Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T05:57:26.398Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

National Trends in Suicides and Male Twin Live Births in the US, 2003 to 2019: An Updated Test of Collective Optimism and Selection in Utero

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 December 2023

Parvati Singh*
Affiliation:
Division of Epidemiology, College of Public Health, The Ohio State University, Ohio, USA
Samantha Gailey
Affiliation:
Department of Forestry, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, USA
Abhery Das
Affiliation:
Program in Public Health, University of California, Irvine, California, USA
Tim A. Bruckner
Affiliation:
Program in Public Health, University of California, Irvine, California, USA Center for Population, Inequality, and Policy, University of California, Irvine, California, USA
*
Corresponding author: Parvati Singh; Email: singh.1704@osu.edu

Abstract

Prior research based on Swedish data suggests that collective optimism, as measured by monthly incidence of suicides, correlates inversely with selection in utero against male twins in a population. We test this finding in the US, which reports the highest suicide rate of all high-income countries, and examine whether monthly changes in overall suicides precede changes in the ratio of male twin to male singleton live births. Consistent with prior work, we also examine as a key independent variable, suicides among women aged 15−49 years. We retrieved monthly data on suicides and the ratio of male twin to singleton live births from CDC WONDER, 2003 to 2019, and applied Box-Jenkins iterative time-series routines to detect and remove autocorrelation from both series. Results indicate that a 1% increase in monthly change in overall suicides precedes a 0.005 unit decline in male twin live births ratio 6 months later (coefficient = −.005, p value = .004). Results remain robust to use of suicides among reproductive-aged women as the independent variable (coefficient = −.0012, p value = .014). Our study lends external validity to prior research and supports the notion that a decline in collective optimism corresponds with greater selection in utero.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Society for Twin Studies

The US reports the highest suicide rate (14 deaths per 100,000 people) among all high-income countries (Tikkanen & Abrams, Reference Tikkanen and Abrams2020). Experts attribute this trend to increased national despair that may approximate the bleak social and economic outlook experienced by a society on average (Case & Deaton, Reference Case and Deaton2020). Not surprisingly, suicides appear to track or correlate positively with some aggregate indicators of national despair (Agrrawal et al., Reference Agrrawal, Waggle and Sandweiss2017; Collins et al., Reference Collins, Cox, Kizys, Haynes, Machin and Sampson2021; Heilbron et al., Reference Heilbron, Franklin, Guerry, Prinstein and Nock2014; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Lee, Myung, Song, Lee, Kim, Carroll and Kim2018; Phillips & Nugent, Reference Phillips and Nugent2014; Won et al., Reference Won, Myung, Song, Lee, Kim, Carroll and Kim2013).

In his seminal treatise on social determinants of suicides, Durkheim (Reference Durkheim, Spaulding and Simpson1897/Reference Durkheim, Spaulding and Simpson1951) posited that suicide prevalence in a population is a consequence of the underlying social structure, beliefs and practices that he referred to as collective inclination (Durkheim, Reference Durkheim, Spaulding and Simpson1897/Reference Durkheim, Spaulding and Simpson1951). A population’s collective inclination towards suicide may exert coercive pressure on individuals towards suicide completion and may vary based on social despair during stressful periods (Durkheim, Reference Durkheim, Spaulding and Simpson1897/Reference Durkheim, Spaulding and Simpson1951). As an extreme consequence of increased despair, trends in suicides may reflect perturbations in aggregate sentiments of despair across place and time (Larsen et al., Reference Larsen, Boonstra, Batterham, O’Dea, Paris and Christensen2015; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Lee, Myung, Song, Lee, Kim, Carroll and Kim2018; Won et al., Reference Won, Myung, Song, Lee, Kim, Carroll and Kim2013). Ecologically, collective despair or (conversely) optimism may reflect a population’s shared outlook (Carver et al., Reference Carver, Scheier and Segerstrom2010) and manifest as contagious, collective expectations about the future (Bennett, Reference Bennett2011; Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020; Herzberg et al., Reference Herzberg, Glaesmer and Hoyer2006; Hooker et al., Reference Hooker, Monahan, Shifren and Hutchinson1992). Suicides, by definition, reflect termination of the future (Baumeister, Reference Baumeister1990; Martin et al., Reference Martin, LaCroix, Novak and Ghahramanlou-Holloway2020; Shneidman, Reference Shneidman, White and Bruner2006). Whereas research on suicides and optimism largely focuses on individual-level mechanisms, national trends in select measures of optimism appear to explain (to an extent) spatial and temporal variation in suicides (Agrrawal et al., Reference Agrrawal, Waggle and Sandweiss2017; Collins et al., Reference Collins, Cox, Kizys, Haynes, Machin and Sampson2021; Heilbron et al., Reference Heilbron, Franklin, Guerry, Prinstein and Nock2014; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Lee, Myung, Song, Lee, Kim, Carroll and Kim2018; Phillips & Nugent, Reference Phillips and Nugent2014; Won et al., Reference Won, Myung, Song, Lee, Kim, Carroll and Kim2013). Collective optimism, however, remains a complex construct that may derive not just from aggregate economic expectations, but also social integration, cohesion and social capital (Anglin et al., Reference Anglin, McKenny and Short2018; Gómez et al., Reference Gómez, Kleinman, Pronk, Gordon, Ochiai, Blakey, Johnson and Brewer2021; Olson, Reference Olson2006; Stack, Reference Stack2000a, Reference Stack2000b).

