Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T12:01:07.109Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Southern California Twin Register at the University of Southern California: III

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 February 2013

Laura A. Baker*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Catherine Tuvblad
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Pan Wang
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Karina Gomez
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Serena Bezdjian
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Sharon Niv
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Adrian Raine
Affiliation:
Departments of Criminology and Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
*
address for correspondence: Laura Baker, Department of Psychology (SGM 501), University of Southern California, 3620 S. McClintock Ave., Los Angeles, CA 90089-1061. E-mail: lbaker@usc.edu

Abstract

The Southern California Twin Register at the University of Southern California (USC) was initiated in 1984 and continues to provide an important resource for studies investigating genetic and environmental influences on human behavior. This article provides an update on the current register and its potential for future twin studies using recruitment through school district databases and voter records. An overview is also provided for an ongoing longitudinal twin study investigating the development of externalizing psychopathology from childhood to young adulthood, the USC Study of Risk Factors for Antisocial Behavior. Characteristics of the twins and their families are presented, including recruitment and participation rates, as well as attrition analyses and a summary of key findings to date.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2013

Large metropolitan areas provide enormous potential for twin research. As the second-largest combined area in the country, the Greater Los Angeles Area — including five counties in southern California, namely Los Angeles, Orange, San Bernardino, Riverside, and Ventura counties — and its enormous population (estimated to be over 17.6 million in 2009; http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Greater_Los_Angeles_Area) makes it a rich resource for recruiting twins across the lifespan. Given the current population and the recent twinning rates in California (incidence: 30.3 per 1,000 births http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nvsr/nvsr61/nvsr61_01.pdf), we estimate there to be over half a million twins residing in this five-county, densely populated area.

We previously described our recruitment efforts in identifying twins in Southern California (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Barton and Raine2002, Reference Baker, Barton, Lozano, Raine and Fowler2006). In this article, we provide an update on the ongoing University of Southern California (USC) longitudinal study of Risk Factors for Antisocial Behavior (RFAB), including a full description of the sample characteristics, recruitment procedures, and a summary of measures across the various occasions (Waves 1–4 are complete; Wave 5 is ongoing). Attrition analyses are presented, along with a summary of key findings. We also provide a brief overview of the potential for recruiting school-age twins based on public school district databases, using the most recent enrollment and multiple birth rate statistics, to highlight the enormous opportunities for doing large-scale twin research in Southern California.

The USC Longitudinal Twin Study of RFAB

The USC RFAB twin study uses a prospective, longitudinal design in which both monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins who have been studied approximately every 2–3 years beginning from age 9–10. The RFAB uses a comprehensive multi-informant, multi-measure approach with a key focus on externalizing psychopathology and its biological and social risk factors. An overview of the study design is presented in Figure 1, which indicates the ages at which data are available, and the major areas of assessment, including anti-social outcomes as well as four major domains of risk factors (psychosocial, psychophysiological, neurocognitive, and personality). Assessments are made using three informants — twins provide self-report as well as ratings of their co-twins, while the twins’ primary caregiver (the biological mother in over 92% of families) and teachers also provide ratings of each twin. Official records are obtained from schools regarding both academic performance and discipline, and criminal justice records will be sought as the twins reach adulthood. To date, twins in the RFAB study have been assessed on at least one of five occasions, including four waves which are complete (Wave 1: age 9–10 years, 614 pairs; Wave 2: age 11–13 years, 445 pairs; Wave 3: age 14–15 years, 604 pairs; and Wave 4: age 16–18 years, 504 pairs), as well as an ongoing assessment (Wave 5: age 19–20 years, with a target sample of 625 pairs).

FIGURE 1 The USC Longitudinal Twin Study of Risk Factors for Antisocial Behavior (RFAB): Design, Constructs and Informants

Sample

The RFAB participants include 780 sets of twins and triplets (N = 1,569 subjects), with males and females represented in approximately equal numbers. Both same-sex MZ and DZ pairs as well as opposite-sex DZ pairs are included. Zygosity of same-sex pairs was determined using DNA micro-satellite analysis, which measures concordance of DNA markers to assess whether DNA is identical (any pair with a discordant marker was to be DZ, while any pair with 7 concordant markers was considered to be MZ). In pairs where results were inconclusive (usually due to weak signal), zygosity was assessed with the Twin Similarity Questionnaire (Lykken, Reference Lykken1978), a measure found to be 90% concordant with DNA zygosity in cases for which both measures were available (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Barton, Lozano, Raine and Fowler2006).

