Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T19:17:05.647Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Voicing or register in Jarai dialects? Implications for the reconstruction of Proto-Chamic and for registrogenesis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 May 2024

Marc Brunelle*
Affiliation:
University of Ottawa
Ke Leb
Affiliation:
Institut National des Langues et des Civilisations Orientales; Paris
Thành Tấn Tạ
Affiliation:
Hà Nội National University of Education
Lư Giang Đinh
Affiliation:
Hồ Chí Minh City University of Social Sciences and Humanities
*
*Corresponding author. Email: mbrunell@uottawa.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Jarai is a Chamic language of Vietnam and Cambodia that is traditionally described as preserving the original Austronesian voicing contrast in onset obstruents. However, there is anecdotal evidence that it has developed a register contrast, i.e. a binary contrast based on a bundle of spectral properties like pitch, voice quality and vowel quality. We conducted production and perception experiments of the voicing/register contrast in two Jarai varieties spoken in Saom Kaning, Cambodia, and Ea Sup, Vietnam, to determine if they preserve voicing and/or have developed a register contrast. Results show that both dialects have a register contrast primarily based on vowel height modulations (F1) and that onset voicing has become at best an optional secondary property of register. F1 is also the primary cue used for register identification in both dialects. Implications for the diachronic development of the register contrast in Chamic languages and in Mainland Southeast Asia in general are discussed.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The International Phonetic Association

1. Introduction

Jarai is a language spoken in Vietnam and Cambodia that belongs to the Chamic branch of the Austronesian family. Until recently, it was described as having a voicing contrast in initial plain stops, like most Chamic languages. However, it has recently been suggested that instead of this expected voicing contrast, Jarai may have a register contrast similar to that of most Austroasiatic languages of the Annamite Cordillera (Williams & Siu Reference Williams, Siu and Williams2013; Jensen Reference Jensen2014). This observation seems to coincide with the realization that Chru and Southern Raglai, two other Chamic languages also described as having a voicing contrast, are actually registral (Brunelle et al. Reference Brunelle, Tấn Tạ, Kirby and Đinh2020; Brunelle, Brown & Hà Reference Brunelle, Brown and Thị Thu Hà2022). This paper aims at determining if two Jarai dialects spoken at the extremes of the Jarai dialectal continuum preserve stop voicing or have developed register, by means of production and perception experiments.

In Section 2, we give an overview of voicing and register in Austroasiatic and Chamic languages. We then provide basic background information about the Jarai language and the two dialects under study in Section 3. In Section 4 and Section 5, we present the acoustic and perceptual experiments that were designed to explore the voicing/register contrasts found in Jarai, with a focus on unaspirated stops. In Section 6, we discuss the implications of the results for models of register development and for the history of Chamic languages.

2. Voicing and register in Mainland Southeast Asia

Scholars of Mon and Khmer, two Austroasiatic languages with well-established Indic scripts, have long observed that the voicing contrast marked in onset obstruents in their classical texts corresponds to vowel modulations in their contemporary spoken varieties (Blagden Reference Blagden1910; Maspero Reference Maspero1915). It is generally accepted that Austroasiatic originally had a voicing contrast in obstruents that was replaced with a register contrast, i.e., a bundle of phonetic properties including vowel quality, voice quality and pitch, realized on the following vowel. Thus, the Old Khmer minimal pair /ta/ ‘grandfather’ ∼ /da/ ‘duck’ is now realized as [taː] ∼ [ti̤ə] in the conservative Khmer dialects of Eastern Thailand (Wayland Reference Wayland1997; Wayland & Jongman Reference Wayland and Jongman2001; Maspong Reference Maspong2021).

In typical register systems, high register vowels (< voiceless onset obstruents) have relatively open initial portions, a modal voice and a higher pitch, while low register vowels (< voiced onset obstruents) have relatively close initial portions, a breathy or lax voice and a lower pitch. However, different languages emphasize different cues. For example, Ban Nakhonchum Mon largely realizes the register distinction through voice quality and pitch (Abramson, Tiede & Luangthongkum Reference Abramson, Tiede and Luangthongkum2015), but vowel quality is the only remaining reflex of register in Standard Khmer (Huffman Reference Huffman1985; Ferlus Reference Ferlus1992). While voicing and register were tacitly treated as mutually exclusive in most research conducted in the past forty years, recent studies suggest that voicing remains an optional secondary cue of register in Chru, Chrau and Mnong (Brunelle et al. Reference Brunelle, Tấn Tạ, Kirby and Đinh2020; Tạ, Brunelle & Nguyễn Reference Tạ, Brunelle and Quý Nguyễn2022; Brunelle, Đinh & Tạ Reference Brunelle, Đinh and Tấn Tạ2023).

The phonetic motivations for the development of register from voicing have been discussed in detail elsewhere (for an overview, cf. Brunelle & Tạ Reference Brunelle, Tấn Tạ, Sidwell and Jenny2021). In a nutshell, they are usually attributed to secondary articulations meant to increase the size of the supraglottal cavity to favor closure voicing by boosting the transglottal airflow (Gregerson Reference Gregerson, Jenner, Thompson and Starosta1976; Ferlus Reference Ferlus1979), to formant cut-back following the aspiration of former voiced stops (Wayland & Jongman Reference Wayland and Jongman2002), or to an auditory low-frequency effect (Kingston et al. Reference Kingston, Macmillan, Walsh Dickey, Thorburn and Bartels1997).

Register is found in many, if not most Austroasiatic languages. It has been studied experimentally in Mon (Lee Reference Lee1983; L.Thongkum Reference L. Thongkum1990; Abramson, Tiede & Luangthongkum Reference Abramson, Tiede and Luangthongkum2015), Kuy (L.Thongkum Reference L. Thongkum1989; Abramson, Luangthongkum & Nye Reference Abramson, Luangthongkum and Nye2004; Lau-Preechathammarach Reference Lau-Preechathammarach2023), Khmer (Wayland Reference Wayland1997; Wayland & Jongman Reference Wayland and Jongman2001; Maspong Reference Maspong2021), Wa (Watkins Reference Watkins2002), Chrau (Tạ, Brunelle & Nguyễn Reference Tạ, Brunelle and Quý Nguyễn2022) and Khmu (Svantesson & House Reference Svantesson and House2006; Abramson, Nye & Luangthongkum Reference Abramson, Nye and Luangthongkum2007; Kirby, Pittayaporn & Brunelle Reference Kirby, Pittayaporn and Brunelle2023) and has been described in many others. However, it is far less common in Austronesian languages, the phylum to which Jarai belongs. A tense-lax contrast that seems equivalent to register has been extensively studied in Javanese (Fagan Reference Fagan1988; Hayward Reference Hayward1993; Hayward et al. Reference Hayward, Grafield-Davies, Howard, Latif and Allen1994; Hayward Reference Hayward1995; Adisasmito-Smith Reference Adisasmito-Smith2004; Thurgood Reference Thurgood2004; Dresser Reference Dresser2005; Brunelle Reference Brunelle, Mercado, Potsdam and Travis2010; Kenstowicz Reference Kenstowicz2021) and voicing-conditioned vowel alternations are well-attested in closely related Madurese (Cohn Reference Cohn1993a, Reference Cohn, Edmondson and Gregerson1993b; Cohn & Lockwood Reference Cohn and Lockwood1994; Cohn & Ham Reference Cohn and Ham1999; Misnadin, Kirby & Remijsen Reference Misnadin and Remijsen2015; Kirby Reference Kirby2020; Misnadin & Kirby Reference Kirby2020), but otherwise, register seems limited to Chamic languages and no reconstruction of Proto-Austronesian or of any branch of Austronesian has to our knowledge ever included a register contrast.

Within Chamic, register has long been reported in all dialects of Cham (Friberg & Hor Reference Friberg, Hor, Thomas, Lee and Liêm Nguyễn1977; Edmondson & Gregerson Reference Edmondson and Gregerson1993; Bùi Reference Bùi1996; Brunelle Reference Brunelle2005, Reference Brunelle, Sidwell and Grant2006, Reference Brunelle2009), even if some authors analyze it as a form of tone (Blood Reference Blood1967; Moussay Reference Moussay1971; Hoàng Reference Hoàng1987; Phú, Edmondson & Gregerson Reference Phú, Edmondson and Gregerson1992; Thurgood Reference Thurgood, Edmondson and Gregerson1993, Reference Thurgood1996, Reference Thurgood1999). A vowel-based register is reported in Haroi (Lee Reference Lee1977; Mundhenk & Goschnick Reference Mundhenk, Goschnick, Thomas, Lee and Đăng Liêm1977; Đoàn Reference Đoàn2009) and recent phonetic evidence establishes that register is also found in Chru (Brunelle et al. Reference Brunelle, Tấn Tạ, Kirby and Đinh2020) and in some Raglai dialects (Lee Reference Lee1998; Tạ Reference Tạ2009; Brunelle, Brown & Hà Reference Brunelle, Brown and Thị Thu Hà2022). While Cham, Chru, Haroi and Raglai are all spoken within 80 km of the coast of Vietnam, Jarai is spoken further afield in the Highlands of the Annamite cordillera and seems to form a distinct branch of Chamic with closely related Rade (Brunelle Reference Brunelle, Adelaar and Schapper2023). Finding register in that language would force us to reconsider reconstructions of Proto-Chamic, as they all assume a voicing contrast in onset obstruents (Lee Reference Lee1966; Burnham Reference Burnham1976; Thurgood Reference Thurgood1999).

3. The Jarai language

Jarai is the largest Chamic language. The geographical distribution of Jarai in Vietnam and Cambodia is given in Figure 1. According to the Vietnamese census, there were 513,930 Jarai in Vietnam in 2019 (General Statistics Office of Vietnam 2020). The Ratanakiri Provincial Planning Department reported 31,359 Jarai in 2021, a figure that probably includes the large majority of Jarai in Cambodia.

Figure 1. Geographical distribution of ethnic Jarai in Vietnam and Cambodia, by commune.

As far as we know, there is no systematic assessment of dialectal variation in Jarai, but the phonology, syntax and basic lexicon are fairly similar across varieties, and speakers report mutual intelligibility. In this paper, we report experiments on two Jarai dialects spoken at the periphery of the Jarai dialectal continuum. The first one is the Western Jarai dialect spoken in Ratanakiri province, Cambodia. Despite minor lexical and phonological differences between villages, it is relatively homogenous. The majority of its speakers are fluent in Khmer (with various degrees of competence) and some also speak Kachok, Tampuan and Vietnamese, depending on the proximity of other language communities. We conducted our experiments in the village Saom Kaning, where most of our participants resided.

The second dialect under investigation is spoken in Ea Sup, Đăk Lăk province, Vietnam. For convenience, we will refer to it as Eastern Jarai. It is geographically separated from other Jarai dialects as its speakers were relocated to Ea Sup from neighbouring areas during and shortly after the Vietnam war, but its speakers do not perceive it to be very different from the varieties spoken in northern Đăk Lăk. Ea Sup Jarai are all fluent in Vietnamese as their village has become surrounded by a Vietnamese town in the past fifty years. Many also speak Rade, a mutually intelligible Chamic language spoken further south, and Lao, which was until recently a lingua franca in the area.