Collective optimism refers to the shared belief or positive outlook that a group of individuals holds regarding the future of their collective endeavors (Anglin et al., Reference Anglin, McKenny and Short2018; Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020). Other scholars interpret it as the collective faith that, as a society, community, or group, things will generally improve, progress, or work out for the better (Anglin et al., Reference Anglin, McKenny and Short2018). This optimism pertains not only to the expectations of individual members but also to a collective sentiment that the group shares (Anglin et al., Reference Anglin, McKenny and Short2018). Collective optimism may play a crucial role in motivating and uniting societies to work towards common goals and may underlie social change, resilience and cooperation, by potentially fostering a sense of hope and purpose (Anglin et al., Reference Anglin, McKenny and Short2018). Various factors, such as shared values, leadership, and the prevailing psychosocial climate may influence the temporal variation of collective optimism in a population (Anglin et al., Reference Anglin, McKenny and Short2018). Quantification of collective optimism in a manner that accounts for all facets of this construct remains difficult. However, as suggested by Durkheim, to the extent that suicides correspond with a society’s collective experiences of despair and future outlook, the patterning of suicides in a population may signal periods of acute decline in collective optimism (Durkheim, Reference Durkheim, Spaulding and Simpson1897/Reference Durkheim, Spaulding and Simpson1951).

Decline in collective optimism may also manifest as risk aversion or reduced willingness to invest in high-cost, uncertain ventures (Gómez et al., Reference Gómez, Kleinman, Pronk, Gordon, Ochiai, Blakey, Johnson and Brewer2021). Uncertainty and unfavorable expectations about the future correspond with reduction or postponement of expensive, long-term investments such as fertility, home purchase or new business creation (Anglin et al., Reference Anglin, McKenny and Short2018; Karasek et al., Reference Karasek, Goodman, Gemmill, Falconi, Hartig, Magganas and Catalano2015; Puri & Robinson, Reference Puri and Robinson2007). Childbearing remains one of the most fundamental, long-term investments over the life course (Hirshfield & Tinkle, Reference Hirshfield and Tinkle1975; Nurmi, Reference Nurmi, Strathman and Joireman2006; Vitzthum, Reference Vitzthum2008). Substantial research documents reproductive risk aversion during stressful ambient circumstances (Bruckner & Catalano, Reference Bruckner and Catalano2018) that, in turn, may also correspond with reduced collective optimism (Karasek et al., Reference Karasek, Goodman, Gemmill, Falconi, Hartig, Magganas and Catalano2015). Parental and social optimism appears to vary inversely with risk aversion, which spills over into conscious and non-conscious decisions regarding offspring conception, sex-specific fetal loss and fitness of resulting live births (Forbes & Mock, Reference Forbes and Mock1998; Trivers & Willard, Reference Trivers and Willard1973).

Selection in utero, defined as spontaneous abortion of gestations (also referred to as fetal loss), offers one mechanism through which populations may respond to heightened uncertainty, or reduced collective optimism (Bruckner & Catalano, Reference Bruckner and Catalano2018). Selection in utero against frail or risky gestations, by way of increased spontaneous abortion or fetal loss, may indicate a pregnant person’s non-conscious, biological risk-averse response to unfavorable external circumstances (Bruckner & Catalano, Reference Bruckner and Catalano2018). These unfavorable circumstances may diminish a frail infant’s survival or reproductive success should the pregnancy result in a live birth (Trivers & Willard, Reference Trivers and Willard1973). Relative to females, male gestations appear particularly vulnerable to selection in utero owing to their greater demand on maternal caregiving resources but lower likelihood of yielding grandchildren (i.e., reproductive success) if born in stressful conditions (Bruckner & Catalano, Reference Bruckner and Catalano2018; Trivers & Willard, Reference Trivers and Willard1973). Male twin gestations, on average, occupy the right tail of the gestational frailty distribution and fare poorer in terms of relative survival, longevity and (future) reproductive success relative to all other types of gestations (Bolund et al., Reference Bolund, Lummaa, Smith, Hanson and Maklakov2016; Lummaa et al., Reference Lummaa, Haukioja, Lemmetyinen and Pikkola1998). Hence, the incidence of male twin live births (or selection in utero against male twins) may indicate the extent to which a population is willing to invest in risky gestations that, in turn, may gauge a population’s sensitivity to variations in collective optimism (Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Saxton, Gemmill and Hartig2016, Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020; Karasek et al., Reference Karasek, Goodman, Gemmill, Falconi, Hartig, Magganas and Catalano2015)

Collective optimism may serve as a shared precursor of both (1) increase in suicides and (2) decline in male twin live births (Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020). If a population exhibits risk aversion following sudden perturbations in collective optimism, temporal changes in one of the most acute indicators of despair — suicides — should precede changes in population-level markers of selection in utero, such as the incidence of male twin live births. Two recent studies support this hypothesis (Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020; Karasek et al., Reference Karasek, Goodman, Gemmill, Falconi, Hartig, Magganas and Catalano2015). Karasek and colleagues (Reference Karasek, Goodman, Gemmill, Falconi, Hartig, Magganas and Catalano2015) report an inverse association between a measure of consumer confidence index among reproductive aged women (indicative of macroeconomic climate) and male twin births in Sweden. This research suggests that women may ‘sense’ ambient macroeconomic disturbances and, as a risk-averse response, yield fewer-than-expected male twins in the short term (two months following extreme decline in consumer confidence). Catalano and colleagues (Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020) test this phenomenon through the lens of collective optimism and examine the relation between suicides among women aged 15−49 years and male twin births in Sweden. They find an inverse association between female suicides and male twinning in that greater-than-expected incidence of suicide among reproductive aged women precedes a decline in male twin live births by three months. To our knowledge, no other studies have examined the relation between collective optimism and male twin live births.

Replication and extension of this work to other national contexts may hold interest among evolutionary theorists, fertility scholars and epidemiologists. The external validity of findings from tests of collective optimism in Sweden to other populations remains unexplored. Sweden differs markedly from the US in terms of healthcare systems, social safety nets, racial/ethnic diversity and income inequality (Shea et al., Reference Shea, Davey, Femia, Zarit, Sundström, Berg and Smyer2003; Starfield, Reference Starfield2000). We examine whether and to what extent sudden variations in collective optimism, as indicated by monthly changes in overall suicides, precede changes in the ratio of male twin to male singleton live births in the US using publicly available, nationally aggregated monthly data from 2003 to 2019. Consistent with prior work, we also test this relation with suicides among women aged 15−49 years (Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020; Karasek et al., Reference Karasek, Goodman, Gemmill, Falconi, Hartig, Magganas and Catalano2015).

