Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T07:31:46.867Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Animal sheltering: A scoping literature review grounded in institutional ethnography

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 January 2023

Katherine E Koralesky*
Affiliation:
Animal Welfare Program, University of British Columbia, 2357 Main Mall, Vancouver, BC V6T1Z4, Canada
Janet M Rankin
Affiliation:
Faculty of Nursing, University of Calgary, 2500 University Drive NW Calgary, AB T2N1N4, Canada
David Fraser
Affiliation:
Animal Welfare Program, University of British Columbia, 2357 Main Mall, Vancouver, BC V6T1Z4, Canada
*
Author for correspondence: Katherine E Koralesky, Email: katie.koralesky@ubc.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

A diverse research literature now exists on the animals, staff and organisations involved in animal sheltering. We reviewed this research through the lens of institutional ethnography, a method of inquiry that focuses on the actual work that people do within institutions. The main topics, identified through a larger ethnographic study of animal sheltering, were: (i) research about shelter staff and officers; (ii) the relinquishment of animals to shelters; and (iii) animals’ length of stay in shelters. After reviewing the literature, we held focus groups with shelter personnel to explore how their work experiences are or are not represented in the research. The review showed that stress caused by performing euthanasia has attracted much research, but the decision-making that leads to euthanasia, which may involve multiple staff and potential conflict, has received little attention. Research on ‘compassion fatigue’ has also tended to focus on euthanasia but a granular description about the practical and emotional work that personnel undertake that generates such fatigue is missing. Published research on both relinquishment and length of stay is dominated by metrics (questionnaires) and often relies upon shelter records, despite their limitations. Less research has examined the actual work processes involved in managing relinquishment as well as monitoring and reducing animals’ length of stay. Institutional ethnography’s focus on people’s work activities can provide a different and more nuanced understanding of what is happening in animal sheltering and how it might better serve the needs of the animals and staff.

Type
Scoping Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

For many decades the sheltering and protection of companion animals have been a primary focus of the animal welfare movement. These topics have also stimulated a large body of research that investigates the animals (e.g. Arhant et al. Reference Arhant, Wogritsch and Troxler2015; Protopopova Reference Protopopova2016), the people (Arluke Reference Arluke1991; Baran et al. Reference Baran, Allen, Rogelberg, Spitzmüller, DiGiacomo, Webb, Carter, Clark, Teeter and Walker2009; Schabram & Maitlis Reference Schabram and Maitlis2017) and the organisations involved (Irvine Reference Irvine2003; Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Patronek, Slater, Garrison and Medicus2013). The extant research, however, has tended to concentrate on certain aspects of animal sheltering and protection such as relinquishment of animals by owners and the stress experienced by shelter staff who perform euthanasia. For the most part, the literature does not provide an integrated understanding of how the policies, processes and functioning of the institutions — which govern the everyday work of the staff — determine what happens to animals.

Institutional ethnography (IE) (Smith Reference Smith1987, Reference Smith1990, Reference Smith1999, Reference Smith2005, Reference Smith2006) is an approach to inquiry that aims to discover how everyday life and work are shaped and organised within ‘institutions’ which sociologist Dorothy Smith (Reference Smith2005; p 68) defined as “complexes organised around a distinctive function”, with healthcare and education as classic examples. In developing IE, Smith proposed that instead of beginning sociological inquiry by applying existing methods and testing existing theories, inquiry should begin in the actual, concrete experiences of what people actually do. This is what Smith means by discovery. Smith thus called for “a sociology for, not of, people”, and an “ontological shift” (Smith Reference Smith2005; p 2) toward building knowledge by focusing on the actual, everyday work of people, and explicitly avoiding theorising about what has been observed. IE’s goal is to materially ‘map’ how experiences are organised, especially by institutionalised policies and practices, to happen as they do.

In parallel with its distinctive approach to research, IE researchers use a distinctive approach to reviewing the research literature as a form of discourse analysis, aligned with Michel Foucault’s (Reference Foucault and Young1981) interest in language as a tool of social regulation. Smith (Reference Smith1987; p 72–73) identified how the sociological discourse regulates and organises how sociologists conduct research including the language they use and what they try to understand. Within any academic discourse, certain topics, concepts and terms become a focus of attention. Researchers then develop and perpetuate ways of thinking about and interpreting those topics, positioning themselves as observers of a topic, rather than looking at what people are doing.

In this review, we used an IE approach to discourse analysis which identified links between the literature and people’s practice. Smith (Reference Smith1987; p 224) noted that academic discourses not only influence researchers but also are taken up by people doing everyday work practices. Thus, in keeping with IE’s interest in discourse and people’s practice, we also included focus group data from people directly involved in animal sheltering. In the focus groups we presented findings of the review and listened to how well or poorly the research literature captures and reflects the everyday problems people encounter. Thus, in this review, we aimed: (i) to identify and analyse how the academic discourse created by researchers has been shaped; and (ii) to begin to analyse how the knowledge being generated by scholars organises, represents, or glosses over the actual work people are doing in animal welfare organisations.

Materials and methods

The topics for this review were identified when the primary author (KEK) was collecting data for an IE project in co-operation with the British Columbia Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals (BC SPCA) investigating what happens to companion animals in British Columbia, Canada, when they become involved with the institution of animal sheltering. The literature review covered the breadth of what has been published about shelter staff and officers, together with two other topics — the relinquishment of animals by owners, and the length of time before an animal is adopted. These topics emerged in the workers’ talk and activity as they were observed doing their work and are well-established topics for research.

We structured the review using the theoretical framework of IE with its interest in the social organisation of knowledge, combined with a framework for conducting scoping reviews (Arksey & O’Malley Reference Arksey and O’Malley2005). We also took guidance on how to modify a scoping review for IE from Dalmer (Reference Dalmer2018, Reference Dalmer2020). We searched for peer-reviewed articles in three databases (Ovid Medline, APA PsycInfo and Web of Science) using keywords, subject headings (i.e. phrases used to index articles by concept) and an asterisk to truncate words or phrases. For the first topic (research about staff and officers) the search terms were (people or staff* or employee* or veterinarian* or officer* or volunteer* or worker* OR subject headings ‘Employee Attitudes’ or ‘Employee Characteristics’ or ‘Employee Retention’ or ‘Employee Motivation’ or ‘Veterinary Medicine’) AND (animal shelter* or animal rescu* OR subject headings ‘Housing, Animal’ or ‘Animal Shelters’). For the second topic (animal relinquishment) the search terms were (surrender* or relinquish*) AND (animal shelter* or animal rescu* OR subject headings ‘Housing, Animal’ or ‘Animal Shelters’) AND (pet or pets or ‘companion animal*’ or cat or cats or dog or dogs or rat or rats or rabbit* or bird* or mice or mouse or gerbil* or hamster* OR subject headings ‘Pets’ or ‘animals, domestic or pets’). For the third topic (length of stay in shelters) the search terms were (length of stay OR subject headings ‘Length of Stay’ or ‘Treatment Duration’) AND (animal shelter* or animal rescu* OR subject headings ‘Housing, Animal’ or ‘Animal Shelters’). We did not set date limits, performed searches on November 3, 2020 and set notification alerts for each topic. We also identified references that our searches missed but were cited in other articles and we selected studies for review based on their relevance to the topics.

On the first topic, focused on shelter staff and officers, we excluded articles mostly or solely involving unpaid personnel (e.g. volunteers). We did not include articles about shelter medicine or dog behavioural assessments, as these articles did not focus on the everyday work of people engaged in practicing shelter medicine or conducting assessments. On the topic of relinquishment, we excluded articles on failed adoptions (animals that were returned soon after they were adopted). The final topic ‘length of stay’ included articles that measured ‘live-release rate’ as well as length of stay.

According to Arksey and O’Malley (Reference Arksey and O’Malley2005) the aim of a scoping review is to identify prominent themes, patterns and findings on topics. However, an IE-based approach also explores how published research generates an academic discourse whereby new knowledge is explicitly built upon prior research, often using ideas, concepts, theories and methods proposed by earlier researchers. Therefore, in reviewing the literature, we also focused on the foundational ideas, concepts and theories that guide how researchers have approached their investigations, and the methods that have become the accepted ways of doing the research. In this we were following Dalmer (Reference Dalmer2018), specifically by identifying the established methods that guide the approaches used by researchers (Rankin Reference Rankin2017).

As suggested by Arksey and O’Malley (Reference Arksey and O’Malley2005) we implemented a consultation exercise with BC SPCA personnel to gather insights into the problems they face that are not captured in the research literature. We did this through four virtual focus groups in January and February of 2021 (Zoom Video Communications Inc, San Jose, CA, USA 2021). Focus groups lasted 40 to 67 min, and two were with shelter staff (n = 2 and 4), one with animal protection officers (n = 5) and one with senior administrators of the shelters (n = 11). We purposefully included individuals from different work locations in the organisation.

We convened the focus groups to contribute to our discourse analysis whereby, following Smith (Reference Smith2005), we wanted to describe how people in the shelter participated in discourse — that is, whether and how the discourse generated by the research affected how staff think about and interpret their work (Smith Reference Smith2005; p 224). In each focus group KEK presented the key findings from the literature review and listened for how topics from the literature were taken up by people engaged in sheltering and protecting animals. From the discussion KEK also identified other problems in the everyday work activities that were not covered in the research literature. The University of British Columbia Behavioural Research Ethics Board (#H19-00009-A002) approved the focus groups.

Results

Following the theoretical approach of IE, we relate each topic of the search to an excerpt of ethnographic data to ground the review in the reality of what people actually do in animal sheltering. We use the excerpts as a tool for examining the concepts and theories discussed in the reviewed literature.

Shelter staff and officers

“Euthanasia for behaviour is a grey area. If we get a fearful cat, shelter managers might say, ‘let’s see what happens in a few days’, but I know what that means. I need to get this cat to like people in three days! I would love not to feel like that. I will spend my lunch break with the cat to make it adoptable.” [Focus group comments by a shelter staff member detailing how they make time to help animals that might be euthanased].