The consonant inventory of Jarai is given in Table 1. It is typical of Chamic languages in that it comprises four series of stops: plain voiceless stops, aspirated voiceless stops, plain voiced stops and implosive stops (described as ‘preglottalized’ in some sources). The plain voiced stops, bolded in Table 1, are the series that may have conditioned a low register on following vowels (and even have devoiced) if recent reports are accurate.

Table 1. Jarai onsets (adapted from Dournes Reference Dournes1976)

A few notational decisions with a limited impact on our research questions must be noted. We adopt a phonological analysis and treat /c/ and /ɟ/ as unaspirated stops (following Lafont Reference Lafont1968; Dournes Reference Dournes1976; Siu Reference Siu1976), even if these consonants are described as affricates in Jensen (Reference Jensen2014). We include /cʰ/ because it appears in a handful of lexical entries in two dictionaries (Dournes Reference Dournes1964; Headley Reference Headley1965). Finally, the complex clusters proposed by Lafont (Reference Lafont1968) are excluded because they can for the most part be attributed to the loss of an underlying presyllabic vowel (see Jensen Reference Jensen2014).

As phonological differences between Jarai dialects appear relatively superficial, the inventory in Table 1 is shared by the Jarai varieties spoken in Saom Kaning and Ea Sup.

4. Production experiment

In order to determine if the two Jarai dialects under study preserve a voicing contrast or have a register system, we conducted an acoustic and electroglottographic investigation of their laryngeal contrasts.

4.1 Methodology

4.1.1 Participants

Data collection in Saom Kaning was carried out by the first and second authors and by Kalan Khi, a native speaking assistant, with twenty-one native speakers of Western Jarai (eleven women, ten men) in January 2019. They were all born between 1952 and 2004 and were all natives of Saom Kaning. All had lived their entire lives in the area, except one older man who had spent thirteen years in Bar Kev, one young woman who had spent four years in Ban Lung (two towns also located in the province of Ratanakiri) and one older man who had spent one year in Vietnam. Otherwise, all participants spoke Khmer (but some women only had very basic proficiency) and several could speak Tampuan or Vietnamese at various levels of fluency.

In Ea Sup, data was collected by the first and fourth authors and by Y Tit Kpă, a native-speaking local assistant, with twenty-two speakers of Eastern Jarai (twelve women, ten men) in February and March 2019. They were all born between 1945 and 1993 and were all natives of Ea Sup, except a man born in Baản Đôn, 30 km south, before his family returned to Ea Sup when he was five. Only three participants spent significant time out of Ea Sup: an older women spent ten years in a Jarai village in Cambodia in her childhood, a middle-aged man spent fifteen years in Baản Đôn in his youth and a younger man spent four years in college in Hồ Chí Minh City. All Eastern Jarai participants spoke Vietnamese fluently, most had at least a passive command of Rade, and a few spoke some Mnong, Lao or Khmer.

4.1.2 Wordlist and procedure

Because of lexical differences between Western and Eastern Jarai, we designed different wordlists for the two locations (see Appendices I and II), but aimed to find target words containing syllables composed of all possible combinations of onset dentals /t, d, tʰ, ɗ, n/ and velars /k, ɡ, kʰ, ŋ/ followed by the vowels /iː, ɛː, aː, ɔː, uː/. Dental and velar places were selected because they have the largest and smallest numbers of consonants, respectively.

In registral Chamic languages, the register of most onsets is directly predictable from their original laryngeal settings. In some languages, however, sonorants developed a register contrast as they underwent register spreading from the previous consonant or syllable and monosyllabization (Friberg & Hor Reference Friberg, Hor, Thomas, Lee and Liêm Nguyễn1977; Thurgood Reference Thurgood1999; Brunelle & Phú Reference Brunelle, Phú, Vittrant and Watkins2019). For this reason, a few pairs of nasal-initial syllables (starting with /n-, ŋ-/) suspected to contrast in register were included. Open monosyllables were preferred; when they were not available, monosyllables closed by sonorants or disyllabic words ending with the target syllables were chosen. There was a total of fifty-six words in the Western Jarai wordlist and fifty-seven in the Eastern Jarai wordlist, but only thirty-three words were identical or had close cognates in the two lists. This is in part due to our decision to favor frequent words with specific phonotactic properties over cognates.

Participants read the wordlist four times in a randomized order while four signals were simultaneously recorded through a Steinberg UR44 preamplifier using SpeechRecorder (Draxler & Jänsch Reference Draxler and Jänsch2004). The first signal was a high-quality audio channel recorded though a Shure BETA 53 microphone. The second signal was a glottal waveform recorded through a Glottal Enterprises EG2-PCX EGG. Two additional channels, a larynx height tracker and a low quality back-up audio signal were recorded through the EG2-PCX but will not be reported here.

As few participants could read in Jarai, they were presented with the words by one of the authors in either Khmer (in Saom Kaning) or Vietnamese (in Ea Sup) and were asked to translate them in Jarai and to pronounce them in a frame sentence. A handful of Western Jarai speakers who spoke limited Khmer (mostly older women) were presented with words in Jarai by Kalan Khi, our native-speaking assistant. Participants then had to insert the target words in one of the frame sentences in (1)–(2) before pronouncing them. Variations in the frame sentence were tolerated to facilitate spontaneous productions as long as the segment preceding the target word was a sonorant. The full recording session took between 30 and 60 minutes, depending on the speaker.

All materials recorded during the production experiment can be accessed from the Pangloss collection and raw data can be downloaded from the Nakala repository. (Eastern Jarai: https://pangloss.cnrs.fr/corpus/Eastern_Jarai?lang=fr&mode=pro; https://nakala.fr/10.34847/nkl.61a5z29q; Western Jarai: https://pangloss.cnrs.fr/corpus/Western_Jarai?lang=fr&mode=pro; https://nakala.fr/10.34847/nkl.f71a8dxx).

4.1.3 Data processing and analysis

After removing tokens produced disfluently or sentences in which the target word was disrupted by background noise (from vehicles, loud music and domestic animals), 4464 Western Jarai and 4789 Eastern Jarai words were annotated in Praat Textgrids (Boersma & Weenink Reference Boersma and Weenink2010). A sample annotation is provided in Figure 2. Stop closures, fricatives and sonorants were labeled based on spectrograms, as well as the open phase of each target syllable, which extends from the consonant release to the end of the vowel. Important voicing landmarks were labeled based on the EGG signal: the onset of voicing was marked in all target syllables, as well as the point of cessation of voicing and the point of resumption of voicing in the case of stops with voicing perturbations (see Section 5.1 for further details).

Figure 2. Annotation of Western Jarai target word /daː/ ‘duck’. Top: spectrogram; Middle: EGG signal; Bottom: acoustic landmarks (ps: previous sonorant, cl: closure, op: open phase, ov: onset of voicing, cv: cessation of voicing, rv: resumption of voicing).

Acoustic and durational measurements were obtained at every millisecond of the audio recordings using PraatSauce (Kirby Reference Kirby2018). Since a 25-ms window was used for acoustic measures, the first and last 12 ms of each vowel will not be reported as their measurement windows span adjacent segments or silence. The acoustic measures reported here include f0, F1, F2, CPP and two spectral tilt measures, H1*–H2* and H1*–A1*. These two spectral tilt measures were chosen as they are the most significant in distinguishing the two voicing series/registers in the Jarai dialects under study (H2*–H4*, H1*–A2* and H1*–A3* were also measured, but are not presented here). They were corrected for formant frequencies (Iseli & Alwan Reference Iseli and Alwan2004).

Outliers were removed following a two-step process. Local tracking errors were first detected by converting f0, F1 and F2 into z-scores, by speaker. Derivatives were then obtained for each of these z-normalized measures. Any measure whose derivative was not between -.5 and .5 z was excluded, as it corresponds to a dramatic jump likely associated with a tracking error. These excluded measures were left blank. Global tracking errors and outliers were then excluded by obtaining mean f0, F1, F2 for each combination of speaker, vowel and voicing/register (after the exclusion of local errors) and excluding any measure distant from the mean by more than three standard deviations. All H1*–H2* and H1*–A1* measures calculated from excluded f0, F1 or F2 measures were also excluded. The proportion of excluded measures, per dialect, is reported in Table 2.

Table 2. Proportion of excluded measures

Since there is significant variation in acoustic ranges across speakers, all non-durational measures were z-normalized by speaker before conducting statistical analyses and plotting data. In order to ease visualization of the data, these z-normalized measures were converted back into familiar scales in the figures by using the means and standard deviations of all speakers (mean of all speakers + z-score * mean standard deviation of all speakers).

4.1.4 Statistical modeling

The significance of differences between key indicators was assessed by fitting linear mixed models on the data with the lmerTest package in R (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff & Christensen Reference Kuznetsova, Brockhoff and Christensen2017). Models were fitted on plain stop VOT and on the means of f0, H1*–H2*, H1*–A1*, CPP, F1 and F2 over the first ten sampling points (10 ms) of vowels after plain stops, which, as we will see shortly, is systematically the area of greatest difference between voicing series/registers (the depth of voicing/register effects in vowels are roughly time-locked rather than proportional to vowel duration). Fixed main effects included voicing/register, vowel quality and place of onsets, and all two-way interactions of these fixed factors were included (three-way interactions were excluded as they resulted in overfitting). Random slopes by-subject and by-word were also included. Models were simplified top-down by iteratively dropping the interaction with the lowest F-value in the ANOVA of the model. Interactions were dropped one by one as long as the resulting models had a lower Akaike information criterion (AIC) score than the previous model or a higher or equal, but not significant different AIC. Note that no attempts were made to run statistical models on the acoustic properties of other consonants, either because they do not contrast in voicing/register or, in the case of sonorants, because we do not have enough target words containing them to fit robust models.

Cohen’s d’s were used to assess the weight of each acoustic property in the voicing/register contrast (Cohen Reference Cohen1988; Clayards Reference Clayards2008; Brunelle et al. Reference Brunelle, Tấn Tạ, Kirby and Đinh2020; Brunelle, Brown & Hà Reference Brunelle, Brown and Thị Thu Hà2022; Tạ, Brunelle & Nguyễn Reference Tạ, Brunelle and Quý Nguyễn2022). They were calculated by dividing the difference between the vowel-weighted and subject-weighted means of each property for each voicing/register category by its standard deviation. A large absolute Cohen’s d (> 0.8) indicates that the two distributions under investigation have a large difference and that they could play a role in contrast maintenance.

4.2 Production results

We will present results from the two dialects in parallel to facilitate comparison. We will start with a description of the onsets (Section 4.2.1), go over the acoustic properties of the following vowels (Section 4.2.2) and then report results on the relative relevance and magnitude of each acoustic property in the voicing/register contrast (Section 4.2.3).

