Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T17:51:51.821Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Backlash after Quotas: Moral Panic as a Soft Repression Tactic against Women Politicians

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 February 2024

Myriam Shiran*
Affiliation:
Postdoctoral Research Associate, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, United States
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Advocates of gender quotas emphasize their transformative potential for women’s political participation. Yet evidence on the symbolic effects of quotas remains inconclusive, with some studies uncovering significant backlash after implementation. Although elite resistance to quotas has been posited as an explanation, the underlying mechanisms generating negative effects remain underexplored. This study proposes the utilization of “moral panic” by elites as a mechanism of resistance. By leveraging their media influence and employing conservative moral rhetoric, elites engineer moral panic, framing women’s political engagement as detrimental to social order and gender hierarchies. Such panic aims to incite public opposition and rationalize elite resistance to progressive changes. Notably, this tactic is more prevalent in countries with reserved-seat quotas, where elites possess limited control over electoral outcomes. Using an original dataset encompassing politician names and genders, I analyze over 150,000 news articles from 2000 to 2021 across 10 sub-Saharan African countries. The findings indicate that quota-induced gains in women’s representation are followed by significant increases in conservative ethical language in news coverage of women politicians, particularly in countries with reserved-seat quotas. These findings bear important implications for gender equality in politics and shed light on the dynamics of backlash after quota implementation.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Women, Gender, and Politics Research Section of the American Political Science Association

Introduction

Electoral gender quotas are celebrated as one of the most extensive electoral reforms worldwide aimed at bolstering women’s presence in legislative bodies, with more than 130 countries currently implementing some type of quota (Hughes et al. Reference Hughes, Paxton, Clayton and Zetterberg2019). Scholars argue that higher political inclusion of women could have positive attitudinal and behavioral (or symbolic) effects by reducing bias against women’s leadership capability, legitimizing them as political actors, and improving the political engagement of women in society (Franceschet, Krook, and Piscopo Reference Franceschet, Krook, Piscopo, Franceschet and Piscopo2012; O’Brien and Piscopo Reference O’Brien, Piscopo, Franceschet, Krook and Tan2019; Pitkin Reference Pitkin1967; Tripp, Konaté, and Lowe-Morna Reference Tripp, Konaté, Lowe-Morna and Dahlerup2006).

However, the nascent literature on the symbolic effects of quotas presents a conflicting picture. Although some evidence supports the positive symbolic effects of quotas (Beaman et al. Reference Beaman, Chattopadhyay, Duflo, Pande and Topalova2009; Burnet Reference Burnet2011; De Paola, Scoppa, and Lombardo Reference De Paola, Scoppa and Lombardo2010; Yoon Reference Yoon2011), several studies have found that quotas have had minimal or no effects in terms of reducing gender bias or improving engagement among women (Clayton Reference Clayton2015; Kerevel and Atkeson Reference Kerevel and Atkeson2017; Kittilson and Schwindt-Bayer Reference Kittilson and Schwindt-Bayer2012; Lloren Reference Lloren2014; Zetterberg Reference Zetterberg2009). In some cases, backlash effects have emerged after the implementation of quotas (Burnet Reference Burnet2011; Kerevel and Atkeson Reference Kerevel and Atkeson2017), normalizing “new forms of oppression” against women (Berry, Bouka, and Kamuru Reference Berry, Bouka and Kamuru2020, 16).

This backlash can have dire consequences, from eroding public perception of women’s leadership abilities to fueling violence against women politicians, diminishing women’s interest in politics, and hindering broader gender role transformations (Berry, Bouka, and Kamuru Reference Berry, Bouka and Kamuru2020; Krook and Sanín Reference Krook and Sanín2016; O’Brien and Piscopo Reference O’Brien, Piscopo, Franceschet, Krook and Tan2019). Some scholars suggest that backlash is generated due to the resistance of elites against transformations brought about by quota reforms and women’s growing foray into politics that threaten their power and privileges. These elites leverage cultural mechanisms to maintain the political status quo (Krook Reference Krook2016). These mechanisms, however, remain understudied and give rise to calls for further research (Krook Reference Krook2016; O’Brien and Piscopo Reference O’Brien, Piscopo, Franceschet, Krook and Tan2019).

Using insights from moral psychology — specifically moral foundations theory — and media studies, I argue that elites leverage the cultural mechanism of moral panic to counter the transformative impacts of quotas (Cohen Reference Cohen1991; Krook Reference Krook2016). Through their definitional power and influence over media, dominant actors such as conservative civil society groups (Larok Reference Larok2018) employ moral rhetoric to accuse women politicians of violating conservative values, subverting gender hierarchies, and endangering social order (Berry, Bouka, and Kamuru Reference Berry, Bouka and Kamuru2020; Biroli and Caminotti Reference Biroli and Caminotti2020; Faludi Reference Faludi1991; Payne and de Souza Santos Reference Payne and de Souza Santos2020).

Elite moral rhetoric has a significant influence on people’s opinion and attitudes (Clifford and Jerit Reference Clifford and Jerit2013) and thus motivates the public’s opposition against progressive policies and women’s political participation by engendering fear and outrage. The moral panic, therefore, serves to undermine the effectiveness of quotas and justifies the elite’s resistance to changes (Krook Reference Krook2016). Moreover, this rhetoric reinforces existing gender norms and values. Therefore, I anticipate an increase in the salience of conservative moral language in news coverage of women politicians as more women enter politics following quota implementation.

However, there are different types of quota systems — reserved-seat, voluntary party, and legislated candidate — that are adopted under different political, institutional, and international contexts and provide elites with varying levels of control over electoral outcomes. I argue that elites are more likely to leverage moral panic in countries where they have less control over the electoral outcomes. I investigate this expectation by focusing on reserved-seat and party quota models that provide elites with the least and most control to manipulate electoral outcomes (David and Nanes Reference David and Nanes2011; Krook, O’Brien, and Swip Reference Krook, O’Brien and Swip2010; Rosen Reference Rosen2017; Tamale Reference Tamale and Ballington2003). I expect women politicians in countries with reserved seat quotas to face stronger backlash and therefore the salience of conservative moral rhetoric to be higher compared to countries with party quotas.

Focusing on 10 countries in sub-Saharan Africa, I explore my theory by analyzing the association between the salience of conservative moral language in news coverage of women politicians and quota-induced increases in the descriptive representation of women. Using an original dataset on the gender of politicians in these countries, I obtain more than 150,000 newspaper articles published between 2000 to 2021 and extract conservative moral language from them using automated content analysis. Then I pair the moral language measure of each article with the gender of the politician discussed in the article, country-level data on women’s descriptive representation, and quota implementation in a cross-national and longitudinal design.

The results confirm my expectations. Findings indicate that quota-induced gains in women’s representation are followed by significant increases in the conservative moral language in news coverage of women politicians. This effect, however, is influenced by the type of quota system. In countries with reserved-seat quotas, there is a higher proportion of conservative language in the elite’s rhetoric about women politicians compared to countries with party quotas.

The present study contributes to the literature on the backlash against women politicians following the implementation of gender quotas in two ways. Firstly, it identifies elite-engineered moral panic as a mechanism of resistance against women’s political participation. By creating moral panic through the strategic use of moralizing rhetoric, elites evoke negative emotions among the public (Ryan Reference Ryan2014, 380), thereby undermining the potential behavioral and attitudinal effects of quotas. Secondly, this study provides a framework for future research to explore similar mechanisms of resistance in different contexts and regions.

Symbolic Representation and Gender Quotas

Throughout history, women’s exclusion from politics has resulted in their underrepresentation and the perception of their lacking leadership capabilities (Mansbridge Reference Mansbridge1999). Symbolic representation scholars argue that increasing gender parity in politics is essential to challenge this perception and generate a “social meaning” of women’s governing capacity (Mansbridge Reference Mansbridge1999). A higher political inclusion of women is expected to challenge gendered perspectives about politics (Burnet Reference Burnet2011; Karp and Banducci Reference Karp and Banducci2008; Mariani, Marshall, and Mathews-Schultz Reference Mariani, Marshall and Mathews-Schultz2015), improve beliefs about their leadership capabilities (Alexander Reference Alexander2012; Franceschet, Krook, and Piscopo Reference Franceschet, Krook, Piscopo, Franceschet and Piscopo2012), and transform traditional gender roles and norms in society into more egalitarian ones (Inglehart and Norris Reference Inglehart and Norris2003; Mansbridge Reference Mansbridge1999). It also gives existence to and constructs the women constituency (Bourdieu Reference Bourdieu1991; Lombardo and Meier Reference Lombardo and Meier2014).

To address the underrepresentation of women in politics, electoral gender quotas have gained worldwide recognition as an accelerated pathway for improving gender parity (Hughes et al. Reference Hughes, Paxton, Clayton and Zetterberg2019). Quota policies require a specific proportion of legislative seats or electoral lists to be allocated to women and have been successful in bolstering women’s presence in political bodies (O’Brien and Piscopo Reference O’Brien, Piscopo, Franceschet, Krook and Tan2019). The implementation of quotas has had a significant impact on the diversification of national legislatures worldwide, with the representation of women rapidly increasing from 13% to 25% over the past 20 years (Clayton Reference Clayton2021), although the success of quotas varies depending on their type. Notably, in Africa, the percentage of women in the legislatures of 15 countries (out of 48 worldwide) has reached 30% or more (Bauer Reference Bauer, Yacob-Haliso and Falola2021).

However, the evidence regarding the symbolic effects of quota reforms is inconclusive and, at times, contradictory. Although scholars have argued that quota reforms can “reshape attitudes, values, and ideas towards women’s roles in politics” (Kittilson Reference Kittilson2005, 29), the impact varies. Some studies have found general proof of positive behavioral and attitudinal effects, such as women seeking political power in other formerly traditionally male-dominated positions like chiefs in Botswana (Bauer Reference Bauer2016; Matemba Reference Matemba2005) and improved perceptions of women as political actors in Rwanda (Burnet Reference Burnet2011), Indian villages (Beaman et al. Reference Beaman, Chattopadhyay, Duflo, Pande and Topalova2009) and municipalities in Italy (De Paola, Scoppa, and Lombardo Reference De Paola, Scoppa and Lombardo2010).

