Introduction
The global refugee crisis is escalating, as exemplified by events in Syria (United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees [UNHCR], 2015). Even once in a safe country, refugees are often plagued by vivid mental images of traumatic events – called ‘intrusive memories’ – that repeatedly spring to mind unbidden (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Exposure to traumatic events, within Syria or on the escape to Europe, have profound mental health consequences (Hassan et al., Reference Hassan, Kirmayer, Mekki-Berrada, Quosh, el Chammay and Deville-Stoetzel2015; Abbott, Reference Abbott2016; Nature Editorial, 2016) that interfere with acquiring skills needed to integrate into the new environment (e.g. language learning), for example by disrupting concentration. The UNHCR report on the mental health of Syrians (Hassan et al., Reference Hassan, Kirmayer, Mekki-Berrada, Quosh, el Chammay and Deville-Stoetzel2015) highlights culture-specific idioms of distress such as ‘mou adder rakkezz’ – ‘I can't focus, I can't concentrate’. Intrusive sensory memories occur across several psychiatric disorders (Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, O'Donnell, Creamer, McFarlane and Silove2011), notably in those disorders with high prevalence in refugees (e.g. Bogic et al., Reference Bogic, Ajdukovic, Bremner, Franciskovic, Galeazzi and Kucukalic2012; Hollander et al., Reference Hollander, Dal, Lewis, Magnusson, Kirkbridge and Dalman2016). These include post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Grey and Holmes, Reference Grey and Holmes2008), depression (Birrer et al., Reference Birrer, Michael and Munsch2007; Williams and Moulds, Reference Williams and Moulds2008) prolonged grief (Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Kenny, Joscelyne, Rawson, Maccallum and Cahill2014), and psychosis (Morrison, Reference Morrison2004). Intrusive sensory memories present a possible ‘transdiagnostic’ target for intervention (Fairburn et al., Reference Fairburn, Cooper and Shafran2003).
Of the more than a million refugees that reached the European Union in 2015 (UNHCR, 2015), Sweden has taken the highest numbers per capita with 162,877 asylum applications in 2015 alone (The Swedish Migration Agency [Migrationsverket], 2015). Almost two-thirds of these applications were from individuals aged 13–24 years, and many from unaccompanied minors, constituting 50.3% of all children between 0 and 17 years who applied for asylum in Sweden in 2015 (The Swedish Migration Agency [Migrationsverket], 2015).
Trauma-associated mental health problems incur heavy costs to individuals and society: there is urgent need for language-free, simple-to-deliver treatments that can reach a large and dispersed refugee population (Ullmann et al., Reference Ullmann, Barthel, Taché, Bornstein, Licinio and Bornstein2015). However, innovation is sorely needed as most psychological treatments cannot reach people with mental illnesses at international scale (Kazdin and Blase, Reference Kazdin and Blase2011; Kazdin and Rabbitt, Reference Kazdin and Rabbitt2013). Refugee host countries lack capacity to deploy evidence-based psychological treatments at the scale needed as they typically require face-to-face contact with a therapist/lay worker (Patel et al., Reference Patel, Chowdhary, Rahman and Verdeli2011) alongside interpreters (Ullmann et al., Reference Ullmann, Barthel, Taché, Bornstein, Licinio and Bornstein2015). Furthermore, the number of psychological treatments tested with refugee populations is low (Nickerson et al., Reference Nickerson, Bryant, Silove and Steel2011), raising questions of cultural acceptability (Hassan et al., Reference Hassan, Kirmayer, Mekki-Berrada, Quosh, el Chammay and Deville-Stoetzel2015).
Approaches could be: (i) to utilize smartphone technology – which is appealing and accessible to young people from Syria (87%; Mohr et al., Reference Mohr, Schueller, Araya, Gureje and Montague2014; Central Intelligence Agency [CIA], 2016) – in a given language, e.g. Arabic; (ii) to take a mechanistically focused cognitive approach from experimental psychopathology and target one tractable symptom (rather than a whole syndrome); and (iii) to focus not only on reducing psychological distress but also downstream improvements in functioning (e.g. adaption to a host country), particularly given refugees’ motivation for improvement, demonstrated by travelling miles through danger and obstacles to reach a better life.
We propose that certain behavioural interventions could be delivered at minimal cost, directly (without a therapist/interpreter), by smartphone, in refugees’ daily life. Our vision is galvanized by the recent concept of ‘massive open online interventions’ (MOOIs) (Muñoz et al., Reference Muñoz, Bunge, Chen, Schueller, Bravin, Shaughnessy and Pérez-Stable2015). ‘MOOIs have the potential to increase the reach, scalability, and affordability of psychological interventions. . . This term was inspired by the growing popularity of massive open online courses (MOOCs). . . which attract millions of students. . . . Digital interventions. . . can be used again and again by people anywhere in the world’ (Muñoz et al., Reference Muñoz, Bunge, Chen, Schueller, Bravin, Shaughnessy and Pérez-Stable2015).
We have experimentally derived simple-to-deliver methods to reduce the frequency of intrusive memories of trauma via computer game-play. A one-session behavioural intervention (involving the game Tetris) at specified time points, reduces the frequency of intrusive memories of experimental trauma (film footage) by approximately half (Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, James, Coode-Bate and Deeprose2009; Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, James, Kilford and Deeprose2010; James et al., Reference James, Bonsall, Hoppitt, Tunbridge, Geddes, Milton and Holmes2015). This suggests a technique that could be developed after real trauma. Mental disorders and attending mental health services is associated with stigma in Syrian cultures (Hassan et al., Reference Hassan, Kirmayer, Mekki-Berrada, Quosh, el Chammay and Deville-Stoetzel2015). Young people particularly can be reluctant to access psychiatric/psychological services, thus computer-gaming in daily life opens new possibilities to provide support in non-/less-stigmatizing ways.
This feasibility study with young refugees recently residing in Sweden is a first step towards future scalable intervention development. We explored monitoring one target symptom, simple measures of functioning, and the acceptability of a novel intervention procedure. Participants were asked to (a) monitor the frequency count of intrusive memories of trauma for one week using a daily intrusion diary; (b) rate concentration disruption and intrusive memory occurrence over a week; and (c) complete a one-session intervention involving Tetris game-play via smartphone. An important aspect of the research was whether these procedures were feasible without an interpreter, given that reliance on interpreters would massively increase treatment costs and impede scalability.
Method
Participants and recruitment procedure
Twenty-two young refugees (mean age 22 years, SD = 2.4; 17 males, 5 females) participated. Sixteen (73%) were from Syria, three from Iraq, and three not specified. Twenty participants (91%) had Arabic as their first language; the remaining two (9%) spoke Arabic, but had a different mother tongue. Eighteen participants (82%) had arrived in Sweden in the last 6 months before study participation; three individuals arrived 7–12 months earlier, and one individual arrived more than 12 months earlier. Ten participants (46%) had had more than 12 years of education, seven participants (32%) had between 10 and 12 years, four participants (18%) had between 6 and 9 years, and one participant had less than 5 years of education.
Participants were recruited via posters in Swedish, English and Arabic, advertised digitally through social media (e.g. Facebook pages of Refugees Welcome and Save the Children) and advertised in paper in relevant institutions (non-governmental organizations [NGOs], religious institutions, cultural initiatives, government agencies, and refugee accommodation), and by handing information sheets to passing individuals at refugee accommodation (with help from local staff members). Sixteen people were recruited via refugee accommodation (73%), two via Swedish language classes (9%), two via direct approach at the central station in Stockholm (9%), one (5%) via Facebook, and one (5%) via a NGO. Recruitment and interviewing was done in Swedish/English (orally and on paper) and in Arabic (on paper), without the presence of an interpreter. Afterwards, the participant's written responses were translated from Arabic/Swedish into English.
Participants did not receive any financial compensation. All gave their written, informed consent before participation; in case of minors consent was given in agreement with their custodian. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of Regional Ethical Review Board Stockholm on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, and its most recent revision (ethics approval number 2015/1915-31/5).
Apparatus and materials
Intrusive memory monitoring via daily intrusion diary
The total number (count data) of intrusive memories in the week after the interview was assessed using a daily pen-and-paper intrusion diary (Fig. 1A) in everyday life, as in previous experimental studies (Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, James, Coode-Bate and Deeprose2009; Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, James, Kilford and Deeprose2010; James et al., Reference James, Bonsall, Hoppitt, Tunbridge, Geddes, Milton and Holmes2015) and clinical studies (Iyadurai and Holmes, Reference Iyadurai and Holmes2015; Horsch and Holmes, Reference Horsch and Holmes2016). Participants ticked a box for the day and time period (morning/afternoon/evening) when the intrusive memory occurred, or marked ‘zero’ if they experienced none. Intrusive memories were described as: ‘images of traumatic events that pop into your mind without warning. They often take the form of visual pictures in your mind's eye, e.g. like a snapshot image or a film clip. They can also include other senses such as sounds and smells, such as the smell of smoke. They may or may not be triggered by something you are aware of, such as telling someone about what happened, or watching something on the news.’ Participants were not to record memories recalled deliberately or general verbal thoughts. Instructions were based on previous protocols (see Supplemental Material) and a short definition of flashback/intrusive memories was printed on the intrusion diary as a reminder. Check marks were summed to yield daily and weekly frequency counts (Fig. 1B).
After 7 days, participants rated ‘How many of your flashbacks do you think you recorded in the diary’ from 0 (none of them) to 10 (all of them). Participants were given a stamped addressed envelope to return the intrusion diary by post.
Ratings of concentration disruption
Participants were asked to estimate, ‘When you have an intrusive memory how long does it disrupt your concentration on average?’, on an ordinal scale depicting different time intervals (<1 min, 1–5 min, 5–10 min, 10–30 min, 30–60 min, >60 min; Fig. 1C).
Two questions rated on 11-point visual analogue scales were: ‘Over the past week, how much difficulty did you have concentrating generally?’ from 0 (no concentration difficulties) to 10 (extreme concentration difficulties) (Fig. 1D); and ‘Over the last week, how much did your intrusive memories/flashbacks disrupt your concentration?’ from 0 (not at all disruptive) to 10 (extremely disruptive).
Rating of intrusive memory re-occurrence over the week preceding interview
Participants were asked ‘How many intrusive memories did you have in the last week?’ anchored from 0 (none) to 10 (numerous – all the time) (Fig. 1D).
Computer game-play and intervention procedure
Participants downloaded and installed the computer game Tetris to their smartphone (e.g. Tetris Free via https://play.google.com/store/apps; version 1.8.03.5593836126994432, developed by EA Mobile Montreal Team). Instructions for game-play were based on previous protocols (Iyadurai and Holmes, Reference Iyadurai and Holmes2015); see Supplemental Material.
The cognitive task intervention procedure involved two key components: thinking back to a traumatic event followed by playing the computer-game Tetris (Iyadurai and Holmes, Reference Iyadurai and Holmes2015; James et al., Reference James, Bonsall, Hoppitt, Tunbridge, Geddes, Milton and Holmes2015; Horsch and Holmes, Reference Horsch and Holmes2016). The following instructions were used to reactivate trauma memory: ‘We asked you in the information sheet about any traumatic events that you may have experienced. You don't need to tell about this, but to make the computer game as helpful as possible, we just need to make sure that the traumatic event is in your mind just before you do the task. So I'd like you to think back to the traumatic event for a brief moment, specifically the worst moments that pop back to mind (intrusive memories that flash to mind). You don't need to think about it in detail. Let me help you – did anything pop into your mind? Are there any images that are flashing to mind or any bits of the traumatic event you can see again? It's OK, you don't have to tell me what happened, as long as the images were in your mind that's fine.’ Following instructions and a practice, participants were asked to play Tetris for at least one uninterrupted period of 15 min (or longer if they wished) on their smartphone.
Feasibility and acceptability
Feedback on the feasibility and acceptability of symptom monitoring and the potential for smartphone-delivered interventions was obtained using ratings scales as well as open-ended questions. For example, acceptability of the intervention was assessed by the following 11-point scale: ‘Do you think computer game play would be an acceptable way to reduce the daily frequency of the intrusive memories?’ from 0 (not at all acceptable) to 10 (very acceptable). An example of an open-ended question related to preference for mode of delivery of the intervention was ‘How would you feel about talking to a doctor/psychologist about your traumatic experiences?’
Procedure
All study documents contained both English and Arabic text interspersed on the same sheets. Communication was done mostly through the translated texts (the interviewers did not speak Arabic) by handing them in the correct order to the participants for them to read and fill out.
Participants completed concentration and intrusive memory ratings, followed by the computer game-play cognitive task procedure. This involved reactivating a memory of trauma and 15–20 min of Tetris game-play on their smartphones. Instructions for the daily intrusion diary were given. Next, feasibility and acceptability feedback questions were collected (see Supplemental Material), with answers written by the participant in their preferred language. The session lasted between 60 and 90 min.
Participants were instructed to return their intrusion diary in the post after 1 week.
Data analysis
Twenty-two participants were tested (Julious, Reference Julious2005). The study aimed to provide estimates of the mean and variance of the number of intrusive memories and concentration difficulties people experience, to inform future studies, but not to conduct tests of statistical significance. However, given a directional hypothesis, the relation between the occurrence of intrusive memories and concentration difficulties was calculated using a Pearson correlation coefficient.
Results
Intrusive memory monitoring via intrusion diary
All 22 participants indicated that they were willing to fill out a daily intrusion diary to measure the frequency of their intrusive memories of trauma (7–10 on a scale from 0 to 10). Seventeen participants (77%) then returned the pen-and-paper intrusion diary by post after 1 week. Of the five that did not return the diary, one felt too sad to complete the study due to receiving bad news from Syria about family (as we were informed by his custodian), and one appeared to not have understood the information properly. The other three dropped out without providing a reason and did not reply to mobile phone SMS (short messaging service).
Critically, diary data indicated a mean of almost two intrusive memories per day (mean = 1.81, SD = 1.39). The mean number of intrusive memories weekly was 12.65 (SD = 9.71). Of the 17 participants who returned the intrusion diary, 13 (59%) rated 5 or higher on a scale from 0 (none) to 10 (all of them) when asked how many of their intrusive memories they had recorded in their diary (mean = 6.53; median = 6.00; mode = 6; SD = 2.62; range 2–10).
Ratings of concentration disruption
Eighteen participants (82%) indicated that a single intrusion disrupts their concentration for longer than a minute on average, a majority (68%) indicated that it disrupts their concentration for longer than 5 min, while almost a third (32%) indicated that it disrupts their concentration for longer than 30 min (Fig. 1C). Difficulties in concentrating (in general) were experienced by most participants (Table 1), with 41% of the participants rating 7–10, on a scale from 0 to 10. When asked how disruptive intrusive memories are for concentration on a scale from 0 to 10, almost one-third of the participants (27%) indicated 5–7, and over a third of the participants (36%) indicated 8–10 (Table 1).
a0 = not at all disruptive; 10 = extremely disruptive; b0 = no concentration difficulties; 10 = extreme concentration difficulties; c0 = not at all useful; 10 = very useful; d0 = not at all; 10 = very easy; e0 = not at all; 10 = extremely easy; f0 = not at all; 10 = very much enjoy; g0 = not at all; 10 = very much; h0 = not at all; 10 = very likely to play; i0 = certainly not; 10 = certainly recommend; j0 = not at all acceptable; 10 = very acceptable; k0 = prefer doctor/psychologist; 10 = prefer smartphone/computer; SD, standard deviation.
Rating of intrusive memory occurrence over the week prior to feasibility study
On a scale from 0 (none) to 10 (numerous), 21/ 22 participants rated that they had experienced intrusive memories of trauma in the past week, with almost a third of the participants (27%) rating 8–10. Only one of the 22 participants indicated that they had not experienced any intrusive memories over the preceding week.
There was a significant positive correlation between the degree to which participants had experienced intrusive memories over the preceding week and the degree to which they experienced concentration difficulties (r = 0.64, n = 22, P = 0.001; Fig. 1D).
Feasibility and acceptability of smartphone-delivered interventions
All participants had access to a smartphone and were able to download the Tetris app. Ratings indicated that it was very easy to download the app (Table 1). The majority of participants (59%) reported that they were not familiar with Tetris prior to the study.
A majority of the participants (77%) rated 5–10 on a scale from 1 to 10 when asked whether they would find it useful to have fewer intrusive memories each day (Table 1).
All participants completed at least 15 min of game-play (mean = 18 min; SD = 2.4). Ratings suggested that most participants found Tetris game-play easy and enjoyable and that it distracted them from having unpleasant thoughts, images or feelings (Table 1). Furthermore, ratings suggested that they would likely play Tetris by themselves, in a different environment and that they would recommend it to a friend (Table 1).
Finally, a majority of the participants believed a computer game would be an acceptable way to reduce the daily frequency of intrusive memories (Table 1). Preference for doctor/psychologist versus technology was equivocal (Table 1). When asked ‘Why?’ replies included: ‘Because I can talk to the doctor and this will give me the feeling that he will understand my problems better’; ‘I like computer games, they are fun and make me feel good and happy. Playing the game makes me forget about my burdens’; ‘It is suitable because we rely on technology in almost everything. Smart phones are accessible and always nearby’; ‘Because it's more easy and the phone now is my best friend to us’.
Discussion
In this study we explored intrusive memories of trauma and concentration in young refugees predominantly from Syria who recently arrived in Sweden. Specifically, we assessed the feasibility and acceptability of methodology required to develop science-driven interventions aimed at reducing the frequency of intrusive memories of trauma. Levels of engagement with study procedures were highly promising. Results suggest a high prevalence of intrusive memories and concentration difficulties in this population and suggest a relationship between these symptoms: more than half of participants indicated that each intrusive memory disrupted their concentration for more than 5 min. Furthermore, participants reporting a higher frequency of intrusive memories also reported more concentration problems in general. Free report data suggested related functional impairment on tasks associated with adaptation to the host country such as language learning.
Supporting the development of novel and scalable psychological intervention procedures (Kazdin and Blase, Reference Kazdin and Blase2011; Kazdin and Rabbitt, Reference Kazdin and Rabbitt2013), our data indicate that young refugees are able and willing to (a) monitor a target symptom – the frequency count of intrusive memories of trauma for 1 week using a daily intrusion diary; (b) rate intrusive memories and concentration levels; and (c) complete one session of computer game-play (Tetris) on their smartphone as part of a mechanistically driven intervention procedure. These procedures were feasible without an interpreter, highlighting the potential large-scale application of such interventions in line with the concept of a future MOOI (Muñoz et al., Reference Muñoz, Bunge, Chen, Schueller, Bravin, Shaughnessy and Pérez-Stable2015).
We have argued that psychological treatments can benefit from advances in neuroscience (Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, Craske and Graybiel2014). Candidate mechanisms to alter trauma memory are offered by understanding the neuroscience of memory reconsolidation (Nader and Einarsson, Reference Nader and Einarsson2010), alongside insights from cognitive psychology (Andrade et al., Reference Andrade, Kavanagh and Baddeley1997) which capitalize on special features of intrusive memories – disrupting their mental imagery-based nature (Grey and Holmes, Reference Grey and Holmes2008; Engelhard et al., Reference Engelhard, van den Hout and Smeets2011; Pearson et al., Reference Pearson, Naselaris, Holmes and Kosslyn2015). Earlier experimental work shows that the effect of the cognitive-task intervention procedure in reducing intrusive memory frequency is specific to visuospatial tasks (e.g. Tetris) when compared with a verbal control game (Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, James, Kilford and Deeprose2010). Further, this method is effective not only for recently viewed trauma footage, but also for established memories (James et al., Reference James, Bonsall, Hoppitt, Tunbridge, Geddes, Milton and Holmes2015). A recent randomized control trial (Iyadurai and Holmes, Reference Iyadurai and Holmes2015) has successfully extended findings to a hospital emergency department setting with patients after a traumatic road traffic accident. A main advantage of this technological intervention, as underscored by results from the current study, is the potential for therapist-free delivery, enabling large-scale dissemination (for example via a MOOI; Muñoz et al., Reference Muñoz, Bunge, Chen, Schueller, Bravin, Shaughnessy and Pérez-Stable2015) and cultural acceptability to young refugees.
Limitations of this feasibility study include the small sample size and the sampling procedure, which may have resulted in a less representative sample, and possibly an underestimation of the scale of the problem (individuals with more severe psychological problems may be reluctant to volunteer for research studies). Further limitations include the lack of a control group and a full clinical interview to assess post-traumatic symptoms. Instead, we took a pragmatic approach, given the urgency of the topic. Whilst the study is modest, alongside the paucity of research in this area and the current ‘epidemic’ scale of traumatized refugees, results offer a small step towards scalable innovation: new ways to (a) develop and deliver psychological treatments for refugees by focusing on one target symptom (not a syndrome); (b) deliver research materials in another language; and (c) bridge a novel behavioural treatment approach for mental health with human memory theory.
Compliance with the 1-week intrusion measurement was high (78% of participants returned the diary by post). The 22% drop-out is remarkably low given that most participants were not familiar with the concept of paper postal services. Following the impact of war there has been no postal service in Syria for some years, with smartphone technology almost entirely replacing its use. We included a photograph of a post-box in the study material to explain the concept of paper diary return. Future studies harnessing technology for symptom monitoring would probably improve compliance (Malik et al., Reference Malik, Goodwin, Hoppitt and Holmes2014). This is corroborated by the finding that attitudes towards using smartphone technology for symptom monitoring and intervention delivery were generally positive.
Participants reported, on average, two intrusions a day, which is higher than the maximum score (‘daily’) on the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5 (CAPS-5) (Elhai et al., Reference Elhai, Gray, Kashdan and Franklin2005). A goal to reduce one intrusion per day could represent a change from CAPS-5 maximum (‘daily’) to mid-minimum (‘once-or-twice a week’/‘never’), representing a clinically meaningful outcome target for future research.
Participants indicated that their intrusive memories typically disrupted their concentration for several minutes (mode 5–10), suggesting that intrusive memories were not only distressing, but also impaired functioning by disrupting focus on daily tasks (see Supplemental Material). Given that persistent intrusive memories are common after trauma, even in the absence of full clinical criteria for PTSD, intrusive memory and their associated ‘hotspots’ in the underlying trauma memory constitute an important target for treatment (Grey et al., Reference Grey, Young and Holmes2002). Reducing intrusions holds promise to prevent the deterioration of mental health, and further to promote improvements in daily functioning, such as language-learning, key to integration for refugees entering a new country (Abbott, Reference Abbott2016; Nature Editorial, 2016), and associated with later employment (Daniel and Zurawan, Reference Daniel and Zurawan2010). Behavioural interventions focusing on a tractable target symptom (here intrusions) may have knock-on effects on other risk factors, promoting resilience and adaptive functioning. Scalable psychological treatments have long been needed (Kazdin and Blase, Reference Kazdin and Blase2011; Kazdin and Rabbitt, Reference Kazdin and Rabbitt2013) and the current refugee crisis makes their development an imperative direction for clinical psychological science.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Tina Morad, refugee hosting schemes, and academic and vocational institutions based in Sweden. We would also like to thank Pia Enebrink and Erik Anderson for helpful suggestions during the design of the study, and Iman Al Haj Ibrahim for helpful comments on the manuscript.
Authorship: E.A.H. and A.G. developed the study concept. Testing and data collection were performed by K.O.L. and E.E. and translation of the data was performed by M.M. and E.A.H. Data analysis and interpretation was conducted by R.M.V., O.M.K. and E.L.J. under the supervision of E.A.H. and A.G., R.M.V., O.M.K., E.L.J. and M.M. drafted the paper, and all authors provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final revision of the paper.
Financial support: E.A.H., E.L.J. and R.M.V. were supported by the Medical Research Council (UK) intramural programme (MC-A060-5PR50). R.M.V. is currently supported by a Marie Skłodowska-Curie Individual Fellowship from the European Union (Horizon 2020; Project ID 705641). O.M.K. was supported by the Erasmus Program Scholarship and the Fredrika Bremer Scholarship foundation. Funding to pay the Open Access publication charges for this article was provided by the UK Medical Research Council.
Conflicts of interest: Emily A. Holmes’ (E.A.H.) primary affiliation is the Department for Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet in Stockholm, Sweden. She also holds an honorary appointment at the Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK. E.A.H. is on the Board of Trustees of the Charity ‘MQ; transforming mental health’, and has received no remuneration for this role. E.A.H. is Associate Editor of Behaviour Research and Therapy, receives an honorarium for this role, and this role has no direct relation to the current contribution. E.A.H. is an Honorary Professor of Clinical Psychology at the University of Oxford, Department of Psychiatry and receives no remuneration for this role. E.A.H. is on the Board of Overseers for the charity ‘Children and War Foundation’, Oslo, Norway and this role has no direct relation to the current contribution. E.A.H. is on the Editorial Board of Cognitive Behaviour Therapy and Psychological Science; these roles have no direct relation to the current contribution. E.A.H. is a principal applicant on a Wellcome Trust Strategic Award, ‘Sleep and Circadian Neuroscience Institute (SCNi) for Mental Health’ (ref. 098461/Z/12/Z) and this role has no direct relation to the current contribution. E.A.H. has a co-authored book on Imagery in Cognitive Therapy (OUP, 2011), which receives royalties. E.A.H. receives travels expenses, some subsistence and associated speaker honorarium for keynote at conferences, e.g. EABCT 2016. E.A.H. presents at clinical training workshops, some of which include a fee, e.g. Copenhagen, Iceland. E.A.H. has received the Humboldt Foundation Friedrich Wilhelm Bessel Research Award (Germany), 2013; and the American Psychological Association (APA) Distinguished Scientific Award for Early Career Contribution to Psychology in the area of Psychopathology, 2014, and these are not directly related to the current contribution. The other authors have no conflicts of interest with respect to this publication.
Supplementary material
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S135246581600062X
Comments
No Comments have been published for this article.