4.2.1 Onsets

A simple look at the VOT distribution in the two Jarai dialects is sufficient to see that onset voicing is no longer the cue that distinguishes the reflexes of Proto-Chamic voiced and voiceless plain onset stops (Figure 3, top row). High register stops (< *plain voiceless stops) systematically have a moderate positive VOT, as expected, but only a small minority of low register stops (< *plain voiced stops) have a negative VOT, the large majority patterning with voiceless stops. For this reason, we will henceforth refer to plain stops as high/low register stops rather than as voiceless/voiced stops. Other stops behave as expected in both dialects: aspirated stops systematically have a long positive VOT and the implosive stop /ɗ/ preserves a strong negative VOT.

Figure 3. VOT distribution in stops in Western Jarai (left) and Eastern Jarai (right).

Mixed models run on plain stops with a positive VOT reveal that in Western Jarai, coronal stops have a slightly longer VOT in the high than the low register before the vowels /aː, ɛː, uː/, but not before /iː, ɔː/. Moreover, this effect is not found in velar stops (RegisterHigh (/ɔː/ as the reference level) β = 4.5 ms, t = 1.5, p = .136, RegisterHigh:Vowel u: β = 9.3 ms, t = 2.1, p = .035, RegisterHigh:PlaceVelar β = –7.6 ms, t = –2.8, p = .005 – Table W1, App. 3). In Eastern Jarai, there is a slightly shorter VOT in high register than in low register velar stops but no similar difference is found in coronal stops (RegisterHigh β = –1.4 ms, t = –0.8, p = .504, RegisterHigh:PlaceVelar β = –6.8 ms, t = –4.2, p = .025 – Table E1, App. 3). As significant VOT differences between registers are all under 10 ms, it is unlikely that they are under speaker control.

Voice onset time, however, is not always a sufficient indicator of voicing. In voiced obstruents, vocal fold duration often ceases before the end of the consonant because of the aerodynamic voicing constraint (Ohala Reference Ohala and MacNeilage1983, Reference Ohala2011), a build-up in supraglottal air pressure that hinders transglottal airflow. An instance of this interruption of voicing in a voiced stop can be seen in Figure 2. Following previous work, we will refer to such cases as closure voicing with a voiceless release (Brunelle, Brown and Hà Reference Brunelle, Brown and Thị Thu Hà2022).

In Figure 4, we report the proportion of low register stops by type of voicing. Voiced stops have vocal fold vibrations over their entire closure. Voiced stops with a voiceless release are similar to the former, but their voicing ceases before stop release. Finally, devoiced stops do not have vocal fold vibrations, except for possible carry-over voicing stemming from a previous sonorant (termed ‘bleeding’ by Davidson Reference Davidson2016). As bleeding can reach up to 30% of the closure even in high register stops (< voiceless stops), only low register stops with voicing over more than the first 30% of their closure were counted as voiced stops with a voiceless release, while voiced stops with a shorter voicing were treated as devoiced.

Figure 4. Proportion of low register stops which are fully devoiced, have a voiceless release or are fully voiced, by dialect and speaker. Speakers are organized by sex (F/M) and year of birth.

The breakdown in Figure 4 suggests that even when looking at nuances in closure voicing, low register stops are devoiced most of the time in Eastern Jarai. Full voicing and voicing with voiceless release are a little more prevalent in Western Jarai, but still make up less than half of low register stops. While some lexical items show more devoicing than others, this does not seem to obey any obvious pattern of phonological conditioning. Three words have devoiced closures more than 85% of the time: Western Jarai /giː/ ‘to be blocked’ and Eastern Jarai /dɛːl/ ‘k.o. bird’ and /dɔːŋ/ ‘to hit a gong’. However, other words pattern less categorically: their rates of devoicing range between 40% and 65% in Western Jarai and between 35% and 70% in Eastern Jarai. In both dialects, men tend to maintain more full voicing or voicing with voiceless release than women, a pattern also encountered in another Chamic language, Raglai (Brunelle, Brown & Hà Reference Brunelle, Brown and Thị Thu Hà2022) and in genetically unrelated languages (Smith Reference Smith1978; Jessen & Ringen Reference Jessen and Ringen2003; van Alphen & Smits 2004; Helgason & Ringen Reference Helgason and Ringen2008; José Reference José2010; Bayley & Holland Reference Bayley and Holland2014; MacKenzie 2018; Michnowicz & Planchón Reference Michnowicz and Planchón2020).

To summarize this section, the modern reflexes of Proto-Chamic voiced stops /d, ɡ/ in Jarai are no longer systematically voiced, even if they preserve some optional closure voicing in many speakers. Our auditory impressions and observation of spectrograms elicited in non-controlled conditions suggest that the other members of the series, /b, ɟ/, are also normally voiceless. In the next section, we establish that the original Chamic voicing contrast has evolved into a register system in Jarai.

4.2.2 Vowels

Now that we have shown that neither dialect preserves a robust voicing contrast, let us look at Jarai vowels to see if they exhibit the type of acoustic modulations expected in a register language. Figure 5 shows normalized f0 after the various onsets recorded in the target words. Western Jarai seems to exhibit a slightly higher f0 during the first 50 ms of vowels after low register plain stops than after high register ones. However, this difference is only significant in the vowel /ɔː/ (RegisterLow β = 36 Hz, t = 8.4, p < .001; all other vowels have significant interactions of Vowel and Register in the opposite direction – Table W2, App. 3). The f0 of high and low register sonorants does not appear to differ. As for other obstruents, aspirates and the fricative /s/ condition a high f0 at the onset of the following vowel while the implosive /ɗ/ is followed by a slightly lower f0 than sonorants.

Figure 5. Normalized f0 of the first 200 ms of vowels following Western Jarai and Eastern Jarai onsets. Thick lines represent means, thin lines individual observations. The implosive /ɗ/ and the fricative /s/ do not contrast in register and are included for comparison.

In Eastern Jarai, high and low register plain coronal stops have indistinct initial f0s, but velars have a lower f0 in the low register (RegisterLow β = 3 Hz, t = 0.7, p = .483; RegisterLow:PlaceVelar β = –20 Hz, t = –3.4, p = .012 – Table E2, App. 3). There does not seem to be any register difference at the onset of vowels following sonorants. As in Western Jarai, aspirates and the fricative /s/ condition a relatively high f0 on following vowels while the implosive /ɗ/ induces a relatively low f0.

Turning to voice quality, we see in Figure 6 that in Western Jarai, vowels following low register plain stops have a much higher H1*–H2* than their high register counterparts, indicating laxness or breathiness (RegisterLow β = 4.8 dB, t = 8.3, p < .001 – Table W3, App. 3). This difference, which lasts about 200 ms, is even greater in velars (RegisterLow:PlaceVelar β = 2.1 dB, t = 3.8, p < .001 – Table W3, App. 3), but is largely canceled out in /iː/ (RegisterLow:Voweliː β = –3.7 dB, t = –4.1, p < .001 – Table W3, App. 3). No noticeable difference in H1*–H2* is found after high and low register sonorants. As for other obstruents, aspirates and fricative /s/ are followed by a high H1*–H2*, a consequence of their wide glottal opening, and the implosive /ɗ/ is followed by a low H1*–H2*, probably caused by the narrowing of the glottis required to produce an ingressive airflow.

Figure 6. Normalized H1*–H2* of the first 200 ms of vowels following Western Jarai and Eastern Jarai onsets. Thick lines represent means, thin lines individual observations. The implosive /ɗ/ and the fricative /s/ do not contrast in register and are included for comparison.

Eastern Jarai vowels show a much higher mean H1*–H2* after low register stops than high ones, but this effect is less robust than in Western Jarai due to a larger interspeaker variation (RegisterLow β = 5.5 dB, t = 2.3, p = .052 – Table E3, App. 3). As in Western Jarai, there is no H1*–H2 difference at the onset of vowels following sonorants. Other obstruents pattern like in Western Jarai.

Our second measure of spectral tilt, H1*–A1* is much less affected by register differences than H1*–H2*, as can be seen in Figure 7. Low register plain stops are followed by a higher H1*–A1* than high ones in Western Jarai (RegisterLow β = 1.8 dB, t = 4.7, p = .004 – Table W4, App. 3), but it is not clear if such a small difference is linguistically relevant. A slightly larger register difference is visible after sonorants, but it goes in the unexpected direction. The patterns found after other consonants largely mirror those found for H1*–H2*.

Figure 7. Normalized H1*–A1* of the first 200 ms of vowels following Western Jarai and Eastern Jarai onsets. Thick lines represent means, thin lines individual observations. The implosive /ɗ/ and the fricative /s/ do not contrast in register and are included for comparison.

The apparently larger register difference in H1*–A1* found after Eastern Jarai plain stops is not significant (RegisterLow β = 2.0 dB, t = 1.8, p = .106 – Table E4, App. 3). There is no clear register difference after sonorants, and other obstruents again pattern as they did for H1*–H2*.

Our last voice quality indicator, CPP, measures the noise component that is usually associated with non-modal phonation (Seyfarth & Garellek Reference Seyfarth and Garellek2018; Garellek & Esposito Reference Garellek and Esposito2021). A high CPP corresponds to a more modal voice. In Western Jarai, low register plain stops are followed by a significantly lower CPP than high register ones (RegisterLow β = –2.1 dB, t = –4.2, p < .001 – Table W5, App. 3), a difference that is even greater after velars (RegisterLow:PlaceVelar β = –1.4 dB, t = –2.7, p = .007 – Table W5, App. 3). Together with the spectral slope measures seen above, this would indicate the presence of breathiness after low register stops. There is no apparent register difference after sonorants, and other obstruents are all followed by a relatively low CPP. In the case of aspirates and of the fricative /s/, this low CPP is probably caused by frication noise. After the implosive /ɗ/, on the other hand, it is probably associated with a greater glottal constriction resulting in greater turbulence noise.

Figure 8 suggests a greater CPP difference after plain stops in Eastern than in Western Jarai, but this apparent effect does not reach significance because of important inter-speaker variation (RegisterLow β = –0.9 dB, t = –1.1, p < .351 – Table E5, App. 3). The patterns found after other onsets are similar to those found in Western Jarai.

Figure 8. Normalized CPP of the first 200 ms of vowels following Western Jarai and Eastern Jarai onsets. Thick lines represent means, thin lines individual observations. The implosive /ɗ/ and the fricative /s/ do not contrast in register and are included for comparison.

Turning to vowel quality, we see in Figure 9 that F1 is significantly higher at vowel onset after high register stops than after low ones, a difference that lasts about 100 ms (RegisterLow β = –191 Hz, t = –10.9, p = .009 – Table W6, App. 3). This difference seems greater in low than in high vowels, and is not significant in /iː/ (RegisterLow:Voweliː β = 185 Hz, t = 6.7, p = .022 – Table W6, App. 3). There is no large register difference after sonorants, except perhaps in /ɔː/, and other obstruents mostly seem to pattern like high register plain stops.