The sample is representative of the ethnic and socio-economic diversity of the greater Los Angeles area (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Jacobson, Raine, Lozano and Bezdjian2007), with 28,6% Caucasians, 34.3% Hispanics, 13.1% Blacks, 4.1% Asians, 0.1% Native Americans, 17.6% mixed, and 2.2% other or unknown.

The numbers of families who participated in each completed wave (1–4) are presented in Table 1, along with expected N's for ongoing Wave 5. Of the 614 original Wave 1 families, 72% (n = 445) returned for Wave 2, 71% (n = 438) returned for Wave 3, and 59% (n = 365) returned for Wave 4. While these retention rates for individual follow-up waves are modest (albeit quite favorable for a diverse urban sample and comparable to similar twin studies (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Gardner and Lichtenstein2009), the overall retention rate for original Wave 1 families returning to any subsequent wave (including 2, 3, and 4) is quite favorable: 529 out of 614 (86%) of original Wave 1 families participated in at least one subsequent wave to date. The twin sample at Wave 5 will entirely consist of previous participants in the RFAB, drawn from the total longitudinal sample of 780 families (i.e., 614 original families who participated at Wave 1, plus 166 new families recruited at Wave 3). We have current contact information on >85% of the total pool of participants.

TABLE 1 Number of Families of Twins Participating in Waves 1–4, and Projected for Wave 5

aWave 5 N's predicted from previous participation; bWave 3 total N includes 166 new families + 438 returning families recruited in Wave 1; cWave 4 total N includes 139 new families + 365 returning families recruited in Wave 1.

Measures

In addition to using three informants, that is, twin self-report, co-twin report, and parent ratings, we also obtain school records for each twin, including both academic performance (e.g., standardized achievement test scores) and behavioral (i.e., discipline) data. The assessments of externalizing psychopathology in this study are summarized in Table 2, indicating key constructs and informants used in each case. Social and biological risk factors for externalizing psychopathology are also assessed during each wave, using both surveys and laboratory tasks. These are summarized in Table 3. The longitudinal data on measures of anti-social behavior (ASB), including psychopathic personality, rule-breaking behavior and aggression, as well as psychiatric symptoms, autonomic measures of arousal and reactivity, are particularly represented across multiple (at least three, and in most cases five) time points. The only measures for which there are only two repeated observations are those involving electrocortical measures (electroencephalogram (EEG) and event-related potentials (ERPs)) and IQ, which are obtained at Waves 1 and 5.

TABLE 2 Key Psychopathology Measures in Twins in USC-RFAB Across Waves 1–5

Y = youth report; P = parent report; T = teacher report; C = co-twin report; O = official records from schools in Waves 1–4 and justice system (for ASB) in Wave 5.

TABLE 3 Measures of Social and Biological Risk Factors for Externalizing Behavior Problems of Twins in USC-RFAB across Waves 1–5

Y = youth report; P = parent report; T = teacher report; * = developed specifically for this study.

Attrition Analysis and Efforts for Tracking and Retention

Attrition analyses were carried out to examine whether those who discontinue participating during Waves 2, 3, or 4 differ from responders on baseline measures at Wave 1. Logistic regression analyses showed non-significant odds ratios (OR) for family socio-economic status, based on the Hollingshead Four-Factor index of Social Status (Hollingshead, Reference Hollingshead1979; OR = 0.99, 95% CI, 0.98–1.01), twin's gender (OR = 0.79, 95% CI, 0.59–1.07), interview language (OR = 1.13, 95% CI, 0.76–1.69), nor for ASB (Child Psychopathy Scale (CPS; total score; Lynam, Reference Lynam1997) OR = 1.00, 95% CI, 0.97–1.03), reactive-proactive aggression total score (Raine et al., Reference Raine, Dodge, Loeber, Gatzke-Kopp, Lynam, Reynolds and Liu2006; OR = 1.00, 95% CI, 0.98–1.02). However, responders and non-responders were significantly different in ethnicity (OR = 0.70, 95% CI, 0.50–0.99), indicating that Caucasians were slightly less likely to drop out. Apart from the slight ethnic difference, those who discontinue participating in our study do so in a random manner.