Although we used broad search terms such as ‘people or staff or officers’ AND ‘animal shelter’ to find research on the actual work of shelter staff and officers, most of the research focused on the topic of ‘euthanasia-related stress’, while a small number of articles examined other topics including the presence of women in animal sheltering, animal intake procedures and staff attitudes toward animals.

Euthanasia-related stress is conceptualised in the research literature as a form of stress that arises among shelter staff because of their involvement with euthanasia of animals (for a review, see Scotney et al. Reference Scotney, McLaughlin and Keates2015). This form of stress is widely regarded as a problem in animal sheltering and has become a major topic of research. Despite the early work by Owens et al. (Reference Owens, Davis and Smith1981) who conducted group discussions with euthanasia technicians about their ‘feelings and concerns’ about euthanasia, the interest in euthanasia-related stress has largely been built upon Arnold Arluke’s ethnographic study in an animal shelter in the USA. Arluke conducted observations and interviews and described shelter staff’s experiences, feelings and coping strategies related to performing euthanasia (Arluke Reference Arluke1991; Arluke & Sanders Reference Arluke and Sanders1996). Arluke coined the term ‘caring-killing paradox’ to describe what he characterised as the clash of feelings that ‘animal people’ (people who love animals and therefore work in animal sheltering) have when institutional practices require them to euthanase animals. The ‘caring-killing paradox’ has become a foundational idea that subsequently influenced a body of research that measures, for example, how staff cope with performing euthanasia (Frommer & Arluke Reference Frommer and Arluke1999; Baran et al. Reference Baran, Allen, Rogelberg, Spitzmüller, DiGiacomo, Webb, Carter, Clark, Teeter and Walker2009), how performing euthanasia affects the occupational health of staff (White & Shawhan Reference White and Shawhan1996; Rogelberg et al. Reference Rogelberg, Reeve, Spitzmüller, DiGiacomo, Clark, Teeter, Walker, Starling and Carter2007a; Andrukonis & Protopopova Reference Andrukonis and Protopopova2020), and shelter manager perspectives on euthanasia (Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Brandt, Lord and Miles2013).

After Arluke’s study, several other USA-based researchers incorporated the concept of euthanasia-related stress in their research, often using standardised questionnaires to determine how performing euthanasia affects staff depression, burn-out, turnover, or substance use. For example, Reeve et al. (Reference Reeve, Rogelberg, Spitzmüller and DiGiacomo2005) measured the extent that animal shelter staff experience euthanasia-related stress through several scales including: (i) the Euthanasia Attitude Scale; (ii) a Work-Family Conflict Scale; (iii) the Hopkins Symptom Checklist (to measure depression); (iv) the Symptom Management Coping Scale (to measure substance use); and (v) a variety of job satisfaction scales. The results showed that staff ‘involved’ with euthanasia had more general job stress, more work-family conflict, greater substance use and overall lower job satisfaction than staff ‘not involved’ with euthanasia. Baran et al. (Reference Baran, Rogelberg, Lopina, Allen, Spitzmüller and Bergman2012) and Lopina et al. (Reference Lopina, Rogelberg and Howell2012) surveyed shelter staff using a variety of scales (e.g. Work-Family Conflict Scale, Maslach Burnout Inventory, Brief Cope Scale and Positive and Negative Affect Scale) within the concept of ‘dirty work’ (i.e. occupations or specific work tasks societally viewed as physically, socially or morally dirty or tainted). Baran et al. (Reference Baran, Rogelberg, Lopina, Allen, Spitzmüller and Bergman2012) reported that 40% of staff ‘directly involved’ with euthanasia reported the task to be the most negative part of their job; the majority, however, reported other issues to be the most negative, for example, supervisor-staff conflict and low pay. Lopina et al. (Reference Lopina, Rogelberg and Howell2012), interested in whether individual characteristics measured through the scales could predict turnover, had staff complete questionnaires on their first day of work and gathered turnover information two months later. At two months, 28% of staff had voluntarily left their positions and those with more access to job information before starting work (e.g. talking with current staff, visiting the shelter and observing work, asking questions during the interview) were less likely to leave their positions.

Other researchers have used surveys to quantify how shelter staff ‘feel’ about euthanasia. White and Shawhan (Reference White and Shawhan1996) surveyed staff and managers on their emotional responses to euthanasia and concluded that individual or group counselling may help alleviate euthanasia-related stress. Rogelberg et al. (Reference Rogelberg, Reeve, Spitzmüller, DiGiacomo, Clark, Teeter, Walker, Starling and Carter2007a) reported a positive association between staff turnover and dog euthanasia rates. In addition, Rogelberg et al. (Reference Rogelberg, DiGiacomo, Reeve, Spitzmüller, Clark, Teeter, Walker, Carter and Starling2007b) solicited recommendations through an open-ended survey question about how shelter management could support staff performing euthanasia. Recommendations included being supportive of staff, offering professional counselling, and rotating staff performing euthanasia. Through a survey to determine shelter managers’ perceptions of how their staff react to performing euthanasia, Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, Brandt, Lord and Miles2013) reported that managers thought staff performing euthanasia were experiencing burn-out but did not believe this led to increased turnover.

The term ‘compassion fatigue’, a concept that was coined in the nursing literature to describe burn-out due to traumatic experiences (Joinson Reference Joinson1992), has been used to discuss what some shelter staff experience in their work, usually performing euthanasia. Schneider and Roberts (Reference Schneider and Roberts2016) conducted interviews about ‘occupational stress’, another conceptualised form of stress, with staff from seven USA and Canadian shelters and reported that in addition to euthanasia-related stress, staff discussed other stresses such as dealing with public perception of high euthanasia rates, negative encounters with human clients, and witnessing animal suffering, all of which contributed to what the researchers categorised as compassion fatigue. Levitt and Gezinski (Reference Levitt and Gezinski2020) interviewed seven shelter staff about compassion fatigue and ‘resiliency.’ Andrukonis and Protopopova (Reference Andrukonis and Protopopova2020) used the Impact of Event Scale-Revised to measure Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder and analysed occupational stress and ‘moral injury’ (an additional concept used to study trauma) using two scales (the Professional Quality of Life Scale and the Moral Injury Event Scale). They reported, for example, that ‘compassion satisfaction’ (i.e. pleasure from helping others through work) was positively associated with live-release rates (ie live animal outcomes divided by total outcomes). Finally, Figley and Roop (Reference Figley and Roop2006) focused on better ways to understand and assess compassion fatigue through surveys and Fournier and Mustful (Reference Fournier, Mustful, Kogan and Blazina2019) offered strategies for clinicians treating compassion fatigue.

A few researchers have written about how staff ‘cope’ with euthanasia-related stress. Frommer and Arluke (Reference Frommer and Arluke1999) observed shelter operations and conducted open-ended interviews with shelter staff and people relinquishing animals to the shelter. They framed their descriptive analysis on a theorised psychological defence mechanism known as ‘blame displacement’; the description concluded that relinquishers tended to blame others (e.g. family members, landlords) for having to relinquish their pet, or they blamed shelter staff if staff were unable to find a home for the animal, while shelter staff blamed relinquishers for being irresponsible or they convinced themselves that euthanasia may be better for animals than being abandoned. Through interviews with individuals working in animal sheltering or animal control, Reeve et al. (Reference Reeve, Spitzmüller, Rogelberg, Walker, Schultz and Clark2004) used an event-based analysis to identify themes based on positive or negative ‘turning-point events’ that marked changes in how individuals felt about and coped with euthanasia-related work. Finally, Baran et al. (Reference Baran, Allen, Rogelberg, Spitzmüller, DiGiacomo, Webb, Carter, Clark, Teeter and Walker2009) used an open-ended survey to ask experienced shelter staff how they would advise less-experienced staff to cope with performing euthanasia. The main advice derived from the thematic content analysis was to express feelings, to avoid forming attachments to animals, and to acknowledge that in some cases euthanasia may be the best option.

In addition to euthanasia-related issues, there is a small body of research on the involvement of women in the sheltering and protection of animals, with a focus on how gender influences the work they do. For example, Markovits and Queen (Reference Markovits and Queen2009) surveyed and interviewed dog rescue organisations in Michigan, USA, and concluded, based on statistical and discourse analysis, that women’s involvement in dog rescue was due in part to their sentimental, maternal and emotional nature. In a literature review focused on gender differences in human-animal interactions studies, Herzog (Reference Herzog2007) noted the preponderance of women in animal protection work — a trend supported by demographic information in several studies (92% in Markovits & Queen Reference Markovits and Queen2009; 86% in Schabram & Maitlis Reference Schabram and Maitlis2017, and 100% in Schneider & Roberts Reference Schneider and Roberts2016; see also Taylor Reference Taylor2010). In fact, Coulter and Fitzgerald (Reference Coulter and Fitzgerald2019) described the “feminisation of animal control work”, arguing that this work has become gendered because the altruistic nature of the work draws many women.

Relatedly, Schabram and Maitlis (Reference Schabram and Maitlis2017) applied a theory about work that may be termed a ‘calling’ — that is, work researchers characterise as a “meaningful beckoning toward activities that are morally, socially, and personally significant.” Applying this theory in an interview-based study, they identified different ‘calling paths’ that shelter staff took as they were doing and were challenged by their work (including, for example, euthanasia). Earlier, Taylor (Reference Taylor2010) theorised that shelter staff do ‘emotion talk’ (i.e. expressing anger and compassion when speaking to staff about engaging with clients or caring for and euthanasing animals), in order to do ‘emotion work’ (i.e. managing their emotions). Although Taylor (Reference Taylor2010) described the study as grounded in actual participants’ activities, the findings are abstracted into sociological theories about emotion management.

