Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T00:39:06.028Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Can food parenting practices explain the association between parental education and children’s food intake? The Feel4Diabetes-study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 April 2022

Paloma Flores-Barrantes
Affiliation:
Growth, Exercise, Nutrition and Development (GENUD) Research Group, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain Instituto Agroalimentario de Aragón (IA2), Instituto De Investigación Sanitaria Aragón (IIS Aragón), Zaragoza, Spain
Christina Mavrogianni
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition and Dietetics, School of Health Sciences & Education, Harokopio University, Athens, Greece
Iris Iglesia*
Affiliation:
Growth, Exercise, Nutrition and Development (GENUD) Research Group, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain Instituto Agroalimentario de Aragón (IA2), Instituto De Investigación Sanitaria Aragón (IIS Aragón), Zaragoza, Spain Red de Salud Materno Infantil y del Desarrollo (SAMID), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, C/ Pedro Cerbuna 12, Madrid50009, Spain
Lubna Mahmood
Affiliation:
Growth, Exercise, Nutrition and Development (GENUD) Research Group, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain Instituto Agroalimentario de Aragón (IA2), Instituto De Investigación Sanitaria Aragón (IIS Aragón), Zaragoza, Spain
Ruben Willems
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health and Primary Care, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
Greet Cardon
Affiliation:
Department of Movement and Sports Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
Flore De Vylder
Affiliation:
Department of Movement and Sports Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
Stavros Liatis
Affiliation:
National and Kapodistrian University of Athens Medical School, Athens, Greece
Konstantinos Makrilakis
Affiliation:
National and Kapodistrian University of Athens Medical School, Athens, Greece
Remberto Martinez
Affiliation:
Extensive Life Oy, Helsinki, Finland
Peter Schwarz
Affiliation:
Department of Prevention and Care of Diabetes, Technical University Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Imre Rurik
Affiliation:
Department of Family and Occupational Medicine, University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary
Emese Antal
Affiliation:
Hungarian Society of Nutrition, Budapest, Hungary
Violeta Iotova
Affiliation:
Department of Paediatrics, Medical University, Varna, Bulgaria
Kaloyan Tsochev
Affiliation:
Department of Paediatrics, Medical University, Varna, Bulgaria
Nevena Chakarova
Affiliation:
Department of Endocrinology, Medical University of Sofia, Sofia, Bulgaria
Jemina Kivelä
Affiliation:
Population Health Unit, Finnish Institute for Health and Welfare, Helsinki, Finland
Katja Wikström
Affiliation:
Population Health Unit, Finnish Institute for Health and Welfare, Helsinki, Finland
Yannis Manios
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition and Dietetics, School of Health Sciences & Education, Harokopio University, Athens, Greece Institute of Agri-food and Life Sciences, Hellenic Mediterranean University Research Centre, Heraklion, Greece
Luis A Moreno
Affiliation:
Growth, Exercise, Nutrition and Development (GENUD) Research Group, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain Instituto Agroalimentario de Aragón (IA2), Instituto De Investigación Sanitaria Aragón (IIS Aragón), Zaragoza, Spain Red de Salud Materno Infantil y del Desarrollo (SAMID), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, C/ Pedro Cerbuna 12, Madrid50009, Spain CIBER Fisiopatología de la Obesidad y Nutrición, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
*
*Corresponding author: Email iglesia@unizar.es
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

This study aimed to investigate the mediating role of food parenting practices (FPP), including home availability of different types of foods and drinks, parental modelling of fruit intake, permissiveness and the use of food as a reward in the relationship between parental education and dietary intake in European children.

Design:

Single mediation analyses were conducted to explore whether FPP explain associations between parents’ educational level and children’s dietary intake measured by a parent-reported FFQ.

Setting:

Six European countries.

Participants:

Parent–child dyads (n 6705, 50·7 % girls, 88·8 % mothers) from the Feel4Diabetes-study.

Results:

Children aged 8·15 ± 0·96 years were included. Parental education was associated with children’s higher intake of water, fruits and vegetables and lower intake of sugar-rich foods and savoury snacks. All FPP explained the associations between parental education and dietary intake to a greater or lesser extent. Specifically, home availability of soft drinks explained 59·3 % of the association between parental education and sugar-rich food intake. Home availability of fruits and vegetables was the strongest mediators in the association between parental education and fruit and vegetable consumption (77·3 % and 51·5 %, respectively). Regarding savoury snacks, home availability of salty snacks and soft drinks was the strongest mediators (27·6 % and 20·8 %, respectively).

Conclusions:

FPP mediate the associations between parental education and children’s dietary intake. This study highlights the importance of addressing FPP in future interventions targeting low-educated populations.

Type
Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Childhood obesity is one of the most serious global public health problems in the twenty-first century(1), and socio-economic status (SES) is associated with this condition, since it has been inversely associated with adiposity in high-income countries and directly associated in medium to low-income countries(Reference Vazquez and Cubbin2,Reference Iguacel, Gasch-Gallén and Ayala-Marín3) . Unequal access to healthy foods is one mechanism by which SES influences the diet and health of the population, given that as income drops, energy-dense and nutrient-poor foods become an important source in affordable diets(Reference Drewnowski4). In this sense, among the obesity-related factors(Reference Littleton, Berkowitz and Grant5), dietary behaviour is one of the most relevant due to its strong impact on maintaining energy balance(Reference Howell and Kones6).

Significant differences have been found in the consumption of fruits according to SES status, for instance children from lower SES consume less fruit and vegetables (F&V) compared with high SES children, and these differences maintain and even grow over time(Reference Hilsen, van Stralen and Klepp7,Reference Joyce, Rosenkranz and Rosenkranz8) . Furthermore, evidence exists confirming that a high SES is associated with healthy eating in youth. For instance, Sandvik et al. observed that 34·0 % of children from the highest SES group reported consuming fruit every day compared with 27·6 % of children in the lower SES groups(Reference Sandvik, Gjestad and Samdal9). In contrast, low SES has been associated with higher consumption of nutrient-poor foods. Parental education has been identified as one of the best proxy indicators of SES(Reference Nixon, Moore and Douthwaite10) and has been widely used in previous studies(Reference Miguel-Berges, Zachari and Santaliestra-Pasias11,Reference Janković, Stojisavljević and Janković12) . In this sense, previous studies have found that children whose mothers had a low level of education tend to consume energy-dense food and drinks(Reference Leech, McNaughton and Timperio13) and those whose parents had higher education levels and the highest household income were more likely to be allocated to the healthy dietary pattern, characterised by the inclusion of low-fat, vitamin-rich and wholegrain foods, among others, and less likely to be allocated to a dietary pattern characterised by high sugar consumption(Reference Fernández-Alvira, Börnhorst and Bammann14).

