by testing phase interaction, F(1, 31) = 5.7, p = .023, η p ² = .15. Whereas there were no significant group differences in performance at pretest, t(1, 31) = −1.47, p = .15, Cohen’s d = .56 the control group (M = 35.88%, S.E. = 1.32%) outperformed individuals with dyslexia (M = 23.5%, SD = 1.03%) at posttest, t(1, 31) = −2.97, p = .005, Cohen’s d = 1.03. Adaptive plasticity drove a 22.83% improvement in word recognition accuracy among the control group, but only a 14.8% improvement among the dyslexia group.
In order the test the possibility that differences in vocabulary knowledge influence adaptive plasticity, we conducted a 2 × 2 mixed-model ANCOVA with testing phase (pretest and posttest), as a within-subjects factor and Group (dyslexia, control) as a between-subjects factor and vocabulary score (Nelson-Deny test, see Table 1) as a covariate. The interaction of testing phase and group was still significant, F(1, 29) = 5.88, p = .022; η p ² = .71, suggesting that differences in accessing lexical knowledge could not fully account for the observed differences in adaptive plasticity across groups.
Relationship of Training Performance with Generalization of Learning Gains
Guediche et al. (Reference Fernandez, Juranek, Romanowska-Pawliczek, Stuebing, Williams and Fletcher2016) reported a correlation between the magnitude of adaptive plasticity observed for an individual listener (posttest minus pretest accuracy in recognizing highly distorted speech) and overall word recognition accuracy in the training block. Here, as shown in Figure 4, we observe a modest correlation of roughly the same magnitude as Guediche et al. for data pooled across the groups, r(33) = .376, p = . 015. More accurate recognition of distorted words during training resulted in the greatest adaptive plasticity. This is consistent with the notion that internally generated predictions available when distorted input can be successfully mapped to word knowledge play a role in adaptive plasticity (Guediche et al., Reference Fernandez, Juranek, Romanowska-Pawliczek, Stuebing, Williams and Fletcher2016).
GENERAL DISCUSSION
In the present study, individuals with dyslexia exhibited less adaptive plasticity than age and intelligence matched peers. Posttest baseline speech recognition for severely distorted speech was very poor for both the dyslexia and control groups, with no significant difference at pretest. Yet, across the full course of 250 training trials in which the signal distortion increased incrementally every 10 trials, the control group was significantly more accurate at word recognition than the dyslexic group. At posttest, recognition of the most severe speech distortion had improved by 22.8% relative to pretest, whereas the dyslexia group had improved by just 14.8%. The dyslexia group exhibited significantly less adaptive plasticity in word recognition than the control group.
Prior results with typical listeners performing this same task have demonstrated that when speech input is moderately distorted and word recognition is therefore somewhat accurate, even mere exposure can be sufficient to drive adaptive plasticity. But, when speech input is so severely distorted that it no longer provides sufficient access to internal predictions through, for example, access to lexical representations that guide accurate word recognition adaptive plasticity can be driven by external (e.g., orthographic feedback) information sources that link the distorted input to internal representations (Guediche et al., Reference Fernandez, Juranek, Romanowska-Pawliczek, Stuebing, Williams and Fletcher2016). In this prior work, and also here among control and dyslexia groups, the accuracy of linguistic predictions as measured by word recognition accuracy was related to the degree of adaptive plasticity observed. The ability to map distorted speech input to internal, here lexical, representations is associated with the degree of adaptive plasticity observed. Guediche et al. (Reference Guediche, Blumstein, Fiez and Holt2014, Reference Guediche, Holt, Laurent, Lim and Fiez2015) propose that when disambiguating information is available with distorted speech input – such as that evoked by activation of an internal linguistic representation (Guediche et al., Reference Fernandez, Juranek, Romanowska-Pawliczek, Stuebing, Williams and Fletcher2016) – a mismatch or “conflict” between the predicted and actual input may generate an internal error signal that may, in turn, guide adaptive plasticity through supervised error-driven learning. Guediche et al. (Reference Fernandez, Juranek, Romanowska-Pawliczek, Stuebing, Williams and Fletcher2016) provide evidence that internal lexical predictions may drive this form of procedural learning and that it may involve the cerebellum (Guediche et al., Reference Guediche, Holt, Laurent, Lim and Fiez2015).