A few studies have addressed other aspects of shelter work. Surveys regarding animal intake procedures have studied disease awareness, screening and vaccination protocols (Steneroden et al. Reference Steneroden, Hill and Salman2011; Spindel et al. Reference Spindel, Slater and Boothe2013; Fagre et al. Reference Fagre, Olea-Popelka and Ruch-Gallie2017) and the various interpretations of intake categories such as ‘stray’ (Vinic et al. Reference Vinic, Dowling-Guyer, Lindenmayer, Lindsay, Panofsky and McCobb2019). Surveys have also been used to understand the relationship between shelter staff and veterinarians (Laderman-Jones et al. Reference Laderman-Jones, Hurley and Kass2016) and to identify challenges of funding and facilities in dealing with horse abuse and investigations of neglect (Stull & Holcomb Reference Stull and Holcomb2014). Arhant and Troxler (Reference Arhant and Troxler2014, Reference Arhant and Troxler2017) used an approach-test (commonly used to test farm animals for their willingness to approach people) and a survey to assess the attitudes of shelter staff toward dogs and cats. Shelter staff had positive attitudes toward cats but there was no clear relationship between these attitudes and cat approach behaviour (Arhant & Troxler Reference Arhant and Troxler2017). While staff generally had positive attitudes toward their work with dogs, the results of the approach-test proved difficult to interpret (Arhant & Troxler Reference Arhant and Troxler2014).

Focus group discussion about the literature on shelter staff and officers

In the opening ethnographic excerpt, a staff member sharing that they spend their breaks with fearful animals provides a glimpse into how shelter staff work with animals with behavioural problems to try and make them adoptable and avoid euthanasia. These activities did not deal with performing euthanasia, but rather the stress arising from euthanasia decisions and euthanasia prevention. Nonetheless, focus group participants were familiar with concepts from the literature, notably ‘caring-killing paradox’ and ‘compassion fatigue.’ This suggests that such conceptual understanding of the work is influential within the animal sheltering discourse.

In discussing these ideas, shelter staff and officers differed slightly from senior administrators. Shelter staff and officers talked about, and gave examples of, individual animals or people they encountered in their work that saddened or challenged them. In contrast, senior administrators talked about being unsure how to best support staff experiencing compassion fatigue caused by dealing repeatedly with people in difficult situations. They pointed out different programmes and counselling that exist, but they expressed concern that the staff and officers did not find the resources helpful.

Moreover, in contrast to how euthanasia-related stress is constructed in the literature, the participants emphasised stress arising from making decisions about euthanasia and their sense of helplessness when they could not rehabilitate or find a new home for an animal. In the organisation under study, where only veterinarians and veterinary technicians actually carry out euthanasia, the decision to euthanase (or save) an animal involves different work activities for individuals located differently within the institution. Shelter staff described their work as assembling information about animals (e.g. medical treatments and behaviour recorded on forms and checklists) for administrators and other people who make the decision. Senior administrators discussed the development and use of frameworks and protocols, such as the Asilomar Accords and Adoptability Guidelines (Gordon Reference Gordon2016) which provide detailed criteria for categorising animals based on their physical and behavioural health. This framework is meant to facilitate their decision, but administrators expressed that a lack of adherence to protocols generates tension between staff and/or between departments. The focus groups thus revealed that the decision to euthanase, which relied on co-ordinated work using institutional texts (a core interest of IE), was a more relevant topic for the participants than the actual euthanasia.

Relinquishment of animals

“When people call or email the shelter and need to relinquish their cat, we usually don’t have room and so we ask if they have a friend or family member that could take it or if they can wait a few weeks. If they can wait, then we send them the relinquishment forms so we can learn a little about the cat and we put them on the relinquishment list so we can call them when we have space.” [Comments by a shelter staff member detailing some of the initial work involved with ‘animal relinquishment’].

As many animals are relinquished to shelters by their owners, the process of animal relinquishment is integral to the everyday work of shelter and animal protection staff. Typically, staff meet with people who bring an animal to the shelter, assess the situation and take people through the process of formally transferring ownership of the animal by signing a form. Relinquishment of animals has generated a large research literature; Coe et al. (Reference Coe, Young, Lambert, Dysart, Borden and Rajić2014) noted 192 citations on the topic, 44.3% of which were primary research articles published since 2006. Protopopova and Gunter (Reference Protopopova and Gunter2017) reviewed research on interventions aimed at decreasing relinquishment of dogs by intervening either with the animals (e.g. training dogs to perform simple behaviours) or with adopters (e.g. providing training or educational materials). Researchers often call for a better understanding of relinquishment because many relinquished animals are euthanased (DiGiacomo et al. Reference DiGiacomo, Arluke and Patronek1998; Salman et al. Reference Salman, New, Scarlett, Kass, Ruch-Gallie and Hetts1998; Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Slater, Garrison, Drain, Dolan, Scarlett and Zawistowski2014; Chua et al. Reference Chua, Rand and Morton2017; Sandøe et al. Reference Sandøe, Jensen, Jensen and Nielsen2019).

Most primary research articles on companion-animal relinquishment use surveys to collect data from people relinquishing animals. Several authors used data from the Regional Shelter Survey, an epidemiological survey carried out under the National Council on Pet Population Study and Policy (NCPPSP) in 1993 in the USA. Members of the NCPPSP and Salman et al. (Reference Salman, New, Scarlett, Kass, Ruch-Gallie and Hetts1998) designed a standardised survey including a list of 66 potential reasons for relinquishing animals plus five additional reasons that the study participants gave during data collection. Subsequently, several studies built on the original survey to analyse and group common relinquishment reasons (Salman et al. Reference Salman, New, Scarlett, Kass, Ruch-Gallie and Hetts1998): relinquishment due to health and personal issues (Scarlett et al. Reference Scarlett, Salman, New and Kass1999), moving (New et al. Reference New, Salman, Scarlett, Kass, Vaughn, Scherr and Kelch1999), owner knowledge about and experience with animal behaviour (New et al. Reference New, Salman, King, Scarlett, Kass and Hutchison2000), and relinquishment specifically for euthanasia (Kass et al. Reference Kass, New, Scarlett and Salman2001). The literature focused almost entirely on the reasons for relinquishment, not on the work of staff who deal with relinquishers and relinquished animals.

In a somewhat different approach, DiGiacomo et al. (Reference DiGiacomo, Arluke and Patronek1998) used open-ended interviews to avoid what they termed the ‘one-word excuses of relinquishers.’ Moving and animal behaviour were common reasons for relinquishment. This research also reported that procrastination was a feature of relinquishers’ experience. Other researchers have developed original surveys that aim to understand relinquishment reasons in greater depth. For example, Weiss et al. (Reference Weiss, Slater, Garrison, Drain, Dolan, Scarlett and Zawistowski2014) used a 26-question survey and reported that most people in their sample cited a change in their household related to people or housing, not the animal’s behaviour, as influencing their decision to relinquish dogs. Using a survey based in part on previous research on relinquishment (e.g. Salman et al. Reference Salman, New, Scarlett, Kass, Ruch-Gallie and Hetts1998; Scarlett et al. Reference Scarlett, Salman, New and Kass1999), Zito et al. (Reference Zito, Morton, Vankan, Paterson, Bennett, Rand and Phillips2016) reported that half of cat owners had multiple reasons for relinquishing their cat related to accommodation, personal factors and financial factors, while most non-owners (i.e. people relinquishing unowned cats) brought found cats to shelters, believing the cat would have better welfare in the shelter. Finally, other researchers have adapted and built upon questions from the 1993 NCPPSP survey and subsequent studies. Weng et al. (Reference Weng, Kass, Hart and Chomel2006) adapted questions from New et al. (Reference New, Salman, King, Scarlett, Kass and Hutchison2000) to understand dog behaviour knowledge in a Taiwanese sample. They reported that more than 90% of participants thought that dogs misbehaved to ‘spite’ owners, higher than the 45% reported for the USA by New et al. (Reference New, Salman, King, Scarlett, Kass and Hutchison2000). Jacobetty et al. (Reference Jacobetty, Lopes, Fatjó, Bowen and Rodrigues2020) adapted scales (e.g. ‘general-trust-in-pets’ and ‘attitudes towards pet relinquishment’) and the relinquishment reasons reported in Salman et al. (Reference Salman, New, Scarlett, Kass, Ruch-Gallie and Hetts1998) to construct a ‘motives-for-pet-relinquishment scale.’ ‘Pragmatic attitudes’ about relinquishment (i.e. rational, justifiable relinquishment) was correlated with past relinquishment (Jacobetty et al. Reference Jacobetty, Lopes, Fatjó, Bowen and Rodrigues2020).

Some research on relinquishment has relied on shelter records where staff record the owner-reported ‘reason for relinquishment’ in databases that often allow only a single reason to be recorded. Cook and McCobb (Reference Cook and McCobb2012) and Ellis et al. (Reference Ellis, McCormick and Tinarwo2017) analysed USA and UK shelter records, respectively, and reported that common reasons for rabbit relinquishment included inability to provide care (or lack of time) and too many rabbits, with rabbit behaviour cited in some cases (Ellis et al. Reference Ellis, McCormick and Tinarwo2017). Casey et al. (Reference Casey, Vandenbussche, Bradshaw and Roberts2009) used open-ended responses from a standardised cat relinquishment form in the UK and grouped the responses into themes which included people ‘finding’ straying cats and ‘owner circumstances’ with sub-themes of ‘moving’, ‘owner death/illness’ and ‘financial problems.’ Alberthsen et al. (Reference Alberthsen, Rand, Morton, Bennett, Paterson and Vankan2016) reported that 91% of cat relinquishments in Australian Royal SPCA records were attributed to a category called ‘owner-related’ such as accommodation (i.e. pets not allowed) and having too many animals. Also in Australia, Hemy et al. (Reference Hemy, Rand, Morton and Paterson2017) reported that 29% of adult dog relinquishments were due to owner-related circumstances (e.g. moving, poor initial decision). Jensen et al. (Reference Jensen, Sandøe and Nielsen2020) reported that reasons for relinquishment were most often ‘owner-related’ rather than ‘animal-related’ for both cats (75%) and dogs (74%), the most common reason being poor owner health. Similarly, using shelter records to generate themes, Shore et al. (Reference Shore, Petersen and Douglas2003) telephoned individuals who had relinquished an animal due to ‘moving.’ Most individuals (57 out of 67) confirmed they were moving, but also cited other factors such as landlord restrictions on pet ownership or pet size; seventeen respondents confirmed other relinquishment reasons that included ‘animal behaviour’ (Shore et al. Reference Shore, Petersen and Douglas2003).