Methods

Data

We retrieved data on monthly counts of (1) male twin and singleton live births and (2) suicides (mortality from intentional self-harm, ICD-10 Codes: X60-X84) from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s online national database — CDC WONDER — for the period 2003 to 2019 (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention & National Center for Health Statistics, 2020; United States Department of Health and Human Services [US DHHS] et al., 2009; US DHHS et al., 2020). CDC WONDER reports data from the National Center for Health Statistics’ Division of Vital Statistics and provides national, regional and temporal aggregates by select subgroups (e.g., gender, age) and health conditions (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention & National Center for Health Statistics, 2020; US DHHS et al., 2009; US DHHS et al., 2020). These data are publicly available and are extensively utilized by epidemiologists worldwide (Friede et al., Reference Friede, Reid and Ory1993). We selected the time period of January 2003 to December 2019 because monthly counts of twin births (by sex) for the US are not available in CDC WONDER prior to 2003. We excluded the year 2020 from our analysis to limit confounding by the COVID-19 pandemic. These restrictions yielded four nationally aggregated data series of 204 months each: (1) male twin live births, (2) male singleton live births, (3) overall suicides (all ages, all sexes), and (4) suicides among women aged 15−49 years. This study was deemed exempt from IRB review as we used publicly available, aggregated, de-identified data.

Variables

We defined as our outcome the ratio of male twins to male singleton live births per month. It is plausible that a general decline in male births may directly reduce the number of male twin births as well. For this reason, we used the monthly counts of male twin and male singleton live births to develop a monthly male twin ratio (male twin live births/male singleton live births) series. Our outcome formulation differs from that of prior research that utilizes the odds ratio of male twins relative to female twin births (Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020). Research on parental optimism and adaptive mechanisms with respect to sex ratios at birth reports an inverse, lagged relation between male and female twin live births (Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Saxton, Bruckner, Goldman and Anderson2009; Forbes & Mock, Reference Forbes and Mock1998). Put simply, a decline in live-born male twins may precede an increase in female twin live births (and vice versa; Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Saxton, Bruckner, Goldman and Anderson2009). We thus contend that the formulation of odds ratio of male twin births, as utilized by Catalano and colleagues (Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020), may distort the temporal lag and/or the magnitude of observed associations owing to endogenous, inverse relations between the numerator (odds of male twin births) and denominator (odds of female twin births) of the outcome.

As our independent variable, we used overall suicides, across all age and sex groups, per month. Overall suicides gauge several common underlying ambient risk factors shared across age and sex groups such as economic inequality and access to mental health care. Consistent with prior work, we also examine the robustness of results to suicides among women aged 15−49 years. This population may show heightened sensitivity to the social environment (Hooker et al., Reference Hooker, Monahan, Shifren and Hutchinson1992; Olson, Reference Olson2006).

We modeled sudden changes in collective optimism (or ‘shocks’) by converting monthly counts of suicides into percent monthly change in suicides. We subtracted the observed count of suicides in the previous month (xm−1) from those in a given month (xm) and divided this difference by the previous month’s observation (i.e., [xm- xm−1]/xm−1). This transformation offers the dual advantage of (1) centering around zero the strong, upward trend in suicides in the US documented in prior research (Curtin, Reference Curtin2020) and (2) yielding sudden variations (i.e., volatility rather than levels) depending on whether suicides increased (positive) or decreased (negative) relative to the previous month. Prior research examining population-level impact of ecological stressors on perinatal and psychiatric outcomes similarly uses change scores when gauging ambient ‘shocks’ (Bruckner, Reference Bruckner2008; Singh, Reference Singh2021).

Analysis

We aim to test whether greater-than-expected change in monthly incidence of overall suicides in the US precedes a reduction in male twins born in the population. We also test this relation with respect to suicides among women aged 15−49 years. Our outcome series, however, may exhibit temporal patterns such as seasonality, trends, and persistence of ‘memory’ from a preceding month into following months. Because of this patterning, also known as autocorrelation, the expected value of the outcome variable (in a given month) may not equal the mean of past values (Catalano & Serxner, Reference Catalano and Serxner1987; Shumway et al., Reference Shumway, Stoffer and Stoffer2000). This circumstance violates the assumptions of correlational tests (Catalano & Serxner, Reference Catalano and Serxner1987; Shumway et al., Reference Shumway, Stoffer and Stoffer2000). Analysis of autocorrelated data, in such cases, may yield spurious relations between the exposure and outcome owing to non-independence of outcome observations (violation of the Independent and identically distributed random variables [i.i.d. assumption]) and non-zero mean of residuals (Catalano & Serxner, Reference Catalano and Serxner1987; Shumway et al., Reference Shumway, Stoffer and Stoffer2000).

We controlled for autocorrelation by modeling the expected value of the outcome series as a function of its past values. We performed this modeling using Autoregressive, Integrated Moving Average (ARIMA) time-series approach wherein we applied iterative pattern recognition routines developed by Box et al. (Reference Box, Jenkins, Reinsel and Ljung2015) to detect autocorrelation parameters and control for them in our analysis (Box et al., Reference Box, Jenkins, Reinsel and Ljung2015). Epidemiologists routinely use ARIMA time-series analytic methods in longitudinal research on the relation between ambient stressors and birth outcomes (Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020; Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Saxton, Bruckner, Goldman and Anderson2009; Karasek et al., Reference Karasek, Goodman, Gemmill, Falconi, Hartig, Magganas and Catalano2015). Identification of autocorrelation parameters allows for prediction of counterfactuals — that is, the monthly series of male twin ratio based solely on its inherent patterns, in the absence of potential perturbations induced by changes in the exposure variable. This counterfactual series thus contains expected values of the outcome under the null hypothesis (Box et al., Reference Box, Jenkins, Reinsel and Ljung2015; Catalano & Serxner, Reference Catalano and Serxner1987; Shumway et al., Reference Shumway, Stoffer and Stoffer2000). Removal of autocorrelation in the outcome also yields normally distributed residuals with a mean of zero (Box et al., Reference Box, Jenkins, Reinsel and Ljung2015; Catalano & Serxner, Reference Catalano and Serxner1987; Shumway et al., Reference Shumway, Stoffer and Stoffer2000).

We conducted ARIMA analysis using software from Scientific Computing Associates (SCA) (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Hudak, Box, Muller and Tiao1992). Our analytic steps appear below:

  1. 1. We used Box-Jenkins time-series methods to identify and remove autocorrelation in the monthly series of male twin ratio (Box et al., Reference Box, Jenkins, Reinsel and Ljung2015).

  2. 2. We applied the Box-Jenkins routines to identify and remove autocorrelation in the percent monthly change in overall suicides series (Box et al., Reference Box, Jenkins, Reinsel and Ljung2015). This exercise yielded exposure residuals that align with the classic correlational test, dating back to Fisher, wherein the residuals indicate deviation from expected values of the series, net of autocorrelation (Fisher, Reference Fisher, Kotz and Johnson1992). We defined this residualized series as our exposure or independent variable.

  3. 3. We applied the exposure residuals obtained in step 2 to the ARIMA model devised in step 1. We specified exposure lags of 2, 3, 4, 5, 6 months based on prior work, the average gestational age of live-born male twins in the US (∼34 to 37 weeks) (US DHHS et al., 2020), and the hypothesized gestational ages at which male twins in utero appear sensitive to ambient stress (Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020; Karasek et al., Reference Karasek, Goodman, Gemmill, Falconi, Hartig, Magganas and Catalano2015).

  4. 4. We inspected the residuals obtained from the time-series model in step 3 for autocorrelation. If any were found, we inserted relevant ARIMA parameters into the error term.

  5. 5. We conducted a sensitivity test by repeating step 3 using log transformed (natural logarithm) male twin ratio series as the outcome to gauge whether results from step 3 were sensitive to heteroskedastic variance and influential outliers.

  6. 6. As a robustness check of whether our findings align with prior research, we repeated steps 2 and 3 with de-trended residuals of percent monthly change in suicides among females aged 15−49 years as the exposure.

Results

Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics of our analytic data. The 204 months in our time-series analysis yielded a total of 1,129,712 male twins and 33,850,336 male singleton births. Total number of overall suicides over our study period equaled 670,893, with 73,826 suicides among females aged 15−49 years. There were approximately 5538 male twin births per month (SD = 327.08) and male twin ratio per month averaged 0.0334 (SD = 0.001). Percent monthly change in overall suicides averaged 0.0045 (SD = 0.071) and among females aged 15−49 years, averaged 0.0075 (SD = 0.107).

Table 1. Counts and distribution of male twin live births, male singleton live births, male twin ratio, overall suicides and suicides among females aged 15 to 49 years in the US, from 2003 to 2019

Figure 1 plots the monthly male twin ratio from January 2003 to December 2019. We observe a slight increase in this ratio starting in 2009 that persists through 2019 (Figure 1). Figure 2 presents the percent monthly change in overall suicides. As expected from our transformation of monthly suicide counts into percent monthly change, this series centers around zero but shows seasonal peaks and troughs (Figure 2). The plot of percent monthly change in female suicides (aged 15−49 years) appears in Supplementary Figure 1.

Figure 1. Plot of monthly male twin ratio (male twin live births/male singleton live births) in the US, from January 2003 to December 2019.

Figure 2. Plot of percent monthly change in overall suicides in the US, from January 2003 to December 2019.

Box-Jenkins routines identified autoregression (i.e., long-term retention or ‘echoes’ of observations) in the monthly series of male twin ratios at lags 1 and 12 months (AR 1, 12). Figure 3a plots the residuals of monthly male twin ratios after removal of AR 1, 12 from the original series and represents the counterfactual. Application of Box-Jenkins ARIMA routines to percent monthly change in overall suicides detected two moving average parameters at lags 1 and 12, indicating high or low values of residual errors being remembered at 1 and 12 months later. We also detected an integration parameter that required differencing of the percent monthly change in overall suicides series over the previous 12 months (I 12). Figure 3b plots the de-trended series of percent monthly change in suicides after removal of detected autocorrelation parameters (MA 1, 12; I 12). This series exhibits no temporal pattern and serves as the exposure or independent variable of our test (Figure 3b).

Figure 3. Residual series (after removal of autocorrelation) of {1) monthly male twin ratios (Figure 3a) and (2) percent monthly change in overall suicides (Figure 3b), from January 2003 to December 2019, USA. Initial 12 observations lost to autocorrelation parameter modeling.

Table 2 shows the estimated coefficients for our time-series test model (corresponding to analytic step 3). A 1% increase in overall suicides corresponds with a 0.005 unit decline in male twin ratio 6 months later (p = .004). Put another way, a 1% increase in overall suicides in a month (relative to the previous month) precedes a decline in 5 male twin live births per 1000 male singleton live births 6 months later. This translates to a 15% decline in the average incidence of male twin births per 1000 male singleton live births (based on 33.4 average monthly male twin births per 1000 male singleton births in our sample; 5/33.4 = 15). Examination of residuals indicates absence of autocorrelation (Supplementary Figure 2). Sensitivity tests support our original inference in that log-transformed series of monthly male twin ratio exhibits similar relation to de-trended residuals of percent monthly change in overall suicides at exposure lag of 6 months (coefficient = −0.137, p = .004; Table 3). As a robustness check and to maintain comparability with prior work, we repeated our main test (from Table 2) with de-trended residuals of percent monthly change in female suicides (aged 15−49 years) as our exposure (Supplementary Figure 3). Results from this robustness check support our main results in terms of exposure lag and statistically detectable decline in male twin ratio (Supplementary Table 1). However, the relation between the outcome and exposure at lag 6 appears attenuated in magnitude (coefficient = −.0012, p = .014), suggesting a stronger relation between male twinning and changes in overall suicides, relative to male twinning and suicides among reproductive-aged females in the US (Supplementary Table 1).

Table 2. Time-Series results for monthly male twin ratios from January 2003 to December 2019, as a function of exposure to de-trended residuals of percent monthly change in overall suicides and autocorrelation parameters

Note: *p < .05; two-sided test, **p < .01; two-sided test, ***p < .001; two-sided test.

Table 3. Time-series results for log-transformed monthly male twin ratios from January 2003 to December 2019, as a function of exposure to de-trended residuals of percent monthly change in overall suicides and autocorrelation parameters

Note: *p < .05; two-sided test, **p < .01; two-sided test, ***p < .001; two-sided test.

Discussion

Volatility in collective optimism, or (conversely) collective despair, as indicated by national frequency of suicides, may precede selection in utero in a population (Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020; Karasek et al., Reference Karasek, Goodman, Gemmill, Falconi, Hartig, Magganas and Catalano2015). Prior evidence from Sweden reports the presence of this relation with respect to extreme variations in female consumer confidence (Karasek et al., Reference Karasek, Goodman, Gemmill, Falconi, Hartig, Magganas and Catalano2015) and suicides among women aged 15−49 years (Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020). We extended this work to the US population and tested whether monthly changes in overall suicides predict changes in the ratio of male twin to male singleton live births — one sensitive indicator of selection in utero. Time-series test results indicate a 0.5% decline in male twinning per 1000 live-born male singletons, 6 months following an increase in overall suicides. We also observe a similar, albeit attenuated, relation between male twin live births and percent monthly change in suicides among 15-49 years old women. Our results align with prior work and suggest that the temporal patterning of suicides may correspond with male twin live births, which in aggregate may indicate a biological risk-averse response to volatility in collective optimism across varied populations (Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020; Karasek et al., Reference Karasek, Goodman, Gemmill, Falconi, Hartig, Magganas and Catalano2015).

Strengths of our study include the use of rigorous time-series modeling approaches that limit confounding from shared patterning (e.g., seasonality) across male twin births and suicides. In addition, we measure the residual series of percent monthly change in suicides in order to capture sudden perturbations (net of autocorrelation). This approach, we argue, provides a more valid test of selection in utero following acute changes in collective optimism, relative to observed monthly suicide counts. We also overcome a potential limitation of prior work by excluding female twin births from our outcome formulation (Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020). Rather, we examine the ratio of male twins to male singleton live births. In addition, our use of publicly available data from CDC WONDER allows independent replication and verification.

Moreover, we show that the theory of collective optimism may not only apply to suicides among reproductive-aged females (Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020), but to overall suicides in the US. The implications of this extension appear twofold. First, our results suggest that male, in addition to female suicides provide a sensitive gauge of collective optimism. Second, given that Catalano and colleagues (Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020) used as a proxy for collective optimism suicides among females of child-bearing age — the only population in whom selection in utero operates — it remains possible that pregnant women (and, by extension, male twin live births) respond not to collective optimism but rather exposure to despair among one’s own sociodemographic group. The inverse relation we observe between male twin live births and suicide in males and females of all ages, in contrast, suggests that the mechanism by which humans signal the need for, and share optimism, appears more fundamental. Future work that tests the concordance of suicides to male twin births by age, sex, and racial/ethnic groups (e.g., the sensitivity of Black male twin births to suicides among Black women) can advance this hypothesis.

Limitations include that, as with most observational studies, we cannot eliminate the possibility of confounding from unobserved factors. Such a factor would (1) not exhibit any correlation with national trends in collective optimism, (2) exhibit simultaneous correlation with monthly changes in suicides and monthly male twin ratios at 6-month lag, and (3) exhibit orthogonality to seasonal patterning in suicides and male twin ratios. Whereas several phenomena such as economic recessions, political unrest, foreign or domestic terrorism and disease outbreaks may correspond with heightened selection in utero (Bruckner et al., Reference Bruckner, Catalano and Ahern2010; Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Zilko, Saxton and Bruckner2010; Gemmill et al., Reference Gemmill, Casey, Catalano, Karasek, Margerison and Bruckner2021) and contemporaneously increase the incidence of suicide (Reeves et al., Reference Reeves, Stuckler, McKee, Gunnell, Chang and Basu2012), we contend that such factors would also manifest as increased collective despair or diminished optimism.

Similar to prior work (Catalano et al., Reference Catalano, Goldman-Mellor, Karasek, Gemmill, Casey, Elser, Bruckner and Hartig2020), another limitation of our study is that we do not distinguish between monozygotic versus dizygotic twins owing to non-reporting of this information in data provided by CDC WONDER. In addition, we cannot comment on whether collective optimism may correspond with increased incidence of vanishing twins (in utero absorption of a fetus by its twin, resulting in singleton live birth; Landy & Keith, Reference Landy and Keith1998), but we encourage future research to examine these relations when the appropriate population-level data become available. We also caution readers that the present study is strictly correlational in that we do not propose a causal link between increase in suicides and decline in male twinning. Our study, rather, suggests that ecological stressors that increase collective despair may extend beyond immediate effects on population-level psychiatric outcomes, into perinatal outcomes as well.

‘Deaths of despair’ comprise suicide, drug overdose, and alcohol use-related mortality (Case & Deaton, Reference Case and Deaton2020). Although our study analyzed suicide, investigation into drug- and alcohol-related deaths may enhance the theoretical context of collective optimism (Case & Deaton, Reference Case and Deaton2020). Monthly incidence of psychiatric emergencies for mood and anxiety disorders, and suicidal ideation and self-harm may also serve as measures of national mood. These indicators exhibit substantial variations across different regions in the US (Owens et al., Reference Owens, Fingar, Heslin, Mutter and Booth2017; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Barrett, Heslin and Stocks2016), which may allow future research to conduct a detailed analysis of subnational relations between regional optimism and male twin births in the US.

In addition to measures of psychiatric morbidity and mortality, indicators of risk-averse behavior may also serve as useful exposures for further tests of collective optimism (Karasek et al., Reference Karasek, Goodman, Gemmill, Falconi, Hartig, Magganas and Catalano2015). Analogous to Karasek and colleagues (Reference Karasek, Goodman, Gemmill, Falconi, Hartig, Magganas and Catalano2015) who utilize a Swedish consumer confidence index to gauge risk aversion, surrogate measures such as new home ownership, home values and purchase of durable assets (e.g., automobiles) may reflect social optimism and willingness to invest in the future. Literature on one of the most widely studied ambient stressors — economic recessions — indicates that decline in consumption of sinful goods (alcohol, tobacco) and reduction in motor vehicular accidents may indicate increased risk aversion in a population (Ruhm, Reference Ruhm2000; Ruhm & Black, Reference Ruhm and Black2002). This literature also reports decline in fertility as a potential risk-averse response to macroeconomic uncertainty (Schneider, Reference Schneider2015; Schneider & Hastings, Reference Schneider and Hastings2015). We encourage future research to examine the relation between these proxy measures of collective optimism and selection in utero with respect to male twinning in the US.

Darwinian expectations from selection in utero would suggest that males born among cohorts with lower-than-expected male twin live births exhibit stronger survival characteristics (Trivers & Willard, Reference Trivers and Willard1973). To explore potential changes in cohort fitness, retests of collective optimism and selection in utero may include examination of preterm births and early neonatal deaths among males (Bruckner et al., Reference Bruckner, Catalano, Das and Lu2021), male-specific infant mortality (Bruckner et al., Reference Bruckner, Helle, Bolund and Lummaa2015), birth defects among live-born males (Singh et al., Reference Singh, Yang, Shaw, Catalano and Bruckner2017), and incidence of other genetic conditions such as childhood cancers (Bruckner et al., Reference Bruckner, Catalano, Das and Lu2021) among conception cohorts exposed in utero to greater-than-expected suicides (Bruckner & Catalano, Reference Bruckner and Catalano2018). We expect these analyses to provide evidence of whether changes in collective optimism affect the survival characteristics of live-born cohorts, or if their relation to selection in utero against male twins diminishes beyond parturition.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2023.49.

Author contributions

PS conceived the study, acquired data, conducted data analyses and developed the manuscript; SG assisted with data acquisition and manuscript preparation; AD contributed to manuscript preparation and review; TAB conducted analyses and supervised manuscript development and review.

Competing interests

All authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

Agrrawal, P., Waggle, D., & Sandweiss, D. H. (2017). Suicides as a response to adverse market sentiment (1980-2016). PloS One, 12, e0186913. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186913 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anglin, A. H., McKenny, A. F., & Short, J. C. (2018). The impact of collective optimism on new venture creation and growth: A social contagion perspective. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 42, 390425. https://doi.org/10.1111/etap.12256 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (1990). Suicide as escape from self. Psychological Review, 97, 90113. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.90 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, O. (2011). Cultures of optimism. Cultural Sociology, 5, 301320. https://doi.org/10.1177/1749975511401 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolund, E., Lummaa, V., Smith, K. R., Hanson, H. A., & Maklakov, A. A. (2016). Reduced costs of reproduction in females mediate a shift from a male-biased to a female-biased lifespan in humans. Scientific Reports, 6, 19. https://doi.10.1038/srep24672 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Box, G. E. P., Jenkins, G. M., Reinsel, G. C., & Ljung, G. M. (2015). Time series analysis: Forecasting and control. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Bruckner, T. A. (2008). Metropolitan economic decline and infant mortality due to unintentional injury. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 40, 17971803. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2008.07.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruckner, T. A., & Catalano, R. (2018). Selection in utero and population health: theory and typology of research. SSM-Population Health, 5, 101113. https://doi.10.1016/j.ssmph.2018.05.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruckner, T. A., Catalano, R., & Ahern, J. (2010). Male fetal loss in the US following the terrorist attacks of September 11, 2001. BMC Public Health, 10, 273. https://doi.10.1186/1471-2458-10-273 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruckner, T. A., Gailey, S., Das, A., Gemmill, A., Casey, J. A., Catalano, R., Shaw, G. M., & Zeitlin, J. (2021). Stillbirth as left truncation for early neonatal death in California, 1989–2015: A time-series study. BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth, 21, 478. https://doi.10.1186/s12884-021-03852-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruckner, T. A., Helle, S., Bolund, E., & Lummaa, V. (2015). Culled males, infant mortality and reproductive success in a pre-industrial Finnish population. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282, 20140835. https://doi.10.1098/rspb.2014.0835 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruckner, T. A., Catalano, R., Das, A., & Lu, Y. (2021). Cohort Selection In Utero against Male Twins and Childhood Cancers: A Population-Based Register Study. Cancer Epidemiology and Prevention Biomarkers, 30, 18341840. https://doi.10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-21-0053 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2010). Optimism. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 879889. https://doi.10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Case, A., & Deaton, A. (2020). Deaths of despair and the future of capitalism. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Catalano, R. A., Goldman-Mellor, S., Karasek, D. A., Gemmill, A., Casey, J. A., Elser, H., Bruckner, T. A., & Hartig, T. (2020). Collective optimism and selection against male twins in utero. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 23, 4550. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2020.2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catalano, R. A., Saxton, K. B., Gemmill, A., & Hartig, T. (2016). Twinning in Norway following the Oslo Massacre: Evidence of a ‘Bruce effect’ in humans. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 19, 485491. https://doi.10.1017/thg.2016.58 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catalano, R. A., Saxton, K., Bruckner, T., Goldman, S., & Anderson, E. (2009). A sex-specific test of selection in utero. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 257, 475479. https://doi.10.1016/j.jtbi.2008.12.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catalano, R., & Serxner, S. (1987). Time series designs of potential interest to epidemiologists. American Journal of Epidemiology, 126, 724731. https://doi.10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a114712 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catalano, R., Zilko, C. E. M., Saxton, K. B., & Bruckner, T. (2010). Selection in utero: A biological response to mass layoffs. American Journal of Human Biology, 22, 396400. https://doi.10.1002/ajhb.21011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, & National Center for Health Statistics. (2020). Underlying Cause of Death 1999-2019 on CDC WONDER Online Database, released in 2020. CDC WONDER. http://wonder.cdc.gov/ucd-icd10.html Google Scholar
Collins, A., Cox, A., Kizys, R., Haynes, F., Machin, S., & Sampson, B. (2021). Suicide, sentiment and crisis. The Social Science Journal, 58, 206223. https://doi.10.1016/j.soscij.2019.04.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curtin, S. C. (2020). Age-adjusted suicide rates, by sex and three most common methods-United States, 2000-2018. MMWR: Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 69, 249.Google Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1897/1951). Suicide, a study in sociology [Spaulding, J. A., & Simpson, G., Trans.]. Routledge.Google Scholar
Fisher, R. A. (1992). Statistical methods for research workers. In Kotz, S. & Johnson, N. L. (Eds.), Breakthroughs in statistics (pp. 6670). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forbes, L. S., & Mock, D. W. (1998). Parental optimism and progeny choice: When is screening for offspring quality affordable. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 192, 314. https://doi.10.1006/jtbi.1997.0596 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friede, A., Reid, J. A., & Ory, H. W. (1993). CDC WONDER: A comprehensive on-line public health information system of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. American Journal of Public Health, 83, 12891294. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(18)30596-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gemmill, A., Casey, J. A., Catalano, R., Karasek, D., Margerison, C. E., & Bruckner, T. (2021). Changes in preterm birth and caesarean deliveries in the United States during the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 36, 485489. https://doi.org/10.1111/ppe.12811 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gómez, C. A., Kleinman, D. V, Pronk, N., Gordon, G. L. W., Ochiai, E., Blakey, C., Johnson, A., & Brewer, K. H. (2021). Practice full report: Addressing health equity and social determinants of health through Healthy People 2030. Journal of Public Health Management and Practice, 27, 249257. https://doi.10.1097/PHH.0000000000001297 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heilbron, N., Franklin, J. C., Guerry, J. D., & Prinstein, M. J. (2014). Social and ecological approaches to understanding suicidal behaviors and nonsuicidal self-injury. In Nock, M. K. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of suicide and self-injury (pp. 206234). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Herzberg, P. Y., Glaesmer, H., & Hoyer, J. (2006). Separating optimism and pessimism: a robust psychometric analysis of the revised Life Orientation Test (LOT-R). Psychological Assessment, 18, 433438. https://doi.10.1037/1040-3590.18.4.433 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirshfield, M. F., & Tinkle, D. W. (1975). Natural selection and the evolution of reproductive effort. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 72, 22272231. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.72.6.2227 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hooker, K., Monahan, D., Shifren, K., & Hutchinson, C. (1992). Mental and physical health of spouse caregivers: The role of personality. Psychology and Aging, 7, 367375. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.7.3.367 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karasek, D., Goodman, J., Gemmill, A., Falconi, A., Hartig, T., Magganas, A., & Catalano, R. (2015). Twins less frequent than expected among male births in risk averse populations. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 18, 314320. https://doi.10.1017/thg.2015.22 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landy, H. J., & Keith, L. G. (1998). The vanishing twin: A review. Human Reproduction Update, 4, 177183. https://doi.10.1093/humupd/4.2.177 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsen, M. E., Boonstra, T. W., Batterham, P. J., O’Dea, B., Paris, C., & Christensen, H. (2015). We feel: mapping emotion on Twitter. IEEE Journal of Biomedical and Health Informatics, 19, 12461252. https://doi.10.1109/JBHI.2015.2403839 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, K. S., Lee, H., Myung, W., Song, G.-Y., Lee, K., Kim, H., Carroll, B. J., & Kim, D. K. (2018). Advanced daily prediction model for national suicide numbers with social media data. Psychiatry Investigation, 15, 344354. https://doi.10.30773/pi.2017.10.15 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, L.-M., Hudak, G. B., Box, G. E. P., Muller, M. E., & Tiao, G. C. (1992). Forecasting and time series analysis using the SCA statistical system (vol. 1). Scientific Computing Associates.Google Scholar
Lummaa, V., Haukioja, E., Lemmetyinen, R., & Pikkola, M. (1998). Natural selection on human twinning. Nature, 394, 533534. https://doi.10.1038/28977 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, J., LaCroix, J. M., Novak, L. A., & Ghahramanlou-Holloway, M. (2020). Typologies of suicide: A critical literature review. Archives of Suicide Research, 24, 2540. https://doi.10.1080/13811118.2018.1564100 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nurmi, J.-E. (2006). Thinking about and acting upon the future: Development of future orientation across the life span. In Strathman, A. & Joireman, J. (Eds.), Understanding behavior in the context of time (pp. 3157). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Olson, K. R. (2006). A literature review of social mood. The Journal of Behavioral Finance, 7, 193203. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15427579jpfm0704_2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owens, P. L., Fingar, K. R., Heslin, K. C., Mutter, R., & Booth, C. L. (2017). Emergency department visits related to suicidal ideation, 2006–2013: Statistical brief# 220. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (US).Google Scholar
Phillips, J. A., & Nugent, C. N. (2014). Suicide and the Great Recession of 2007–2009: The role of economic factors in the 50 US states. Social Science & Medicine, 116, 2231. https://doi.10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.06.015 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puri, M., & Robinson, D. T. (2007). Optimism and economic choice. Journal of Financial Economics, 86, 7199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfineco.2006.09.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reeves, A., Stuckler, D., McKee, M., Gunnell, D., Chang, S.-S., & Basu, S. (2012). Increase in state suicide rates in the USA during economic recession. The Lancet, 380, 18131814. https://doi.10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61910-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruhm, C. J. (2000). Are recessions good for your health? The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115, 617650. https://doi.10.3386/w5570 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruhm, C. J., & Black, W. E. (2002). Does drinking really decrease in bad times? Journal of Health Economics, 21, 659678. https://doi.10.1016/s0167-6296(02)00033-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, D. (2015). The great recession, fertility, and uncertainty: Evidence from the United States. Journal of Marriage and Family, 77, 11441156. https://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12212 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, D., & Hastings, O. P. (2015). Socioeconomic variation in the effect of economic conditions on marriage and nonmarital fertility in the United States: Evidence from the Great Recession. Demography, 52, 18931915. https://doi.10.1007/s13524-015-0437-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shea, D., Davey, A., Femia, E. E., Zarit, S. H., Sundström, G., Berg, S., & Smyer, M. A. (2003). Exploring assistance in Sweden and the United States. The Gerontologist, 43, 712721. https://doi.10.1093/geront/43.5.712 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shneidman, E. S. (2006). Orientations toward death: A vital aspect of the study of lives. In White, R. W. (Ed.) & Bruner, K. F. (Collaborator), The study of lives: Essays on Personality in Honor of Henry A. Murray (pp. 200227). Routledge. https://www.taylorfrancis.com/chapters/edit/10.4324/9781351302562-11/orientations-toward-death-edwin-shneidman Google Scholar
Shumway, R. H., Stoffer, D. S., & Stoffer, D. S. (2000). Time series analysis and its applications (vol. 3). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, P. (2021). Psychiatric emergencies following the 2008 economic recession: An Ecological examination of population-level responses in four US states. The Journal of Mental Health Policy and Economics, 24, 1330.Google ScholarPubMed
Singh, P., Yang, W., Shaw, G. M., Catalano, R., & Bruckner, T. A. (2017). Selected birth defects among males following the United States terrorist attacks of 11 September 2001. Birth Defects Research, 109, 12771283. https://doi.10.1002/bdr2.1072 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stack, S. (2000a). Suicide: A 15-year review of the sociological literature part I: Cultural and economic factors. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 30, 145162. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1943-278X.2000.tb01073.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stack, S. (2000b). Suicide: A 15-year review of the sociological literature part II: Modernization and social integration perspectives. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 30, 163176. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1943-278X.2000.tb01074.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Starfield, B. (2000). Is US health really the best in the world? JAMA, 284, 483485. https://doi.10.1001/jama.284.4.483 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tikkanen, R., & Abrams, M. K. (2020). US health care from a global perspective, 2019: higher spending, worse outcomes. The Commonwealth Fund. https://www.commonwealthfund.org/publications/issue-briefs/2020/jan/us-health-care-global-perspective-2019 Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L., & Willard, D. E. (1973). Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring. Science, 179, 9092. https://doi.10.1126/science.179.4068.90 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
United States Department of Health and Human Services (US DHHS), Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS), & Division of Vital Statistics. (2009). Natality public-use data 2003-2006, on CDC WONDER Online Database. CDC WONDER. http://wonder.cdc.gov/natality-v2006.html Google Scholar
United States Department of Health and Human Services (US DHHS), Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS), & Division of Vital Statistics. (2020). Natality public-use data 2007-2019, on CDC WONDER Online Database. CDC WONDER. http://wonder.cdc.gov/natality-current.html Google Scholar
Vitzthum, V. J. (2008). Evolutionary models of women’s reproductive functioning. Annual Review of Anthropology, 37, 5373. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.37.081407.085112 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, A. J., Barrett, M. L., Heslin, K. C., & Stocks, C. (2016). Trends in emergency department visits involving mental and substance use disorders, 2006–2013. In Healthcare Cost and Utilization Project (HCUP) Statistical Briefs [Internet]. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (US).Google Scholar
Won, H.-H., Myung, W., Song, G.-Y., Lee, W.-H., Kim, J.-W., Carroll, B. J., & Kim, D. K. (2013). Predicting national suicide numbers with social media data. PloS One, 8, e61809. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061809 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Counts and distribution of male twin live births, male singleton live births, male twin ratio, overall suicides and suicides among females aged 15 to 49 years in the US, from 2003 to 2019

Figure 1

Figure 1. Plot of monthly male twin ratio (male twin live births/male singleton live births) in the US, from January 2003 to December 2019.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Plot of percent monthly change in overall suicides in the US, from January 2003 to December 2019.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Residual series (after removal of autocorrelation) of {1) monthly male twin ratios (Figure 3a) and (2) percent monthly change in overall suicides (Figure 3b), from January 2003 to December 2019, USA. Initial 12 observations lost to autocorrelation parameter modeling.

Figure 4

Table 2. Time-Series results for monthly male twin ratios from January 2003 to December 2019, as a function of exposure to de-trended residuals of percent monthly change in overall suicides and autocorrelation parameters

Figure 5

Table 3. Time-series results for log-transformed monthly male twin ratios from January 2003 to December 2019, as a function of exposure to de-trended residuals of percent monthly change in overall suicides and autocorrelation parameters

Supplementary material: File

Singh et al. supplementary material

Singh et al. supplementary material

Download Singh et al. supplementary material(File)
File 47.6 KB