To stay in contact with participating families we send out newsletters, annual birthday cards, and email personalized notecards to remind participants of upcoming lab visits. We also use various tracking procedures, including telephone number searches, returned mailings’ forwarding address information, and utilizing popular media (e.g., searches of Facebook and MySpace). We also periodically request that they verify/update current contact information. Additionally, we establish communication with their close friends and family members to learn their whereabouts if we have lost contact with them over time. Families are also invited to attend organized events of interest to twins, including a recent panel discussion on twins by top experts in the area of twins’ development, genetics, and psychotherapy, which helps create an atmosphere of inclusion. We provide each twin with a list of phone numbers and Web sites of agencies they can turn to for help. Finally, we offer a small monetary incentive for referrals.

Key Findings From the USC-RFAB

Several important findings have emerged from the RFAB, including both phenotypic and genetic analyses of externalizing psychopathology and its various social and biological risk factors. Below is a short summary of some of the key findings to date.

Genetic Effects in Externalizing Behavior Problems: Variations Across Definitions and Informants

Our findings show ASB (measured by symptom counts of conduct disorder, ratings of aggression, delinquency, and psychopathic personality) can be defined by a common ASB factor with strong heritability (96%; Baker et al., Reference Baker, Jacobson, Raine, Lozano and Bezdjian2007). Similarly, the covariance among attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), and conduct disorder (CD) symptoms could be explained by a latent externalizing behavior factor at ages 9–10 years. Genetic influences explained 57% of variance in this externalizing factor (Tuvblad et al., Reference Tuvblad, Zheng, Raine and Baker2009b). We also examined the genetic and environmental influences on the two factor scores Callous/Disinhibited and Manipulative/Deceitful (CPS; Lynam, Reference Lynam1997 Reference Lynam2002) and found significant genetic influences between 46 and 64% (Bezdjian et al., Reference Bezdjian, Raine, Baker and Lynam2010). These findings together show that genetic influences vary depending on how ASB is defined, and that there are measurement-specific genetic and environmental influences, suggesting some etiological independence among different types of ASB. Further, our findings show that different informants only partly share a ‘common view’ regarding the same behaviors, thus, additional factors that influence the ‘rater-specific’ view of the child's ASB vary for different informants, especially for complex traits including psychopathic personality (Bezdjian et al., Reference Bezdjian, Raine, Baker and Lynam2010) and aggression (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Jacobson, Raine, Lozano and Bezdjian2007).

Relationships of ASB With Biological Risk Factors

A novel finding from the current sample was that the association between low-resting heart rate measured at ages 9–10 and ASB measured at ages 9–10 and 11–13 was entirely explained by genetic covariation. Although the heritable component of heart rate explained only a small portion (1–4%) of the substantial genetic variance in ASB, children with low-resting heart rate appear genetically predisposed toward externalizing behavior problems as early as 9 years old (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Tuvblad, Reynolds, Zheng, Lozano and Raine2009).

Reduced skin conductance response (SCR) magnitude (hyporeactivity) was only characteristic of boys with higher psychopathic scores, suggesting that it is a biological marker of a manipulative and deceitful orientation in males. No association was found between SCRs and psychopathic personality in girls, suggesting sex-specific etiologies of these traits in childhood (Isen et al., Reference Isen, Raine, Baker, Dawson, Bezdjian and Lozano2010).

Atypical electrodermal and cardiovascular response patterns in psychopathic individuals are thought to be biological indicators of fearless and disinhibition. Larger heart rate acceleration and fewer non-specific SCRs (NS-SCRs) were found to be significantly associated with psychopathic traits during anticipation of signaled white-noise bursts when the twins were 9–10 years old. These findings suggest that autonomic deficits present in children at risk may predispose them to later psychopathy (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Baker, Gao, Raine and Lozano2012).