Researchers have also looked for relationships between animal intake data (including relinquishment) and census-based socioeconomic data. Rinzin et al. (Reference Rinzin, Stevenson, Probert, Bird, Jackson, French and Weir2008) reported a positive but weak tendency for more cats and dogs to be brought to shelters from areas categorised as socioeconomically deprived by the New Zealand Deprivation Index. In Georgia, USA, Dyer and Milot (Reference Dyer and Milot2019) reported that dogs from areas ranked high on the Social Vulnerability Index were at higher risk of being euthanased due to behaviours such as aggression and fearfulness which are often associated with social neglect. To investigate socioeconomic factors in geographic areas of high dog intake, Spencer et al. (Reference Spencer, Behar-Horenstein, Aufmuth, Hardt, Applebaum, Emanuel and Isaza2017) generated themes based on field observations and interviews with 39 community members and reported, for instance, that 40% of participants believed pet abandonment (which could lead to increased shelter intake) was due to: (i) inability to provide proper care for an animal; and (ii) uncontrolled breeding. Morris and Steffler (Reference Morris and Steffler2011) compared relinquishment data and home-foreclosure data in California, USA, finding that while relinquishments and foreclosures were concentrated in the same areas, only one of the 248 relinquished dogs came from the address of a foreclosed house. Weng and Hart (Reference Weng and Hart2012), using relinquishment data from 2000 to 2010 in Chicago, determined that the economic recession of 2008–2010 led to an increase in the relinquishment of older dogs.

Costs of animal ownership and regulations that generate expenses have also been studied in relation to relinquishment of animals. Based on a survey, Dolan et al. (Reference Dolan, Scotto, Slater and Weiss2015) concluded that animal cost, along with other factors, was strongly associated with the relinquishment of dogs. Similarly, Carter and Clark (Reference Carter and Clark2020) developed cost-related themes from interviews with people who had relinquished animals in Australia finding that relinquishment due to cost was mentioned, but only in combination with another factor, and that individuals often attempted to re-home their animal before taking it to the shelter. Others have performed statistical analyses on animal intake data over longer periods (eight to thirteen years) and concluded that a free (Kass et al. Reference Kass, Johnson and Weng2013) or subsidised (Scarlett & Johnston Reference Scarlett and Johnston2012) spay/neuter programme led to a decline in the number of cats brought into shelters. Sandøe et al. (Reference Sandøe, Jensen, Jensen and Nielsen2019) concluded that regulations that require dogs to be registered and controlled have decreased the number of dogs relinquished.

Focus group discussion about the literature on relinquishment

In the opening ethnographic excerpt, the staff member’s description of what happens when a person calls to relinquish a pet provides a glimpse into practical aspects of this work. The work processes, and the tensions they may generate, were absent from the research literature on animal relinquishment which mostly aims to understand the reasons for relinquishment by surveying owners or analysing reasons recorded in shelter databases. For shelter staff, the relinquishment process typically begins with phone calls or emails from individuals who want to relinquish a pet. This requires shelter staff to listen, to evaluate the animal’s situation, to determine if the shelter has available space to house the animal, and to decide whether other resources (e.g. animal training, social service referral) might help. Such calls sometimes come from people who are struggling with complex social circumstances. Focus group participants described this work as challenging and sometimes (in the words of one participant) like managing a ‘crisis hotline.’ This detailed information about actual work processes is important in IE projects because it can help identify where tensions arise.

Animal protection officers’ work related to relinquishment typically involves visiting people’s homes in response to a report alleging abuse or neglect. Officers described how they follow set procedures to require compliance with applicable laws, and sometimes provide resources (bedding, food, referral to a free veterinary clinic) so that people can address problems. Their work is focused mostly on keeping animals and people together as long as animals are not in ‘distress’, but they may encourage relinquishment or remove animals where this is not achievable.

The work of both shelter staff and officers is guided by the need to maintain sufficient space in shelters as determined in part by the Capacity for Care (C4C) programme (Koret Shelter Medicine Program 2021). C4C calculates the optimal shelter animal population that can be provided with humane care based on available resources and other factors. This may require staff to triage cases and give priority to animals most at risk of harm such as animals rescued from abuse or neglect. Relinquishment by owners is the lowest triage level unless animals are at imminent risk of harm. Thus, according to the focus group participants, how the topic of relinquishment is handled in the research literature seems to have little relevance to the everyday ‘relinquishment work’ that shelter staff do.

Shelter staff also noted that the databases they use in their work have limitations when used for research. The database category ‘stray’, for instance, does not necessarily denote an unowned or unwanted animal. Staff are required to assign this category to animals that are abandoned outside the shelter, or that people bring to the shelter after finding them unattended. Staff also noted that there is no database category for the increasing number of animals relinquished by citizens who believe they have ‘rescued’ them from online sources (e.g. Craigslist, Kijiji) where animals are given away or sold.

Length of stay in shelters

“Henry’s been in shelter for 32 days now and I was wondering if we could put something on the website, generate pictures saying we have this kind of dog here available for adoption?”

This quotation is from a daily staff meeting that included a brainstorming session on how staff might facilitate adoption of a dog named Henry whose length of stay (LOS, defined as the number of days between entering and leaving the shelter) had greatly exceeded the shelter’s average LOS of eleven days. Minimising LOS emerged as a key topic in the animal sheltering literature, and research has conceptualised the topic in two main ways. One acknowledges that many animals are euthanased if they are not adopted promptly and investigates LOS (or live-release rate) to reduce euthanasia by increasing adoptions (Brown et al. Reference Brown, Davidson and Zuefle2013; Protopopova et al. Reference Protopopova, Mehrkam, Boggess and Wynne2014; Gunter et al. Reference Gunter, Barber and Wynne2016; Hawes et al. Reference Hawes, Kerrigan and Morris2018; Patronek & Crowe Reference Patronek and Crowe2018). The other, noting decreasing rates of euthanasia in shelters, is focused on reducing LOS because long periods in a shelter can contribute to physical and mental health problems for animals (Kay et al. Reference Kay, Coe, Young and Pearl2018; Voslářová et al. Reference Voslářová, Žák, Večerek and Bedáňová2019; review by Protopopova Reference Protopopova2016). In either case, the goal of the research is generally to identify how shelters can adopt animals more quickly.

Many researchers have tried to identify adopter ‘preferences’ and other factors that may decrease LOS, often focusing on ‘phenotypic’ traits of animals such as breed group, sex, size, coat colour and age as listed in shelter databases. For example, Lepper et al. (Reference Lepper, Kass and Hart2002), Brown et al. (Reference Brown, Davidson and Zuefle2013), Brown and Morgan (Reference Brown and Morgan2015), Kay et al. (Reference Kay, Coe, Young and Pearl2018) and Voslářová et al. (Reference Voslářová, Žák, Večerek and Bedáňová2019) used statistical analyses to compare animal traits to LOS, live-release rate (Patronek & Crowe Reference Patronek and Crowe2018) or general outcomes of adopted, euthanased or transferred (Carini et al. Reference Carini, Sinski and Weber2020). A common finding from such analyses is that younger dogs have a shorter LOS than adults (Brown et al. Reference Brown, Davidson and Zuefle2013; Žák et al. Reference Žák, Voslářová, Večerek and Bedáňová2015; Patronek & Crowe Reference Patronek and Crowe2018). The evidence linking LOS to coat colour is more mixed. Two studies reported that black dogs had a longer LOS than white or yellow dogs (Kay et al. Reference Kay, Coe, Young and Pearl2018; Voslářová et al. Reference Voslářová, Žák, Večerek and Bedáňová2019), and one reported that white cats had a longer LOS than black cats (Miller et al. Reference Miller, Ward and Beatty2019). However, Patronek and Crowe (Reference Patronek and Crowe2018) reported no difference in live-release rate for different coloured dogs, and both Sinski et al. (Reference Sinski, Carini and Weber2016) and Carini et al. (Reference Carini, Sinski and Weber2020) concluded that coat colour was not a significant predictor of outcome (adoption, euthanasia, or transfer) for either dogs or cats. Studies often conclude that understanding shelter ‘context’ and adopter preference is important (Brown et al. Reference Brown, Davidson and Zuefle2013; Carini et al. Reference Carini, Sinski and Weber2020).

Some authors have related LOS to other information in shelter databases, often using statistical analysis to interpret findings. Noting the source of animals, Notaro (Reference Notaro2004) reported that animals brought to shelters by animal control officers had a longer LOS than animals brought by the public. Hawes et al. (Reference Hawes, Kerrigan and Morris2018) used categorical information from animal intake (e.g. body condition, health problems) to determine what influenced LOS for older animals. Patronek and Crowe (Reference Patronek and Crowe2018) reported that dogs in foster homes or dogs returned after an unsuccessful adoption had increased odds of live release while Kay et al. (Reference Kay, Coe, Young and Pearl2018) determined that dogs in shelters located in larger human population centres had faster adoption times than those in smaller centres.

Other approaches have also been used to study LOS. Protopopova et al. (Reference Protopopova, Mehrkam, Boggess and Wynne2014) video-recorded dogs and analysed the types of behaviour in the kennel that influenced LOS; they concluded that LOS was higher for dogs leaning on the wall, facing away from the front of the kennel and standing. Other authors have investigated how LOS is influenced by aspects of shelter organisation. Using statistical analysis of Canadian shelter data, Janke et al. (Reference Janke, Berke, Flockhart, Bateman and Coe2017) and Karsten et al. (Reference Karsten, Wagner, Kass and Hurley2017) concluded that implementation of the C4C programme decreased LOS for cats. A case study report about C4C implementation in Canada provided additional insights by asking and reporting what shelter staff considered worked well (e.g. additional cage space for cats) and did not work well (e.g. visitors being bitten when staff were not present in cat adoption rooms) (Humane Canada 2018). Weiss et al. (Reference Weiss, Patronek, Slater, Garrison and Medicus2013) reported improved live-release rates for cats and dogs in ASPCA shelters that had ‘partnerships’ with other animal protection and rescue agencies, although they did not include details about the actual work processes and activities of the partnership that might explain the improvement. In addition, Abrams et al. (Reference Abrams, Brennen and Byosiere2020) reported that dogs in a New York shelter were more likely to be adopted if they received the antidepressant trazadone.