Figure 9. Normalized F1 of the first 200 ms of vowels following Western Jarai and Eastern Jarai onsets. Thick lines represent means, thin lines individual observations. The implosive /ɗ/ and the fricative /s/ do not contrast in register and are included for comparison.

Figure 10. Normalized F2 of the first 200 ms of vowels following Western Jarai and Eastern Jarai onsets. Thick lines represent means, thin lines individual observations. The implosive /ɗ/ and the fricative /s/ do not contrast in register and are included for comparison.

The same general pattern seems to hold after Eastern Jarai plain stops, where register-conditioned F1 differences reach a high t-value, even if their p-value is high (RegisterLow β = –117 Hz, t = –2.0, p = .117 – Table E6, App. 3). The apparent reversal in /ɛː/ in Figure 9 is not significant. Sonorants seem to have a smaller F1 difference between the high and the low registers and other obstruents again pattern with the high register plain stops.

Differences in F2 are more subtle, as can be seen in Figure 10. In Western Jarai, there is a higher F2 at the beginning of vowels following low register stops (RegisterLow β = 173 Hz, t = 3.6, p = .069 – Table W7, App. 3). F2 after sonorants does not seem to pattern consistently and other obstruents do not clearly pattern with one register or the other.

Eastern Jarai shows some complex but robust F2 trends. While the register of plain stops does not condition a systematic F2 difference across vowels, there is a strong effect in at least /aː/ and /iː/ (RegisterLow:Vowelaː β = 337 Hz, t = 10.1, p = .002; RegisterLow:Voweliː β = 215 Hz, t = 6.4, p = .008 – Table E7, App. 3). No clear patterns emerge for other onset consonants.

4.2.3 Production cue weights

In order to get a better idea of the variation in the cues used to distinguish registers across speakers and dialects, Cohen’s d’s were computed for each speaker. Cohen’s d’s above 0.8 and below –0.8 indicate a large separability between the distribution of the two registers. Positive Cohen’s d’s denote larger values in the high register, while negative Cohen’s d’s denote larger values in the low register. In Figure 11, we can see that individual production cue weights are similar across ages and sexes and that they vary little between the two dialects.

Figure 11. Cohen’s d’s of each acoustic property associated with the Jarai register contrast, per dialect and speaker. Speakers are organized by sex (F/M) and year of birth.

F1 seems to be the most robust production cue in both dialects, with a Cohen’s d of more than 1 in all speakers. Voice quality cues also seem to distinguish the two registers: in most speakers, H1*–H2* has a Cohen’s d below -1 and CPP has a Cohen’s d above 1, and these two acoustic properties generally weigh heavier in Eastern than Western Jarai, but H1*–A1* is more variable and tends to have Cohen’s d’s much closer to 0. F2 also seems to have some distinctive value as it has consistently negative Cohen’s d but some speakers have values very close to 0. The other two cues, f0 and VOT are extremely variable, with some speakers having positive Cohen’s d’s while others have negative ones.

Overall, this confirms the results presented in Section 4.2.2: the two Jarai dialects investigated here no longer reliably distinguish voiced and voiceless plain stops but have developed a register contrast on following vowels. The production cues that are used to distinguish these registers are F1, voice quality and, to a certain extent, F2. The only clear difference between dialects is a slightly stronger reliance on H1*–H2* in the production of voice quality in Eastern Jarai.

5. Perception experiment

A perception experiment was conducted to determine if the cues used in register identification match the acoustic properties uncovered in the previous section and if they vary across dialects and speakers. In Section 5.1, we describe the methodology used for this experiment and in Section 5.2, we present identification results. In Section 5.3, we look at the relation between production and perception in the two dialects and across speakers.

5.1 Methodology

An experiment was designed in which listeners of both Western and Eastern Jarai had to listen to stimuli varying in acoustic parameters mirroring those found to be relevant in Section 4 and to identify them as either high or low register words. In Section 5.1.1, we describe the stimuli used in the experiment. In Section 5.1.2, we provide details about the participants and the experimental procedure. An overview of the statistical analysis is given in Section 5.1.3.

5.1.1 Stimuli

Stimuli were created using Klattgrid synthesis in Praat (Boersma & Weenink Reference Boersma and Weenink2010). Two minimal pairs were synthesized: /taː/ ‘we’ ∼ /daː/ ‘duck’ (Western Jarai), ‘chest’ (Eastern Jarai) and /tuː/ ‘closet’ ∼ /duː/ ‘deflated’. /d/ is used to mark the low register stop because there is no standard IPA diacritic for register and because the low register has optional closure voicing.

The stimuli were resynthesized based on natural utterances produced by a middle-aged male speaker whose register contrast was representative of the mean acoustic properties presented in Section 4. Vowel duration was set to 350 ms. We manipulated the acoustic parameters shown to play the most important role in the acoustic study: voice quality, F1, F2 and onset voicing. No attempt was made at manipulating f0, the least reliable acoustic property of register (if reliable at all), as this would have resulted in an unreasonably long experiment. Three-step continua were generated for each property using the following parameters.

  • Voicing. Three types of dental onset stops were generated: (1) a stop with full closure voicing (70 ms); (2) a stop with a voiceless release: voicing over the first 40 ms of its closure, a period of voicelessness at the end of the closure and 10 ms of aspiration after the release; and (3) a stop with a voiceless closure and 10 ms of aspiration after the release.

  • Voice quality. Voice quality was manipulated by using two Klatt parameters: open phase (or open quotient, OQ), which modulates spectral tilt (H1*–H2* and H1*–A1* in the production study), and breathiness amplitude (BA), which adds aspiration noise to the vowel (CPP in the production study). At vowel onset, the breathy step had a OQ of .6 and a BA of 60 dB, the middle step had a OQ of .5 and BA of 30 dB and the modal step had a OQ of .4 and a BA of 0 dB. All three synthesized steps then reached an OQ target of .5 at 150 ms and a BA target of 0 dB at 300 ms. Manipulation of these parameters yielded stimuli with voice qualities closely mirroring those of the production results, as illustrated in Figure 13.

  • F1. For /a/, targets at vowel onset were 500, 675 and 850 Hz. They all returned to 800 at 150 ms and remained stable until vowel end. For /u/, targets at vowel onset were 290, 415 and 540 Hz. They all returned to 300 at 150 ms and remained stable until vowel end.

  • F2. For /a/, targets at vowel onset were set to 1600, 1750 and 1900 Hz. They all returned to 1500 Hz at 100 ms and remained stable until vowel end. For /u/, targets at vowel end were 1300, 1425 and 1550 Hz. They all returned to 900 Hz at 100 ms and remained stable until vowel end.

  • f0. Pitch did not vary over the stimuli and was the same in /a/ and /u/ stimuli. It started at 120 Hz, dropped to 115 Hz at 100 ms and to 110 at vowel end.

All possible combinations of these acoustic values were synthesized, yielding eighty-one stimuli (3 voicing steps X 3 voice quality steps X 3 F1 steps X 3 F2 steps). Spectrograms of stimuli representing high register /ta/ and low register /da/ are given in Figure 12. These stimuli differ in all four acoustic dimensions discussed above and mirror natural productions of the target words. The reliability of the synthesis parameters was controlled by measuring the stimuli with PraatSauce (Kirby Reference Kirby2018). The distribution of the stimuli along the four acoustic dimensions shown to be the most relevant in Section 4 is given in Figure 13.

Figure 12. Spectrograms of sample stimuli. Top: Stimulus mirroring natural productions of high register /ta/ (Targets at vowel onset: OQ .4, BA 0 dB, F1 850 Hz, F2 1600 Hz). Bottom: Stimulus mirroring natural productions of low register /da/, with optional voicing (Targets at vowel onset: OQ .6, BA 60 dB, F1 500 Hz, F2 1900 Hz).

Figure 13. Mean values of the acoustic parameters manipulated in the stimuli used for the identification experiment. Top panel: /ta∼da/. Bottom panel /tu∼du/. The ribbons show one standard deviation above and below the mean (the large H1*–H2* ribbons for /tu∼du/ are due to the effect of F1 on spectral slope).

5.1.2 Participants and procedure

The perception experiment on Western Jarai was conducted in July 2022 by the first and the second authors. Forty-seven participants (twenty-four women, twenty-three men) took part in it (three additional participants were excluded because they were unable to use the computer). Out of the forty-seven participants, eighteen had been speakers for the production experiment three years before. Participants all resided in Saom Kaning or the vicinity. They were either born in Saom Kaning (40/48) or within 10 kilometers (7/48), except one who was born in Mondulkiri from parents originally from Saom Kaning and returned there at the age of four. Two participants spent a few years each in Vietnam and Kompong Cham. All participants spoke Khmer (most with a high proficiency), and several also spoke Tampuan and Vietnamese.

The perception experiment on Eastern Jarai was also conducted in July 2022, by the first, third and fourth authors. Forty-four participants (twenty-two women, twenty-two men) took part in it (two additional participants were excluded because they could not be trained on the identification task). Seventeen of the forty-four participants had been speakers for the production experiment three years before. They were all born in Ea Sup, but four had spent a few years in other Vietnamese cities for study or work. Participants all spoke Vietnamese, and several also spoke Rade, Mnong and Lao.

In both venues, participants were asked to sit in front of a computer placed on a table in a quiet room or underneath a stilt house. Up to three participants took part in the experiment simultaneously, in which case the different computers were positioned on separate tables facing different directions. They had to follow instructions presented on the screen, to listen to stimuli in Sennheiser HD 280 PRO headphones and to identify the stimuli by pressing one of two computer keys associated with images representing response choices. Images were identical in the two experiments, except those associated with the word /da/, which means ‘chest’ in Western Jarai but ‘duck’ in Eastern Jarai. For each pair of target words, participants underwent three training phases: one with three repetitions of the two stimuli most closely mirroring natural productions (with feedback), one with five repetitions of the same two near-natural stimuli (without feedback), and one with ten random stimuli. They then had to identify each set of eighty-one stimuli three times, in alternating blocks. As few participants could read Jarai, visual instructions were provided, along with short written instructions in Khmer (Western Jarai) and Vietnamese (Eastern Jarai).

5.1.3 Analysis

Mixed logistic regressions were used to analyze the identification results, by syllable and dialect. The dependent variable was the responses provided by participants. The fixed effects were the types of voicing and voice quality (VQ), F1 and F2 steps. Random slopes for each main effect by participant were also included. Models were simplified using the same top-down approach as in Section 4. Interactions were dropped one by one, starting with that with the lowest F-value, as long as the resulting models had a lower Akaike information criterion (AIC) score than the previous model or a higher or equal, but not significantly different AIC.

Figure 14. Proportion of high register /t/ responses for each type of /a/ stimulus, by F1, VOT (type of voicing), OQ (representing voice quality as a whole) and F2, for all listeners. Left panel: Western Jarai. Right panel: Eastern Jarai.

Figure 15. Coefficients and statistical significance of logistic regression models conducted on the responses given by Western Jarai listeners (left) and Eastern Jarai listeners (right) for /a/ stimuli. The full model summaries are provided in Tables W8 and E8, Appendix 3.

5.2 Perception results

Figure 14 plots the proportion of high register responses to the /a/ stimuli (/ta/ vs. /da/). In both dialects, F1 is by far the factor that plays the greater role in register identification, a high F1 favoring high register responses. Voicing (VOT) seems to play a weaker role, but is not negligible in stimuli with ambiguous F1 (green lines): a negative VOT is associated with the low register, while a 10 ms VOT biases responses towards the high register. The effects of voice quality (represented by open quotient, OQ) and F2 are not immediately apparent.

The results of the mixed logistic regressions largely confirm these patterns. They are plotted in Figure 15 (the full models are provided in Tables W8 and E8 in Appendix 3). We see that F1 is the dominant identification cue in both dialects, a high F1 triggering more high register responses, especially in Eastern Jarai. Voicing comes second: in both dialects, stimuli with a 10 ms positive VOT are more associated with the high register than stimuli with a negative VOT, while stimuli with a voiceless release fall in between. Note however that the role of voicing is greater in Eastern Jarai than in Western Jarai. Other main effects, voice quality and F2 turn out to play a weak role that was not visible in Figure 14: breathier phonation and a higher F2 both bias responses towards the low register. There are finally some weak, but significant interactions. In Western Jarai, the high register bias towards F1 is weaker when stimuli are breathier (VQ x F1). In Eastern Jarai, the effect of voice quality is reduced or cancelled out in stimuli that are not fully voiced (Voicing[vr] x F1, Voicing[10] x F1) and the effect of F2 is nullified when there is a 10 ms VOT (Voicing[10] x F2).

Figure 16. Proportion of high register /t/ responses for each type of /u/ stimulus, by F1, VOT, OQ (representing voice quality as a whole) and F2, for all listeners. Left panel: Western Jarai. Right panel: Eastern Jarai.

Figure 17. Coefficients and statistical significance of logistic regression models conducted on the responses given by Western Jarai listeners (left) and Eastern Jarai listeners (right) for /u/ stimuli. The full model summaries are provided in Tables W9 and E9, Appendix 3.

Figure 18. Log-odd estimates of each perceptual property by dialect and speaker, /a/ stimuli (Eastern Jarai participants F88, F89 and M86 have F1 log-odds greater than 20 that are off-scale).

The overall picture is very similar for /u/ stimuli. In Figure 16, we see that a high F1 favors high register responses in both dialects. Voicing is also important: negative VOT is associated to low register responses while a 10 ms VOT yields more high register responses, especially when F1 is ambiguous. The effects of voice quality (OQ) and F2 are more subtle.

These results are again confirmed by the statistical analysis. F1 is the factor with the largest log-odds estimate in both dialects. The effect of F1 is weaker in /u/ than /a/, which is likely due to its narrower F1 range. The effect of voicing is roughly comparable to what was found in /a/. Stimuli with a 10 ms VOT yield more high register responses than stimuli with a voiceless release, which in turn yield more high register responses than stimuli with a negative VOT, and the global effect of voicing is greater in Eastern than Western Jarai. Voice quality and F2 are also significant: breathier stimuli and stimuli with a high F2 weakly bias responses towards the low register. There are also significant interactions. In Western Jarai, the effect of F1 is unexpectedly greater in stimuli with a voiceless release and a 10 ms VOT than in stimuli with a negative VOT (Voicing[vr] x F1, Voicing[10] x F1). The high register bias towards F1 is also weaker when stimuli are breathier (VQ x F1). Eastern Jarai shows the same significant interactions, but in addition, the main effect of voice quality is canceled out in stimuli that do not have a negative voicing (Voicing[vr] x VQ, Voicing[10] x VQ).

Figure 19. Log-odd estimates of each perceptual property by dialect and speaker, /u/ stimuli (Eastern Jarai participants F77, F89 and M86’ have F1 log-odds greater than 20 that are off-scale).

In order to determine if the results of mixed logistic regressions by dialect hide individual differences, logistic regressions were conducted on each participant’s data. Voicing was here coded as a continuous variable (negative VOT = 0; voiceless release = 1, 10 ms VOT = 2). As there are only 243 observations per syllable (three repetitions of each of the eighty-one stimuli), these models include no interactions and no random effects and are not as robust as the models presented in Figures 15 and 17. Yet, they only show limited variation across vowels, speakers and dialects, as can be seen in Figures 18 and 19. F1 is the strongest perceptual cue for all participants, with estimates typically ranging between 2.5 and 5 for /a/ and between 1.25 and 3.75 in /u/. This slightly weaker weight of F1 in /u/ duplicates what was observed in the global mixed logistic regressions models above. Other cues are all much weaker, but Voicing seems to weigh a bit heavier in Eastern Jarai, which also matches the results of the global models.

5.3 Relation between production and perception

The Cohen’s d’s used as a proxy for production cue weights in Section 4.2.3, can be compared with the log-odds estimates used to assess perception cue weights in the previous section. A first general observation is that there is limited variation in cue weights (production or perception) across speakers and that this variation does not seem structured by age and sex. The second important observation is that production and perception cue weights largely match each other. F1 is the dominant property for all speakers in both production and perception. Voicing is a stronger cue in perception than production, but this seems to reflect biases in our stimuli more than natural speech. The stimuli with a strong negative VOT that we tested in the identification experiment are relatively rare in natural production, as can be seen in Figure 4. The identification weight of voicing would be much weaker if we focused exclusively on stops with a voiceless release and stops with a 10 ms VOT, which are more representative of natural productions. Voice quality seems to be stronger in production (CPP and H1*–H2* in Figure 11) than in perception (VQ in Figures 18 and 19), which could indicate that speakers produce a relatively salient voice quality contrast between registers but do not use it as systematically for identification. It could also be due to the difficulty of synthesizing stimuli with voice quality modulations perfectly matching those used in Jarai, given the rich and non-monotonic acoustic properties of voice quality. Finally, F2 is a weak cue in both production and perception.

6. Discussion and conclusion

The acoustic results presented in Section 4 show that neither of the two Jarai dialects described here preserve the original voicing contrast that is reconstructed for Proto-Chamic (Lee Reference Lee1966; Burnham Reference Burnham1976; Thurgood Reference Thurgood1999). Full closure voicing is rare (especially in women), and more than half of the stops that were described as voiced in previous descriptions of Jarai are totally voiceless. Saom Kaning and Ea Sup Jarai have both developed register contrasts in which closure voicing is at best an optional secondary cue of the low register.

The acoustic properties of register are almost identical in the two dialects. Register is primarily realized through modulations of F1 that result in ongliding immediately after onset stops. As can be seen in Figure 9, low register vowels start with a lower F1. In low vowels this results in a falling diphthong (/a/ realized as [ɛa]), while in high vowels, it is the high register that is realized with a weak rising onglide (e.g., /i/ realized as [ɪi]). This diphthongization pattern is widely attested in register languages (Huffman Reference Huffman1985).

Vowels also bear weaker register cues like voice quality and F2 modulations. The first 150 ms of the low register vowels has a higher H1*–H2* than that of high register vowels, which indicates breathiness or laxness (Figure 6). Other spectral slope measurements, like H1*–A1* show a weaker difference (Figure 7). CPP differences between registers (Figure 8) are minimal and do not clearly reach significance in Western Jarai, suggesting that there is little breathiness noise and that the low register may contrast a lax voice with the modal voice of the high register, but overall, voice quality (H1*–H2*) seems to be slightly more salient in Eastern than in Western Jarai. F2 differences between registers are subtle, but there tends to be a slightly higher F2 in the low register immediately at vowel onset. Whether this is caused by active tongue-fronting or by a lengthening of the supraglottal cavity remains unclear. Finally, f0 does not seem to be a reliable register cue, contrary to what was found in related Eastern and Western Cham (Phú, Edmondson and Gregerson Reference Phú, Edmondson and Gregerson1992; Brunelle Reference Brunelle2005, Reference Brunelle, Sidwell and Grant2006, Reference Brunelle2009).

Against our initial expectations, there is little evidence that the register contrast found after stops was extended to vowels following sonorants. Since we tested a relatively small number of sonorant-initial syllables, this should be further investigated, but we can safely say that if there were any register contrast in that context, it would be more subtle than after stops.

Our perception experiment establishes that the cues used in register identification largely match those used in production, with F1 being the primary identification cue and voice quality and F2 playing secondary roles. It also confirms that closure voicing, even if it is optional in production, is associated with the low register.

The absence of structured variation in age and sex across participants, in perception as well as production, suggests that both dialects under study have stable register systems. More importantly, the near absence of differences in the phonetic realization of register in two Jarai dialects that are not in contact and have no recent genetic relation is a strong indication that Jarai as a whole may be registral. An investigation of dialects spoken in Gia Lai province, Vietnam, would be needed to confirm this contention.

If Jarai has such a clear register system, how can we explain that it has always been described as preserving the original Proto-Chamic obstruent voicing contrast (Dournes Reference Dournes1964; Headley Reference Headley1965; Lafont Reference Lafont1968), except for brief mentions of a register contrast in Williams and Siu (Reference Williams, Siu and Williams2013) and Jensen (Reference Jensen2014)? A first possible explanation is that it underwent registrogenesis recently. However, it is unlikely that two Jarai dialects that have no direct contact would have developed registers independently, especially since they are not in contact with the same languages. Moreover, the fact that even a recent Jarai dictionary omits any mention of register suggests that there is more at play (Siu Reference Siu2009). We hypothesize that the pioneer linguists that developed Jarai orthography were unaware of the existence of register, which was first explicitly discussed by Henderson (Reference Henderson1952) but only became a well-known concept in the late 1960s, and transcribed the low register syllables with voiced stops because that was the closest available category in their native languages (French and English). This L1 bias may have been reinforced by an overrepresentation of optional closure voicing in the careful speech typically used in the elicitation sessions that are a necessary first stage of language documentation. In fact, Jarai is not the only language in which register was ‘missed’ by descriptive linguists before the 1960s: other examples include Chrau (Tạ, Brunelle and Nguyễn Reference Tạ, Brunelle and Quý Nguyễn2022), Chru (Brunelle et al. Reference Brunelle, Tấn Tạ, Kirby and Đinh2020) and Central Mnong (Brunelle, Đinh and Tạ Reference Brunelle, Đinh and Tấn Tạ2023).