However, several studies have found limited or no evidence of the quotas’ positive symbolic effect in terms of political engagement, efficacy, or evaluations of women’s capability to lead (Clayton Reference Clayton2018; Kittilson and Schwindt-Bayer Reference Kittilson and Schwindt-Bayer2012; Zetterberg Reference Zetterberg2009). In some cases, implementing quotas has resulted in a backlash. For instance, political engagement among women decreased following the implementation of gender quotas in Mexico (Kerevel and Atkeson Reference Kerevel and Atkeson2017). Similarly in Lesotho, women in districts reserved for female representatives were significantly less engaged with politics, and quota adoption created a backlash effect against women elected through affirmative action (Clayton Reference Clayton2015). The backlash effect in Rwanda manifested itself in the form of increased marital discord and resentment among men, with some perceiving that gender role upheaval undermined the institution of marriage (Burnet Reference Burnet2011). Similarly in Kenya, women politicians experienced a “patriarchal backlash” after quota implementation, leading to the emergence of new forms of oppression and violence against women in politics (Berry, Bouka, and Kamuru Reference Berry, Bouka and Kamuru2020).

The significant implications of backlash for the success of quota policies highlight the need to comprehend the underlying mechanisms through which it manifests in society. However, the existing evidence on conservative backlash is limited and predominantly relies on single-country case studies that do not investigate its underlying mechanism (Berry, Bouka, and Kamuru Reference Berry, Bouka and Kamuru2020; Matfess, Kishi, and Berry Reference Matfess, Kishi and Berry2022; Muhammad, Awan, and Hussain Reference Muhammad, Awan and Hussain2020). Drawing on moral psychology, media studies, and theories of backlash, this study adopts a comparative and longitudinal approach to present a more nuanced understanding of the underlying mechanisms of backlash against women politicians after the implementation of gender quotas.

Transformative Effects of Quotas and Backlash

Scholars have conceptualized backlash as the “dynamic resistance” (Mansbridge and Shames Reference Mansbridge and Shames2008) of dominant groups who experience a decline in their power (Lipset and Raab Reference Lipset and Raab1978, 29) due to the “intrusion” and challenge of historically excluded and marginalized actors. Backlash seeks to protect elite interests, values, and status by diminishing the real or perceived gains of marginalized groups and restoring social conditions to their prior state when the “do-not-cross” boundary of existing power hierarchies had not been breached and structural inequality was normalized (Alter and Zürn Reference Alter and Zürn2020; Flood, Dragiewicz, and Pease Reference Flood, Dragiewicz and Pease2021: Rowley Reference Rowley2020; Townsend-Bell Reference Townsend-Bell2020).

Quota reforms and the subsequent empowerment of women in politics can be perceived as a threat by the elites who benefit from existing power arrangements (Franceschet, Krook, and Piscopo Reference Franceschet, Krook, Piscopo, Franceschet and Piscopo2012; Krook Reference Krook2016; O’Brien and Piscopo Reference O’Brien, Piscopo, Franceschet, Krook and Tan2019). These policies transform the political landscape by reforming political structures and increasing women’s access to leadership positions within political parties and the executive branch. These positions enable women elected through quotas to exert influence over policymaking, engage in negotiations, and foster collaborations with other lawmakers. Countries with electoral gender quotas are more likely to see the emergence of women’s legislative caucuses, which provide a platform for engaging with feminist civil society and driving legislative reforms on women’s rights issues. Additionally, the implementation of gender quotas disrupts traditional networks and selection criteria that have historically favored men over women (Adams, Scherpereel, and Wylie Reference Adams, Scherpereel and Wylie2019; Barnes Reference Barnes2012; Franceschet and Piscopo Reference Franceschet and Piscopo2008; Zetterberg Reference Zetterberg2013).

In reaction to these challenges to the political status quo and in an attempt to protect their privileges, elites seek to delegitimize women as political actors, keep them “subordinated within the political system” (Berry, Bouka, and Kamuru Reference Berry, Bouka and Kamuru2020, 19), and even force them to withdraw from the public sphere of politics (Krook Reference Krook2015). Backlash may take different forms. The elites may intentionally challenge or refuse to comply with affirmative action policies (Lang Reference Lang2015; Lee and Shin Reference Lee and Shin2016), resort to overt force such as violence or threats of violence against women politicians (Berry, Bouka, and Kamuru Reference Berry, Bouka and Kamuru2020; Matfess, Kishi, and Berry Reference Matfess, Kishi and Berry2022) or employ soft repression tactics (Faree Reference Faree, Davenport, Johnston and Mueller2005; Mansbridge and Shames Reference Mansbridge and Shames2008). Soft repression involves the use of cultural mechanisms to repress or exclude collective identities, such as women politicians, or ideas that challenge dominant groups in society, such as gender parity in politics (Faree Reference Faree, Davenport, Johnston and Mueller2005; Mansbridge and Shames Reference Mansbridge and Shames2008).

Backlash and Moral Panic

One of the soft repression tactics that elites utilize when faced with a “crisis of hegemony” that challenges the legitimacy of the dominant ideology in society is moral panic (Hall et al. Reference Hall, Critcher, Jefferson, Clarke and Roberts2013). Moral panic happens when a social condition or a group of people is portrayed as a threat to societal values and interests by the political discourse and the media, while “moral barricades are manned by editors, bishops, politicians and other right-thinking people” (Cohen Reference Cohen2011, 1). In other words, elites respond to perceived threats against their power and hegemony by creating a moral panic against the challenging groups using their definitional power and influence over media that allows them to control the debate around controversial issues (Hall et al. Reference Hall, Critcher, Jefferson, Clarke and Roberts2013).

Using moral rhetoric, the elites portray the challenging groups as deviants who have violated the official morality of the society and are therefore detrimental to conservative values, norms, and social order (Becker Reference Becker1967; Cohen Reference Cohen1991). Moral panic is not necessarily a genuine panic, and one of its features is the small or negligible likelihood of social harm compared to the response of the dominant groups. The moral panic serves to orchestrate public opinion by creating negative emotions of fear and anger, and even inciting violence. It therefore justifies the disproportionate reaction of elites to a challenge that has risen due to serious structural problems that resolving them would undermine the dominant hegemony (Cohen Reference Cohen1991; Goode and Ben-Yehuda Reference Goode and Ben-Yehuda1994; Hall et al. Reference Hall, Critcher, Jefferson, Clarke and Roberts2013; McRobbie and Thornton Reference McRobbie and Thornton1995).

During the “mugging crisis” in the early 1970s in the US, a moral panic was created by the dominant groups including the police and government that portrayed the black community as the cause of increasing social problems. This moral panic allowed the elites to suppress a crisis of legitimacy through adopting increasingly harsh and authoritarian measures (Hall et al. Reference Hall, Critcher, Jefferson, Clarke and Roberts2013). Similarly, in Europe, a moral panic surrounding Middle Eastern immigrants led to the normalization of racism and provided justification for implementing restrictive immigration policies (Krzyżanowski Reference Krzyżanowski2020).

In the context of gender quotas and women’s presence in politics, dominant actors employ moral panic to depict women politicians and progressive policies as violating the official morality of society, traditional values, and institutions. These actors consist of elites such as male legislators as well as conservative civil society groups like religious fundamentalist societies that have been on the rise over the past few decades and have influence over media (Biroli and Caminotti Reference Biroli and Caminotti2020; Fowler et al. Reference Fowler, Youngs, Larok, Marczewski, Vijayan, Nodia, Shapovalova, Sombatpoonsiri, von Bülow and Zihnioğlu2018; Panchaud et al. Reference Panchaud, Keogh, Stillman, Awusabo-Asare, Motta, Sidze and Silvia Monzón2019; Pousadela and Perera Reference Pousadela and Perera2021). Furthermore, in African countries, the media landscape is largely controlled by the state or the political elites and serves dominant interests and ideology. In the case of Zimbabwe, for instance, the ruling party ZANU-PF controls Zimbabwe Broadcasting Corporation and did not issue new broadcasting licenses in the country (Mano Reference Mano, Flew, Iosifidis and Steemers2016; Sparks Reference Sparks2009).

The example of Uganda’s fiercely contested Marriage and Divorce Bill — aimed at regulating marriage, separation, and divorce — illustrates the strategic use of moral panic. In the highly publicized 2013 debate over the bill, an alliance comprising religious institutions, traditionalists, and proponents of the patriarchy, led by male legislators, successfully defeated the bill. These groups, such as the Uganda Joint Christian Council and the Inter-Religious Council of Uganda, argued that it would disrupt traditional gender roles and family values, promote sexual promiscuity, and tarnish the sanctity of marriage.Footnote 1 The women politicians sponsoring the bill were branded prostitutes seeking to “spoil the institution of marriage so that everyone would become a prostitute” (Larok Reference Larok2018, 160). This rhetoric incited nation-wide opposition and protests against the bill, resulting in its defeat.