Finally, a few researchers have investigated how potential adopters ‘perceive’ animal photographs, profiles and breed labels. Using data available through existing online pet-adoption platforms some authors have explored how dog photographs (Lampe & Witte Reference Lampe and Witte2015; Nakamura et al. Reference Nakamura, Dhand, Wilson, Starling and McGreevy2020) or written profiles (Nakamura et al. Reference Nakamura, Dhand, Starling and McGreevy2019) affected speed of adoption. Rix et al. (Reference Rix, Westman, Allum, Hall, Pockett, Pegram and Serlin2021) concluded that cat profiles written in the third person (rather than the first person) were associated with shorter LOS, while Nakamura et al. (Reference Nakamura, Dhand, Starling and McGreevy2019) focused on specific words used in dog profiles. For example, Staffordshire Terriers and Jack Russell Terriers had the shortest LOS when the word ‘gentle’ was used. Interestingly, Lampe and Witte (Reference Lampe and Witte2015) reported that dogs photographed outdoors were adopted more quickly while Nakamura et al. (Reference Nakamura, Dhand, Wilson, Starling and McGreevy2020) reported that dogs photographed in a kennel setting were adopted more quickly. Gunter et al. (Reference Gunter, Barber and Wynne2016) designed a study that used shelter database records and an experiment that showed members of the public photographs of pit-bull-type dogs and ‘lookalike’ dogs with and without labels to identify the breed. They reported that a label of ‘pit-bull’ could increase LOS because of a negative perception of the breed. Finally, Cohen et al. (Reference Cohen, Chodorow and Byosiere2020) examined shelter records before and after the removal of breed labels and reported that removal of breed labels for dogs decreased LOS.

Focus group discussion about the literature on length of stay

In the opening ethnographic excerpt, the staff member proposing how to facilitate the adoption of a dog provides a glimpse of their concern over an individual animal with an extended LOS in the shelter. Noting that much of the research concentrates on animal characteristics that influence LOS, focus group participants identified other features of their work that affect LOS but are not covered in the literature. This work, which was completed mostly by shelter staff, included what they described as ‘veterinary treatment’ and ‘behavioural modification’ of animals.

Regarding veterinary treatment work, both shelter staff and senior administrators noted that the need to follow protocols for veterinary interventions (e.g. treating ringworm) or referring animals to specialists took time and thus increased LOS for animals. Regarding behavioural modification work, senior administrators noted that animals currently being relinquished seem to have more behavioural problems than in years past yet there are not enough trained staff to carry out behavioural modification. Shelter staff, however, reported feeling responsible and sometimes compelled to do behavioural modification work so animals could be adopted more quickly, for instance by sitting with fearful animals, bringing food rewards so the animals would associate people with positive occurrences, and habituating dogs to wearing collars and leashes. Officers also recognised that staff had limited time in their daily work to do what they knew about and talked about as behavioural modification. This influenced their efforts to try and keep animals with their owners, when possible, for example by donating supplies. These instances of feeling pressure, responsibility and a lack of time are tensions that could be explored through IE.

Finally, shelter staff and officers identified a need for research on how transfer programmes affect animal behaviour and health. Transfer programmes aim to decrease LOS by moving animals to larger centres with more adopters, but also involve additional work activities such as preparing kennels, performing medical intake procedures and moving animals around within the shelter to accommodate those with special needs.

Discussion

In this review, we attempted to identify how published research approaches and ways of thinking seen in the research literature have shaped the current understanding of animal sheltering, and how the research literature relates (or not) to problems and challenges identified by people doing the actual work of animal sheltering.

The growing literature on euthanasia-related stress, which has received much attention since the 1990s, shows that this topic remains a strong focus of research framed by concepts like compassion fatigue and the caring-killing paradox. Scientists are now using established social-science research methods, such as standardised questionnaires and psychometric constructs, to build on prior knowledge, with the proposed aim of understanding the phenomenon as it is currently conceptualised. When Arluke (Reference Arluke1991) and Arluke and Sanders (Reference Arluke and Sanders1996) began to report study findings, euthanasia of shelter animals was very common in North America, but it is now declining sharply. In Canada, for example, the percentage of animals euthanased in shelters declined between 1993 and 2019 from 30 to 10 percent of dogs and from 60 to 15 percent of cats (Humane Canada 2021). In the USA, a survey in 1973 reported that 13.5 million animals were euthanased (Rowan & Kartal Reference Rowan and Kartal2018) and estimates remained high in the late 1980s and early 1990s (Bartlett et al. Reference Bartlett, Bartlett, Walshaw and Halstead2005); a more recent estimate, however, reported that 920,000 cats and dogs are currently euthanased each year (ASPCA 2021a). The continued strong focus on euthanasia-related stress may be an example of how, once a topic has become established in the research literature, it can continue to be a conceptual focus of study despite what is actually happening in sheltering work.

Focus group participants were fluent in the language of euthanasia-related stress and compassion fatigue and they used these terms in relating their own experiences. Indeed, institutional ethnographers look for traces of how topics within the academic discourse circulate in institutions, discovering how people actively participate in them through their use of texts and language in their everyday work (Smith Reference Smith2005; p 224). However, the participants — working in an institution where the modest number of euthanasias are done by veterinary staff — noted that the challenges are now very different from how they are represented in the research literature. For focus group participants, the stress associated with euthanasia was linked more with making, discussing and sometimes defending the decision to euthanase rather than conducting euthanasia routinely. To better understand these tensions, more attention is needed on the actual work involved in an animal’s institutional pathway that results in euthanasia.

In the research literature, descriptions about the work involved with performing euthanasia are broad. Arluke (Reference Arluke1991) recognised two distinct roles: the ‘holder’ who controls the animal and the ‘shooter’ who administers the injection. Baran et al. (Reference Baran, Rogelberg, Lopina, Allen, Spitzmüller and Bergman2012) also positioned euthanasia as a work process but also discussed the work as categorical ‘roles’ that included selecting animals for euthanasia and confirming death. Both these authors suggested that these specific work procedures may lead to different responses and levels of stress. In this research, however, euthanasia is categorised as a distinct ‘episode’ of work — disconnected from the series of events and decisions that contribute to a final decision to euthanase. Moreover, most studies have identified shelter staff simply as those who ‘participate in’ (White & Shawhan Reference White and Shawhan1996), are ‘involved with’ (Reeve et al. Reference Reeve, Spitzmüller, Rogelberg, Walker, Schultz and Clark2004, Reference Reeve, Rogelberg, Spitzmüller and DiGiacomo2005), ‘perform’ (Rogelberg et al. Reference Rogelberg, Reeve, Spitzmüller, DiGiacomo, Clark, Teeter, Walker, Starling and Carter2007a; Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Brandt, Lord and Miles2013), are ‘directly involved with’ (Baran et al. Reference Baran, Allen, Rogelberg, Spitzmüller, DiGiacomo, Webb, Carter, Clark, Teeter and Walker2009) or have ‘direct or indirect contact with’ (Lopina et al. Reference Lopina, Rogelberg and Howell2012) euthanasia. Attention to the actual work activities that lead to a decision to euthanase (or not), rather than the use of generic categories, could lead to greater insight into how tensions and conflict may arise.

While most existing research on compassion fatigue in shelters is focused on euthanasia, focus group participants noted the emotional toll on staff and officers whose work involves dealing with people in ‘distress.’ This work often includes listening to people and referring them to available services — tasks that are more related to social work than to traditional animal sheltering yet are important for the widely accepted priority of keeping people and animals together (e.g. LaVallee et al. Reference LaVallee, Kiely Mueller and McCobb2017; Baker et al. Reference Baker, Kutz, Toews, Edwards and Rock2018). Some research has discussed shelter staff interacting with relinquishers (DiGiacomo et al. Reference DiGiacomo, Arluke and Patronek1998; Frommer & Arluke Reference Frommer and Arluke1999; Irvine Reference Irvine2003) and with the public (Schneider & Roberts Reference Schneider and Roberts2016) but the focus has been on negative interactions. An IE approach would shift the focus to examine the actual work staff and officers do to keep people and their animals together including co-ordinating and collaborating with social service agencies that may also be involved.

Only a few research studies on shelter staff have applied the methods of ethnography rather than relying on questionnaires and scales. Taylor (Reference Taylor2010) used observations and interviews with shelter staff to understand how they expressed emotions. However, unlike an IE approach that stays firmly connected to how happenings and experiences are organised, findings are analysed within sociological theories about ‘emotion management.’ Irvine (Reference Irvine2003) made ethnographic observations of the relinquishment process and conducted in-depth interviews with people relinquishing animals. She pointed out that the current understanding of relinquishment may be limited by research relying on information from standardised intake forms or a drop-down list of relinquishment reasons in a database (following, for example, Salman et al. Reference Salman, New, Scarlett, Kass, Ruch-Gallie and Hetts1998; New et al. Reference New, Salman, Scarlett, Kass, Vaughn, Scherr and Kelch1999; Kass et al. Reference Kass, New, Scarlett and Salman2001) or if the records allow people to report only a single reason for relinquishment. Irvine’s misgivings over the current knowledge about relinquishment is supported by a review by Protopopova and Gunter (Reference Protopopova and Gunter2017) who noted that much of the understanding about dog relinquishment has been influenced by the 1993 NCPPSP survey. As noted by Levitt and Gezinski (Reference Levitt and Gezinski2020), future research should expand to include other aspects of animal shelter work beyond euthanasia “to gain a more holistic understanding of [workers’] experiences and needs.”

