Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-12T23:52:03.405Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Counterpoint: Clinical neuroscience is not ready for clinical use

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Mark D. Rego*
Affiliation:
Yale University School of Medicine, 63 Lookout Hill Road, Milford, CT 06461, USA. Email: mark.rego@yale.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Radical proposals are being made to change the practice, teaching and research basis of psychiatry to that of clinical neuroscience. Such changes would affect practice via what is studied, published, recommended as standard treatment and what is decided in medico-legal forums. These proposed changes are very premature and misguided. Here, I refute these proposals.

Type
Editorials
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2016 

In recent years the psychiatric literature has contained several prominent opinion pieces which propose to change the way psychiatry is practised and taught. Reference Ross, Travis and Arbuckle1,Reference Reynolds, Lewis, Detre, Schatzberg and Kupfer2 These authors contend that neuroscience has progressed to the point that psychiatry should either merge with neurology or become a specialty of clinical neuroscience. I believe this is premature.

History

Psychiatric neuroscience came of age in the 1990s when neuroimaging and genetic testing – along with more advanced molecular studies of receptors, messenger systems, etc. – were applied in aggressive research programmes to psychiatric disorders. It can be stated with no controversy that in the 25 years since, in spite of a large accumulation of data, we do not know the cause for any psychiatric disorder. We know many associations with neuroscientific findings, but the theories these generate remain far from proven.

Furthermore and more to the point we have no diagnostic tests or biomarkers (the dementias are exceptions to all I am about to say). There are also no tests for prognosis, treatment choice or treatment response. In all cases there are studies showing associations, but none that do better than clinical assessment. Even for the tests that do show promising associations, the explained degree of variance between participants and controls is usually small enough that the test could not even serve as an adjunct to clinical evaluation.

The final area of promise in neuroscience has been the development of new drugs for psychiatric disorders. Again, there has been no progress on this front. Even the neuroscientifically generated hypotheses about drugs that are already available have not borne out (an example would be naltrexone for the treatment of borderline personality disorder Reference Bohus, Landwehrmeyer, Stiglmayr, Limberger, Böhme and Schmahl3 ).

Neuroscientists can hardly be blamed for this record. The massive complexity of the brain still baffles all of science. The most basic things like how structure and function are related remain mysterious (even leaving alone the problem of how consciousness exists). No one knows where or how information is stored, in what format, or how it is retrieved and processed.

Rather than be a clinical neuroscience, psychiatry is an eclectic use of tools from different disciplines to help those with mental disorders and other emotional and cognitive problems. Situated in the centre of this endeavour are psychopathology, normal psychology and neuropsychology. These all currently rely on construct validity Reference Feighner, Robins, Guze, Woodruff, Winokur and Munoz4 not on aetiological explanations. To be sure neuroscience plays an important role in construct validity, as do descriptive studies, epidemiology, genetics, medication response and cognitive neuroscience. Additionally, psychopharmacology depends heavily on receptor science as well as clinical neurology. We all would like to see and appreciate the need for biological progress in all the areas I have mentioned. But needing these things and having them are quite different.

What we know

We should be mindful of the areas in which neuroscience has changed our thinking about psychiatry. As mentioned above, neuroscience has added to the ongoing work in construct validity in many areas. These include, but are not limited to, psychopathology; neuropsychology (e.g. the better understanding of memory systems Reference Budson and Price5 ); appreciation of the complexity of psychiatric genetics; the knowledge that successful psychotherapy has concrete neurophysiological impact; Reference Collerton6 the growing literature on new neuropsychological concepts such as salience Reference Menon7 and conclusion formation, which better connect pathology to normal function, and the shift in focus from the limbic system to the prefrontal cortex as an essential part of all psychopathology (and within this refocus a better understanding of the prefrontal cortex such as the complementary functions of the dorsal and ventral frontal systems). Last, I will mention the development in thinking about the genesis of mental illness that was unclear and controversial as recently as the 1980s. Were the syndromes we commonly observed complex psychological problems, undiagnosed medical disorders or many different psychiatric illnesses? Because of neuroscience research as well as clinical investigation we now know that most mental illness includes some degree of inherited vulnerability that can be modified positively or negatively by experience and may or may not express itself in the form of a final common pathway due to stress or developing vulnerability.

This misattribution of the role of neuroscience is doubly a shame as there is much to advertise about the way we now conduct treatment. The combination of empirically validated constructs, many choices of medicines to work with, multiple forms of validated psychotherapy as well as empirically tested social interventions such as assisted employment Reference Kinoshita, Furukawa, Kinoshita, Honyashiki, Omori and Marshall8 now equip us with the tools to either remit or mitigate the suffering of most patients at the same level as many medical specialties. Psychiatry and allied mental health disciplines have much to offer in the form of relatively safe, inexpensive, empirically based and clinically reasoned treatments for very common and debilitating health problems. Psychiatry has long been the object of scepticism from the public, potential patients and our colleagues. Clarity on all we have to offer is in order.

References

1 Ross, DA, Travis, MJ, Arbuckle, MR. The future of psychiatry as clinical neuroscience: Why not now? JAMA Psychiatry 2015; 72: 413–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2 Reynolds, CF, Lewis, DA, Detre, T, Schatzberg, AF, Kupfer, DJ. The future of psychiatry as clinical neuroscience. Acad Med 2009; 84: 446–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3 Bohus, MJ, Landwehrmeyer, GB, Stiglmayr, CE, Limberger, MF, Böhme, R, Schmahl, CG. Naltrexone in the treatment of dissociative symptoms in patients with borderline personality disorder: an open-label trial. J Clin Psychiatry 1999; 60: 598603.Google ScholarPubMed
4 Feighner, JP, Robins, E, Guze, SB, Woodruff, RA Jr, Winokur, G, Munoz, R. Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric research. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1972; 26: 5763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Budson, AE, Price, BH. Memory dysfunction. N Engl J Med 2005; 352: 692–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 Collerton, D. Psychotherapy and brain plasticity. Front Psychol 2013; 4: 548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7 Menon, V. Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn Sci 2011; 15: 483506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Kinoshita, Y, Furukawa, TA, Kinoshita, K, Honyashiki, M, Omori, IM, Marshall, M, et al. Supported employment for adults with severe mental illness. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2013; 9: CD008297.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.