The existence of register in Jarai forces us to reconsider what is known about the development of register in Chamic languages, a difficult task because there is no consensus on Chamic internal subgrouping (Lee Reference Lee1966; Burnham Reference Burnham1976; Thurgood Reference Thurgood1999; Brunelle Reference Brunelle, Adelaar and Schapper2023). The only point of agreement between authors is that Jarai and Rade form a subgroup, here Highlands Chamic; other subgroups are more controversial and have been proposed based on geographical criteria (see Map 30.1 in Brunelle and Jensen Reference Brunelle, Jensen, Adelaar and Schapper2023) or ill-described innovations (Brunelle Reference Brunelle, Adelaar and Schapper2023: for a recent review). Despite this absence of consensus on subgrouping, previous reconstructions of proto-Chamic all postulated that it had a voicing contrast in obstruents, assuming that only three Chamic languages spoken close to the coast, Tsat, Cham and Haroi, have developed register (Blood Reference Blood1967; Friberg & Hor Reference Friberg, Hor, Thomas, Lee and Liêm Nguyễn1977; Lee Reference Lee1977; Mundhenk & Goschnick Reference Mundhenk, Goschnick, Thomas, Lee and Đăng Liêm1977; Hoàng Reference Hoàng1987; Headley Reference Headley and Davidson1991; Maddieson & Pang Reference Maddieson, Pang, Edmondson and Gregerson1993; Thurgood Reference Thurgood, Edmondson and Gregerson1993; Đoàn Reference Đoàn2009). The recent discovery of a register contrast in three other Chamic languages spoken in the foothills of the Annamite Cordillera, Cát Gia Raglai, Chru, Southern Raglai was already a problem for these reconstructions (Lee Reference Lee1998; Brunelle et al. Reference Brunelle, Tấn Tạ, Kirby and Đinh2020; Brunelle, Brown & Hà Reference Brunelle, Brown and Thị Thu Hà2022), but now that register is even attested in a Highlands Chamic language like Jarai, there seems to be sufficient evidence to propose that register has been a feature of Chamic languages for much longer than previously assumed. That said, two elements currently prevent us from reconstructing it all the way to Proto-Chamic. First, Northern Raglai still clearly has an obstruent voicing contrast (Brunelle, Brown and Hà Reference Brunelle, Brown and Thị Thu Hà2022). Unless we claim that this voicing contrast is a modern reflex of an earlier register contrast, this forces us to maintain a conservative standpoint. Second, the register systems of Chamic languages do not all have the same primary register cue. While Tsat and Cham are primarily pitch-based (Phú, Edmondson and Gregerson Reference Phú, Edmondson and Gregerson1992; Maddieson & Pang Reference Maddieson, Pang, Edmondson and Gregerson1993; Brunelle Reference Brunelle2005, Reference Brunelle, Sidwell and Grant2006, Reference Brunelle2009), Southern Raglai, Haroi, Chru and Jarai mainly rely on F1 (Đoàn Reference Đoàn2009; Brunelle et al. Reference Brunelle, Tấn Tạ, Kirby and Đinh2020; Brunelle, Brown and Hà Reference Brunelle, Brown and Thị Thu Hà2022). Unless we can establish paths of change through which register systems can drift from one primary cue to others, it remains unclear if all Chamic register systems have a common source.

This in turn has implications for models of the development of register systems in Mainland Southeast Asian languages. Many of these models assume that voice quality is the primary acoustic property of register and that other cues developed as a consequence of original voice quality modulations (Huffman Reference Huffman1976; Thurgood Reference Thurgood2002; Wayland & Jongman Reference Wayland and Jongman2002). However, the fact that voice quality is never a primary property of register in the varieties of Chamic studied so far suggests that voice quality may not be as instrumental in registrogenesis as previously claimed. Two alternative scenarios emerge. The first one is that early register is always realized through multiple phonetic features, like vowel quality, voice quality and possibly pitch, and that each language then enhances and drops some of these properties. The other is that various types of phonetic properties can transphonologize directly as a result of the loss of onset voicing. This would be parallel with cases of tonal contrasts developing from the loss of onset voicing without any sign of voice quality developments (Svantesson & House Reference Svantesson and House2006; Howe Reference Howe2017; Coetzee et al. Reference Coetzee, Patrice Speeter Beddor, Styler and Wissing2018; Kirby, Pittayaporn & Brunelle Reference Kirby, Pittayaporn and Brunelle2023).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank several people for their help in conducting the research presented in this paper. Our work on Western Jarai would have been impossible without the help of Joshua Jensen, who helped us with logistic matters in Saom Kaning and introduced us to key community members. We are also indebted to Kalan Khi, who recruited participants for us and acted as our Khmer-Vietnamese-Jarai translator in the village, and would like to thank local authorities for allowing us to conduct research in the area. Our work in Ea Sup was made possible by Y Tit Kpă (†) and Y Khăm Ta Niê, who acted as local recruiters and fixers and as Vietnamese-Jarai translators. We also thank the administration of the province of Đăk Lăk and the district of Ea Sup for granting us research authorizations. We finally thank all our participants, who gracefully took part in our uncanny experiments with patience and good humour, and our annotators, Jeanne Brown, Sabrina McCullough and Sue-Anne Richer. This project was funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (grants 435-2017-0498 ‘Voicing and its transphonologization: The initiation and actuation of a sound change in Southeast Asia” and 435-2022-0047 ‘Sound change and the interaction between production and perception: Register in Austronesian and Austroasiatic’).

Appendix 1. Western Jarai wordlist

(‘ ̥ ‘ is used for purported low register sonorants)

Appendix 2. Eastern Jarai wordlist

(‘ ̥ ‘ is used for purported low register sonorants)

Appendix 3. Mixed models

Table W1. Table of estimates for mixed model on VOT in Western Jarai plain stops with positive VOT

Table E1. Table of estimates for mixed model on VOT in Eastern Jarai plain stops with positive VOT

Table W2. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized f0 over the first ten sampling points after Western Jarai plain stops

Table E2. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized f0 over the first ten sampling points after Eastern Jarai plain stops

Table W3. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized H1*–H2* over the first ten sampling points after Western Jarai plain stops

Table E3. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized H1*–H2* over the first ten sampling points after Eastern Jarai plain stops

Table W4. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized H1*–A1* over the first ten sampling points after Western Jarai plain stops

Table E4. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized H1*–A1* over the first ten sampling points after Eastern Jarai plain stops

Table W5. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized CPP over the first ten sampling points after Western Jarai plain stops

Table E5. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized CPP over the first ten sampling points after Eastern Jarai plain stops

Table W6. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized F1 over the first ten sampling points after Western Jarai plain stops

Table E6. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized F1 over the first ten sampling points after Eastern Jarai plain stops

Table W7. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized F2 over the first ten sampling points after Western Jarai plain stops

Table E7. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized F2 over the first ten sampling points after Eastern Jarai plain stops

Table W8. Table of estimates of the final logistic regression model for /a/ stimuli in Western Jarai. Estimates represent the log odds of high register responses. VQ, F1 and F2 are centered.

Table E8. Table of estimates of the final logistic regression model for /a/ stimuli in Eastern Jarai. Estimates represent the log odds of high register responses. VQ, F1 and F2 are centered.

Table W9. Table of estimates of the final logistic regression model for /u/ stimuli in Western Jarai. Estimates represent the log odds of high register responses. VQ, F1 and F2 are centered.

Table E9. Table of estimates of the final logistic regression model for /u/ stimuli in Eastern Jarai. Estimates represent the log odds of high register responses. VQ, F1 and F2 are centered.