In discussing relinquishment, the focus group participants suggested that understanding may be limited by the prevailing reliance on shelter data which often were not collected with research purposes in mind. For example, the term ‘stray’ in shelter data is sometimes used for various categories of animals (Zito et al. Reference Zito, Morton, Vankan, Paterson, Bennett, Rand and Phillips2016; Vinic et al. Reference Vinic, Dowling-Guyer, Lindenmayer, Lindsay, Panofsky and McCobb2019). Shelter staff reported that owners often express multiple reasons for relinquishing an animal, and it is difficult to select a single reason as required by the database. ‘Following up with relinquishers’, as done by Shore et al. (Reference Shore, Petersen and Douglas2003) and Irvine (Reference Irvine2003), could also help overcome the limitations inherent in the use of shelter records for research.

Like research on relinquishment, conventional statistical research on LOS uses shelter database records and this approach has organised how LOS is understood. Shelter records are typically collected to track average LOS for the purpose of generating annual reports and information for the organisation. However, certain variables (e.g. breed, coat colour) may be recorded in an inconsistent way (Kay et al. Reference Kay, Coe, Young and Pearl2018; Patronek & Crowe Reference Patronek and Crowe2018; Carini et al. Reference Carini, Sinski and Weber2020), a point also discussed by shelter staff in the focus groups. Thus, the variables recorded, and hence used by researchers, may be entered inconsistently and may not be the most important determinants of LOS. Detailed descriptions of the broad work processes being undertaken in shelters (that lead to a lengthy or reduced LOS) are overlooked in the current published approaches.

Focus group participants clearly appreciated LOS research that could help shelters achieve more prompt adoption. However, their own knowledge about the protocols related to animal health (e.g. treating fungal infections, collecting samples for testing) which increase LOS is excluded from current published work. The large ‘shelter medicine’ literature on prevention and treatment of disease (excluded from this review) mainly reports animal-based outcomes such as upper respiratory infection (e.g. Gourkow et al. Reference Gourkow, Lawson, Hamon and Phillips2013) and enteric parasites (e.g. Villeneuve et al. Reference Villeneuve, Polley, Jenkins, Schurer, Gilleard, Kutz, Conboy, Benoit, Seewald and Gagné2015). An alternative approach would be to shift attention to descriptions of the everyday work routines and knowledge of staff involved with animal health and care to more effectively support their work.

Research on LOS has tended to emphasise adopter preferences and effective ways to present animals to potential adopters. Focus group participants, however, emphasised the increasing number of animals with behavioural problems that prevent prompt adoption and require the extra work of behaviour modification. Behaviours that need to be modified are identified in part through formal behavioural ‘assessments’, but the validity and utility of such assessments is debated (Patronek & Bradley Reference Patronek and Bradley2016). Importantly, the actual work involved in performing such assessments is less clear although Mornement et al. (Reference Mornement, Coleman, Toukhsati and Bennett2010) noted through interviews that shelter staff thought the assessment could be improved by assessing more behaviours and having more time for the assessment.

The work of conducting behavioural modification in shelters has been explored by some researchers (e.g. Orihel et al. Reference Orihel, Ledger and Fraser2005 and Orihel & Fraser Reference Orihel and Fraser2008 on inter-dog aggression; Mohan-Gibbons et al. Reference Mohan-Gibbons, Weiss and Slater2012 on food guarding; Marder Reference Marder, Miller and Zawistowski2013 on behavioural pharmacology), and animal sheltering organisations have created training materials on this topic (e.g. ASPCA 2021b). Recent research has reported that fearful animals from hoarding situations require more time from shelter staff (McMillan et al. Reference McMillan, Vanderstichel, Stryhn, Yu and Serpell2016; Strong et al. Reference Strong, Federico, Banks and Williams2019). A more nuanced understanding of the actual work involved with behavioural modification (e.g. how behavioural modification is done, by whom, the training required) and a better understanding of the benefits that are accrued by informal human-animal interactions could give insight into how shelter staff monitor the progress animals make toward becoming adoption candidates and how that daily work contributes to the ‘adoptability’ decisions about animals in care.

Animal welfare implications and conclusion

The research literature on animal sheltering, while involving many fields such as veterinary medicine and social sciences, tends to be organised around a few key theories and frameworks. Many studies apply high-level concepts (compassion fatigue, euthanasia-related stress) and attempt to quantify these through standardised scales. Studies on animal relinquishment often use standardised questionnaires or shelter database records to understand the reasons why people relinquish animals. Studies on LOS commonly rely on shelter database records to understand animal characteristics that increase time spent in the shelter.

An institutional ethnographic approach would complement such research by shifting the focus to observing the actual work practices of shelter staff and officers and identifying how texts are taken up in their work. It would focus on what is difficult and challenging in these work practices — with both people and animals — and then track the social organisation of those tensions. It would map and track the actual work involved with making decisions to euthanase and identify how those decisions create tensions for staff. It would describe the actual practices involved in the relinquishment of animals and the processes that owners, staff and animals are subject to. From here, policies, protocols and routines could be modified to better serve the interests of the animals and the staff. Finally, it would complement metrics on LOS by identifying the work practices that lead to shorter or longer LOS for individual animals whose stay far exceeds the average.

Acknowledgements

We thank the BC SPCA for kindly co-operating with this research, and all study participants for their time, interest and ideas. We also thank Katherine Miller, Reference Librarian at the University of British Columbia, for sharing her knowledge about scoping reviews.

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: DF, JMR, KEK; Data curation: KEK; Formal analysis: KEK; Investigation: KEK; Methodology: JMR, KEK; Project administration: KEK; Supervision: DF, JMR; Visualization: DF, JMR, KEK; Writing – original draft: KEK; Writing – review and editing: DF, JMR, KEK.