Women politicians themselves are often accused by their male rivals and in the news reports of violating gendered norms and values such as modesty and sexual propriety, disrespecting their husbands, and disregarding their families as well as their duties as wives and mothers once they enter politics. For instance, when a well-known Kenyan politician Gladys Boss Shollei got a divorce, the reports, apart from covering all the gory details of the scandal with sensationalized titles such as “politics and marriage make a highly volatile mix for women,” blamed her political career as the reason for the breakdown of her marriage and questioned Shollei’s leadership capability as she had failed to “nurture a family.”Footnote 2 In another article that was widely circulated online, the husband of a woman politician lamented allowing his wife to go into politics as she had become a prostitute and had illicit relationships with her male colleagues.Footnote 3

As people seldom have direct contact with politicians, the public’s perceptions about them are predominantly shaped through mediated exposure (Bligh et al. Reference Bligh, Schlehofer, Casad and Gaffney2012; Chong and Druckman Reference Chong and Druckman2007; Reese, Gandy, and Grant Reference Reese, Gandy and Grant2001). The nature of this mediation, as documented by a robust scholarship, is gendered and exposes existing power dynamics in society (Aaldering and Van Der Pas Reference Aaldering and Van Der Pas2018; Courtney et al. Reference Courtney, Breen, McGing, McMenamin, O’Malley and Rafter2020; Gidengil and Everitt Reference Gidengil and Everitt1999, Reference Gidengil and Everitt2003; Goodyear-Grant Reference Goodyear-Grant2009). A comprehensive meta-analysis of 90 studies on gendered political news coverage confirms the presence of gender bias. Women politicians often face heightened scrutiny of their appearance and personal lives, receive more negative assessments of their viability, and encounter a greater emphasis on gender stereotypes in relation to their traits and policy issues (Van der Pas and Aaldering Reference Van der Pas and Aaldering2020).

This mediated exposure, influenced by dominant actors with definitional power, plays a crucial role in the construction of symbolic representation. The effect of symbolic representation resides in its capacity to evoke “emotional, affective, irrational psychological response” (Pitkin Reference Pitkin1967, 100). A symbol is politically constructed and associated with certain representative meanings by shaping social habits and practices through manipulating the emotions and attitudes of the public (Lombardo and Meier Reference Lombardo and Meier2014; Pitkin Reference Pitkin1967). Consequently, this process perpetuates patterns of domination and inequality, reinforcing privileged and marginalized positions within society (Connell Reference Connell2002; Dovi Reference Dovi2007).

From this perspective, the symbolic effect of women’s representation and the social meaning that their political presence generates largely depends on the affective responses that people have been trained to experience upon exposure to women politicians who “stand for” women citizens in general (Lombardo and Meier Reference Lombardo and Meier2014; Meier Reference Meier, Franceschet, Krook and Piscopo2012; Pitkin Reference Pitkin1967). Research indicates that the moral rhetoric of elites can strongly influence individuals’ opinions and attitudes by eliciting powerful emotions (Clifford et al. Reference Clifford, Jerit, Rainey and Motyl2015; Clifford and Jerit Reference Clifford and Jerit2013; Graham and Haidt Reference Graham, Haidt, Mikulincer and Shaver2012; Tamborini et al. Reference Tamborini, Prabhu, Lewis, Grizzard and Eden2018).

Through the use of moral rhetoric, therefore, elites can construct or reinforce the connection between a symbol and its meaning. Consequently, by generating a moral panic against women politicians and moralizing women’s presence in politics, elites establish a link between women’s political representation and either the violation or the necessity of upholding conservative values. The moralization of an issue often bears negative consequences as it has the potential to elicit strong negative emotions, provoke hostile attitudes, and lead to punitive actions (Kraaijeveld and Jamrozik Reference Kraaijeveld and Jamrozik2022; Ryan Reference Ryan2014). In this study, I employ moral foundations theory to measure the presence of conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of women politicians.

Moral Foundations of Moral Panic

Moral foundations theory posits that people’s conceptions of morality have evolved as mental systems along five sets of intuitions or foundations throughout history in response to “adaptive challenges” facing communities (Graham et al. Reference Graham2013, 67). These foundations fall into five categories and each category is divided into violation (vice) and upholding (virtue) dimensions: (1) care/harm, (2) fairness/cheating, (3) loyalty/betrayal, (4) authority/subversion, and (5) sanctity/degradation. For instance, the foundation of loyalty/betrayal is developed in response to the challenge of creating cohesion within the group by suppressing individual selfishness, and authority/subversion in response to the challenge of forming and maintaining social hierarchies (Graham et al. Reference Graham2013; Graham, Haidt, and Nosek Reference Graham, Haidt and Nosek2009).

The first two (individualizing) foundations emphasize individual rights, avoidance of harm, fairness, and equality. Prioritizing these foundations is associated with support for progressive policies, social justice, and equality. In this paper, I focus on the three binding foundations of loyalty, authority, and sanctity. These foundations “bind people into roles, duties, and mutual obligations” (Graham et al. Reference Graham, Nosek, Haidt, Iyer, Koleva and Ditto2011, 368) and serve to strengthen social groups and institutions by promoting attitudes that uphold traditional social order, values, and norms. Loyalty is associated with prioritizing and self-sacrificing for the group; authority pertains to respecting long-standing traditions and established social hierarchy as well as complying with official authority; and sanctity is related to protecting physical and spiritual purity, such as observance of traditional religious values and sexual propriety.

Research consistently indicates a strong association between prioritizing binding moral foundations with conservativism (Day et al. Reference Day, Fiske, Downing and Trail2014). Conservatives are more averse to change and more concerned with preserving traditional social order and hierarchies, and therefore have a higher tolerance for inequality and discrimination (Jost et al. Reference Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski and Sulloway2003). In making moral judgments, conservatives tend to prioritize the foundations of loyalty, respect for authority, and sanctity (Haidt and Graham Reference Haidt and Graham2007; van Leeuwen and Park Reference van Leeuwen and Park2009), which promote conservative values such as group cohesion and self-sacrifice and respect for authority. Endorsing binding foundations can perpetuate and justify patterns of discrimination and inequality, particularly impacting marginalized social groups like women (Hoover et al. Reference Hoover, Atari, Mostafazadeh Davani, Kennedy, Portillo-Wightman, Yeh and Dehghani2021; Janoff-Bulman and Carnes Reference Janoff-Bulman, Carnes and Zia2016; Malka et al. Reference Malka, Osborne, Soto, M. Greaves, Sibley and Lelkes2016; Strupp-Levitsky et al. Reference Strupp-Levitsky, Noorbaloochi, Shipley, Jost and Wisneski2010).

Moreover, endorsing binding moral foundations is associated with sexist attitudes that maintain traditional gender roles and hierarchy (Vecina and Piñuela Reference Vecina and Piñuela2017). The authority foundation, for example, contributes to the belief that men should be leaders in society, while women should be subservient to men and focus on their traditional roles as mothers and caregivers (Lakoff Reference Lakoff2014). Women politicians, therefore, are perceived as violating the foundation of authority and loyalty as they prioritize the pursuit of political power over their families (Harper and Harris Reference Harper and Harris2017). Unsurprisingly, higher regard for binding foundations is also correlated with more negative attitudes toward women in managerial positions (Baldner and Pierro Reference Baldner and Pierro2019), as powerful women are perceived as subverting gender hierarchies and threatening the status quo (Brescoll, Okimoto, and Vial Reference Brescoll, Okimoto and Vial2018).

Additionally, the sanctity foundation contributes to the belief that women’s bodies should be controlled and protected in private spheres, and those who engage in “immoral” behaviors deserve punishment. In news coverage of women politicians, women politicians are accused of violating the sanctity foundation by consorting with their male counterparts, rendering sexual favors, and “opening their legs to men” for political gain.Footnote 4 Moreover, they are encouraged to respect the traditional gendered norms and hierarchies, be “humble” wives, and treat their husbands as the “boss” of the household.Footnote 5

By invoking binding moral values in their rhetoric, elites activate conservative-relevant moral intuitions in individuals and shape the attitudes of people by tapping into deeply held moral beliefs and values (Clifford and Jerit Reference Clifford and Jerit2013). Hence, moral rhetoric can serve as a powerful tool in constructing symbolic representation as it can evoke an emotional response from individuals (Brescoll, Okimoto, and Vial Reference Brescoll, Okimoto and Vial2018) and reinforce the connection between symbols and their meaning. In the case of moral panic against women politicians, the conservative rhetoric connects women’s presence in politics to the violation or necessity of upholding conservative values and norms in society.

In conclusion, moral foundations theory provides a useful framework for measuring conservative moral language in news articles. Analyzing the salience of binding moral foundations of loyalty, authority, and sanctity allows us to understand how often conservative values are invoked in news coverage of women politicians as compared to their male counterparts.

To recapitulate, I argue that elites perceive quota reforms and gains in women empowerment as a threat to their power and use their influence over media to engineer a moral panic against women politicians using conservative moral rhetoric to portray them as endangering social order and values. This rhetoric may discourage women from engaging in politics, undermine their legitimacy as political actors, and overall impede the descriptive, substantive, and symbolic success of quota reforms. Furthermore, moral panic may increase violence against women politicians by creating the perception that they are endangering society by violating conservative moral values. Research indicates that people support harsher punitive measures when social order and collective norms — such as group cohesion, traditional hierarchies, and adherence to moral values — are perceived to be threatened by the actions of individuals (Silver and Silver Reference Silver and Silver2017). Based on these, I propose the following hypothesis:

Hypothesis A: An increase in the descriptive representation of women is associated with a higher salience of conservative moral rhetoric in the news coverage of women politicians, conditional on the adoption of gender quotas.

Backlash and Type of Quota

There are different types of quota policies — including reserved-seat, legislated candidate, and party quota models — that provide elites with varying levels of control over implementing quotas provisions (Darhour and Dahlerup Reference Darhour and Dahlerup2013; Krook Reference Krook2007). Reserved-seat quotas allocate a specific number of legislative seats to women by law and ensure the electoral outcome (Dahlerup Reference Dahlerup2005). Legislated candidate and party quotas target the gender composition of candidate lists, with the difference that the former is binding for all parties, whereas the latter is voluntarily adopted at the parties’ discretion.

Reserved-seat models are typically adopted in a top-down manner and imposed on elites when a strong executive seeks to maintain their strategic interests and dominance, or in response to pressure from international actors rather than as a measure to address the under-representation of women (Chowdhury Reference Chowdhury2002; Darhour Reference Darhour, Darhour and Dahlerup2020; Krook Reference Krook2006; Muriaas and Wang Reference Muriaas and Wang2012). Party quotas, in contrast, are often presented as political parties’ commitment to promoting gender equality. However, scholars argue that the decision to adopt party quotas is driven by various factors, including pressure from the electorate, a desire to improve the party’s electability by attracting support from women voters (Tripp and Kang Reference Tripp and Kang2008), and an attempt to consolidate their power in relation to other candidates while maintaining control over the nomination process (Baldez Reference Baldez2004). Furthermore, evidence indicates that in single-party dominant countries such as South Africa and Namibia, the implementation of party quotas can lead to a significant increase in women’s presence in the legislature similar to the reserved seat model.