Relationships of ASB With Social Risk Factors

We also found a significant biosocial interaction, whereby risky decision making was associated with psychopathic tendencies, but only in children from benign (i.e., high socio-economic status), non-adverse home environments. This finding supports the biosocial interaction perspective, suggesting that risky decision making may particularly predispose psychopathic personality in children from benign home backgrounds (Gao et al., Reference Gao, Baker, Raine, Wu and Bezdjian2008).

Yet another study found that parenting has less impact on ASB among children who are high on psychopathic personality traits. Children low on psychopathy scores showed decreasing aggressive and conduct disorder symptoms as positive affect increased. Children high on psychopathy scores showed elevated aggressive and conduct disorder symptoms regardless of affect level. This interaction was not found for negative affect. Results imply that negative parenting detrimentally affects children's problem behavior regardless of existing psychopathic traits, while children high on psychopathy scores may be less responsive to ineffective parenting (Yeh et al., Reference Yeh, Jacobson, Baker and Raine2011).

We recently examined the direction and the genetic and environmental etiology of the association between negative parent-to-child affect and psychopathic personality from ages 9–10 to 14–16 years. Negative parent-to-child affect at ages 9–10 years influenced psychopathic personality at ages 14–16 years. Both genetically and environmentally mediated parent-driven effects were important in development of psychopathic personality. Psychopathic personality at ages 9–10 years influenced negative parent-to-child affect at ages 14–16 years. Thus, children's genetically influenced psychopathic personality seemed to evoke parental negativity at ages 14–16 years, These results illustrate the importance of investigating causal relationships in the development of psychopathic personality (Tuvblad et al., Reference Tuvblad, Gao, Isen, Botwick, Raine and Baker2012).

Stability and Change in ASB and Its Risk Factors

Genetic and environmental stability in reactive and proactive aggression was investigated when the twins were 9–10 and 11–13 years old. We found that the stability in reactive aggression was due to genetic and non-shared environmental influences, whereas the continuity in proactive aggression was primarily genetically mediated (Tuvblad et al., Reference Tuvblad, Raine, Zheng and Baker2009a).

The genetic and environmental stability of impulsivity was investigated using data from the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) in Waves 2 and 3. Univariate genetic analyses for the three sub-scales (inattention, motor, and non-planning impulsivities) yielded moderate heritability estimates of 38–55% at ages 11–13, and 31–37% at ages 14–15. Multivariate genetic analyses at each wave suggest a latent common impulsivity factor, with 72% of the stability between these two latent impulsivity factors is accounted for by genetic effects (Niv et al., Reference Niv, Tuvblad, Raine, Wang and Baker2012).

We also investigated longitudinal stability and change in motor impulsivity using errors of commission from the Go/NoGo task in Waves 1, 2, and 3. Latent growth curve analyses revealed variation in initial levels in motor impulsivity was predominantly due to genetic factors, while non-shared environmental factors also played a significant role. Change (slope) variation in motor impulsivity was due to both genetic and non-shared environmental factors (Bezdjian et al., Reference Bezdjian, Tuvblad, Wang, Raine and Baker2012). Taken together, the genetic influences on impulsive behavior appear to take effect early in (pre-adolescent) childhood, and non-shared environmental influences play an increasingly important role during adolescence. It appears that the temporal stability of impulsivity, both in NoGo errors and self-reported BIS scores, is due to early appearing genetic influences, while changes across adolescence are primarily due to unique individual environments.

We examined the genetic and environmental etiology of skin conductance orienting response (SCOR) at ages 9–10, 11–13, and 14–16 years. The SCOR is an autonomic response to novel stimuli and indirectly reflects how much a person attends to and processes novel stimuli in the environment (Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Schell, Filion, Cacioppo, Tassinary and Berntson2007). SCOR deficits have been linked to a variety of externalizing problems, including psychopathy, ASB, and ADHD (see Tuvblad et al., Reference Tuvblad, Bezdjian, Raine and Baker2012). Genetic influences explained 55%, 82%, and 48% of the total variance in SCOR at Waves 1, 2, and 3, respectively, with the remaining variance explained by non-shared environment. SCOR was moderately stable phenotypically across ages (r = .38 to .51). Genetic influences contributed between 7% and 30% to the stability of SCOR. The genetic correlations among the three waves were high, ranging between 0.53 and 0.90, indicating a substantial continuity in genetic influences from ages 9 to 16 (Tuvblad et al., in press).