References

Abrams, J, Brennen, R and Byosiere, S-E 2020 Trazodone as a mediator of transitional stress in a shelter: Effects on illness, length of stay, and outcome. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 36 : 13e18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2020.01.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alberthsen, C, Rand, J, Morton, J, Bennett, P, Paterson, M and Vankan, D 2016 Numbers and characteristics of cats admitted to Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals (RSPCA) shelters in Australia and reasons for surrender. Animals 6 : 23. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani6030023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, KA, Brandt, JC, Lord, LK and Miles, EA 2013 Euthanasia in animal shelters: Management’s perspective on staff reactions and support programs. Anthrozoös 26: 569578. https://doi.org/10.2752/175303713X13795775536057CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrukonis, A and Protopopova, A 2020 Occupational health of animal shelter employees by live release rate, shelter type, and euthanasia-related decision. Anthrozoös 33: 119131. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2020.1694316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arhant, C and Troxler, J 2014 Approach behaviour of shelter dogs and its relationships with the attitudes of shelter staff to dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 160 : 116126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2014.08.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arhant, C, Wogritsch, R and Troxler, J 2015 Assessment of behavior and physical condition of shelter cats as animal-based indicators of welfare. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 10 : 399406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2015.03.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arhant, C and Troxler, J 2017 Is there a relationship between attitudes of shelter staff to cats and the cats’ approach behaviour? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 187 : 6068. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.11.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arksey, H and O’Malley, L 2005 Scoping studies: Towards a methodological framework. International Journal of Social Research Methodology 8 : 1932. https://doi.org/10.1080/1364557032000119616CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arluke, A 1991 Coping with euthanasia: A case study of shelter culture. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 198 : 11761180.Google ScholarPubMed
Arluke, A and Sanders, CR 1996 The institutional self of shelter workers. In: Regarding Animals pp 82106. Temple University Press: Philadelphia, Pennsylvania USA.Google Scholar
ASPCA 2021a Pet statistics. American Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals. https://www.aspca.org/animal-homelessness/shelter-intake-and-surrender/pet-statisticsGoogle Scholar
ASPCA 2021b Behavioral Rehabilitation Center. American Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals. https://www.aspcapro.org/about-programs-services/aspcar-behavioral-rehabilitation-centerGoogle Scholar
Baker, T, Kutz, S, Toews, L, Edwards, N and Rock, M 2018 Are we adequately evaluating subsidized veterinary services? A scoping review. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 157 : 5969. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prevetmed.2018.05.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baran, BE, Allen, JA, Rogelberg, SG, Spitzmüller, C, DiGiacomo, NA, Webb, JB, Carter, NT, Clark, OL, Teeter, LA and Walker, AG 2009 Euthanasia-related strain and coping strategies in animal shelter employees. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 235: 8388. https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.235.1.83CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baran, BE, Rogelberg, SG, Lopina, EC, Allen, JA, Spitzmüller, C and Bergman, M 2012 Shouldering a silent burden: The toll of dirty tasks. Human Relations 65: 597626. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018726712438063CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartlett, PC, Bartlett, A, Walshaw, S and Halstead, S 2005 Rates of euthanasia and adoption for dogs and cats in Michigan animal shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 8: 97104. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0802_2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, WP, Davidson, JP and Zuefle, ME 2013 Effects of phenotypic characteristics on the length of stay of dogs at two no kill animal shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 16: 218. http://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2013.740967CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, WP and Morgan, KT 2015 Age, breed designation, coat color, and coat pattern influenced the length of stay of cats at a no-kill shelter. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 18 : 169180. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2014.971156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carini, RM, Sinski, J and Weber, JD 2020 Coat color and cat outcomes in an urban U.S. shelter. Animals 10 : 1720. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10101720CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, J and Clark, CS 2020 Socio-economic factors in companion animal relinquishment on the Sunshine Coast, Australia. Society & Animals 28: 531549. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685306-12341473CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, RA, Vandenbussche, S, Bradshaw, JWS and Roberts, MA 2009 Reasons for relinquishment and return of domestic cats (Felis Silvestris catus) to rescue shelters in the UK. Anthrozoös 22 : 347358. https://doi.org/10.2752/089279309X12538695316185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chua, D, Rand, J and Morton, J 2017 Surrendered and stray dogs in Australia—Estimation of numbers entering municipal pounds, shelters and rescue groups and their outcomes. Animals 7 : 50. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani7070050CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coe, JB, Young, I, Lambert, K, Dysart, L, Borden, LN and Rajić, A 2014 A scoping review of published research on the relinquishment of companion animals. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 17 : 253273. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2014.899910CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, NP, Chodorow, M and Byosiere, S-E 2020 A label’s a label, no matter the dog: Evaluating the generalizability of the removal of breed labels from adoption cards. PloS ONE 15: e0238176. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238176CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, AJ and McCobb, E 2012 Quantifying the shelter rabbit population: An analysis of Massachusetts and Rhode Island animal shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 15 : 297312. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2012.709084CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coulter, K and Fitzgerald, A 2019 The compounding feminization of animal cruelty investigation work and its multispecies implications. Gender, Work and Organization 26: 288302. https://doi.org/10.1111/gwao.12230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalmer, NK 2018 ‘Add info and stir’: An institutional ethnographic scoping review of family care-givers’ information work. Ageing & Society 40: 663689. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X18001125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalmer, NK 2020 Unsettling knowledge synthesis methods using institutional ethnography: Reflections on the scoping review as a critical knowledge synthesis tool. Qualitative Health Research 30 : 23612373. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049732320949167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DiGiacomo, NA, Arluke, A and Patronek, GJ 1998 Surrendering pets to shelters: The relinquisher’s perspective. Anthrozoös 11: 4151. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.1998.11425086CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolan, ED, Scotto, J, Slater, M and Weiss, E 2015 Risk factors for dog relinquishment to a Los Angeles municipal animal shelter. Animals 5 : 13111328. doi: 10.3390/ani5040413CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dyer, JL and Milot, L 2019 Social vulnerability assessment of dog intake location data as a planning tool for community health program development: A case study in Athens-Clarke County, GA, 2014-2016. PloS ONE 14: e0225282. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225282CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, CF, McCormick, W and Tinarwo, A 2017 Analysis of factors relating to companion rabbits relinquished to two United Kingdom rehoming centers. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 20 : 230239. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2017.1303381CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fagre, A, Olea-Popelka, F and Ruch-Gallie, R 2017 Intake procedures in Colorado animal shelters. Animals 7 : 38. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani7050038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figley, CR and Roop, RG 2006 Compassion Fatigue in the Animal-Care Community. Humane Society Press: Washington, DC, USA.Google Scholar
Foucault, M 1981 The order of discourse. In: Young, R (ed) Untying the Text: A Post-Structuralist Reader pp 5178. Routledge and Kegan Paul: Boston, USA.Google Scholar
Fournier, AK and Mustful, B 2019 Compassion fatigue: Presenting issues and practical applications for animal-caring professionals. In: Kogan, L and Blazina, C (Eds.) Clinician’s Guide to Treating Companion Animal Issues pp 511534. Academic Press: London, UK. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-812962-3.00027-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frommer, SS and Arluke, A 1999 Loving them to death: Blame-displacing strategies of animal shelter workers and surrenderers. Society & Animals 7: 116. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853099X00121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, E 2016 Asilomar Accords and Adoptability Guidelines. The British Columbia Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals: Vancouver, BC, Canada. https://spca.bc.ca/wp-content/uploads/Asilomar_Accords_and_Adoptability_Guidelines_Policy_V_2016-08-04.pdfGoogle Scholar
Gourkow, N, Lawson, JH, Hamon, SC and Phillips, CJC 2013 Descriptive epidemiology of upper respiratory disease and associated risk factors in cats in an animal shelter in coastal western Canada. Canadian Veterinary Journal 54: 132138.Google Scholar
Gunter, LM, Barber, RT and Wynne, CDL 2016 What’s in a name? Effect of breed perceptions & labeling on attractiveness, adoptions & length of stay for pit-bull-type dogs. PloS ONE 11: e0146857. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0146857CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawes, SM, Kerrigan, J and Morris, KN 2018 Factors informing outcomes for older cats and dogs in animal shelters. Animals 8 : 36. https://doi.org/doi:10.3390/ani8030036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hemy, M, Rand, J, Morton, J and Paterson, M 2017 Characteristics and outcomes of dogs admitted into Queensland RSPCA shelters. Animals 7 : 67. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani7090067CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herzog, HA 2007 Gender differences in human–animal interactions: A review. Anthrozoös 20 : 721. https://doi.org/10.2752/089279307780216687CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humane Canada 2018 Capacity for Care (C4C) Case Studies Final Report. https://humanecanada.ca/wp-content/uploads/2020/03/Capacity-For-Care-English.pdfGoogle Scholar
Irvine, L 2003 The problem of unwanted pets: A case study in how institutions ‘think’ about clients’ needs. Social Problems 50 : 550566. https://doi.org/10.1525/sp.2003.50.4.550CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobetty, R, Lopes, D, Fatjó, J, Bowen, J and Rodrigues, DL 2020 Psychological correlates of attitudes toward pet relinquishment and of actual pet relinquishment: The role of pragmatism and obligation. Animals 10: 63. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10010063CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janke, N, Berke, O, Flockhart, T, Bateman, S and Coe, JB 2017 Risk factors affecting length of stay of cats in an animal shelter: A case study at the Guelph Humane Society, 2011–2016. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 148 : 4448. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.prevetmed.2017.10.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, JBH, Sandøe, P and Nielsen, SS 2020 Owner-related reasons matter more than behavioural problems—A study of why owners relinquished dogs and cats to a Danish animal shelter from 1996 to 2017. Animals 10 : 1064. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10061064CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joinson, C 1992 Coping with compassion fatigue. Nursing 22 : 116120.Google ScholarPubMed
Karsten, CL, Wagner, DC, Kass, PH and Hurley, KF 2017 An observational study of the relationship between Capacity for Care as an animal shelter management model and cat health, adoption and death in three animal shelters. The Veterinary Journal 227 : 1522. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2017.08.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kass, PH, New, JC Jr., Scarlett, JM and Salman, MD 2001 Understanding animal companion surplus in the United States: Relinquishment of nonadoptables to animal shelters for euthanasia. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 4: 237248. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327604JAWS0404_01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kass, PH, Johnson, KL and Weng, H-Y 2013 Evaluation of animal control measures on pet demographics in Santa Clara County, California, 1993–2006. PeerJ 1: e18. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, A, Coe, JB, Young, I and Pearl, D 2018 Factors influencing time to adoption for dogs in a provincial shelter system in Canada. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 21 : 375388. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2018.1436917CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koret Shelter Medicine Program 2021 Shelter Operations/Capacity for Care Resources. https://www.sheltermedicine.com/library/capacity-for-care-c4c-resources/Google Scholar
Laderman-Jones, BE, Hurley, KF and Kass, PH 2016 Survey of animal shelter managers regarding shelter veterinary medical services. Veterinary Journal 210 : 6876. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2016.02.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lampe, R and Witte, TH 2015 Speed of dog adoption: Impact of online photo traits. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 18 : 343354. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2014.982796CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaVallee, E, Kiely Mueller, M and McCobb, E 2017 A systematic review of the literature addressing veterinary care for underserved communities. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 20 : 381394. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2017.1337515CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lepper, M, Kass, PH and Hart, LA 2002 Prediction of adoption versus euthanasia among dogs and cats in a California animal shelter. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 : 2942. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327604JAWS0501_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levitt, AL and Gezinski, LB 2020 Compassion fatigue and resiliency factors in animal shelter workers. Society & Animals 28 : 633650. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685306-12341554CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopina, EC, Rogelberg, SG and Howell, B 2012 Turnover in dirty work occupations: A focus on pre-entry individual characteristics. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology 85 : 396406. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8325.2011.02037.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marder, A 2013 Behavioral pharmacotherapy in the animal shelter. In: Miller, L and Zawistowski, S (Eds.) Shelter Medicine for Veterinarians and Staff, Second Edition pp 569576. John Wiley & Sons, Inc: Ames, Iowa USA. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119421511.ch36Google Scholar