From this perspective, disorders that impact the fidelity of the linguistic representations that provide the internal predictions hypothesized to drive adaptive plasticity (or the learning mechanisms that make use of these predictions to adjust the mapping of distorted speech input) would be expected to impact the degree of adaptive plasticity observed in speech recognition. In this regard, evidence indicating that individuals with developmental dyslexia exhibit impairments in the fidelity of speech processing (Kast, Elmer, Jancke, & Meyer, Reference Loebach, Pisoni and Svirsky2010) and in vocabulary size (van Viersen et al., Reference Gabay, Karni and Banai2017), and parallel literatures demonstrating procedural learning impairments (Gabay et al., Reference Gabay, Schiff and Vakil2012a, Reference Gabay, Schiff and Vakil2012c; Gabay, Vakil, et al., Reference Gabay, Thiessen and Holt2015; Hedenius et al., Reference Hedenius, Persson, Alm, Ullman, Howard, Howard and Jennische2013; Howard et al., Reference Menghini, Hagberg, Caltagirone, Petrosini and Vicari2006; Lum et al., Reference Lum, Ullman and Conti-Ramsden2013; Pavlidou & Williams, Reference Pavlidou and Williams2014; Stoodley et al., Reference Bradlow and Bent2008; Stoodley et al., Reference Howard, Howard, Japikse and Eden2006; Vicari et al., Reference Davis, Johnsrude, Hervais-Adelman, Taylor and McGettigan2005) and neurobiological differences in the cerebellum (Fernandez et al., Reference Guediche, Fiez and Holt2016; Stanberry et al., Reference Howard, Howard, Japikse and Eden2006; Stoodley, Reference Stoodley2014), potentially involved in guiding adaptive plasticity in speech (Guediche et al., Reference Guediche, Holt, Laurent, Lim and Fiez2015), align with the present observation of less efficient learning in individuals with developmental dyslexia.
For example, the more limited vocabularies among the dyslexia group (Table 2) might affect the fidelity of linguistic representations, and thus adaptive plasticity in speech perception. Greater vocabulary knowledge likely supports ready word recognition from unfamiliar or ambiguous auditory input (Borovsky, Elman, & Fernald, Reference Borovsky, Elman and Fernald2012). One may argue, therefore, that poorer vocabularies among the dyslexia group may have made it less possible to make use of word knowledge to generate internal predictions to drive adaptive plasticity. However, this explanation is weakened by evidence that there were no group differences in word recognition for the most severely distorted speech at pretest. Furthermore, group differences remained when variability derived from an estimate of vocabulary (the Nelson-Denny test) were factored into analyses.
Problems with reading and writing could have influenced the ability of people with dyslexia to perform the task. However, all stimuli were high-frequency monosyllabic English words, the duration of orthographic feedback was long, and response accuracy coding as done in such a way that misspellings were not considered errors. Similarly, poorer auditory or phonetic fidelity of speech processing in dyslexia (Kast et al., Reference Loebach, Pisoni and Svirsky2010) may have played a role in the less efficient adaptive plasticity observed among the dyslexic group. However, these representational challenges would be expected to influence participants’ performance at baseline pretest, as well. The observation of no group differences at baseline reduces the possibility that problems in speech sound representations can account for the findings.