References

Abramson, Arthur S., Luangthongkum, Therapan & Nye, Patrick W.. 2004. Voice register in Suai (Kuai): An analysis of perceptual and acoustic data. Phonetica 61(2–3), 147171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abramson, Arthur S., Nye, Patrick W. & Luangthongkum, Therapan. 2007. Voice register in Khmu’: Experiments in production and perception. Phonetica 64(2–3), 80104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abramson, Arthur S., Tiede, Mark K. & Luangthongkum, Therapan. 2015. Voice register in Mon: Acoustics and electroglottography. Phonetica 72(4), 237256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adisasmito-Smith, Niken (2004). Phonetic influences of Javanese on Indonesian. Ph.D. Linguistics, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Bayley, Robert & Holland, Cory. 2014. Variation in Chicano English: The case of final (z) devoicing. American Speech 89(4), 385407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blagden, Charles Otto. 1910. Quelques notions sur la phonétique du Talain et son évolution historique. Journal Asiatique 15, 477505.Google Scholar
Blood, David L. 1967. Phonological Units in Cham. Anthropological Linguistics 9(8), 1532.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul & Weenink, David. 2010. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer. http://www.praat.org Google Scholar
Brunelle, Marc. (2005). Register in Eastern Cham: Phonological, phonetic and sociolinguistic approaches. Ph.D. Linguistics, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Brunelle, Marc. 2006. A phonetic study of Eastern Cham register. In Sidwell, Paul & Grant, Anthony (eds.), Chamic and beyond, 136. Sidney: Pacific Linguistics.Google Scholar
Brunelle, Marc. 2009. Contact-induced change? Register in three Cham dialects. Journal of Southeast Asian Linguistics 2, 122.Google Scholar
Brunelle, Marc. 2010. The role of larynx height in the Javanese tense ∼ lax stop contrast. In Mercado, Raphael, Potsdam, Eric & Travis, Lisa (eds.), Austronesian contributions to linguistic theory: Selected proceedings of AFLA, 724. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunelle, Marc. 2023. Historical linguistics of the Chamic languages. In Adelaar, Sander & Schapper, Antoinette (eds.), The Oxford guide to the Malayo-Polynesian languages of Southeast Asia, 143149. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Brunelle, Marc, Brown, Jeanne & Thị Thu Hà, Phạm. 2022. Northern Raglai voicing and its relation to Southern Raglai register: Evidence for early stages of registrogenesis. Phonetica 79(2), 151188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunelle, Marc, Đinh, Lu Giang & Tấn Tạ, Thành. 2023. Voicing and register in Mnông Râlâm. Proceedings of the 20th Congress of Phonetic Sciences.Google Scholar
Brunelle, Marc & Jensen, Joshua. 2023. Chamic languages. In Adelaar, Sander & Schapper, Antoinette (eds.), The Oxford guide to the Malayo-Polynesian languages of Southeast Asia, 455469. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Brunelle, Marc & Phú, Văn Hẳản. 2019. Colloquial Eastern Cham. In Vittrant, Alice & Watkins, Justin (eds.), The Mainland Southeast Asia Linguistic Area. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Brunelle, Marc & Tấn Tạ, Thành. 2021. Register in languages of Mainland Southeast Asia: The state of the art. In Sidwell, Paul & Jenny, Matthias (eds.), The languages and linguistics of Mainland Southeast Asia: A comprehensive guide, 683706. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunelle, Marc, Tấn Tạ, Thành, Kirby, James & Đinh, Lu Giang. 2020. Transphonologization of voicing in Chru: Studies in production and perception. Laboratory Phonology: Journal of the Association for Laboratory Phonology 11 (1)(15).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bùi, Khánh Thế 1996. Ngữ Pháp Tiếng Chăm [Cham grammar]. Hà Nội: Nhà Xuất Baản Giáo Dục.Google Scholar
Burnham, Eugene. (1976). The place of Haroi in the Chamic languages. Linguistics. University of Texas at Arlington.Google Scholar
Clayards, Meghan. (2008). The ideal listener: Making optimal use of acoustic-phonetic cues for word recognition. Linguistics. University of Rochester.Google Scholar
Coetzee, Andries W., Patrice Speeter Beddor, Kerby Shedden, Styler, Will & Wissing, Daan. 2018. Plosive voicing in Afrikaans: Differential cue weighting and tonogenesis. Journal of Phonetics 66(Complete), 185216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, Jacob. 1988. The effect size index: d. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Cohn, Abigail C. & Lockwood, Katherine. 1994. A phonetic description of Madurese and its phonological implications. Working Papers of the Cornell Phonetics Laboratory 9, 6792.Google Scholar
Cohn, Abigail C. 1993a. Consonant-vowel interactions in Madurese: The feature lowered larynx. Papers from the regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society 29, 105119.Google Scholar
Cohn, Abigail C. 1993b. Voicing and vowel height in Madurese: A preliminary report. In Edmondson, Jerold & Gregerson, Kenneth (eds.), Tonality in Austronesian Languages, 107122. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Cohn, Abigail C. & Ham, William H.. 1999. Temporal properties of Madurese consonants: A preliminary report. In Elizabeth Zeitoun & Paul Jen-kuei Li (eds.), Selected papers from the eighth international conference on Austronesian linguistics, 227–249. Taipei: Academia Sinica.Google Scholar
Davidson, Lisa. 2016. Variability in the implementation of voicing in American English obstruents. Journal of Phonetics 54, 3550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Đoàn, Văn Phúc. 2009. Hế thống ng âm tiếng Chăm Hroi (trong sụ so sánh vói hệ thống ng âm tiếng Chăm) [The phonetic system of Haroi (in comparison with the Cham phonetic system)]. In Văn Thông Tạ (ed.), Tìm hiêảu ngôn ng các dân tộc oả Việt NAm, 161–189. H̀ Ṇi: Nh̀ xuát baả̉n khoa ḥc x ḥi.Google Scholar
Dournes, Jacques. 1964. Ebauche de dictionnaire de la Langue Jörai. Cheo Reo.Google Scholar
Dournes, Jacques. 1976. Le parler des Jörai et le style oral de leur expression. Publications orientalistes de France.Google Scholar
Draxler, Christoph & Jänsch, Klaus. 2004. SpeechRecorder-A universal platform independent multi-channel audio recording software. LREC. Google Scholar
Dresser, Craig. 2005. A phonological account of Javanese stops. Presented at the midcontinental workshop on phonology (MCWOP11). Ann Arbor, Michigan.Google Scholar
Edmondson, Jerold & Gregerson, Kenneth. 1993. Western Cham as a register language. In Jerold Edmondson & Kenneth Gregerson (eds.), Tonality in Austronesian languages, 61–74. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Fagan, Joel L. 1988. Javanese intervocalic stop phonemes. Studies in Austronesian Linguistics 76, 173202.Google Scholar
Ferlus, Michel. 1979. Formation des Registres et mutations consonantiques dans les langues Mon-Khmer. Mon Khmer Studies VIII, 176.Google Scholar
Ferlus, Michel. 1992. Essai de phonétique historique du Khmer (Du milieu du premier millénaire de notre ère à l’époque actuelle). Mon-Khmer Studies 21, 5789.Google Scholar
Friberg, Timothy & Hor, Kvoeu. 1977. Register in Western Cham phonology. In Thomas, David D., Lee, Ernest W. & Liêm Nguyễn, Đăng (eds.), Papers in Southeast Asian Linguistics No.4, vol. 48, 1738. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.Google Scholar
Garellek, Marc & Esposito, Christina M.. 2021. Phonetics of White Hmong vowel and tonal contrasts. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 120.Google Scholar
General Statistics Office of Vietnam 2020. Completed results of the 2019 Vietnam population and housing census. Hanoi: Statistical Publishing House.Google Scholar
Gregerson, Kenneth. 1976. Tongue-root and register in Mon-Khmer. In Jenner, Philip N., Thompson, Laurence & Starosta, Stanley (eds.), Austroasiatic Studies 1, 323369. Honolulu: University Press of Hawaii.Google Scholar
Hayward, Katrina. 1993. /p/ vs. /b/ in Javanese: Some preliminary data. Working Papers in Linguistics and Phonetics 3, 1–33.Google Scholar
Hayward, Katrina. 1995. /p/ vs. /b/ in Javanese: The role of the vocal folds. Working Papers in Linguistics and Phonetics 5, 1–11.Google Scholar
Hayward, Katrina, Grafield-Davies, D., Howard, B. J., Latif, J. & Allen, Ray. 1994. Javanese stop consonants: The role of the vocal folds. London: School of Oriental and African Studies.Google Scholar
Headley, Robert K. 1991. The phonology of Kompong Thom Cham. In Davidson, Jeremy (ed.), Austroasiatic languages essays in honour of H. L. Shorto, 105121. London: School of Oriental and African Studies.Google Scholar
Headley, Robert K. Jr. 1965. A dictionary of the Jarai language. Washington.Google Scholar
Helgason, Pétur & Ringen, Catherine. 2008. Voicing and aspiration in Swedish stops. Journal of Phonetics 36(4), 607628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henderson, Eugenie. 1952. The main features of Cambodian pronunciation. Bulletin of the School of Oriental and African Studies 14(1), 453476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoàng, Thị Châu. 1987. Hệ thống thanh điệu tiếng Chàm và các kí hiệu [The Cham tone system and diacritics]. Ngôn Ngữ 1–2, 3135.Google Scholar
Howe, Penelope Jane (2017). Tonogenesis in central dialects of Malagasy: Acoustic and perceptual evidence with implications for synchronic mechanisms of sound change. Linguistics. Rice University.Google Scholar
Huffman, Franklin E. 1976. The register problem in fifteen Mon-Khmer languages. Oceanic Linguistics special publication Austroasiatic Studies, part 1(13), 575589.Google Scholar
Huffman, Franklin E. 1985. Vowel permutations in Austroasiatic languages: Papers presented to Paul K. Benedict for his 71st birthday. In Graham Thurgood, James Matisoff & David Bradley (eds.), Linguistics of the Sino-Tibetan area: The state of the art., 141–145. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics Series C 87, Australian National University.Google Scholar
Iseli, Markus & Alwan, Abeer. 2004. An improved correction formula for the estimation of harmonic magnitudes and its application to open quotient estimation. Proceedings of the International Conference on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing (ICASSP’04 ) 72(1), 669672.Google Scholar
Jensen, Joshua M. 2014. Jarai clauses and noun phrases: Syntactic structures in an Austronesian language. Berlin and New York: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jessen, Michael & Ringen, Catherine. 2003. Laryngeal features in German. Phonology 19(2), 189218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
José, Brian. 2010. The Apparent-Time Construct and stable variation: Final/z/devoicing in northwestern Indiana 1. Journal of Sociolinguistics 14(1), 3459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenstowicz, Michael J. 2021. Phonetic correlates of the Javanese Voicing contrast in stop consonants. NUSA: Linguistic Studies of Languages in and around Indonesia (70), 137.Google Scholar
Kingston, John, Macmillan, Neil A., Walsh Dickey, Laura, Thorburn, Rachel & Bartels, Christine. 1997. Integrality in the perception of tongue root position and voice quality in vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 101(3), 16961709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirby, James. 2018. Praatsauce: Praat-based tools for spectral analysis (version 0.2.4). https://github.com/kirbyj/praatsauce Google Scholar
Kirby, James. 2020. Madurese. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 50(1), 109126.Google Scholar
Kirby, James, Pittayaporn, Pittayawat & Brunelle, Marc. 2023. Transphonologization of onset voicing: Revisiting Northern and Eastern Kmhmu’. Phonetica 79(6), 591629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B. & Christensen, R. H. B.. 2017. lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software 82(13), 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
L. Thongkum, Therapan. 1989. An acoustic study of the register complex in Kui (Suai). Mon-Khmer Studies 15, 119.Google Scholar
L. Thongkum, Therapan. 1990. The interaction between pitch and phonation type in Mon: Phonetic implications for a theory of tonogenesis. Mon-Khmer Studies 16–17, 1124.Google Scholar
Lafont, Pierre-Bernard 1968. Lexique Jarai. Paris: École Française d’Extrême-Orient.Google Scholar
Lau-Preechathammarach, Raksit Tyler. 2023. The interaction between language usage and acoustic correlates of the Kuy register distinction. Laboratory Phonology 14(1).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, Ernest W. (1966). Proto-Chamic phonologic word and vocabulary. microform. Language and Literature. Indiana University.Google Scholar
Lee, Ernest W. 1977. Devoicing, aspiration, and vowel split in Haroi: Evidence for register (contrastive tongue-root position). In David D. Thomas, Ernest W. Lee & Nguyễn Đăng Liêm (eds.), Papers in Southeast Asian Linguistics no.4, vol. 48, 87–104. Canberra: Australian National University.Google Scholar
Lee, Ernest W. 1998. The contribution of Cat Gia Roglai to Chamic. In David D. Thomas (ed.), Papers in Southeast Asian Linguistics no.15: Further Chamic Studies, vol. 89, 31–54. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics – Series A.Google Scholar
Lee, Thomas. 1983. An acoustical study of the register distinction in Mon. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 57, 7996.Google Scholar
Mackenzie, Laurel. 2018. Variable stem-final fricative voicing in American English plurals: Different pa [ð∼ θ] s of change. Language Variation and Change 30(2), 147174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maddieson, Ian & Pang, Keng-Fong. 1993. Tone in Utsat. In Edmondson, Jerold & Gregerson, Kenneth (eds.), Tonality in Austronesian languages, 7589. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Maspero, Georges. 1915. Grammaire de la langue khmère (cambodgien). Paris: Imprimerie nationale.Google Scholar
Maspong, Sireemas. 2021. The temporal unfolding of register reveals distinct mechanisms at play: A case study of Western Khmer. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 150(4), A68A68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michnowicz, Jim & Planchón, Lucía. 2020. Sheísmo in Montevideo Spanish. Variation and Evolution: Aspects of language contact and contrast across the Spanish-speaking world 29, 163.Google Scholar
Misnadin & James Kirby. 2020. Acoustic correlates of plosive voicing in Madurese. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 147(4), 27792790.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Misnadin, James Kirby & Remijsen, Bert. 2015. Temporal and spectral properties of Madurese stops. Proceedings of the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, paper 789.Google Scholar
Moussay, Gérard 1971. Dictionnaire cam-vietnamien-franais. Phan Rang: Trung-tâm Văn hoá Chăm.Google Scholar
Mundhenk, Alice Tegenfeldt & Goschnick, Hella. 1977. Haroi Phonemes. In Thomas, David D., Lee, Ernest W. & Đăng Liêm, Nguyễn (eds.), Papers in Southeast Asian Linguistics no. 4, vol. 48, 115. Canberra: Australian National University.Google Scholar
Ohala, John J. 1983. The origin of sound patterns in vocal tract constraints. In MacNeilage, Peter (ed.), The production of speech, 189216. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohala, John J. 2011. Accommodation to the aerodynamic voicing constraint and its phonological relevance. Proceedings of the International Congress of Phonetic Sciences XVII, 64–67. Hong Kong.Google Scholar
Phú, Văn Hẳản, Edmondson, Jerold & Gregerson, Kenneth. 1992. Eastern Cham as a tone language. Mon Khmer Studies 20, 3143.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, Scott & Garellek, Marc. 2018. Plosive voicing acoustics and voice quality in Yerevan Armenian. Journal of Phonetics 71, 425450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siu, Ha Diếu 1976. Tơoloơi Hrăm kơo Tơolơoi Jơorai [Jarai langage lessons]. Summer Institute of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Siu, Lap Minh (2009). Developing the first preliminary dictionary of North American Jarai. Master’s Thesis, Texas Tech University, Lubbock.Google Scholar
Smith, Bruce L. 1978. Effects of place of articulation and vowel environment on voiced stop consonant production. Glossa 12(2), 163175.Google Scholar
Svantesson, Jan-Olof & House, David. 2006. Tone production, tone perception and Kammu tonogenesis. Phonology 23, 309333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tạ, Thành Tấn, Brunelle, Marc & Quý Nguyễn, Trần. 2022. Voicing and register in Ngãi Giao Chrau: Production and perception studies. Journal of Phonetics 90, 101115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tạ, Văn Thông. 2009. Tiếng Ra glai oảở ćc địa phuơong [The Raglai language in its dialects]. In Văn Thông Tạ (ed.), T̀m hiêả̉u ngôn ng ćc dân tộc oả ở Việt Nam, 222–245. H̀ Nội: Nh̀ xuất baả̉n khoa học x hội.Google Scholar
Thurgood, Ela. 2004. Phonation types in Javanese. Oceanic Linguistics 43(2), 277295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thurgood, Graham. 1993. Phan Rang Cham and Utsat: Tonogenetic themes and variants. In Edmondson, Jerold & Gregerson, Kenneth (eds.), Tonality in Austronesian Languages, 91106. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Thurgood, Graham. 1996. Language contact and the directionality of internal drift: The development of tones and registers in Chamic. Language 72(1), 131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thurgood, Graham 1999. From ancient Cham to modern dialects : Two thousand years of language contact and change. Honolulu: University of Hawai’i Press.Google Scholar
Thurgood, Graham. 2002. Vietnamese and tonogenesis: Revising the model and the analysis. Diachronica 19, 333363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alphen, Van, Petra, M. & Smits, Roel. 2004. Acoustical and perceptual analysis of the voicing distinction in Dutch initial plosives: The role of prevoicing. Journal of Phonetics 32(4), 455491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watkins, Justin 2002. The phonetics of wa: Experimental phonetics, phonology, orthography and sociolinguistics. Canberra: Australian National University.Google Scholar
Wayland, Ratree (1997). Acoustic and perceptual investigation of breathy and clear phonation in Chanthaburi Khmer: Implications for the history of Khmer phonology. Ph.D. Linguistics. Cornell.Google Scholar
Wayland, Ratree & Jongman, Allard. 2001. Chanthaburi Khmer vowels: Phonetic and phonemic analyses. Mon-Khmer Studies 31, 6582.Google Scholar
Wayland, Ratree & Jongman, Allard. 2002. Registrogenesis in Khmer: A phonetic account. Mon-Khmer Studies 32, 101115.Google Scholar
Williams, Jeffrey P. & Siu, Lap M.. 2013. The aesthetics of Jarai echo morphology. In Williams, Jeffrey P. (ed.), The aesthetics of grammar: Sound and meaning in the languages of mainland Southeast Asia, 191206. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Geographical distribution of ethnic Jarai in Vietnam and Cambodia, by commune.

Figure 1

Table 1. Jarai onsets (adapted from Dournes 1976)

Figure 2

Figure 2. Annotation of Western Jarai target word /daː/ ‘duck’. Top: spectrogram; Middle: EGG signal; Bottom: acoustic landmarks (ps: previous sonorant, cl: closure, op: open phase, ov: onset of voicing, cv: cessation of voicing, rv: resumption of voicing).

Figure 3

Table 2. Proportion of excluded measures

Figure 4

Figure 3. VOT distribution in stops in Western Jarai (left) and Eastern Jarai (right).

Figure 5

Figure 4. Proportion of low register stops which are fully devoiced, have a voiceless release or are fully voiced, by dialect and speaker. Speakers are organized by sex (F/M) and year of birth.

Figure 6

Figure 5. Normalized f0 of the first 200 ms of vowels following Western Jarai and Eastern Jarai onsets. Thick lines represent means, thin lines individual observations. The implosive /ɗ/ and the fricative /s/ do not contrast in register and are included for comparison.

Figure 7

Figure 6. Normalized H1*–H2* of the first 200 ms of vowels following Western Jarai and Eastern Jarai onsets. Thick lines represent means, thin lines individual observations. The implosive /ɗ/ and the fricative /s/ do not contrast in register and are included for comparison.

Figure 8

Figure 7. Normalized H1*–A1* of the first 200 ms of vowels following Western Jarai and Eastern Jarai onsets. Thick lines represent means, thin lines individual observations. The implosive /ɗ/ and the fricative /s/ do not contrast in register and are included for comparison.

Figure 9

Figure 8. Normalized CPP of the first 200 ms of vowels following Western Jarai and Eastern Jarai onsets. Thick lines represent means, thin lines individual observations. The implosive /ɗ/ and the fricative /s/ do not contrast in register and are included for comparison.

Figure 10

Figure 9. Normalized F1 of the first 200 ms of vowels following Western Jarai and Eastern Jarai onsets. Thick lines represent means, thin lines individual observations. The implosive /ɗ/ and the fricative /s/ do not contrast in register and are included for comparison.

Figure 11

Figure 10. Normalized F2 of the first 200 ms of vowels following Western Jarai and Eastern Jarai onsets. Thick lines represent means, thin lines individual observations. The implosive /ɗ/ and the fricative /s/ do not contrast in register and are included for comparison.

Figure 12

Figure 11. Cohen’s d’s of each acoustic property associated with the Jarai register contrast, per dialect and speaker. Speakers are organized by sex (F/M) and year of birth.

Figure 13

Figure 12. Spectrograms of sample stimuli. Top: Stimulus mirroring natural productions of high register /ta/ (Targets at vowel onset: OQ .4, BA 0 dB, F1 850 Hz, F2 1600 Hz). Bottom: Stimulus mirroring natural productions of low register /da/, with optional voicing (Targets at vowel onset: OQ .6, BA 60 dB, F1 500 Hz, F2 1900 Hz).

Figure 14

Figure 13. Mean values of the acoustic parameters manipulated in the stimuli used for the identification experiment. Top panel: /ta∼da/. Bottom panel /tu∼du/. The ribbons show one standard deviation above and below the mean (the large H1*–H2* ribbons for /tu∼du/ are due to the effect of F1 on spectral slope).

Figure 15

Figure 14. Proportion of high register /t/ responses for each type of /a/ stimulus, by F1, VOT (type of voicing), OQ (representing voice quality as a whole) and F2, for all listeners. Left panel: Western Jarai. Right panel: Eastern Jarai.

Figure 16

Figure 15. Coefficients and statistical significance of logistic regression models conducted on the responses given by Western Jarai listeners (left) and Eastern Jarai listeners (right) for /a/ stimuli. The full model summaries are provided in Tables W8 and E8, Appendix 3.

Figure 17

Figure 16. Proportion of high register /t/ responses for each type of /u/ stimulus, by F1, VOT, OQ (representing voice quality as a whole) and F2, for all listeners. Left panel: Western Jarai. Right panel: Eastern Jarai.

Figure 18

Figure 17. Coefficients and statistical significance of logistic regression models conducted on the responses given by Western Jarai listeners (left) and Eastern Jarai listeners (right) for /u/ stimuli. The full model summaries are provided in Tables W9 and E9, Appendix 3.

Figure 19

Figure 18. Log-odd estimates of each perceptual property by dialect and speaker, /a/ stimuli (Eastern Jarai participants F88, F89 and M86 have F1 log-odds greater than 20 that are off-scale).

Figure 20

Figure 19. Log-odd estimates of each perceptual property by dialect and speaker, /u/ stimuli (Eastern Jarai participants F77, F89 and M86’ have F1 log-odds greater than 20 that are off-scale).

Figure 21

Table W1. Table of estimates for mixed model on VOT in Western Jarai plain stops with positive VOT

Figure 22

Table E1. Table of estimates for mixed model on VOT in Eastern Jarai plain stops with positive VOT

Figure 23

Table W2. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized f0 over the first ten sampling points after Western Jarai plain stops

Figure 24

Table E2. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized f0 over the first ten sampling points after Eastern Jarai plain stops

Figure 25

Table W3. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized H1*–H2* over the first ten sampling points after Western Jarai plain stops

Figure 26

Table E3. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized H1*–H2* over the first ten sampling points after Eastern Jarai plain stops

Figure 27

Table W4. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized H1*–A1* over the first ten sampling points after Western Jarai plain stops

Figure 28

Table E4. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized H1*–A1* over the first ten sampling points after Eastern Jarai plain stops

Figure 29

Table W5. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized CPP over the first ten sampling points after Western Jarai plain stops

Figure 30

Table E5. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized CPP over the first ten sampling points after Eastern Jarai plain stops

Figure 31

Table W6. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized F1 over the first ten sampling points after Western Jarai plain stops

Figure 32

Table E6. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized F1 over the first ten sampling points after Eastern Jarai plain stops

Figure 33

Table W7. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized F2 over the first ten sampling points after Western Jarai plain stops

Figure 34

Table E7. Table of estimates for mixed model on mean normalized F2 over the first ten sampling points after Eastern Jarai plain stops

Figure 35

Table W8. Table of estimates of the final logistic regression model for /a/ stimuli in Western Jarai. Estimates represent the log odds of high register responses. VQ, F1 and F2 are centered.

Figure 36

Table E8. Table of estimates of the final logistic regression model for /a/ stimuli in Eastern Jarai. Estimates represent the log odds of high register responses. VQ, F1 and F2 are centered.

Figure 37

Table W9. Table of estimates of the final logistic regression model for /u/ stimuli in Western Jarai. Estimates represent the log odds of high register responses. VQ, F1 and F2 are centered.

Figure 38

Table E9. Table of estimates of the final logistic regression model for /u/ stimuli in Eastern Jarai. Estimates represent the log odds of high register responses. VQ, F1 and F2 are centered.