Parental behaviours or actions performed for child-rearing purposes in the contexts of food and feeding are defined as food parenting practices (FPP)(Reference Vaughn, Ward and Fisher15). FPP influence children’s dietary intake, as well as home food environment characteristics, such as the home availability of foods or the use of food as a reward, and they vary across SES.

Regarding associations between parental education and FPP, results from a nationally representative sample from the USA also indicated that both education and SES were positively associated with home availability of foods, such as fruits, vegetables and specifically, education was negatively associated with salty snacks and sugary drinks availability at home. On the other hand, income was positively associated with dark green vegetables, low-fat milk products and salty snacks availability at home(Reference Chai, Fan and Wen16). In addition, a previous study by Campbell et al.(Reference Campbell, Crawford and Jackson17) found that families with higher education levels reported a lower frequency of family meals, whereas lower fresh F&V availability at home was reported by families with lower education levels. Moreover, the accumulation of social vulnerabilities, such as lack of social network and migrant background, has also been associated with a processed dietary pattern(Reference Iguacel, Fernández-Alvira and Bammann18).

Given that in the last years, parenting styles and specifically food parenting styles have been studied to a greater extent than FPP, it is worth mentioning that general parenting styles can be conceived as more distal, higher-order constructs, whereas parenting practices are more proximal determinants of child behaviour(Reference Power, Sleddens and Berge19). Regarding parenting styles and dietary intake in children, a previous systematic review in pre-school aged children reported that two out of three studies reported that authoritative parenting style was associated with higher intake of F&V and from the two studies that examined associations between parenting styles and unhealthy/non-core foods, no significant associations were observed(Reference Burnett, Lamb and McCann20). Also, in a 3-year-longitudinal study in Australian children, 6–9-year-old boys whose mothers reported using the authoritarian style were less likely to consume F&V. In the same study, boys and girls with authoritative and permissive fathers, and girls with authoritative mothers at 4–5 years, were more likely to consume F&V 2 and 4 years later(Reference Alsharairi and Somerset21).

The relevance of studying these practices lies in the fact that, for example when parents use emotional feeding for a prolonged period of time, children may eventually learn to calm themselves by eating(Reference Braden, Rhee and Peterson22), which may increase their future overweight risk. A recent systematic review aimed to conclude that FPP receiving the most attention within prospective studies were generally not associated with children’s weight outcomes over time(Reference Beckers, Karssen and Vink23). Nevertheless, several FPP, such as home food availability(Reference van Grieken, Renders and van de Gaar24) or parental modelling of food intake(Reference Draxten, Fulkerson and Friend25), are associated with children’s dietary intake.

In this sense, given that previous research has showed significant associations between SES indicators, such as parental education and children’s dietary intake, it is relevant to examine through mediation analyses to what extent FPP explain this relationship. In fact, FPP such as food availability and food accessibility have been previously evaluated as potential mediators of such associations(Reference Pinket, De Craemer and De Bourdeaudhuij26,Reference Gebremariam, Lien and Torheim27) . Also, a previous systematic review aiming to summarise existing evidence regarding the mediators of socio-economic differences in dietary behaviours among youth at the interpersonal level found that availability at home, accessibility at home, food rules and modelling were consistent mediators of this association(Reference Mekonnen, Havdal and Lien28). Nevertheless, some parenting practices, such as permissiveness and allowance, had not been previously assessed as potential mediators of the associations between SES and children’s dietary intake.

However, to our concern, FPP such as the use of food as a reward have not yet been evaluated as potential mediators. Such information might help construct more effective nutrition interventions aiming to alter children’s eating behaviour and promote healthy eating, particularly in vulnerable and low-SES groups. The understanding of these practices as possible mediators of the previously mentioned associations might be useful for future interventions aiming to improve dietary intake in young children through parental behaviour modifications such as avoiding certain FPP and making efforts to use those known to have a positive impact on their children’s dietary intake. Ultimately, this can result in minimising social inequalities in diet and health(Reference Ayala-Marín, Iguacel and Miguel-Etayo29). Thus, this study aimed to examine the mediating role of FPP in explaining the relationship between parental education and children’s dietary intake.

Materials and methods

Study design and setting

This cross-sectional analysis used baseline data from the ‘Families across Europe following a hEalthy Lifestyle FOR Diabetes prevention’ (Feel4Diabetes-study), a cluster-randomised study that included a school- and community-based intervention aiming to promote a healthy lifestyle and tackle obesity and obesity-related metabolic risk factors for the prevention of type 2 diabetes among families from vulnerable groups in six European countries. The recruitment of participants was performed in children of 1st, 2nd and 3rd grade (aged 6–9 years at baseline) and their parent or parents through a standardised, multistage sampling approach. More details on the recruitment strategy can be found in the study of Manios et al. (2018)(Reference Manios, Androutsos and Lambrinou30). The initial study sample included 11 396 families (12 280 children) and were recruited between January and November 2016 in schools from the participating countries. These countries represented low/middle-income countries (Bulgaria and Hungary), high-income countries (Belgium and Finland) and high-income countries under austerity measures (Greece and Spain). Details of the study protocol have been previously published (https://feel4diabetes-study.eu/)(Reference Manios, Androutsos and Lambrinou30).