Elites in countries with party quotas have the ability to utilize informal tactics, such as allocating women to unwinnable positions, manipulating election outcomes, and maintaining existing power dynamics, even with the presence of quotas (Verge and Troupel Reference Verge and Troupel2011). In contrast, elites in countries with reserved-seat models do not have access to such strategies. As a result, I argue that elites in reserved-seat model countries are more likely to perceive quota reforms as a threat to their privileges. To counter this perceived threat, they may employ soft repression tactics, such as generating moral panic, to mobilize the public against women’s political participation. Based on this analysis, I propose the second hypothesis:

Hypothesis B: An increase in the number of women in parliaments with a reserved-seat model is more likely to be associated with a higher proportion of conservative moral foundations in news coverage of women politicians.

Data and Method

Investigating the two hypotheses of the study (A and B) requires capturing conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of women politicians, which I measure by applying automated content analysis of moral foundations in news articles. In the following sections, I explain the sample selection for the study and summarize the process of collecting news articles and measuring conservative moral language in them. Then I explain the dependent and independent variables as well as the method of analysis.

Sample Selection

In this study, I focus on 10 selected countries in sub-Saharan Africa, including Nigeria, Namibia, Zimbabwe, Zambia, Uganda, Ghana, Rwanda, South Africa, Tanzania, and Kenya. These countries were selected based on several key considerations, including the variation in the percentage of women’s representation, the diversity in the adoption and types of quota policies, and the availability of data. Some of the countries in the sample have witnessed an increase in women’s descriptive representation attributed primarily to the implementation of gender quotas (Barnes and Burchard Reference Barnes and Burchard2013; Bush and Zetterberg Reference Bush and Zetterberg2020; Edgell Reference Edgell2017; Hughes and Tripp Reference Hughes and Tripp2015).

The selected sample offers a wide range of variation in terms of women’s descriptive representation. For example, at the beginning of the study, women occupied approximately 4% or less of the seats in both Nigeria and Kenya. While the percentage has remained relatively unchanged in Nigeria, it significantly increased to more than 21% in Kenya after the implementation of the reserved-seat quota in 2013. These countries also exhibit diversity in the adoption and implementation of quota systems. Ghana, Nigeria, and Zambia have not adopted quotas, whereas Kenya, Zimbabwe, Rwanda, and Namibia implemented the policy during the study period (Clayton Reference Clayton2014). South Africa, Tanzania, and Uganda had quotas in place before 2000. Kenya, Zimbabwe, Rwanda, Tanzania, and Uganda have implemented the reserved-seats model, whereas South Africa and Namibia adopted voluntary party quotas. This variation provides an ideal environment for investigating the potential backlash effect against women politicians following the implementation of gender quotas.

To gather newspaper articles for content analysis, I utilized the Nexis Data Lab service, a cloud-based platform that offers access to news articles from around the world. However, it is important to note that the availability of articles for non-English-speaking countries in sub-Saharan Africa was limited within the archive, and the coverage in these counties spanned a short period of time. Given this consideration, a careful assessment of data availability led to the selection of 10 countries in the region to ensure variation in terms of the percentage of women’s representation and the presence/type of quota policies.

Although the selection of these 10 countries provides valuable insights, it is important to acknowledge certain limitations in case selection. The lack of data for non-English-speaking countries in sub-Saharan Africa may limit the generalizability of findings. Nevertheless, the selected countries offer a meaningful context for investigating the potential backlash effect against women politicians following the implementation of gender quotas. Future research can expand the case selection to include a broader range of countries and incorporate additional sources of data to further enhance our understanding of this phenomenon.

News Collection Process

To obtain news articles about politicians in sample countries, I first collected the names of more than 20,000 aspiring former and current members of parliaments in these countries through official outlets including parliament websites and election results published by governments. Then I identified their gender by consulting various sources, including governmental gazettes, news articles, and social media platforms. This dataset was then used to collect around 500,000 articles published between 2000 and 2020 in the sample countries from Nexis Data Lab service, a recently launched cloud-based platform that provides access to news around the globe. The selection criterion is that politicians should be discussed in the headline or the lead, which ensures they are the main focus of the article (O’Keeffe and McCarthy Reference O’Keeffe and McCarthy2010). Using the data on the gender of politicians, I dropped articles where names from both genders were included in the lead.

As the number of articles for men far exceeds those on women, I randomly selected an equal number of stories about men to match the size of the sample for women to create a balanced corpus and ensure that results are not driven by sample size (Hopp et al. Reference Hopp, Fisher, Cornell, Huskey and Weber2020). After removing duplicate titles and articles published by non-domestic news outlets, the final corpus included more than 150,000 articles in which the sample size for each gender is roughly the same across the corpus (a total of approximately 75,000 for men and women each) within each country. Supplementary Figure 1 in SI showcases the distribution of articles in sample countries.

Measuring Moral Rhetoric in Text

For measuring conservative moral language in news articles, I utilize the Extended Moral Foundation Dictionary (eMFD) developed by Hopp et al. (Reference Hopp, Fisher, Cornell, Huskey and Weber2020), which builds on the original and relatively small Moral Foundations Dictionary (MFD) (Graham, Haidt, and Nosek Reference Graham, Haidt and Nosek2009). The eMFD offers an open-source Python package (eMFDscore) for analyzing text.Footnote 6 The developers of eMFD constructed their dictionary by leveraging an extensively validated, crowd-sourced annotation procedure. Moreover, the eMFDscore is trained on a corpus exclusively build from newspaper articles, and it has been utilized in several recent empirical analyses (Hopp et al. Reference Hopp, Fisher, Cornell, Huskey and Weber2020; Malik et al. Reference Malik, Hopp, Chen and Weber2021). These advantages are important for the task of extracting moral rhetoric from news and make eMFD a well-suited method for this study.

The eMFD takes into consideration the context in which the word appears and detects latent, morally relevant information in the text, whereby words are assigned a probability of belonging to a foundation. For example, the word kill might belong to the foundation of care/harm with a probability of 80% and the foundation of loyalty with a probability of 30%. The open-source text analysis tool developed for scoring the documents — eMFDscore — allows the researcher to score the document using only the foundation with the highest probability (single vice-virtue method), therefore avoiding correlation among various foundations. Here, I use “bow emfd single vice-virtue” to extract moral language from the text corpus. Supplementary Table 1 in SI provides an example of the package’s output.

Dependent and Independent Variables

Dependent variable — The dependent variable is a [0, 1]-bounded conservative moral rhetoric score that measures the salience of binding moral foundations invoked in articles. Higher values of the dependent variable indicate higher salience of conservative moral rhetoric. Previous studies have measured moral rhetoric in terms of separate foundations (e.g., purity/degradation, care/harm). Recent research, however, suggests foundations cluster together, and invoking one of them triggers other intuitions as well. These studies, therefore, use aggregate moral foundation scores (Malik et al. Reference Malik, Hopp, Chen and Weber2021). Following this approach, after obtaining the scores for each binding foundation separately, I aggregate them to create the conservative moral rhetoric score to better capture the overall proportion of moral rhetoric in text. Figure 1 shows the annual trend of the conservative moral rhetoric score in news coverage of women politicians for each country in the sample over the duration of the study.

Figure 1. Trends in the salience of conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of politicians, 2001–2021. The dashed lines indicate year of quota adoption. As can be seen in countries with reserved-seat quotas — Kenya or Uganda for instance — there is a surge in the salience of conservative moral rhetoric compared to men and relative to the pre-quota period. It is noteworthy that in the case of South Africa, although quotas were in place prior to 2000, the country’s ruling party — the African National Congress — adopted a 50% gender quota in national elections 2009 when we observe an upward trend in the salience of conservative moral rhetoric in the news of women politicians. In countries without quota, however, the salience slightly varies or decreases for women across the years. Note: The annual values of the conservative moral rhetoric score are created by summing up and normalizing the scores of individual binding foundations for all news articles published by domestic news outlets in one year in each country.

Independent variables — Main independent variables include the percentage of women’s descriptive representation, quota adoption, type of quota system, and the gender of the politician discussed in the article.

Data on the proportion of seats held by women in single or lower chambers of national parliaments is obtained through the Inter-Parliamentary Union archives.Footnote 7 Figure B in SI demonstrates the changes in the proportion of women in parliaments across countries in the sample. The gender of the politician is a binary variable that indicates whether the lawmaker discussed in the article is male or female.

The binary variable of quota adoption indicates whether and when a country has adopted the policy, the data on which is collected from the International IDEA.Footnote 8 The type of quota is a categorical variable with three levels of no quota, reserved seat, and party quota, which is also created using data from the International IDEA. As the other main independent variable is the percentage of women’s representation in single or lower chambers of parliament, quota type is determined based on the affirmative action implemented in the lower chamber in case of bicameral countries.

Empirical Strategy

In this study, I analyze cross-national, longitudinal, and hierarchical data from domestic newspapers in the sample countries using a random-effects model. I also estimate fixed-effects models and present the results — that are consistent with random-effects models — in the SI. Clustering of observations in longitudinal data introduces two types of variances for variables: within- and between-country that correspond to two different relationships in response to a variable (i.e., percentage of women in single or lower chambers of parliaments). Random-effects estimation combines both types of variation for estimating the effect of the explanatory variable on the outcome. It accounts for the dependence of observations in the same group — articles published within the same country — and therefore leads to unbiased estimates of standard errors.