In fact, group differences emerged in training. In light of evidence that individuals with developmental dyslexia exhibit less efficient procedural learning across a variety of domains (Gabay et al., Reference Gabay, Schiff and Vakil2012a, Reference Gabay, Schiff and Vakil2012c; Gabay, Vakil, et al., Reference Gabay, Thiessen and Holt2015; Howard et al., Reference Menghini, Hagberg, Caltagirone, Petrosini and Vicari2006; Lum et al., Reference Lum, Ullman and Conti-Ramsden2013; Stoodley et al., Reference Bradlow and Bent2008; Stoodley et al., Reference Howard, Howard, Japikse and Eden2006; Vicari et al., Reference Davis, Johnsrude, Hervais-Adelman, Taylor and McGettigan2005), it is possible that the present results may reflect procedural-learning-related group differences. Gabay and Holt (Reference Gabay and Holt2015) have argued that procedural learning impairment in dyslexia may lead to impaired perceptual learning that results in impoverished representations of the phonological characteristics of speech and concomitant difficulties in grapheme-phenome conversion and in learning to read (see also, Krishnan, Watkins, & Bishop, Reference Krishnan, Watkins and Bishop2016; Ullman et al., Reference Ullman, Earle, Walenski and Janacsek2020). Therefore, it is possible that an impairment or inefficiency of the procedural learning systems could influence phonological processing by a perceptual route in addition the motor route originally suggested by Nicolson and Fawcett (Nicolson & Fawcett, Reference Idemaru and Holt2011). Indeed, there is emerging evidence that individuals with dyslexia may acquire sound categories through procedural learning less efficiently than typical listeners (Gabay & Holt, Reference Gabay, Thiessen and Holt2015; Lim, Fiez, & Holt, Reference Lim, Fiez and Holt2019).
The present results suggest the possibility that individuals with dyslexia also may exhibit less efficient adaptation across existing phonological representations in the context of online listening. As reviewed briefly above, speech processing is remarkably dynamic and adjusts rapidly to the mapping of acoustic speech input to linguistically relevant representations in online speech processing (Bertelson, Vroomen, & De Gelder, Reference Fu and Galvin2003; Bradlow & Bent, Reference Bradlow and Bent2008; Idemaru & Holt, Reference Nicolson and Fawcett2011; Norris et al., Reference Bertelson, Vroomen and De Gelder2003; Samuel & Kraljic, Reference Pernet, Poline, Demonet and Rousselet2009; Schwab et al., Reference Schwab, Nusbaum and Pisoni1985; Zhang & Holt, Reference Alvarez and Fiez2018). Although the mechanisms driving these adaptive plasticity effects are still unknown, multiple groups have proposed supervised error-driven learning as a possibility (Guediche, Reference Gabay, Karni and Banai2017; Guediche et al., Reference Guediche, Blumstein, Fiez and Holt2014; Norris et al., Reference Bertelson, Vroomen and De Gelder2003; Vroomen et al., Reference Brookes, Nicolson and Fawcett2007) and at least one study has implicated the cerebellum as a key component of the neurobiological network that is involved (Guediche et al., Reference Guediche, Holt, Laurent, Lim and Fiez2015). Thus, adaptive plasticity may represent another form of procedural learning postulated by Nicolson and Fawcett (Reference Idemaru and Holt2011) to be impaired in developmental dyslexia.
In line with this possibility, Gabay, Karni, and Banai (Reference Alvarez and Fiez2018) recently observed that individuals with developmental dyslexia were on par with control participants in a task involving adaptive plasticity to time-compressed speech so long as stimuli were repeated across the task. When the potential for reliance on declarative memorization strategies was eliminated by introducing entirely novel speech stimuli (as in the present experiment), individuals with dyslexia exhibited significantly less adaptive plasticity than control listeners.
The present results indicate that even well-compensated adults with a history of dyslexia exhibit inefficiencies in adaptive plasticity in speech processing. Less robust adaptive plasticity in speech perception may affect those with dyslexia and, in turn, their speech comprehension abilities in real-world listening environments. Further research examining this and other phenomena of adaptive plasticity among individuals with dyslexia will be informative in revealing the nature of speech processing in dyslexia and, perhaps, in revealing mechanisms of adaptive plasticity among typical listeners, as well.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Research was supported by the National Institutes of Health (R01DC004674) to LLH and by Binational Scientific Foundation (2015227) and the National Science Foundation-Binational Scientific Foundation (2016867) grants to LLH and YG. Thanks to Christi Gomez for stimulus creation and support in testing human participants.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors have nothing to disclose.