Participants

Altogether, 6705 (58·84 %) parent–child dyads (50·7 % girls and 88·8 % mothers) were included from the 11 396 families assessed at baseline. Children with complete information on parental education, children’s food intake, FPP, parental self-reported weight and height and children’s weight and height were included in the present study. In order to avoid duplicate parental information, since some families included more than one child and shared the same reporting parent, we randomly selected one child per family. After this step, 800 children were removed from the main dataset; these represented siblings of participants included in the subsequent analysis. Hence, one child from each family was included and was linked to the reported parental information. A flow diagram of the inclusion of participants is presented in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1 Flow diagram of participant selection

Furthermore, to avoid the effect of possible outliers, children consuming more than seven servings per day of F&V were removed from the analyses (n 255).

Measures

One parent per child, either the mother or the father, completed a self-administered questionnaire that assessed socio-demographic characteristics, FPP and their child’s dietary intake, among other energy balance-related behaviours. Anthropometric measurements were conducted according to standardised protocols(Reference Androutsos, Anastasiou and Lambrinou31). Children received the parent questionnaire in a closed envelope to take home for completion by one of the parents.

Parental education level

Education level of both parents was reported by the parent who answered the questionnaire and was asked in a 6-point Likert-type scale question, ranging from ‘less than 6 years’ to ‘more than 16 years’ of education (< 6, 7–9, 10–12, 13–14, 15–16, and > 16). For this study, the education of the reporting parent was considered and dichotomised into ≤ 14 (low-education) and > 14 years (high-education), considering that > 14 years implies attendance of higher education (e.g. a bachelor’s program).

Dietary intake

Children’s dietary intake was reported by parents with a FFQ, using the question: ‘How often does your child usually consume the following foods and drinks?’, which they could answer by choosing one of the following options: on a weekly (less than 1, 1–2, 3–4 or 5–6 times/week) or daily basis (1–2, 3–4, 5–6 and more than 6 times/d). Beverages assessed were water, fruit juices (freshly squeezed or prepacked without sugar), soft drinks and fruit juices containing sugar and soft drinks without sugar. Foods assessed were fruits and berries (fresh or frozen), fruits and berries (canned), vegetables, sweets and salty snacks and fast food. Intra-class coefficients (ICC) of test–retest showed good reliability of reported food items (ICC = 0·633, (0·371, 0·822)) and have previously been reported in more detail(Reference Anastasiou, Fappa and Zachari32). Range categories in times per week (t/w) and times per day (t/d) of the food intake items were recoded to reflect daily intake of servings (s/d) prior to data analyses (less than 1 t/w = 0·14 s/d, 1–2 t/w = 0·21 s/d, 3–4 t/w = 0·5 s/d, 5–6 t/w = 0·79 s/d, 1–2 t/d = 1·5 s/d, 3–4 t/d = 3·5 s/d, 5–6 t/d = 5·5 s/d, and > 6 t/d = 6 s/d).

In this study, four dietary outcomes were assessed: water, F&V, sugar-rich foods and salty snacks and fast food, herein referred to as savoury snacks. Water and savoury snacks were used as single food items, as they were reported. The F&V variable was calculated by summing the total number of daily servings of fruits and berries, canned fruits, 100 % fruit juice and vegetables. The total number of sugar-rich foods was calculated by summing up servings per day of soft drinks and sugary juices and sweets.

Food parenting practices

The following FPP were included in the questionnaire:

  • Home availability of three foods considered to be healthy: fresh fruit, fresh fruit juice and vegetables and home availability of five food items considered to be energy-dense/nutrient-poor: sugary juices, soft drinks, light soft drinks, sweets and salty snacks.

  • Parental role modelling of fruit intake: parental consumption of fruit in front of their children.

  • Permissiveness: allowance of sweets and salty snacks whenever the child asks for them.

  • Use of food as a reward: defined as using sweets, salty snacks or fast food as a reward for their children.

Questions, response options and analytic coding for the analyses are shown in online supplemental Table S1. ICC showed good reliability for home availability of foods (ICC = 0·720 (0·625, 0·794)) and parental role modelling of fruit intake, permissiveness and the use of food as a reward (ICC = 0·695 (0·563, 0·793))(Reference Anastasiou, Fappa and Zachari32). Response options on a 5-point Likert scale ranged from ‘very often’ (‘always’ for home food availability) to ‘never’. These categories were reordered to denote increasing use of the practice, from ‘never’ to ‘very often’ (‘always’ for home food availability). To facilitate interpretation, home availability of nutrient-dense foods and parental modelling of fruit intake was classified as positive FPP, while home availability of unhealthy foods, permissiveness of sweets and salty snacks and using food as a reward were classified as negative FPP.

Covariates: country, age, sex and BMI

Socio-demographic variables included parents’ and children’s age and sex. Children underwent anthropometric measurements that were conducted at school by trained researchers(Reference Androutsos, Anastasiou and Lambrinou31) using standard procedures and equipment. Body weight was measured to the nearest 100 g using a portable SECA scale (SECA 213, 214, 217, and 225, Hamburg, Germany). Height was measured to the nearest 0·1 cm using a SECA stadiometer (type SECA 217). Two readings were obtained for each measurement and the mean was used for the analysis. A third measurement was conducted if the previous measurements differed > 100 g for weight and > 1 cm for height. BMI was calculated by dividing weight in kilograms by height in meters squared (as kg/m2), and BMI Z-scores (Z-BMI) were calculated for age and sex according to Cole et al. (Reference Cole, Bellizzi and Flegal33). Parental weight (kilograms) and height (meters) were self-reported, and BMI (kg/m2) was calculated.

Data analyses

Normality of the outcome variables was checked with Shapiro–Wilk tests for continuous variables. Given that all continuous variables were not normally distributed, a Mann–Whitney U test was performed for continuous variables for between-group comparisons, while a Pearson’s Chi-square test was used to compare percentages between groups according to SES (Table 1) and sex (see online supplemental Table S2).

Table 1 Study participants’ characteristics at baseline by parental education level; n 6705

SES, socio-economic status, Z-BMI, BMI Z score according to Cole et al. (2012).

n 6705, except for salty snacks, n 5765. Boldface indicates statistical significance between SES at P < 0·05.

Chi-square test was used to test differences by SES for categorical data.

Mann–Whitney U tests were performed to test differences by education in log-transformed continuous variables.

Low-education was defined as <14 years of parental education and high-education was defined as > 14 years of parental education.