Scholars have recently begun to use a hybrid method that disentangles within- and between-unit variation errors (Barr et al. Reference Barr, Levy, Scheepers and Tily2013; Bell and Jones Reference Bell and Jones2015; Certo, Withers, and Semadeni Reference Certo, Withers and Semadeni2017; Malik et al. Reference Malik, Hopp, Chen and Weber2021). In this method, the between variable is calculated by obtaining the cluster mean of the explanatory variable $ \overline{x_i} $ and within variable by obtaining the group-centered variation $ \left({x}_{i\mathrm{j}}-\overline{x_i}\right) $ as shown in Equation (1):

(1) $$ {y}_{ij}={\beta}_1\overline{x_i}+{\beta}_2\left({x}_{ij}-\overline{x_i}\right)+{u}_i+{e}_{ij} $$

where within-country variation — $ {\beta}_2 $ — cannot be correlated with country-level effects $ {u}_i $ , as $ {u}_i $ does not vary within countries. By disentangling the variation in the data, the coefficients capture the within-country effect, specifically how changes in women’s descriptive representation within a country correlate with the salience of conservative moral language in interaction with quota adoption or the type of quota system.

I run two multilevel models for testing my proposed hypotheses, as shown in Equations (2) and (3). In Equation 2, the dependent variable is conservative moral rhetoric index that measures the salience of moral language in observation (article) $ i $ within the publication $ j $ in country $ k $ at occasion (month) $ t $ . $ Woman. Representation{(demean)}_{kt} $ indicates the within-country variation in the percentage of women’s parliamentary presence in country $ k $ at occasion $ t $ , whereas $ Quota.{Adoption}_{kt} $ indicates whether country $ k $ has implemented quota policy at occasion $ t $ , and $ {Gender}_i $ refers to the gender of politicians discussed in the article. $ Woman. Representation{(mean)}_k $ measures the time-invariant mean of women’s parliamentary presence in country. In Equation 3, $ Quota.{Type}_{kt} $ is the type of quota implemented in country $ k $ at occasion $ t $ . $ {u}_k $ , $ {u}_j $ , and $ {u}_t $ are random effects for country, publication, and occasion (month), respectively.

(2) $$ {\displaystyle \begin{array}{c}\hskip-30.12em Conservative. Rhetoric. Scor{e}_{\left\{ ijkt\right\}}\\ {}\hskip10.12em ={\beta}_1 Woman. Representation{(demeaned)}_{\left\{ tk\right\}}+{\beta}_2 Quota. Adoptio{n}_{\left\{ tk\right\}}\\ {}\hskip-1.12em +{\beta}_3 Gende{r}_i+{\beta}_4 Woman. Representation{(mean)}_k\\ {}\hskip10.12em +{\beta}_5 Woman. Representation(demeaned) \times Quota. Adoptio n \times Gender\\ {}\hskip-18.0em +{u}_k+{u}_j+{u}_t+{e}_{\left\{ ijkt\right\}}\end{array}} $$
(3) $$ {\displaystyle \begin{array}{c}\hskip-30em Conservative. Rhetoric. Scor{e}_{\left\{ ijkt\right\}}\\ {}\hskip7em ={\beta}_1 Woman. Representation{(demeaned)}_{\left\{ tk\right\}}+{\beta}_2 Quota. Typ{e}_{\left\{ tk\right\}}\\ {}\hskip1em +\beta \_3\; Gender\_i+\beta \_4\; Woman. Representation{(mean)}_k\\ {}\hskip8em +\beta \_5\; Woman. Representation(demeaned) \times Quota. Typ e \times Gender\\ {}\hskip-18em +{u}_k+{u}_j+{u}_t+{e}_{\left\{ ijkt\right\}}\end{array}} $$

The main term of interest in both models is the three-way interaction, which indicates the effect of the increase in the percentage of women in the legislature on the salience of conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of women compared to men, conditional on the adoption or the type of gender quotas. Analyzing articles for both men and women serves to capture the overall salience of conservative moral rhetoric in the news coverage of politicians in the country. There is no reason to assume that the proportion of moral rhetoric in the news about men is meaningfully associated with the percentage of women’s parliamentary presence. Therefore, the result of interaction would reveal whether changes in the salience of conservative moral rhetoric for women are the result of overall trends in the endorsement of moral foundations in the countries under study or associated with women’s presence in politics.

Results

I examine two hypotheses in this study to explore the backlash effect against women politicians following the implementation of gender quotas. Hypothesis A predicts that an increase in the proportion of seats held by women in the single or lower chambers of national parliaments is positively associated with a higher salience of conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of women politicians, conditional on the adoption of a quota system. In reporting resuts, I focus on the within-country variation in women’s representation in parliaments as it reflects changes within the country and is hereby referred to in the analysis as %Woman Representation. The compelete tables with mean value of women’s representation are presented in the SI.

Table 1 and Figure 2 present the results of the within-between random effects model that tests this expectation. The coefficient for %Woman Representation indicates changes in the proportion of conservative moral rhetoric in news the coverage of men in countries with no quota system. The coefficient is statistically indistinguishable from zero ( $ \beta =-0.002,t=-0.7\Big) $ , suggesting that in countries with no gender quota, the proportion of conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of men does not vary in relation with changes in women’s representation (see left panel of Figure 2).

Table 1. Within-between random effects model for the effect of quota adoption on conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of women politicians

* p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01

Figure 2. In countries that have adopted gender quota, increase in the percentage of women in parliaments is followed by significant increase in the salience of conservative moral language in news coverage of women politicians.

Meanwhile, the coefficient for the interaction between %Woman Representation and binary variable of Gender (woman) is negative and statistically significant ( $ \beta =-0.028,t=-8.4 $ ). It suggests that in the absence of a quota system, a higher presence of women in the legislature is associated with a lower proportion of conservative moral language, as can be seen in the left panel of Figure 2. This result could be indicative of the positive attitudinal effect of descriptive representation as theorized by the literature, whereby a more prominent presence of women in the positions of power improves gender role attitudes and beliefs about their leadership capabilities (Allen and Cutts Reference Allen and Cutts2018; Beaman et al. Reference Beaman, Chattopadhyay, Duflo, Pande and Topalova2009). However, the coefficient for Gender (woman) and Quota Adoption is positive and significant, suggesting that the implementation of gender quota is associated with increase in conservative moral rhetoric in the news about women even when there is no change in women’s descriptive representation within the country (%Woman Representation = 0).

The primary term of interest in the model is the three-way interaction between the three independent variables. The interaction captures the changes in the salience of conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of women politicians, in response to variation in women’s parliamentary presence, conditional on the adaptation of gender quota. The coefficient for the interaction term is positive and significant ( $ \beta =0.044,\mathrm{t}=11.7 $ ), which, as can be seen in the right panel of Figure 2, shows that in countries with gender quota, the increase in the descriptive representation of women is associated with a higher proportion of conservative moral language in news on women politicians. Furthermore, the positive association is not due to an overall increase in the conservative moral rhetoric in the news, as its salience is decreasing in the news of male politicians (right panel of Figure 2).

The results confirm my expectation. The salience of conservative moral language in news coverage of women politicians increases following the implementation of gender quotas, which suggests dominant actors leverage their definitional power to generate a moral panic around women’s political participation and enforce long-established social order and values. This tactic serves to motivate public opposition to women’s political pariticpation and help these elites to justify and/or normalize their resistance to progressitve quota reforms (Goode Reference Goode2014; Krook Reference Krook2016).

While the first hypothesis explores the conservative backlash against women following their increased participation after the implementation of gender quotas, hypothesis B disentangles the trends in countries with different types of quota systems. Hypothesis B predicts that the type of quota influences the backlash against women politicians. I expect an increase in the proportion of seats held by women in legislatures of countries with a reserved-seat system to be associated with a higher endorsement of conservative moral language compared to countries with a party quota system.

To test this hypothesis, I focus on countries that have already implemented quotas and run a model with the type of quota policy as one of the independent variables. The dependent variable is the conservative moral rhetoric score, and the main term of interest is once again the three-way interaction between the main independent variables. I include gender in this model as well. Although the results of the previous model revealed an overall decrease in the salience of conservative moral language in news coverage of men in countries with a quota system, the trend might differ across countries with different quota types. Changes in moral rhetoric in news coverage of women politicians should be interpreted in the context of the overall endorsement of moral foundations by the media in news coverage of both men and women.

Table 2 and Figure 3 showcase the results of the random-effects model for Hypothesis B. In this model, the reference group is party quota type and male politicians. The coefficient for %Woman Representation is statistically significant and negative ( $ \beta =-0.013,t=-4.05 $ ), which suggests a decrease in the endorsement of conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of male politicians in countries with a party quota model over the duration under study. The positive and statistically significant coefficient for the interaction between %Woman Representation and Gender[woman] ( $ \beta =0.008,t=2.2 $ ) indicates that increase in the proportion of women in legislature in countries with party quota system is correlated with an increase in salience of conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of women compared to their male counterparts.

Table 2. Within-between random effects model for relationship between quota type and conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of women politicians

* p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01

Figure 3. The influence of quota type on the salience of conservative moral language in news coverage of women politicians. Media reporting of women politicians in countries with reserved seat quotas has a higher proportion of conservative moral rhetoric.

The main term of interest, the coefficient of the three-way interaction, captures the changes in the salience of conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of women in response to the independent variables conditional on the type of quota system and given the overall salience of conservative moral rhetoric in the media. The interaction term is positive and significant ( $ \beta =0.012,t=2.9 $ ), which confirms my expectation. The coefficient indicates that an increase in the percentage of women in the legislature of countries with a reserved-seat model is associated with a higher salience of conservative moral foundations in news coverage of women politicians compared to countries with party quotas, given the overall salience of conservative moral language in news in these countries. Although, as can be seen in the left panel of Figure 3, the proportion of conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of women is still increasing compared to that of their male counterparts in countries with a party quota model.

These results are consistent with the evidence on the implications of quota types, that suggests in countries with reserved-seat quota models, women politicians are more likely to experience tokenism, as well as less political legitimacy and efficacy, and be sidelined (Darhour Reference Darhour, Darhour and Dahlerup2020). Reserved-seat quotas are likely to be adopted as a strategy by the government to improve the country’s international reputation and signal commitment to democratic values and norms. However, these top-down policies are arguably more aimed at maintaining the dominant groups’ hold on power than empowering women. Furthermore, as elites have less control over the electoral outcome, they are more likely to use soft repression tactics such as moral panic to subordinate women within politics and limit the transformative potential of quota reforms.

Discussion and Conclusion

Some scholars argue that quota reforms and subsequent gains in women’s empowerment in politics are often faced with resistance from elites, in reaction to a sense of decline in power and threat to their privileges, which creates a backlash against women politicians. These actors utilize soft repression tactics to counter or undermine the transformative effects of quota reforms and limit women’s political efficacy. While opposition to quota implementation and structural factors affecting their impact has been extensively explored, elites’ resistance after affirmative action goes into effect remains understudied. In this study, I investigated one of the soft repression tactics that dominant actors employ to maintain the status quo and protect their power following the implementation of gender quotas.

I argued that elites create a moral panic around women’s intrusion into politics using conservative moral language as a soft repression tactic. The moral panic serves to incite people’s opposition to women’s political participation and justifies the elites’ resistance against the transformative effects of gender quotas. I offered two hypotheses: First, quota-induced increases in women’s representation would be followed by a wave of moral panic that I measured using the salience of conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of women politicians. Second, elites in countries with a reserved-seat model are more likely to leverage moral panic to push back against quota reforms as they have less control over electoral outcomes compared to countries with party quota models.

I investigated these expectations using automatic content analysis and a cross-national and longitudinal design. The findings of empirical analyses confirm my hypotheses, showing that gains in women’s representation after quotas are followed by significant increases in the conservative moral language in news coverage of women politicians. The moral panic may dissuade women from entering politics or limit their presence to gender-appropriate political roles and activities. It can also potentially affect women’s legislative effectiveness and undermine public opinion about women’s political capability. However, examining the association between conservative moral language in news and support for women’s political participation as well as their willingness to enter politics is outside of the scope of this paper, and should be addressed in future studies. Another venue of investigation is exploring the impact of conservative moral language in news on individuals’ gender role attitudes and biases.

Identifying soft repression in the form of moral panic as a mechanism of backlash holds significant implications for advocates of gender equality in politics and for ensuring the electoral outcomes of gender quotas. Countering the negative impacts of moral panic around women’s political participation requires employing different persuasive strategies as moral-based attitudes are more resistant to change.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S1743923X24000011.

Competing interest

The author declares none.

Funding statement

The author has not received any funding for this project.

Acknowledgements

The author thanks Ryan Kennedy and Scott Clifford for their invaluable mentorship and support, and anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments that helped improve the study.