Genetic and Environmental Bases of Biological Risk Factors

We examined the genetic and environmental etiology of the associations among respiratory sinus arrhythmia, heart rate, skin conductance level, and NS-SCRs. Nearly all of the genetic and environmental influences on these measures were accounted for by two latent factors, supporting the validity of separate sympathetic and parasympathetic constructs (Tuvblad et al., Reference Tuvblad, Isen, Baker, Raine, Lozano and Jacobson2010).

Resting frontal asymmetry and alpha power were examined when the twins were 9–10 years old. Results displayed a modest but significant amount of variance in frontal asymmetry accounted for by genetic factors (11–27%), with the remainder accounted for by non-shared environmental influences. Alpha power was highly heritable, with 70–85% of the variance accounted for by genetic factors (Gao et al., Reference Gao, Tuvblad, Raine, Lozano and Baker2009).

Identifying Twins for Future Studies

Public school districts are a valuable resource for future twin studies. Sort-match procedures of school district databases can be used to identify twins as two students with the same last name, birth date, and home address. We successfully used this method previously and found >99% accuracy identifying twins. Based on the average twinning birthrate in California from 1995 to 2007 (3% of all live births; http://multiples.about.com/od/funfacts/a/twinbirthrate.htm) and the recently published enrollments for grades K–12 in 2011–2012 (3,058,535 students; California Department of Education: http://data1.cde.ca.gov, accessed on 9/6/2012), we estimate the number of school-age twin pairs living in the five-county area of Greater Los Angeles to be over 45,000 pairs. This would provide a sufficiently large potential from which to draw future twin samples. Families of identified twins are generally sent letters (from district offices) inviting them to participate in studies, and those interested are asked to return postage-paid information forms or call us to register their twins for study. Based on past experience, we expect >30% return rate among identified twins. Additional efforts to identify twins within private schools, advertisements and referrals can be used to add more twin pairs beyond those from public schools, that is, at least another 15%. Using these same recruitment methods, we previously demonstrated success in forming a sample that is representative of the ethnic and socio-economic distribution in Southern California (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Barton and Raine2002, Reference Baker, Barton, Lozano, Raine and Fowler2006). As previously described (see Baker et al., Reference Baker, Barton, Lozano, Raine and Fowler2006), Los Angeles County birth records can also be used to identify twins from the target birth years, and these can be cross-matched to voter records to find recent addresses for either parents or the twins themselves (i.e., for age 18+ twins).

Technological developments in the last decade have made the recruitment of twins from large urban populations highly accessible with minimal resources. Computerized databases from schools and public records through state and county sources can yield large samples of twins interested in participating in research projects. We have successfully used these methods in the past to identify and recruit twins for research at USC, and our methods could be used to successfully recruit large samples for future research.