Fruits and vegetables: fresh or frozen fruit and berries, canned fruit, fresh fruit juices and vegetables.

Sugar-rich foods: Sugar-sweetened beverages (sugar juices and soft drinks) and sweets.

Savoury snacks: salty snacks and fast food (e.g. one small hamburger, one small bag of chips, one slice of pizza).

As the four dietary outcome variables were not normally distributed, the data were log-transformed for the analyses (ln (x + 10). For interpretation purposes, results describing the association between SES and dietary intake of foods and beverages are also reported using the non-transformed data (normal scale).

Mediation models are used to examine the possible causal processes through which a predictor leads to an outcome(Reference von Eye, Mun and Mair34). To investigate whether FPP mediated the associations between parental education and children’s food intake, Baron & Kenny’s four-step approach for mediation analyses was used(Reference Baron and Kenny35). First, to determine whether FPP were significant mediators of the relationship between parental education and children’s dietary intake, it was necessary to show that: (i) the predictor (parental education) is associated with the mediator (FPP); and (ii) the mediator (FPP) is associated with the outcome (children’s dietary intake) while simultaneously controlling for the tested predictor (parental education). These facts explain the reason for running adjusted linear regressions to examine the associations between parental education and the potential mediators (FPP) and between children’s dietary intake and the FPP (Table 2). Then, based on the hypothesis that FPP could mediate the associations between parental education and dietary intake, the mediation models were examined using the PROCESS macro 3.1.4 software for SPSS by Andrew Hayes(Reference Hayes and Rockwood36). In PROCESS, Model 4 software was applied for simple mediations

Table 2 Associations between parental education, food parenting practices and dietary intake in children, n 6705

β , Standardised coefficients; HA, home availability.

* n 6705, except for salty snacks and fast food (savoury snacks), n 5765.

Individual linear regressions were performed using the log-transformed scales of outcome variables and were adjusted for country and parental and children’s sex, age and BMI.

Parental education was also included as covariate to test the associations between FPP and dietary intake.

Boldface indicates statistical significance.

Mediation models were performed individually to examine the mediating role of each FPP on the association between parental education and the four outcomes. As shown in Fig. 2, the independent variable was parental education, dependent variables were children’s food intake and the potential mediator variables were the positive (e.g. home availability of fruit) and negative (e.g. permissiveness) FPP. Included covariates were country; parental age, sex and BMI; and children’s age, sex and BMI. In the model, the associations between parental education and FPP (a’ – coefficient), FPP and dietary outcomes (b’ – coefficient) and parental education and dietary outcomes (c’ – coefficient) are illustrated. The indirect effects (A * B) data, which is obtained by multiplying a’ x b’ coefficient, are generated with 95 % CI, representing P-values < 0·05 rather than generating exact P-values. Significant mediations were then calculated as percentages by dividing A * B by the total effect (c – coefficient).

Fig. 2 Graphical illustration of the possible interactions between parent’s educational level, FPP and children’s food intake. Simple mediation analyses adjusted by country, parental age, BMI and sex, and children’s age, BMI and sex. Pathway A’: Association between parent’s educational level and FPP. Pathway B’: Association between FPP and children’s food intake. Pathway C’: Direct association between parent’s educational level and children’s food intake after adjustment for each mediator (FPP). Pathway C: The total effect (C) shows the association between parent’s educational level and dietary intake. A * B: Indirect effect of each FPP on the association between parent’s educational level and dietary intake. Abbreviations: FPP, food parenting practices

Descriptive and mediation analyses were completed using IBM SPSS Statistics (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 26.0. IBM Corp.).

Results

Participant characteristics

In total, 6705 child–parent dyads from the six participating countries from the Feel4Diabetes-study were included in this study. Table 1 shows the characteristics of the children (50·7 % girls) and the parents (88·8 % mothers) by parental education group. The mean age of the children was 8·15 ± 0·96 years. Parents’ mean age was 38·54 ± 5·09 years. Of all participating parents, 75·8 % stated that they had completed 14 years of education or more. Regarding dietary intake, children whose parents had a low education level had higher consumption of water, sugar-rich foods and savoury snacks than children from parents with high education level. Conversely, the consumption of F&V was significantly higher in the high-education level group. The description of participants’ characteristics by sex is shown in online Supplemental Table S2.

Associations between SES, children’s dietary intake and FPP

First, the associations between parental education, FPP and children’s food intake adjusting for covariates were assessed (Table 2). Parental education was associated with higher water and F&V intake and with lower sugar-rich food and savoury snack consumption.

Parental education was associated with nine of the eleven FPP. No associations were observed between parental education and home availability of light soft drinks or sweets, and these FPP were therefore not considered for subsequent analyses.

Out of a total of fourty-four associations between FPP and children’s food intake, fourty-one were significant. Regarding FPP and children’s food intake, several direct significant associations were observed between positive FPP, like modelling of fruit intake, and water and F&V intake. Conversely, several inverse significant associations were observed between positive FPP and energy-dense/nutrient-poor foods. Nevertheless, no associations were found between the FPP of home availability of fresh fruit juice and water or savoury snack intake or the FPP of home availability of light soft drinks and F&V.

Mediating effect of FPP on the associations between SES and children’s dietary intake

The potential mediating effect of positive and negative FPP in the association between parental education and children’s dietary intake of water, F&V, sugar-rich foods and savoury snacks was evaluated while adjusting for covariates. To facilitate interpretation of the models, a graphical illustration of the mediation pathways between parental education, FPP and dietary intake is shown in Fig. 2 and another to illustrate the proportions mediated by each FPP in Fig. 3.

Fig. 3 Mediating effect of FPP on the association between parental education and dietary intake of water, fruits & vegetables, sugar-rich foods, and savoury snacks. HA, home availability

Home availability of fruits and soft drinks appeared to be the most important mediator explaining the association between parental education and children’s water intake (Table 3). It is worth pointing out that the other FPP assessed were also found to be significant mediators but to a lesser extent.

Table 3 Total associations (c)* direct associations (c’) and indirect effects between parental education and water intake adjusted for significant mediators

FPP, food parenting practices; HA, home availability.

n 6705.