References

Aaldering, Loes, and Van Der Pas, Daphne Joanna. 2018. “Political Leadership in the Media: Gender Bias in Leader Stereotypes during Campaign and Routine Times.” British Journal of Political Science 50 (3): 911–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, Melinda J., Scherpereel, John A., and Wylie, Kristin N.. 2019. “The Adoption of Women’s Legislative Caucuses Worldwide.” International Feminist Journal of Politics 21 (2): 249–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, Amy C. 2012. “Change in Women’s Descriptive Representation and the Belief in Women’s Ability to Govern: A Virtuous Cycle.” Politics & Gender 8 (04): 437–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, Peter, and Cutts, David. 2018. “How Do Gender Quotas Affect Public Support for Women as Political Leaders?West European Politics 41 (1): 147–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alter, Karen J, and Zürn, Michael. 2020. “Conceptualising Backlash Politics: Introduction to a Special Issue on Backlash Politics in Comparison.” British Journal of Politics and International Relations 22 (4): 563–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldez, Lisa. 2004. “Elected Bodies: The Gender Quota Law for Legislative Candidates in Mexico.” Legislative Studies Quarterly 29 (2): 231–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldner, Conrad, and Pierro, Antonio. 2019. “The Trials of Women Leaders in the Workforce: How a Need for Cognitive Closure Can Influence Acceptance of Harmful Gender Stereotypes.” Sex Roles 80 (9–10): 565–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, Tiffany D. 2012. “Gender and Legislative Preferences: Evidence from the Argentine Provinces.” Politics & Gender 8 (04): 483507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, Tiffany D., and Burchard, Stephanie M.. 2013. “Engendering Politics: The Impact of Descriptive Representation on Women’s Political Engagement in Sub-Saharan Africa.” Comparative Political Studies 46 (7): 767–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, Dale J., Levy, Roger, Scheepers, Christoph, and Tily, Harry J.. 2013. “Random Effects Structure for Confirmatory Hypothesis Testing: Keep It Maximal.” Journal of Memory and Language 68 (3): 255–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauer, Gretchen. 2016. “‘What Is Wrong with a Woman Being Chief?’ Women Chiefs and Symbolic and Substantive Representation in Botswana.” Journal of Asian and African Studies 51 (2): 222–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, Gretchen. 2021. “Women in African Parliaments: Progress and Prospects.” In The Palgrave Handbook of African Women’s Studies, eds. Yacob-Haliso, Olajumoke and Falola, Toyin, 335–52. Cham, Switzerland: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaman, Lori, Chattopadhyay, Raghabendra, Duflo, Esther, Pande, Rohini, and Topalova, Petia. 2009. “Powerful Women: Does Exposure Reduce Bias?Quarterly Journal of Economics 124 (4): 14971540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, Howard S. 1967. “Whose Side Are We On?Social Problems 14 (3): 239–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, Andrew, and Jones, Kelvyn. 2015. “Explaining Fixed Effects: Random Effects Modeling of Time-Series Cross-Sectional and Panel Data.” Political Science Research and Methods 3 (1): 133–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, Marie E., Bouka, Yolande, and Kamuru, Marilyn Muthoni. 2020. “Implementing Inclusion: Gender Quotas, Inequality, and Backlash in Kenya.” Politics & Gender 17 (4): 125.Google Scholar
Biroli, Flávia, and Caminotti, Mariana. 2020. “The Conservative Backlash against Gender in Latin America.” Politics & Gender 16 (1): 138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bligh, Michelle C., Schlehofer, Michèle M., Casad, Bettina J., and Gaffney, Amber M.. 2012. “Competent Enough, But Would You Vote for Her? Gender Stereotypes and Media Influences on Perceptions of Women Politicians: WOMEN POLITICIANS.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology 42 (3): 560–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourdieu, Pierre. 1991. Language and Symbolic Power. Reprinted. Cambridge: Harvard Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Brescoll, Victoria L., Okimoto, Tyler G., and Vial, Andrea C.. 2018. “You’ve Come a Long Way…Maybe: How Moral Emotions Trigger Backlash against Women Leaders: Moral Emotions in Backlash.” Journal of Social Issues 74 (1): 144–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burnet, Jennie E. 2011. “Women Have Found Respect: Gender Quotas, Symbolic Representation, and Female Empowerment in Rwanda.” Politics & Gender 7 (03): 303–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bush, Sarah Sunn, and Zetterberg, Pär. 2020. “Gender Quotas and International Reputation.” American Journal of Political Science 65 (2): 326–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Certo, S. Trevis, Withers, Michael C., and Semadeni, Matthew. 2017. “A Tale of Two Effects: Using Longitudinal Data to Compare within- and between-Firm Effects: Longitudinal Relationships in Strategy Research.” Strategic Management Journal 38 (7): 1536–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chong, Dennis, and Druckman, James N.. 2007. “A Theory of Framing and Opinion Formation in Competitive Elite Environments.” Journal of Communication 57 (1): 99118.Google Scholar
Chowdhury, Najma. 2002. “The Implementation of Quotas: Bangladesh Experience - Dependence and Marginality in Politics.” In The Implementation of Quotas: Asian Experiences. Jakarta, Indonesia, 5058. Stockholm: International Institute for Democracy and Electoral Assistance.Google Scholar
Clayton, Amanda. 2014. Namibia at a Crossroads: 50/50 and the Way Forward. Windhoek, Namibia: Institute for Public Policy Research (IPPR).Google Scholar
Clayton, Amanda. 2015. “Women’s Political Engagement Under Quota-Mandated Female Representation: Evidence from a Randomized Policy Experiment.” Comparative Political Studies 48 (3): 333–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, Amanda. 2018. “Do Gender Quotas Really Reduce Bias? Evidence from a Policy Experiment in Southern Africa.” Journal of Experimental Political Science 5 (3): 182–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, Amanda. 2021. “How Do Electoral Gender Quotas Affect Policy?Annual Review of Political Science 24 (1): 235–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifford, Scott, and Jerit, Jennifer. 2013. “How Words Do the Work of Politics: Moral Foundations Theory and the Debate over Stem Cell Research.” Journal of Politics 75 (3): 659–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifford, Scott, Jerit, Jennifer, Rainey, Carlisle, and Motyl, Matt. 2015. “Moral Concerns and Policy Attitudes: Investigating the Influence of Elite Rhetoric.” Political Communication 32 (2): 229–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, Stanley. 1991. “Moral Panics and Folk Concepts.” Paedagogica Historica 35 (3): 585–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, Stanley. 2011. Folk Devils and Moral Panics: The Creation of the Mods and Rockers. Abingdon, Oxon; New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connell, Raewyn. 2002. Gender. Cambridge, UK: Malden, MA: Polity; Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Courtney, Michael, Breen, Michael, McGing, Claire, McMenamin, Iain, O’Malley, Eoin, and Rafter, Kevin 2020. “Underrepresenting Reality? Media Coverage of Women in Politics and Sport.” Social Science Quarterly 101 (4): 12821302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahlerup, Drude. 2005. “Increasing Women’s Political Representation: New Trends in Gender Quotas.” In Women in Parliament: Beyond Numbers, 141–53. Stockholm, Sweden: International Institute for Democracy and Electoral Assistance (IDEA).Google Scholar
Darhour, Hanane. 2020. “Whose Empowerment? Gender Quota Reform Mechanisms and De-Democratization in Morocco.” In Double-Edged Politics on Women’s Rights in the MENA Region, Gender and Politics, eds. Darhour, Hanane and Dahlerup, Drude, 279302. Cham: Springer International Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darhour, Hanane, and Dahlerup, Drude. 2013. “Sustainable Representation of Women through Gender Quotas: A Decade’s Experience in Morocco.” Women’s Studies International Forum 41: 132–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
David, Assaf, and Nanes, Stefanie. 2011. “The Women’s Quota in Jordan’s Municipal Councils: International and Domestic Dimensions.” Journal of Women, Politics & Policy 32 (4): 275304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Day, Martin V., Fiske, Susan T., Downing, Emily L., and Trail, Thomas E.. 2014. “Shifting Liberal and Conservative Attitudes Using Moral Foundations Theory.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 40 (12): 1559–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Paola, Maria, Scoppa, Vincenzo, and Lombardo, Rosetta. 2010. “Can Gender Quotas Break down Negative Stereotypes? Evidence from Changes in Electoral Rules.” Journal of Public Economics 94 (5–6): 344–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovi, Suzanne. 2007. “Theorizing Women’s Representation in the United States.” Politics & Gender 3(03): 297319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edgell, Amanda B. 2017. “Foreign Aid, Democracy, and Gender Quota Laws.” Democratization 24 (6): 1103–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faludi, Susan. 1991. Backlash: The Undeclared War against American Women. United StatesCrown.Google Scholar
Faree, Maria Marx. 2005. “Soft Repression: Ridicule, Stigma, and Silencing in Gender-Based Movements.” In Repression and Mobilization, eds. Davenport, Christian, Johnston, Hank, and Mueller, Carol McClurg, 138–56. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Flood, Michael, Dragiewicz, Molly, and Pease, Bob. 2021. “Resistance and Backlash to Gender Equality.” Australian Journal of Social Issues 56 (3): 393408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, Gareth, Youngs, Richard, Larok, Arthur, Marczewski, Paweł, Vijayan, MJ, Nodia, Ghia, Shapovalova, Natalia, Sombatpoonsiri, Janjira, von Bülow, Marisa, and Zihnioğlu, Özge. 2018. The Mobilization of Conservative Civil Society. CEIP: Carnegie Endowment for International Peace. United States of America. https://policycommons.net/artifacts/433868/the-mobilization-of-conservative-civil-society/1404936/. CID: 20.500.12592/51fhbc.Google Scholar
Franceschet, Susan, Krook, Mona Lena, and Piscopo, Jennifer M. 2012. “Conceptualizing the Impact of Quotas.” In The Impact of Gender Quotas, eds. Franceschet, Susan and Piscopo, Jennifer M, 326. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franceschet, Susan, and Piscopo, Jennifer M.. 2008. “Gender Quotas and Women’s Substantive Representation: Lessons from Argentina.” Politics & Gender 4 (03): 393425.Google Scholar
Gidengil, Elisabeth, and Everitt, Joanna. 1999. “Metaphors and Misrepresentation: Gendered Mediation in News Coverage of the 1993 Canadian Leaders’ Debates.” Harvard International Journal of Press/Politics 4 (1): 4865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gidengil, Elisabeth, and Everitt, Joanna. 2003. “Talking Tough: Gender and Reported Speech in Campaign News Coverage.” Political Communication 20 (3): 209–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goode, Erich. 2014. Drugs in American Society, 9th edition. Dubuque: McGraw-Hill Education.Google Scholar
Goode, Erich, and Ben-Yehuda, Nachman. 1994. “Moral Panics: Culture, Politics, and Social Construction.” Annual Review of Sociology 20: 149–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodyear-Grant, Elizabeth. 2009. “Crafting a Public Image: Women MPs and the Dynamics of Media Coverage.” In Opening Doors Wider: Women’s Political Engagement in Canada, 147–66. Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada: UBC Press.Google Scholar
Graham, Jesse, Nosek, Brian A., Haidt, Jonathan, Iyer, Ravi, Koleva, Spassena, and Ditto, Peter H.. 2011. “Mapping the Moral Domain.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101 (2): 366–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, Jesse, Graham, Jesse, Jonathan Haidt, Sena Koleva, Matt Motyl, Ravi Iyer, Sean P. Wojcik, and Peter H. Ditto. 2013. “Moral Foundations Theory: The Pragmatic Validity of Moral Pluralism.” In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 55130. United States.Google Scholar
Graham, Jesse, and Haidt, Jonathan. 2012. “Sacred Values and Evil Adversaries: A Moral Foundations Approach.” In The Social Psychology of Morality: Exploring the Causes of Good and Evil, eds. Mikulincer, Mario and Shaver, Phillip R., 1131. Washington: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, Jesse, Haidt, Jonathan, and Nosek, Brian A.. 2009. “Liberals and Conservatives Rely on Different Sets of Moral Foundations.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96 (5): 1029–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, Jonathan, and Graham, Jesse. 2007. “When Morality Opposes Justice: Conservatives Have Moral Intuitions That Liberals May Not Recognize.” Social Justice Research 20 (1): 98116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, Stuart, Critcher, Chas, Jefferson, Tony, Clarke, John, and Roberts, Brian. 2013. Policing the Crisis: Mugging, the State, and Law and Order, 2nd edition. Houndmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harper, Craig A., and Harris, Andrew J.. 2017. “Applying Moral Foundations Theory to Understanding Public Views of Sexual Offending.” Journal of Sexual Aggression 23 (2): 11123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoover, Joe, Atari, Mohammad, Mostafazadeh Davani, Aida, Kennedy, Brendan, Portillo-Wightman, Gwenyth, Yeh, Leigh, and Dehghani, Morteza. 2021. “Investigating the Role of Group-Based Morality in Extreme Behavioral Expressions of Prejudice.” Nature Communications 12 (1): 4585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopp, Frederic R., Fisher, Jacob T., Cornell, Devin, Huskey, Richard, and Weber, René 2020. “The Extended Moral Foundations Dictionary (eMFD): Development and Applications of a Crowd-Sourced Approach to Extracting Moral Intuitions from Text.” Behavior Research Methods 53: 232–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, Melanie M., Paxton, Pamela, Clayton, Amanda B., and Zetterberg, Pär. 2019. “Global Gender Quota Adoption, Implementation, and Reform.” Comparative Politics 51 (2): 219–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, Melanie M., and Tripp, Aili Mari. 2015. “Civil War and Trajectories of Change in Women’s Political Representation in Africa, 1985–2010.” Social Forces 93 (4): 1513–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglehart, Ronald, and Norris, Pippa. 2003. Rising Tide: Gender Equality and Cultural Change around the World. Cambridge, UK ; New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janoff-Bulman, Ronnie, and Carnes, Nate C.. 2016. “Social Justice and Social Order: Binding Moralities across the Political Spectrum” ed. Zia, Asim. PLOS ONE 11 (3): e0152479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jost, John T., Glaser, Jack, Kruglanski, Arie W., and Sulloway, Frank J.. 2003. “Political Conservatism as Motivated Social Cognition.” Psychological Bulletin 129 (3): 339–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karp, Jeffrey A., and Banducci, Susan A.. 2008. “When Politics Is Not Just a Man’s Game: Women’s Representation and Political Engagement.” Electoral Studies 27 (1): 105–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerevel, Yann P., and Atkeson, Lonna Rae. 2017. “Campaigns, Descriptive Representation, Quotas and Women’s Political Engagement in Mexico.” Politics, Groups, and Identities 5 (3): 454–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kittilson, Miki Caul. 2005. “In Support of Gender Quotas: Setting New Standards, Bringing Visible Gains.” Politics & Gender 1 (04). http://www.journals.cambridge.org/abstract_S1743923X05230192.Google Scholar