Acknowledgments

The RFAB was funded by NIMH (R01 MH58354). AR was supported by NIMH (Independent Scientist Award K02 MH01114-08). We thank the Southern California Twin Project staff for their assistance in recruiting twins and collecting data, as well as Fred Hodshon and Yaling Yang for their contributions to the graphic design in Figure 1. We are indebted to the twins and their families for their participation.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Baker, L. A., Barton, M., Lozano, D. I., Raine, A., & Fowler, J. H. (2006). The Southern California Twin Register at the University of Southern California: II. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 933940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, L. A., Barton, M., & Raine, A. (2002). The Southern California Twin Register at USC. Twin Research, 5, 456459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, L. A., Jacobson, K. C., Raine, A., D Lozano, D. I., & Bezdjian, S. (2007). Genetic and environmental bases of childhood antisocial behavior: A multi-informant twin study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 219235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, L. A., Tuvblad, C., Reynolds, C., Zheng, M., Lozano, D. I., & Raine, A. (2009). Resting heart rate and the development of antisocial behavior from age 9 to 14: Genetic and environmental influences. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 939960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The Empathy Quotient (EQ). An investigation of adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Functioning Autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bezdjian, S., Raine, A., Baker, L. A., & Lynam, D. R. (2010). Psychopathic personality in children: Genetic and environmental contributions. Psychological Medicine, 41, 589600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bezdjian, S., Tuvblad, C., Wang, P., Raine, A., & Baker, L. A. (2012). Motor impulsivity during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal biometric analysis of the Go/NoGo Task in 9- to 18-year-old twins. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Cherek, D. R., Moeller, F. G., Schnapp, W., & Dougherty, D. (1997). Studies of violent and nonviolent male parolees: I. Laboratory and psychometric measurements of aggression. Biological Psychiatry, 41, 514522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Filion, D. L. (2007). The electrodermal system. In Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G. & Berntson, G. G. (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 159181). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Deater-Deckard, K. (1996). Within family variability in parental negativity and control. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 17, 407422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliot, D. S., & Huizinga, D. (1988). Improving self-reported measures of delinquency. Paper presented at the 1988 NATO Workshop, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., Smith Benjamin, L., & Williams, J. B. W. (1997). User's guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Forth, A. E., Kosson, D. S., & Hare, R. D. (2003). Hare PCL: Youth version (PCL:YV). Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Frick, P. J., & Hare, R. (2001). The antisocial process screening device. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Gao, Y., Baker, L. A., Raine, A., Wu, H., & Bezdjian, S. (2008). Brief report: Interaction between social class and risky decision-making in children with psychopathic tendencies. Journal of Adolescence, 32, 409414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gao, Y., Tuvblad, C., Raine, A., Lozano, D. I., & Baker, L. A. (2009). Genetic and environmental influences on frontal EEG asymmetry and alpha power in 9-10 year old twins. Psychophysiology, 46, 787796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, D. A., & Berg, E. A. (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 404411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halperin, J. M., Wolf, L. E., Greenblatt, E. R., & Young, G. (1991). Subtype analysis of commission errors on the continuous performance test in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7, 207217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, R. D. (1965). Temporal gradient of fear arousal in psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 70, 442445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, R. D. (1978). Psychopathy and electrodermal responses to a nonsignal stimulation. Biological Psychiatry, 6, 237246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, R. D. (2003). Hare PCL-R (2nd ed.). Toronto: Multi-Health Systems, Inc.Google Scholar
Hollingshead, A. G. (1979). Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of Socio-Economic Status. Unpublished Manuscript, Yale University, USA.Google Scholar
Isen, J., Raine, A., Baker, L., Dawson, M., Bezdjian, S., & Lozano, D. I. (2010). Sex-specific association between psychopathic traits an electrodermal reactivity in children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jern, S., Pilhall, M., Jern, C., & Carlsson, S. G. (1991). Short-term reproducibility of a mental arithmetic stress test. Clinical Sciences, 81, 593601.Google ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Gardner, C. O., & Lichtenstein, P. (2009). A developmental twin study of symptoms of anxiety and depression: Evidence for genetic innovation and attenuation. Psychological Medicine, 38, 15671575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2000). What parents know, how they know it, and several forms of adolescent adjustment: Further support for a reinterpretation of monitoring. Developmental Psychology, 36, 366380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiehl, K. A., Hare, R. D., Liddle, P. F., & McDonald, J. J. (1999). Reduced P300 responses in criminal psychopaths during a visual oddball task. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 14981507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P. J., & Hnatiow, M. (1962). Stimulus repetition and the heart rate response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 781785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lykken, D. T. (1957). A study of anxiety in the sociopathic personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 55, 610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lykken, D. T. (1978). The diagnosis of zygosity in twins. Behavior Genetics 8, 437473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynam, D. R. (1997). Pursuing the psychopath: Capturing the fledgling psychopath in a nomological net. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 424438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynam, D. (2002). Fledgling psychopathy: A view from personality theory. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 255259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNair, D. M., Frankenthaler, L. M., Czerlinsky, T., White, T. W., Sasson, S., & Fisher, S. (1982). Simulated public speaking as a model of clinical anxiety. Psychopharmacology, 77, 710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morey, L. (1991). The personality assessment inventory: Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Niv, S., Tuvblad, C., Raine, A., Wang, P., & Baker, L. A. (2012). Heritability and longitudinal stability of impulsivity in adolescence. Behavior Genetics, 42, 378392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotion in the criminal psychopath: Startle reflex modulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 8292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pokorny, A. D., Miller, B. A., & Kaplan, H. B. (1972). The brief MAST: A shortened version of the Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test. American Journal of Psychiatry, 129, 342345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pomerleau, O. F., Pomerleau, C. S., & Namenek, R. J. (1998). Early experiences with tobacco among women smokers, ex-smokers, and never-smokers. Addiction, 93, 595599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porges, S. W. (1995). Orienting in a defensive world: Mammalian modifications of our revolutionary heritage. A polyvagal theory. Psychophysiology, 32, 301318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porteus, S. D. (1965). Porteus maze test: Fifty years’ application. Palo Alto, CA: Pacific Books Publishers.Google Scholar
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raine, A., Dodge, K., Loeber, R., Gatzke-Kopp, L., Lynam, D., Reynolds, C., . . . Liu, J. (2006). The Reactive-Proactive Aggression Questionnaire: Differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 159171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A., Venables, P. H., & Williams, M. (1990). Autonomic orienting responses in 15-year-old male subjects and criminal behavior at age 24. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 933937.Google ScholarPubMed
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2004). The Trail Making Test as an initial screening procedure for neuropsychological impairment in older children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 281288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaffer, D., Fisher, P., Dulcan, M. K., Davies, M., Piacentini, J., Schwab-Stone, M. E., . . . Regier, D. A. (2000). NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): Description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 2838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Finkelhor, D., Moore, D. W., & Runyan, D. (1998). Identification of child maltreatment with the parent-child conflict tactics scales: Development and psychometric data for a national sample of american parents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 249270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuvblad, C., Bezdjian, S., Raine, A., & Baker, L. (in press). Psychopathic personality and negative parent-to-child affect: A longitudinal cross-lag twin study. Developmental Psychology.Google Scholar
Tuvblad, C., Gao, Y., Isen, J., Botwick, T., Raine, A., & Baker, L. A. (2012). The heritability of the skin conductance orienting response: A longitudinal twin study. Biological Psychology, 89, 4753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuvblad, C., Isen, J., Baker, L. A., Raine, A., Lozano, D. I., & Jacobson, K. C. (2010). The genetic and environmental etiology of sympathetic and parasympathetic activity in children. Behavior Genetics, 40, 452466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuvblad, C., Raine, A., Zheng, M., & Baker, L. A. (2009a). The genetic and environmental stability differs in reactive and proactive aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 35, 437452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuvblad, C., Zheng, M., Raine, A., & Baker, L. A. (2009b). A common genetic factor explains the covariation among ADHD, CD, and ODD symptoms in 9-10 year old boys and girls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 153167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, P., Baker, L. A., Gao, Y., Raine, A., & Lozano, D. I. (2012). Psychopathic traits and physiological responses to aversive stimuli in children aged 9–11 years. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 759769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R. W., & Mather, N. (1989). Woodcock Johnson — Revised test of achievement: Examiner's manual. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar
Yeh, M., Jacobson, K., Baker, L. A., & Raine, A. (2011). Negative parenting and antisocial behavior A twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 50, 10541064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

FIGURE 1 The USC Longitudinal Twin Study of Risk Factors for Antisocial Behavior (RFAB): Design, Constructs and Informants

Figure 1

TABLE 1 Number of Families of Twins Participating in Waves 1–4, and Projected for Wave 5

Figure 2

TABLE 2 Key Psychopathology Measures in Twins in USC-RFAB Across Waves 1–5

Figure 3

TABLE 3 Measures of Social and Biological Risk Factors for Externalizing Behavior Problems of Twins in USC-RFAB across Waves 1–5