Single mediation analyses were adjusted for country and parental and children’s sex, age, and BMI.

* Total effect (C pathway) of the association between parental education and water intake: β log = 0·066 (0·028, 0 104); β normal = 0·064 (-0·030, 0 158).

Results on the log-scale were used for determining statistical significance.

Results on the normal scale (servings/d) are used for interpretation purposes only.

For the association between parental education and children’s F&V intake (Table 4), home availability of fruits, vegetables and soft drinks and parental modelling of fruit intake were found to be significant mediators, explaining 77·3 %, 51·5 %, 21·8 % and 19·7 % of this association, respectively.

Table 4 Total associations (c)*, direct associations (c’) and indirect effects between parental education and fruit and vegetable intake adjusted for significant mediators

FPP, food parenting practices; HA, home availability.

n 6705.

Single mediation analyses were adjusted for country and parental and children’s sex, age and BMI.

* Total effect (C pathway) of the association between parental education and F&V intake: β log = 0·066, 95 % CI (0·035, 0·097); β normal = 0·107 (0·025, 0 189).

Results on the log-scale were used for determining statistical significance.

Results on the normal scale (servings/d) are used for interpretation purposes only.

Regarding the association between parental education and children’s sugar-dense food intake (Table 5), all FPP assessed were found to be significant mediators. However, the most significant mediators were found to be negative FPP, particularly home availability of soft drinks and permissiveness, for which proportions mediated were 59·3 and 45·1 %, respectively.

Table 5 Total associations (c)*, direct associations (c’) and indirect effects between parental education and sugar-rich foods intake adjusted for significant mediators

FPP, food parenting practices; HA, home availability.

n 6705.

Single mediation analyses were adjusted for country and parental and children’s sex, age and BMI.

Sugar-rich foods included the sum of sweets (e.g. one chocolate bar or half a cup of sweets, cookies or icecream) and soft drinks (e.g. one glass of one cup of soft drinks and juices containing sugar).

* Total effect (C pathway) of the association between parental education and sugar-rich foods intake: β log= −0·091, 95 % CI (−0·132, -0·050); β normal = −0·249 (−0·316, −0·183).

Results on the log-scale were used for determining statistical significance.

Results on the normal scale (servings/d) are used for interpretation purposes only.

The association between parental education and savoury snack intake (Table 6) was explained by home availability of salty snacks (27·6 %) and home availability of soft drinks (20·8 %) as well as the other FPP assessed, which also proved to be significant mediators, but to a lesser extent.

Table 6 Total associations (c)*, direct associations (c’) and indirect effects between parental education and savoury snacks adjusted for significant mediators

FPP, food parenting practices; HA, home availability.

n 5765.

Single mediation analyses were adjusted for country and parental and children’s sex, age and BMI.

Savoury snacks include salty snacks and fast-food items like one small hamburger, one bag of chips or one slice of pizza.

* Total effect (C pathway) of the association between SES and savoury snacks intake: β log= −0·192, 95 % CI (−0·232, −0·152); β normal = −0·117 (−0·146, −0·089).

Results on the log-scale were used for determining statistical significance.

£ Results on the normal scale (servings/d) are used for interpretation purposes only.

Discussion

The current study shows that inequalities in food intake according to parental education were partly mediated by the addressed FPP in European children from the Feel4Diabetes-study. In fact, almost all of them appeared to explain the associations to a greater or lesser extent. Given that parental education is difficult to modify in the short term, FPP appear to be interesting factors for potential modification.

Intake of assessed food items was significantly affected by parental education for all items except water (see online supplemental Table S3), being directly associated with water, F&V and inversely associated with sugar-rich foods and savoury snacks; nevertheless, water intake was used in later analyses because of the observed associations in the adjusted regression models. An explanation for this may be that the assessment method for water intake was not ideal, since repeated 24-h recalls or specific tools are preferable to FFQ for water intake(Reference Gandy37). Parents may have found it difficult to estimate an average daily consumption since water intake is usually distributed throughout the day and might be difficult to quantify properly. Also, water intake might not strictly depend on parental education, since all the families from our study have drinking water access, and water was therefore available in every household.

No associations were observed between parental education and home availability of light soft drinks or sweets; therefore, these FPP were not considered for subsequent mediation analyses. Nevertheless, results from the adjusted linear regressions showed that home availability of light soft drinks was inversely associated with water intake, indicating that the presence of these beverages might reduce the amount of water intake. These associations are in line with the findings of Galastri et al.(Reference Galastri Baraldi, Martinez Steele and Louzada38), which aimed to assess the association between ultra-processed food consumption and total water intake in a national representative sample from the USA, indicated that the consumption of artificially sweetened beverages was associated with a reduction in water consumption. On the other hand, home availability of light soft drinks was directly associated with consumption of sugar-rich foods and savoury snacks, indicating that even though light soft drinks might not be a substantial source of calories, their availability at home may have an association with a pattern of high consumption of energy-dense foods such as savoury snacks.

In our study, permissiveness and home availability of both sugary juices and soft drinks were inversely associated with water intake. In a previous study in 6- to 8-year-old European children that aimed to evaluate the associations between parenting practices towards fruit juices and soft drinks and water consumption of children, children’s water intake was found to be favourably influenced by less parental allowance, low home availability and high parental self-efficacy in managing intake(Reference Mantziki, Renders and Seidell39). On this basis, we assessed the mediation effect of FPP on the association between parental education and water intake and found that home availability of soft drinks was the strongest mediator. This finding indicates that the association between parental education and water consumption is significantly explained by the presence of soft drinks at home, which may replace water intake in children. Interestingly, fresh fruit juice was not a significant mediator, indicating that the consumption of water is not affected or replaced by that of fresh fruit juice. As shown in our study, the physical presence of sugar-sweetened beverages at home is inversely associated with water intake; however, as shown in a randomised controlled trial aiming to decrease sugar-sweetened beverages intake in Dutch adolescents(Reference Veitch, Singh and van Stralen40), the decrease in sugar-sweetened beverages consumption over time does not necessarily lead to an increase in water consumption. This indicates that water intake promotion may be necessary to achieve the corresponding recommendations.

In a previous study in 11-year-old Dutch children, that evaluated the potential mediating effect of home environment characteristics in the association between maternal educational level and children’s healthy eating behaviour, results indicated that home availability of fruit significantly mediated this association. In fact, home availability appeared to be a significant mediator when evaluated separately and in combination with other factors, such as parental fruit intake and fruit consumption rules(Reference van Ansem, Schrijvers and Rodenburg41). It is worth mentioning that parental intake of fruit refers to their diet and differs from parental modelling of fruit intake, which is often used to refer to more intentional efforts made by parents to actively demonstrate healthy eating for the child(Reference Vaughn, Ward and Fisher15).

In our study, important significant mediators on the association between parental education and sugar-rich food intake were primarily those considered as negative FPP, specifically, home availability of soft drinks and permissiveness. As observed by Robert et al.(Reference Roberts, Marx and Musher-Eizenman42), parents with the highest mean scores for permissive parenting frequently used rewards, defined as the use of both tangible items and food to reward children for eating and behaviours. It was also found in the current study that the use of food as a reward was significantly correlated with permissiveness (data not shown), indicating that these practices may be used in combination. Moreover, the link between parental education and the use of food as a reward could be explained by the fact that food may be recognised as an easy and affordable reward that will be accepted by the child.

A previous study in 3- to 11-year-old Australian children found that covert control feeding strategies, defined as the way in which parents promote the consumption of healthy food by managing the child’s environment by providing primarily healthy foods, were significantly associated with lower unhealthy snack intake but not with healthy snack food intake by children(Reference Boots, Tiggemann and Corsini43), indicating a positive effect of healthy home food availability in terms of lower unhealthy snack intake over time.

Other FPP, such as food accessibility, have been explored for their potential role as mediators in the association between SES and dietary intake. For instance, a previous study in Norwegian adolescents showed that food accessibility and perceived rules were significant mediators of the associations between parental education and soft drink consumption(Reference Gebremariam, Lien and Torheim27), indicating that other FPP besides the ones we explored may also play an important role.

An option for replacing the use of food as a reward and its negative effects on diet, and consequently on health, would be the use of social rewards, which are inexpensive or free and can be even more powerful than material rewards. Examples of social rewards include those characterised by affection, such as hugs and smiles, and those including attention and activities, such as playing the child’s favourite game together, reading a story or encouraging them to help with home tasks like preparing dinner(44). Permissiveness regarding food intake might be accompanied by permissiveness in other aspects of the child’s activities; in this sense, it could be interesting to evaluate if permissiveness is also applied to other aspects of life, such as physical activity and sedentary behaviours like the use of screens.

Humans learn by imitation and reference(Reference Rizzolatti and Craighero45); therefore, it is also possible that parents, regardless of their educational level, were raised surrounded by the same FPP they use. This means that they might have inherited lifestyle habits and family rules and they use these with their own children. At this point, it is important to break the cycle, so they recognise their behaviours and can make efforts to improve.

A major strength of our study is the large and pan-European sample and the standardisation of measurements, which was followed across all centres. Also, to our knowledge, this is the first study examining the mediating role of permissiveness and the use of food as a reward explaining differences in European children’s dietary intake by SES. However, our study has several limitations. Firstly, a FFQ was used to assess regular dietary intake, which may have introduced self-report bias, but this weakness is very hard to overcome when studying food intake(Reference Livingstone, Robson and Wallace46). In addition, parents reporting both food intake and FPP might overestimate the association between the variables. However, as some of the children were only first graders, it was not possible to get self-reported food intake data from the children. In this study, education was used as the main determinant of dietary intake; nevertheless, it could be important to include other SES variables besides education, such as occupation(Reference Costa de Oliveira Forkert, de Moraes and Carvalho47), employment status or income or even composite indices(Reference Shavers48), considering that each of them should be chosen according to their strengths, limitations and depending on the sample‘s characteristics. We selected the education of the reporting parent as the exposure variable since we considered that it was relevant to consider how education reflects on parents’ behaviour and whether it determines if they are more permissive or tend to use food as a reward with their children. Nevertheless, for future studies, it might be important to consider the educational level and FPP of both parents and, for example, to evaluate whether they are consistent with each other or contrary in some respects.

A recent study that aimed to evaluate the associations between parents’ work status and the dietary consumption patterns of Australian pre-school children(Reference Alsharairi and Somerset49) found that depending on the work status and educational level attained by mothers or fathers, children presented significant differences in terms of F&V, high-fat foods and high-sugar foods consumption. This indicates that not only education but also work status has an important role in determining the dietary intake of children at this age.

Even though there was an initial sample loss of 41·2 %, the proportion of low-educated and high-educated parents was very similar in the included sample and in the excluded sample, which indicates that no selection bias might have occur.

Parents should be aware that there are modifiable practices that they can use in the home food environment, such as home availability, and they can try to enhance these to improve their children’s diet. There is a need for family-focused research that identifies social aspects of the home environment that potentially impact on dietary intake of children(Reference Dollman, Ridley and Magarey50). It remains important to encourage parents to understand the importance of avoiding negative FPP.

In conclusion, this study highlighted the role of FPP in explaining the associations between parental education and children’s intake of water, fruits and vegetables, sugar-rich foods and savoury snacks. Encouraging parents, especially those with a low level of education, to increase the use of positive FPP, such as modelling of fruit intake, and to avoid the use of negative FPP, such as home availability of soft drinks, might help to tackle health and dietary inequalities by improving children’s intake of these food groups. Health professionals should understand not only the challenges but also the opportunities and possibilities that parents can have if they improve the FPP they use. These findings may broaden the understanding of potential pathways by which various factors might influence children’s dietary intake, helping researchers to better design nutrition-focused interventions.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors thank the participating families from the Feel4diabetes-study. Financial support: The Feel4Diabetes-study was funded by the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under grant agreement n° 643708. The funding body did not have any role in the design of the study and collection, analysis and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript. The content of this article reflects only the authors’ views and the European Community is not liable for any use that may be made of the information contained therein. PF-B received financial support by a grant from the Aragón’s Regional Government (Diputación General de Aragón, DGA). Authorship: P.F.B. conducted the statistical analyses, wrote the first draft of the manuscript and made the greatest contribution to the paper. C.M. also contributed to the statistical analysis and write-up of the first draft. All the authors have critically revised the manuscript and read and approved the final version. Ethics of human subject participation: The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and ethical approval was obtained by the Ethical Committees of all participating European centres. In Belgium, the study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the Ghent University Hospital (ethical approval code: B670201524437); in Bulgaria, by the Ethics Committee of the Medical University of Varna (ethical approval code: 52/10-3-2016) and the Municipalities of Sofia and Varna, as well as the Ministry of Education and Science’s local representatives; in Finland, by the hospital district of Southwest Finland’s ethical committee (ethical approval code: 174/1801/2015); in Greece, by the Bioethics Committee of Harokopio University (ethical approval code: 46/3-4-2015) and the Greek Ministry of Education; in Hungary, by the National Committee for Scientific Research in Medicine (ethical approval code: 20095/2016/EKU) and in Spain, by the Clinical Research Ethics Committee and the Department of Consumers’ Health of the Government of Aragón (ethical approval code: CP03/2016).

Conflicts of interest:

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980022000891

References

World Health Organization (2016) Report of the Commission on Ending Childhood Obesity. http://www.who.int/end-childhood-obesity/en/ (accessed March 2021).Google Scholar
Vazquez, C & Cubbin, C (2020) Socioeconomic status and childhood obesity: a review of literature from the past decade to inform intervention research. Curr Obes Rep 9, 562570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iguacel, I, Gasch-Gallén, Á, Ayala-Marín, A et al. (2005) Social vulnerabilities as risk factor of childhood obesity development and their role in prevention programs. Int J Obes 45, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drewnowski, A (2009) Obesity, diets, and social inequalities. Nutr Rev 67, Suppl. 1, S36S39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Littleton, S, Berkowitz, R & Grant, S (2020) Genetic determinants of childhood obesity. Mol Diagn Ther 24, 653663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howell, S & Kones, R (2017) “Calories in, calories out” and macronutrient intake: the hope, hype, and science of calories. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 313, E608E612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilsen, M, van Stralen, MM, Klepp, KI et al. (2011) Changes in 10–12 year old’s fruit and vegetable intake in Norway from 2001 to 2008 in relation to gender and socioeconomic status – a comparison of two cross-sectional groups. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 8, 108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joyce, J, Rosenkranz, R & Rosenkranz, S (2020) Evaluation of variability in dietary quality of school lunches meeting national school lunch program guidelines by socioeconomic status and rurality. Int J Environ Res Public Health 17, 8012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandvik, C, Gjestad, R, Samdal, O et al. (2010) Does socio-economic status moderate the associations between psychosocial predictors and fruit intake in schoolchildren? The Pro Children study. Health Educ Res 25, 121134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nixon, CA, Moore, HJ, Douthwaite, W et al. (2012) Identifying effective behavioural models and behaviour change strategies underpinning preschool- and school-based obesity prevention interventions aimed at 4–6-year-olds: a systematic review. Obes Rev 13, Suppl. 1, 106117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miguel-Berges, M, Zachari, K, Santaliestra-Pasias, A et al. (2017) Clustering of energy balance-related behaviours and parental education in European preschool children: the ToyBox study. Br J Nutr 118, 10891096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janković, S, Stojisavljević, D, Janković, J et al. (2014) Association of socioeconomic status measured by education, and cardiovascular health: a population-based cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 4, e005222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leech, RM, McNaughton, SA & Timperio, A (2014) Clustering of children’s obesity-related behaviours: associations with sociodemographic indicators. Eur J Clin Nutr 68, 623628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández-Alvira, J, Börnhorst, C, Bammann, K et al. (2015) Prospective associations between socio-economic status and dietary patterns in European children: the identification and prevention of dietary- and lifestyle-induced health effects in children and infants (IDEFICS) study. Br J Nutr 113, 517525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaughn, AE, Ward, DS, Fisher, JO et al. (2016) Fundamental constructs in food parenting practices: a content map to guide future research. Nutr Rev 74, 98117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chai, W, Fan, J & Wen, M (2018) Association of individual and neighborhood factors with home food availability: evidence from the national health and nutrition examination survey. J Acad Nutr Diet 118, 815823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, K, Crawford, D, Jackson, M et al. (2002) Family food environments of 5–6-year-old-children: does socioeconomic status make a difference? Asia Pac J Clin Nutr 11, Suppl. 3, S553S561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iguacel, I, Fernández-Alvira, J, Bammann, K et al. (2016) Associations between social vulnerabilities and dietary patterns in European children: the identification and prevention of dietary- and lifestyle-induced health EFfects In Children and infantS (IDEFICS) study. Br J Nutr 116, 12881297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, T, Sleddens, E, Berge, J et al. (2013) Contemporary research on parenting: conceptual, methodological, and translational issues. Child Obes 9, Suppl. 1, S87S94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burnett, A, Lamb, K, McCann, J et al. (2020) Parenting styles and the dietary intake of pre-school children: a systematic review. Psychol Health 35, 13261345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alsharairi, NA & Somerset, S (2015) Associations between Parenting Styles and Children’s Fruit and Vegetable Intake. https://doiorg/101080/036702442014953248 (accessed December 2021).Google Scholar
Braden, A, Rhee, K, Peterson, C et al. (2014) Associations between child emotional eating and general parenting style, feeding practices, and parent psychopathology. Appetite 80, 3540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beckers, D, Karssen, L, Vink, J et al. (2021) Food parenting practices and children’s weight outcomes: a systematic review of prospective studies. Appetite 158, 105010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Grieken, A, Renders, CM, van de Gaar, VM et al. (2015) Associations between the home environment and children’s sweet beverage consumption at 2-year follow-up: the 'Be active, eat right’ study. Pediatr Obes 10, 126133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Draxten, M, Fulkerson, JA, Friend, S et al. (2014) Parental role modeling of fruits and vegetables at meals and snacks is associated with children’s adequate consumption. Appetite 78, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinket, AS, De Craemer, M, De Bourdeaudhuij, I et al. (2016) Can parenting practices explain the differences in beverage intake according to socio-economic status: the Toybox-study. Nutrients 8, 591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gebremariam, MK, Lien, N, Torheim, LE et al. (2016) Perceived rules and accessibility: measurement and mediating role in the association between parental education and vegetable and soft drink intake. Nutr J 15, 76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mekonnen, T, Havdal, H, Lien, N et al. (2020) Mediators of socioeconomic inequalities in dietary behaviours among youth: a systematic review. Obes Rev 21, e13016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayala-Marín, A, Iguacel, I, Miguel-Etayo, P et al. (2020) Consideration of social disadvantages for understanding and preventing obesity in children. Front Public Health 8, 423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manios, Y, Androutsos, O, Lambrinou, CP et al. (2018) A school- and community-based intervention to promote healthy lifestyle and prevent type 2 diabetes in vulnerable families across Europe: design and implementation of the Feel4Diabetes-study. Public Health Nutr 21, 32813290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Androutsos, O, Anastasiou, C, Lambrinou, C, et al. (2020) Intra- and inter- observer reliability of anthropometric measurements and blood pressure in primary schoolchildren and adults: the Feel4Diabetes-study. BMC Endocr Disord 20, Suppl. 1, 27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anastasiou, CA, Fappa, E, Zachari, K et al. (2020) Development and reliability of questionnaires for the assessment of diet and physical activity behaviors in a multi-country sample in Europe the Feel4Diabetes study. BMC Endocr Disord 20, Suppl. 1, 135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, T, Bellizzi, M, Flegal, K et al. (2000) Establishing a standard definition for child overweight and obesity worldwide: international survey. BMJ 320, 12401243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Eye, A, Mun, E & Mair, P (2009) What carries a mediation process? Configural analysis of mediation. Integr Psychol Behav Sci 43, 228247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, RM & Kenny, DA (1986) The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51, 11731182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, AR & Rockwood, NJ (2017) Regression-based statistical mediation and moderation analysis in clinical research: observations, recommendations, and implementation. Behav Res Ther 98, 3957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gandy, J (2015) Water intake: validity of population assessment and recommendations. Eur J Nutr 54, Suppl. 2, 1116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galastri Baraldi, L, Martinez Steele, E, Louzada, M et al. (2021) Associations between ultraprocessed food consumption and total water intake in the US population. J Acad Nutr Diet 121, 16951703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mantziki, K, Renders, CM & Seidell, JC (2017) Water consumption in European children: associations with intake of fruit juices, soft drinks and related parenting practices. Int J Environ Res Public Health 14, 583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veitch, J, Singh, A, van Stralen, M et al. (2011) Reduction in sugar-sweetened beverages is not associated with more water or diet drinks. Public Health Nutr 14, 13881393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Ansem, WJC, Schrijvers, CTM, Rodenburg, G et al. (2014) Maternal educational level and children’s healthy eating behaviour: role of the home food environment (cross-sectional results from the INPACT study). Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 11, 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, L, Marx, JM & Musher-Eizenman, DR (2018) Using food as a reward: an examination of parental reward practices. Appetite 120, 318326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boots, SB, Tiggemann, M & Corsini, N (2018) “That’s enough now!” A prospective study of the effects of maternal control on children’s snack intake. Appetite 126, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2019) Essentials for Parenting Toddlers and Preschoolers; How to Use Rewards. https://www.cdc.gov/parents/essentials/consequences/rewards.html (accessed: March 2021).Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G & Craighero, L (2004) The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci 27, 169192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingstone, M, Robson, P & Wallace, J (2004) Issues in dietary intake assessment of children and adolescents. Br J Nutr 92, Suppl. 2, S213S222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa de Oliveira Forkert, E, de Moraes, A, Carvalho, H et al. (2017) Abdominal obesity and its association with socioeconomic factors among adolescents from different living environments. Pediatr Obes 12, 110119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shavers, V (2007) Measurement of socioeconomic status in health disparities research. J Natl Med Assoc 99, 10131023.Google ScholarPubMed
Alsharairi, N & Somerset, S (2018) Parental work status and children’s dietary consumption: Australian evidence. Int J Consum Stud 42, 522532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollman, J, Ridley, K, Magarey, A et al. (2007) Dietary intake, physical activity and TV viewing as mediators of the association of socioeconomic status with body composition: a cross-sectional analysis of Australian youth. Int J Obes 31, 4552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Flow diagram of participant selection

Figure 1

Table 1 Study participants’ characteristics at baseline by parental education level; n 6705

Figure 2

Table 2 Associations between parental education, food parenting practices and dietary intake in children, n 6705

Figure 3

Fig. 2 Graphical illustration of the possible interactions between parent’s educational level, FPP and children’s food intake. Simple mediation analyses adjusted by country, parental age, BMI and sex, and children’s age, BMI and sex. Pathway A’: Association between parent’s educational level and FPP. Pathway B’: Association between FPP and children’s food intake. Pathway C’: Direct association between parent’s educational level and children’s food intake after adjustment for each mediator (FPP). Pathway C: The total effect (C) shows the association between parent’s educational level and dietary intake. A * B: Indirect effect of each FPP on the association between parent’s educational level and dietary intake. Abbreviations: FPP, food parenting practices

Figure 4

Fig. 3 Mediating effect of FPP on the association between parental education and dietary intake of water, fruits & vegetables, sugar-rich foods, and savoury snacks. HA, home availability

Figure 5

Table 3 Total associations (c)* direct associations (c’) and indirect effects between parental education and water intake adjusted for significant mediators

Figure 6

Table 4 Total associations (c)*, direct associations (c’) and indirect effects between parental education and fruit and vegetable intake adjusted for significant mediators

Figure 7

Table 5 Total associations (c)*, direct associations (c’) and indirect effects between parental education and sugar-rich foods intake adjusted for significant mediators

Figure 8

Table 6 Total associations (c)*, direct associations (c’) and indirect effects between parental education and savoury snacks adjusted for significant mediators

Supplementary material: File

Flores-Barrantes et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S3

Download Flores-Barrantes et al. supplementary material(File)
File 39.9 KB