Kittilson, Miki Caul, and Schwindt-Bayer, Leslie A.. 2012. The Gendered Effects of Electoral Institutions: Political Engagement and Participation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraaijeveld, Steven R., and Jamrozik, Euzebiusz. 2022. “Moralization and Mismoralization in Public Health.” Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy 25 (4): 655–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krook, Mona Lena. 2006. “Gender Quotas, Norms, and Politics.” Politics & Gender 2 (01): 110–18.Google Scholar
Krook, Mona Lena. 2007. “Candidate Gender Quotas: A Framework for Analysis.” European Journal of Political Research 46 (3): 367–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krook, Mona Lena. 2015. “Empowerment versus Backlash: Gender Quotas and Critical Mass Theory.” Politics, Groups, and Identities 3 (1): 184–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krook, Mona Lena. 2016. “Contesting Gender Quotas: Dynamics of Resistance.” Politics, Groups, and Identities 4 (2): 268–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krook, Mona Lena, O’Brien, Diana Z., and Swip, Krista M.. 2010. “Military Invasion and Women’s Political Representation: Gender Quotas in Post-Conflict Afghanistan and Iraq.” International Feminist Journal of Politics 12 (1): 6679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krook, Mona Lena, and Sanín, Juliana Restrepo. 2016. “Gender and Political Violence in Latin America. Concepts, Debates and Solutions.” Política y gobierno 23 (1): 127–62.Google Scholar
Krzyżanowski, Michał. 2020. “Discursive Shifts and the Normalisation of Racism: Imaginaries of Immigration, Moral Panics and the Discourse of Contemporary Right-Wing Populism.” Social Semiotics 30 (4): 503–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, George. 2014. The All-New Don’t Think of an Elephant! Know Your Values and Frame the Debate. White River Junction, Vermont: Chelsea Green Publishing.Google Scholar
Lang, Sabine. 2015. Thirty Years of Gender Quotas in Germany: Policy Adoption between Mainstreaming and Minimal Compliance. Italy: European University Institute.Google Scholar
Larok, Arthur. 2018. The Conservative Alliance against Gender Justice and Equality in Uganda. Washington DC, US: Carnegie Endowment for International Peace.Google Scholar
Lee, Hyunji, and Shin, Ki-Young. 2016. “Gender Quotas and Candidate Selection Processes in South Korean Political Parties.” Pacific Affairs 89 (2): 345–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Leeuwen, Florian, and Park, Justin H.. 2009. “Perceptions of Social Dangers, Moral Foundations, and Political Orientation.” Personality and Individual Differences 47 (3): 169–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lipset, Seymour Martin, and Raab, Earl. 1978. The Politics of Unreason: Right-Wing Extremism in America, 1790-1977, 2nd edition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lloren, Anouk. 2014. “Gender Quotas in Morocco: Lessons for Women’s Descriptive and Symbolic Representation.” Representation 50 (4): 527–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lombardo, Emanuela, and Meier, Petra. 2014. The Symbolic Representation of Gender: A Discursive Approach. Farnham, Surrey: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Malik, Musa, Hopp, Frederic R., Chen, Yibei, and Weber, René. 2021. “Does Regional Variation in Pathogen Prevalence Predict the Moralization of Language in COVID-19 News?Journal of Language and Social Psychology 40 (5–6): 653–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malka, Ariel, Osborne, Danny, Soto, Christopher J., M. Greaves, Lara, Sibley, Chris G., and Lelkes, Yphtach. 2016. “Binding Moral Foundations and the Narrowing of Ideological Conflict to the Traditional Morality Domain.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 42 (9): 1243–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mano, Winston. 2016. “The State and Public Broadcasting: Continuity and Change in Zimbabwe.” In Global Media and National Policies, eds. Flew, Terry, Iosifidis, Petros, and Steemers, Jeanette, 190205. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK. http://link.springer.com/10.1057/9781137493958_12.Google Scholar
Mansbridge, Jane. 1999. “Should Blacks Represent Blacks and Women Represent Women? A Contingent ‘Yes.’Journal of Politics 61 (3): 628–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mansbridge, Jane, and Shames, Shauna L.. 2008. “Toward a Theory of Backlash: Dynamic Resistance and the Central Role of Power.” Politics & Gender 4 (04): 623.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mariani, Mack, Marshall, Bryan W., and Mathews-Schultz, A. Lanethea. 2015. “See Hillary Clinton, Nancy Pelosi, and Sarah Palin Run? Party, Ideology, and the Influence of Female Role Models on Young Women.” Political Research Quarterly 68 (4): 716–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matemba, Yonah Hisbon. 2005. “A Chief Called ‘Woman’: Historical Perspectives on the Changing Face of Bogosi (Chieftainship) in Botswana, 1834º2004.” JENDA: A Journal of Culture and African Women Studies 7: 122.Google Scholar
Matfess, Hilary, Kishi, Roudabeh, and Berry, Marie E.. 2022. “No Safety in Numbers: Political Representation and Political Violence Targeting Women in Kenya.” International Feminist Journal of Politics 25(3): 123.Google Scholar
McRobbie, Angela, and Thornton, Sarah L.. 1995. “Rethinking ‘Moral Panic’ for Multi-Mediated Social Worlds.” British Journal of Sociology 46 (4): 559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, Petra. 2012. “Paradoxes in the Meaning of Quotas in Belgium.” In The Impact of Gender Quotas, eds. Franceschet, Susan, Krook, Mona Lena, and Piscopo, Jennifer M., 157–72. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muhammad, Sher, Awan, Muhammad, and Hussain, Misbah. 2020. “Violence against Women in Politics: A Study of Backlash Effect of Gender Quota in Parliament of Pakistan.” Research Journal of Social Sciences and Economics Review (RJSSER) 1 (4): 361–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muriaas, Ragnhild, and Wang, Vibeke. 2012. “Executive Dominance and the Politics of Quota Representation in Uganda.” Journal of Modern African Studies 50 (2): 309–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Brien, Diana Z., and Piscopo, Jennifer M.. 2019. “The Impact of Women in Parliament.” In The Palgrave Handbook of Women’s Political Rights, eds. Franceschet, Susan, Krook, Mona Lena, and Tan, Netina, 5372. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Keeffe, Anne, and McCarthy, Michael, eds. 2010. The Routledge Handbook of Corpus Linguistics, 1st edition. London; New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Panchaud, Christine, Keogh, Sarah C., Stillman, Melissa, Awusabo-Asare, Kofi, Motta, Angélica, Sidze, Estelle, and Silvia Monzón, Ana. 2019. “Towards Comprehensive Sexuality Education: A Comparative Analysis of the Policy Environment Surrounding School-Based Sexuality Education in Ghana, Peru, Kenya and Guatemala.” Sex Education 19 (3): 277–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, Leigh A., and de Souza Santos, Andreza Aruska. 2020. “The Right-Wing Backlash in Brazil and Beyond.” Politics & Gender 16 (1): 3238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pitkin, Hanna Fenichel. 1967. The Concept of Representation. University of California: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pousadela, Ines M., and Perera, Dominic R.. 2021. “The Enemy Within? Anti‐Rights Groups and Restrictions on Civil Society.” Global Policy 12 (S5): 3444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, Stephen D., Gandy, Oscar H., and Grant, August E.. 2001. Framing Public Life: Perspectives on Media and Our Understanding of the Social World. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosen, Jennifer. 2017. “Gender Quotas for Women in National Politics: A Comparative Analysis across Development Thresholds.” Social Science Research 66: 82101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowley, Michelle V. 2020. “Anything but Reactionary: Exploring the Mechanics of Backlash.” Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and Society 45 (2): 278–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, Timothy J. 2014. “Reconsidering Moral Issues in Politics.” Journal of Politics 76 (2): 380–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silver, Jasmine R., and Silver, Eric. 2017. “Why Are Conservatives More Punitive than Liberals? A Moral Foundations Approach.” Law and Human Behavior 41: 258–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sparks, Colin. 2009. “South African Media in Transition.” Journal of African Media Studies 1 (2): 195220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strupp-Levitsky, Michael, Noorbaloochi, Sharareh, Shipley, Andrew, and Jost, John T.. 2010. “Moral ‘Foundations’ as the Product of Motivated Social Cognition: Empathy and Other Psychological Underpinnings of Ideological Divergence in ‘Individualizing’ and ‘Binding’ Concerns” ed. Wisneski, Daniel. PLOS ONE 15 (11): e0241144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamale, Sylvia. 2003. “Introducing Quotas in Africa: Discourse and Legal Reform in Uganda.” In The Implementation of Quotas: African Experiences, Quota Report Series, edited by Ballington, Julie, 3845. Stockholm, Sweden: International IDEA.Google Scholar
Tamborini, Ron, Prabhu, Sujay, Lewis, Robert J., Grizzard, Matthew, and Eden, Allison. 2018. “The Influence of Media Exposure on the Accessibility of Moral Intuitions and Associated Affect.” Journal of Media Psychology 30 (2): 7990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townsend-Bell, Erica. 2020. “Backlash as the Moment of Revelation.” Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and Society 45 (2): 287–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tripp, Aili Mari, and Kang, Alice. 2008. “The Global Impact of Quotas: On the Fast Track to Increased Female Legislative Representation.” Comparative Political Studies 41 (3): 338–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tripp, Aili Mari, Konaté, Dior, and Lowe-Morna, Colleen. 2006. “Sub-Saharan Africa: On the Fast Track to Women’s Political Representation.” In Women, Quotas, and Politics, ed. Dahlerup, Drude, 112. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Van der Pas, Daphne Joanna, and Aaldering, Loes. 2020. “Gender Differences in Political Media Coverage: A Meta-Analysis.” Journal of Communication 70 (1): 114–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vecina, Maria L., and Piñuela, Raul. 2017. “Relationships between Ambivalent Sexism and the Five Moral Foundations in Domestic Violence: Is It a Matter of Fairness and Authority?Journal of Psychology 151 (3): 334–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verge, Tània, and Troupel, Aurélia. 2011. “Unequals among Equals: Party Strategic Discrimination and Quota Laws.” French Politics 9 (3): 260–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoon, Mi Yung. 2011. “More Women in the Tanzanian Legislature: Do Numbers Matter?Journal of Contemporary African Studies 29 (1): 8398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zetterberg, Pär. 2009. “Do Gender Quotas Foster Women’s Political Engagement?: Lessons from Latin America.” Political Research Quarterly 62 (4): 715–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zetterberg, Pär. 2013. “The Dynamic Relationship between Gender Quotas and Political Institutions.” Politics & Gender 9 (03): 316–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Trends in the salience of conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of politicians, 2001–2021. The dashed lines indicate year of quota adoption. As can be seen in countries with reserved-seat quotas — Kenya or Uganda for instance — there is a surge in the salience of conservative moral rhetoric compared to men and relative to the pre-quota period. It is noteworthy that in the case of South Africa, although quotas were in place prior to 2000, the country’s ruling party — the African National Congress — adopted a 50% gender quota in national elections 2009 when we observe an upward trend in the salience of conservative moral rhetoric in the news of women politicians. In countries without quota, however, the salience slightly varies or decreases for women across the years. Note: The annual values of the conservative moral rhetoric score are created by summing up and normalizing the scores of individual binding foundations for all news articles published by domestic news outlets in one year in each country.

Figure 1

Table 1. Within-between random effects model for the effect of quota adoption on conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of women politicians

Figure 2

Figure 2. In countries that have adopted gender quota, increase in the percentage of women in parliaments is followed by significant increase in the salience of conservative moral language in news coverage of women politicians.

Figure 3

Table 2. Within-between random effects model for relationship between quota type and conservative moral rhetoric in news coverage of women politicians

Figure 4

Figure 3. The influence of quota type on the salience of conservative moral language in news coverage of women politicians. Media reporting of women politicians in countries with reserved seat quotas has a higher proportion of conservative moral rhetoric.

Supplementary material: File

Shiran supplementary material

Shiran supplementary material
Download Shiran supplementary material(File)
File 17.3 MB