Markovits, AS and Queen, R 2009 Women and the world of dog rescue: A case study of the state of Michigan. Society & Animals 17 : 325342. https://doi.org/10.1163/106311109X12474622855147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMillan, FD, Vanderstichel, R, Stryhn, H, Yu, J and Serpell, JA 2016 Behavioural characteristics of dogs removed from hoarding situations. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 178 : 6979. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.02.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, H, Ward, M and Beatty, JA 2019 Population characteristics of cats adopted from an urban cat shelter and the influence of physical traits and reason for surrender on length of stay. Animals 9 : 940. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9110940CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohan-Gibbons, H, Weiss, E and Slater, M 2012 Preliminary investigation of food guarding behavior in shelter dogs in the United States. Animals 2: 331346. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani2030331CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mornement, KM, Coleman, GJ, Toukhsati, S and Bennett, PC 2010 A review of behavioral assessment protocols used by Australian animal shelters to determine the adoption suitability of dogs. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 13 : 314329. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2010.483856CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, GD and Steffler, J 2011 Was pet relinquishment related to foreclosure? A spatial research note from California during the height of foreclosure. The Social Science Journal 48 : 739745. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.soscij.2011.03.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M, Dhand, N, Starling, MJ and McGreevy, PD 2019 Descriptive texts in dog profiles associated with length of stay via an online rescue network. Animals 9 : 464. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9070464CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakamura, M, Dhand, N, Wilson, BJ, Starling, MJ and McGreevy, PD 2020 Picture perfect pups: How do attributes of photographs of dogs in online rescue profiles affect adoption speed? Animals 10 : 152. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10010152CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
New, JC Jr, Salman, MD, Scarlett, JM, Kass, PH, Vaughn, JA, Scherr, S and Kelch, WJ 1999 Moving: Characteristics of dogs and cats and those relinquishing them to 12 US animal shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2 : 8396. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0202_1Google ScholarPubMed
New, JC Jr, Salman, MD, King, M, Scarlett, JM, Kass, PH and Hutchison, JM 2000 Characteristics of shelter-relinquished animals and their owners compared with animals and their owners in US pet-owning households. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 3 : 179201. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327604JAWS0303_1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Notaro, SJ 2004 Disposition of shelter companion animals from nonhuman animal control officers, citizen finders, and relinquished by caregivers. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 7: 181188. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0703_4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orihel, JS, Ledger, RA and Fraser, D 2005 A survey of the management of inter-dog aggression by animal shelters in Canada. Anthrozoös 18: 273287. https://doi.org/10.2752/089279305785594144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orihel, JS and Fraser, D 2008 A note on the effectiveness of behavioural rehabilitation for reducing inter-dog aggression in shelter dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 112 : 400405. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2007.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owens, CE, Davis, R and Smith, BH 1981 The psychology of euthanizing animals: The emotional components. International Journal for the Study of Animal Problems 2 : 1926.Google Scholar
Patronek, GJ and Bradley, J 2016 No better than flipping a coin: Reconsidering canine behavior evaluations in animal shelters. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 15 : 66e77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2016.08.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patronek, GJ and Crowe, A 2018 Factors associated with high live release for dogs at a large, open-admission, municipal shelter. Animals 8 : 45. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8040045CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Protopopova, A, Mehrkam, LR, Boggess, MM and Wynne, CDL 2014 In-kennel behavior predicts length of stay in shelter dogs. PloS ONE 9: e114319. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114319CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Protopopova, A 2016 Effects of sheltering on physiology, immune function, behavior, and the welfare of dogs. Physiology & Behavior 159 : 95103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.03.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Protopopova, A and Gunter, LM 2017 Adoption and relinquishment interventions at the animal shelter: a review. Animal Welfare 26 : 3548. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.26.1.035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rankin, JM 2017 Conducting analysis in institutional ethnography: Analytical work prior to commencing data collection. International Journal of Qualitative Methods 16 : 19. https://doi.org/10.1177/1609406917734484Google Scholar
Reeve, CL, Spitzmüller, C, Rogelberg, SG, Walker, A, Schultz, L and Clark, OL 2004 Employee reactions and adjustment to euthanasia-related work: Identifying turning-point events through retrospective narratives. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 7 : 125. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0701_1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reeve, CL, Rogelberg, SG, Spitzmüller, C and DiGiacomo, NA 2005 The caring-killing paradox: Euthanasia-related strain among animal-shelter workers. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 35 : 119143. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02096.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rinzin, K, Stevenson, MA, Probert, DW, Bird, RG, Jackson, R, French, NP and Weir, JA 2008 Free-roaming and surrendered dogs and cats submitted to a humane shelter in Wellington, New Zealand, 1999–2006. New Zealand Veterinary Journal 56 : 297303. https://doi.org/10.1080/00480169.2008.36850CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rix, C, Westman, M, Allum, L, Hall, E, Pockett, J, Pegram, C and Serlin, R 2021 The effect of name and narrative voice in online adoption profiles on the length of stay of sheltered cats in the UK. Animals 11: 62. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11010062CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogelberg, SG, Reeve, CL, Spitzmüller, C, DiGiacomo, NA, Clark, OL, Teeter, L, Walker, AG, Starling, PG and Carter, NT 2007a Impact of euthanasia rates, euthanasia practices, and human resource practices on employee turnover in animal shelters. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 230 : 713719. https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.230.5.713CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogelberg, SG, DiGiacomo, NA, Reeve, CL, Spitzmüller, C, Clark, OL, Teeter, L, Walker, AG, Carter, NT and Starling, PG 2007b What shelters can do about euthanasia-related stress: An examination of recommendations from those on the front line. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 10 : 331347. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888700701353865CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowan, A and Kartal, T 2018 Dog population & dog sheltering trends in the United States of America. Animals 8 : 68. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8050068CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salman, MD, New, JG Jr, Scarlett, JM, Kass, PH, Ruch-Gallie, R and Hetts, S 1998 Human and animal factors related to relinquishment of dogs and cats in 12 selected animal shelters in the United States. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 : 207226. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0103_2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandøe, P, Jensen, JBH, Jensen, F and Nielsen, SS 2019 Shelters reflect but cannot solve underlying problems with relinquished and stray animals—A retrospective study of dogs and cats entering and leaving shelters in Denmark from 2004 to 2017. Animals 9 : 765. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9100765CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarlett, JM, Salman, MD, New, JG Jr and Kass, PH 1999 Reasons for relinquishment of companion animals in US animal shelters: Selected health and personal issues. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2 : 4157. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0201_4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarlett, JM and Johnston, N 2012 Impact of a subsidized spay neuter clinic on impoundments and euthanasia in a community shelter and on service and complaint calls to animal control. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 15 : 5369. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2012.624902CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schabram, K and Maitlis, S 2017 Negotiating the challenges of a calling: Emotion and enacted sensemaking in animal shelter work. Academy of Management Journal 60 : 584609. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2013.0665CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, M and Roberts, J 2016 Shelter-specific occupational stress among employees in animal shelters. Human-Animal Interaction Bulletin 4 : 1938.Google Scholar
Scotney, RL, McLaughlin, D and Keates, HL 2015 A systematic review of the effects of euthanasia and occupational stress in personnel working with animals in animal shelters, veterinary clinics, and biomedical research facilities. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 247 : 11211130. https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.247.10.1121CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shore, ER, Petersen, CL and Douglas, DK 2003 Moving as a reason for pet relinquishment: A closer look. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 : 3952. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327604JAWS0601_04CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinski, J, Carini, RM and Weber, JD 2016 Putting (big) black dog syndrome to the test: Evidence from a large metropolitan shelter. Anthrozoös 29 : 639652. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2016.1228769CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, DE 1987 The Everyday World as Problematic: A Feminist Sociology. Northeastern University Press: Boston, Massachusetts USA.Google Scholar
Smith, DE 1990 The Conceptual Practices of Power: A Feminist Sociology of Knowledge. University of Toronto Press: Toronto, Ontario Canada.Google Scholar
Smith, DE 1999 Writing the Social. University of Toronto Press: Toronto, Ontario, Canada.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, DE 2005 Institutional Ethnography: A Sociology for People. AltaMira Press, A Division of Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, Inc: Lanham, Maryland USA.Google Scholar
Smith, DE 2006 Institutional Ethnography as Practice. Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, Inc: Lanham, Maryland USA.Google Scholar
Spencer, T, Behar-Horenstein, L, Aufmuth, J, Hardt, N, Applebaum, JW, Emanuel, A and Isaza, N 2017 Factors that influence intake to one municipal animal control facility in Florida: A qualitative study. Animals 7 : 48. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani7070048CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spindel, ME, Slater, MR and Boothe, D 2013 A survey of North American shelter practices relating to feline upper respiratory management. Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery 15: 323327. https://doi.org/10.1177/1098612X13477801CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steneroden, KK, Hill, AE and Salman, MD 2011 A needs-assessment and demographic survey of infection-control and disease awareness in western US animal shelters. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 98 : 5257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prevetmed.2010.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strong, S, Federico, J, Banks, R and Williams, C 2019 A collaborative model for managing animal hoarding cases. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 22 : 267278. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2018.1490183CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stull, CL and Holcomb, KE 2014 Role of U.S. animal control agencies in equine neglect, cruelty, and abandonment investigations. Journal of Animal Science 92 : 23422349. https://doi.org/10.2527/jas.2013-7303CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, N 2010 Animal shelter emotion management: A case of in situ hegemonic resistance? Sociology 44 : 85101. https://doi.org/10.1177/0038038509351629; https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1440783318816214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villeneuve, A, Polley, L, Jenkins, E, Schurer, J, Gilleard, J, Kutz, S, Conboy, G, Benoit, D, Seewald, W and Gagné, F 2015 Parasite prevalence in fecal samples from shelter dogs and cats across the Canadian provinces. Parasites & Vectors 8 : 281. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13071-015-0870-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vinic, T, Dowling-Guyer, S, Lindenmayer, J, Lindsay, A, Panofsky, R and McCobb, E 2019 Survey of Massachusetts animal shelter record-keeping practices in 2015. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 23 : 385401. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2019.1646135CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voslářová, E, Žák, J, Večerek, V and Bedáňová, I 2019 Coat color of shelter dogs and its role in dog adoption. Society & Animals 27 : 2535. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685306-12341491CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, E, Patronek, GJ, Slater, M, Garrison, L and Medicus, K 2013 Community partnering as a tool for improving live release rate in animal shelters in the United States. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 16 : 221238. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2013.803816CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, E, Slater, M, Garrison, L, Drain, N, Dolan, ED, Scarlett, JM and Zawistowski, SL 2014 Large dog relinquishment to two municipal facilities in New York City and Washington DC: Identifying targets for intervention. Animals 4 : 409433. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani4030409CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weng, H-Y, Kass, PH, Hart, LA and Chomel, BB 2006 Risk factors for unsuccessful dog ownership: An epidemiologic study in Taiwan. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 77 : 8295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prevetmed.2006.06.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weng, H-Y and Hart, LA 2012 Impact of the economic recession on companion animal relinquishment, adoption, and euthanasia: A Chicago animal shelter’s experience. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 15 : 8090. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2012.624908CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, DJ and Shawhan, R 1996 Emotional responses of animal shelter workers to euthanasia. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 208 : 846849.Google ScholarPubMed
Žák, J, Voslářová, E, Večerek, V and Bedáňová, I 2015 Sex, age and size as factors affecting the length of stay of dogs in Czech shelters. Acta Veterinaria Brunensis 84 : 407413. https://doi.org/10.2754/avb201584040407CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zito, S, Morton, J, Vankan, D, Paterson, M, Bennett, PC, Rand, J and Phillips, CJC 2016 Reasons people surrender unowned and owned cats to Australian animal shelters and barriers to assuming ownership of unowned cats. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 19 : 303319. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2016.1141682CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed