Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T16:28:36.111Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Voter responses to female candidates' voice pitch: experimental evidence from Japan

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 February 2023

Claire Bowern
Affiliation:
Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
Rieko Kage*
Affiliation:
University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
Frances Rosenbluth
Affiliation:
Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
Seiki Tanaka
Affiliation:
University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: kage@waka.c.u-tokyo.ac.jp
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Politicians' voice pitch is known to affect voters' evaluation of the candidates in the US. But to what extent is this true outside of the American context? To address this question, we conducted an original survey experiment in Japan. Our findings are threefold. First, in contrast to previous studies, voters in Japan do not systematically prefer lower-pitched over higher-pitched female politicians. Second, our findings suggest heterogeneity in the effect of voice pitch by voters' gender – while Japanese women are indifferent as to female candidates' pitch levels, men are more likely to prefer female candidates who speak at lower pitch. Third, preliminary analyses reveal limited evidence that female candidates' political experience conditions the effect of voice pitch over voters' willingness to vote for that candidate. Our findings suggest that lowering pitch is likely to increase female candidates' electoral prospects by attracting male voters without backlash from female voters.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

That voters assess political candidates based on how they look and sound is well-known. Candidates who are considered to be physically attractive tend to be viewed more favorably than those seen as less attractive (Sigelman et al., Reference Sigelman, Sigelman and Fowler1987; Verhulst et al., Reference Verhulst, Lodge and Lavine2010; Stockemer and Praino, Reference Stockemer and Praino2015). Voters typically prefer taller, as opposed to shorter, political candidates, not only in the US (McCann, Reference McCann2001; Stulp et al., Reference Stulp, Buunk, Verhulst and Pollet2013), but elsewhere as well (Sorokowski, Reference Sorokowski2010). Attire also impacts voter assessments of candidates (Rosenberg et al., Reference Rosenberg, Kahn and Tran1991), while smiling can improve one's chances of winning an election (Horiuchi et al., Reference Horiuchi, Komatsu and Nakaya2012).Footnote 1 These studies suggest that in addition to various correlates of electoral success such as candidates' ideological positions and political experiences, voters are also influenced, whether directly or indirectly, by candidates' physical cues.

Among different physical traits, this study zeroes in on the impact of female candidates' voice pitch on voters' assessments of those candidates. Recent studies of female representation typically find limited evidence of outright discrimination against female candidates (Lawless and Pearson, Reference Lawless and Pearson2008; Palmer and Simon, Reference Palmer and Simon2010; Teele et al., Reference Teele, Kalla and Rosenbluth2018; Kage et al., Reference Kage, Rosenbluth and Tanaka2019; Schwarz and Coppock, Reference Schwarz and Coppock2021). Yet studies in the field of psychology show that individuals' voice pitch has a considerable impact over how speakers are perceived, including their strength (Sell et al., Reference Sell, Bryant, Cosmides, Tooby, Sznycer, Von Rueden, Krauss and Gurven2010; Puts et al., Reference Puts, Apicella and Cárdenas2012; Armstrong et al., Reference Armstrong, Lee and Feinberg2019), formidability (Aung et al., Reference Aung, Rosenfield and Puts2021), hierarchical rank (Ko et al., Reference Ko, Sadler and Galinsky2015), attractiveness (Pisanski et al., Reference Pisanski, Mishra and Rendall2012; Tigue et al., Reference Tigue, Borak, O'Connor, Schandl and Feinberg2012; Leongómez et al., Reference Leongómez, Binter, Kubicová, Stolařová, Klapilová, Havlíček and Craig Roberts2014), trustworthiness (Montano et al., Reference Montano, Tigue, Isenstein, Barclay and Feinberg2017) and even aversiveness (O'Connor and Barclay, Reference O'Connor and Barclay2018). In recent years, political scientists have begun to build on these insights and have shown voice pitch to shape perceptions of leadership capacity (Klofstad et al., Reference Klofstad, Anderson and Peters2012), vote choice and/or election outcomes (Klofstad, Reference Klofstad2016, Reference Klofstad2017; Klofstad et al., Reference Klofstad, Nowicki and Anderson2016; Banai et al., Reference Banai, Laustsen, Banai and Bovan2018), and the amount of political discussion (Dietrich et al., Reference Dietrich, Hayes and O'Brien2019). Female politicians are often aware of the potential impacts of voice pitch. As the examples of Hillary Clinton and Angela Merkel illustrate, female politicians often change their voice pitch to attract more voters, while male politicians do not need to do so (Jones, Reference Jones2016; Boussalis et al., Reference Boussalis, Coan, Holman and Müller2021), suggesting that gender bias continues to affect female politicians in subtler ways, if not through blatant discrimination.

Studies find that US voters typically prefer both female and male candidates with lower pitch (Klofstad et al., Reference Klofstad, Anderson and Peters2012; Klofstad, Reference Klofstad2016), but we know little about the generalizability of these findings beyond the American context.Footnote 2 It is important to assess the extent to which voters' preferences for lower-pitched candidates travel to other contexts.

This study draws on an original survey experiment conducted to examine whether voters in Japan, as in the US, prefer low-pitched female candidates. As is well-known, Japan is a more gender-stratified society than the US. At 9.7%, Japan has the lowest rate of female national legislators in the developed world (Inter-Parliamentary Union, n.d.). A recent study finds surprisingly little discrimination against female candidates among the Japanese electorate (Kage et al., Reference Kage, Rosenbluth and Tanaka2019), but it is possible that Japan's female politicians suffer from subtler bias via other channels, including their voice pitch. Japanese women typically speak at higher pitch levels than Caucasian women (e.g., speakers of American English (Yamazawa and Hollien, Reference Yamazawa and Hollien1992) or Dutch (Van Bezooijen, Reference Van Bezooijen1995)), and this has been attributed to the cultural norm of exhibiting powerlessness among Japanese women as well as a cultural preference for higher pitch (Van Bezooijen, Reference Van Bezooijen1995). On the one hand, if these cultural preferences extend to candidate choice, voters may prefer higher-pitched female candidates over lower-pitched candidates. Alternatively, Japanese voters, much like their American counterparts, may prefer low-pitched as opposed to high-pitched political candidates. Whether, and to what extent, Japanese women's relatively high voice pitch impacts their electability thus poses an important empirical puzzle.

We also ask whether different segments of the population prefer female candidates of different pitch levels. If Japanese voters do respond to candidates' pitch levels, an exploration of who among the voters reward or punish candidates with higher or lower pitch would illuminate which voters could be won over or lost if candidates changed their pitch. This study takes first steps in this direction by examining the differential impacts of different voice pitch.

We conducted a survey experiment in March 2021 in Japan. We asked respondents to listen to a recording of a hypothetical female candidate's speech and to indicate whether they would vote for that candidate. We find that in contrast to American voters, who typically prefer female candidates with lower voice pitch, Japanese respondents were, on average, indifferent between female candidates of higher and lower pitch. But Japanese preferences over voice pitch were conditioned by gender, with male voters being more willing to vote for low-pitched female candidates than female voters. In addition, contrary to our expectation, we find that the effect of voice pitch on voters' willingness to vote for a candidate is little affected by that candidate's political experience.

Existing studies report that outside the realm of politics, men typically find women with higher voice pitch to be more attractive (Borkowska and Pawlowski, Reference Borkowska and Pawlowski2011; Fraccaro et al., Reference Fraccaro, O'Connor, Re, Jones, DeBruine and Feinberg2013).Footnote 3 Our findings suggest that in Japan as well, men use a different metric for evaluating whether women are fit for political office than when they assess general ‘attractiveness.’ Further, our finding that male voters prefer lower-pitched female candidates more than female voters also points to different mechanisms of candidate evaluation by voters' gender. While male voters may prefer those who sound more authoritative, female voters do not associate voice pitch with candidate quality. Moreover, while previous studies suggest the existence of backlash effect of women unnaturally lowering their voice pitch (Fraccaro et al. (Reference Fraccaro, O'Connor, Re, Jones, DeBruine and Feinberg2013), Levon and Ye (Reference Levon and Ye2020), see also Jones (Reference Jones2016)), our findings suggest that all else equal, female candidates in Japan may be able to attract more votes from male voters by lowering their voice pitch while avoiding backlash from female voters.

More broadly, our findings provide new evidence to recent findings from the US, Japan and other developed democracies that voters do not exhibit outright discrimination against female politicians (Kage et al., Reference Kage, Rosenbluth and Tanaka2019),Footnote 4 but that their sensitivity to female candidates' physical cues, including physical appearance and voice pitch (Carpinella et al., Reference Carpinella, Hehman, Freeman and Johnson2016; Klofstad, Reference Klofstad2016, Reference Klofstad2017; Klofstad et al., Reference Klofstad, Nowicki and Anderson2016; Bauer and Carpinella, Reference Bauer and Carpinella2021; Boussalis et al., Reference Boussalis, Coan, Holman and Müller2021) indirectly place an additional burden on women who seek to run for office. In the concluding section, we discuss the implications of our findings in more detail.

2. Voice pitch and voting behavior: the argument

Voice pitch is a perceptual measure of how high or low a voice sounds. There are several acoustic characteristics that contribute to voice pitch, and the easiest measure is F0, or fundamental frequency. F0 is typically measured in Hertz (Hz), and it may be perceived as being ‘high’ or ‘low’ – lower values of Hz correspond to lower pitch, and vice versa. Pitch is inversely correlated with the thickness of the vocal fold (Hollien, Reference Hollien2014), and women typically speak at higher pitch than men. Other factors also shape voice pitch, such as height and weight (Fitch and Giedd, Reference Fitch and Giedd1999), age (Pisanski et al., Reference Pisanski, Oleszkiewicz, Plachetka, Gmiterek and Reby2018) as well as socio-cultural factors (Van Bezooijen, Reference Van Bezooijen1995).

Studies in the field of psychology show that individuals' voice pitch has a considerable impact over how speakers are perceived, including their strength (Sell et al., Reference Sell, Bryant, Cosmides, Tooby, Sznycer, Von Rueden, Krauss and Gurven2010; Puts et al., Reference Puts, Apicella and Cárdenas2012; Armstrong et al., Reference Armstrong, Lee and Feinberg2019), formidability (Aung et al., Reference Aung, Rosenfield and Puts2021), hierarchical rank (Ko et al., Reference Ko, Sadler and Galinsky2015), attractiveness (Pisanski et al., Reference Pisanski, Mishra and Rendall2012; Tigue et al., Reference Tigue, Borak, O'Connor, Schandl and Feinberg2012; Leongómez et al., Reference Leongómez, Binter, Kubicová, Stolařová, Klapilová, Havlíček and Craig Roberts2014), trustworthiness (Montano et al., Reference Montano, Tigue, Isenstein, Barclay and Feinberg2017) and aversiveness (O'Connor and Barclay, Reference O'Connor and Barclay2018). Accordingly, changing one's pitch also shifts assessments of the speaker (Fraccaro et al., Reference Fraccaro, O'Connor, Re, Jones, DeBruine and Feinberg2013; Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Tracy, Ho and Henrich2016; Sorokowski et al., Reference Sorokowski, Puts, Johnson, Żółkiewicz, Oleszkiewicz, Sorokowska, Kowal, Borkowska and Pisanski2019). Importantly, these are character traits that are valued in politicians, and not surprisingly, political scientists have recently begun to build on these insights to show voice pitch to impact perceptions of leadership capacity (Klofstad et al., Reference Klofstad, Anderson and Peters2012), vote choice and/or election outcomes (Klofstad, Reference Klofstad2016, Reference Klofstad2017; Klofstad et al., Reference Klofstad, Nowicki and Anderson2016; Banai et al., Reference Banai, Laustsen, Banai and Bovan2018), and the amount of political discussion (Dietrich et al., Reference Dietrich, Hayes and O'Brien2019).

Existing studies find that both female and male voters in the US prefer low-pitched over high-pitched candidates, regardless of candidate gender (e.g., Klofstad et al., Reference Klofstad, Anderson and Peters2012, Klofstad, Reference Klofstad2016). Klofstad (Reference Klofstad2016, Reference Klofstad2017), in particular, finds that voice pitch can influence vote choice and, by implication, electoral outcomes. However, previous studies of the impact of voice pitch on voting behavior have been largely conducted on US voters, and we know little about the effect of voice pitch in other contexts.

The small handful of studies that have been conducted outside of the US largely concur that voters prefer lower-pitched candidates. A study of 51 presidential elections around the world found lower-pitched candidates to be more likely to emerge victorious than higher-pitched candidates (Banai et al., Reference Banai, Banai and Bovan2017). Another study of 69 presidential and prime ministerial countries also found lower-pitched candidates more likely to win elections than higher-pitched (Banai et al., Reference Banai, Laustsen, Banai and Bovan2018). Although these are valuable studies, they are observational in design, making it difficult to isolate the effects of pitch from other confounding factors that may have been correlated with pitch, such as age, height, weight, and socio-cultural factors. Both studies also lack information on voter attributes, so we know little about the extent to which assessments of voice pitch differ across different segments of the population. For instance, do female voters prefer low-pitched female candidates more than male voters, or vice versa?

In a pioneering study of the impact of voice pitch on Japanese voters, Okada (Reference Okada2016) reports that as in the US, Japanese respondents, regardless of gender, prefer lower-pitched female politicians over those who are higher-pitched, but the sample size is small, at just 74 college students, and the voice treatments were not randomly assigned. We thus know little as to whether Japan's female candidates would fare better electorally by changing their voice pitch, and by attracting the votes of which segments of the population. We empirically examine these questions by using a broader sample of the Japanese population as well as random assignments of voice pitch.

We also seek to push the literature forward by assessing how candidate experience mediates the effects of voice pitch. Klofstad et al. (Reference Klofstad, Anderson and Nowicki2015) finds that voters typically prefer lower-pitched candidates because it serves as a signal for age as well as experience. But pitch can also signal other candidate qualities such as authoritativeness and/or leadership capacity. If Japanese voters do prefer low-pitched over high-pitched female candidates, to what extent is it because they prefer those with more political experience? This paper seeks to disentangle the effects of pitch and political experience and to ask whether, and to what extent, political experience mediates the effect of voice pitch on voters.

With our motivations in mind, we now derive theoretical expectations about the effect of voice pitch from two contrasting perspectives: (1) the leadership role perspective, and (2) the societal norm perspective.

2.1 Leadership role perspective

First, Japanese voters may assess women of more professional occupations, including politicians, by different standards than women of less professional occupations. If this is the case, like US voters, Japanese voters may prefer female politicians with lower voice pitch, as low pitch is often associated with professional candidate qualities such as competence and trustworthiness (Klofstad et al., Reference Klofstad, Anderson and Peters2012; Carpinella et al., Reference Carpinella, Hehman, Freeman and Johnson2016; Klofstad, Reference Klofstad2016). From this perspective, we hypothesize that voters in Japan prefer female candidates who speak with lower voice pitch than those with higher.

As noted earlier, voter impressions of candidates' voice pitch may also be conditioned by that candidate's political experience. To the extent that perceived authority is an important trait for political leaders and low voice pitch signals a candidate's authority, we may expect voters to prefer female candidates with low voice pitch over those with high pitch. From this perspective, high-pitched female candidates with less political experience should be the least preferred, with high-pitched candidates with more experience and low-pitched candidates with less experience falling somewhere in between. Which is more valued by voters, more political experience or lower voice pitch, presents an empirical question.

Women and men may also have different expectations for female leaders. Female more than male voters may welcome female candidates who are breaking out of the traditional mold to pursue political careers. By the same token, we may expect female voters to be more willing to vote for low-pitched female candidates than those who are high-pitched, as lower pitch serves as a signal of greater liberation from ‘traditional’ gender norms (Ohara, Reference Ohara1999).

We summarize three hypotheses from the leadership role perspective as follows:

H1-1: Japanese voters are more likely to prefer female candidates with lower voice pitch than those with higher pitch.

H1-2: Japanese voters are more likely to prefer experienced female candidates with lower voice pitch more than less experienced female candidates with lower pitch or more experienced candidates with higher pitch. They are least likely to prefer less experienced candidates with high pitch.

H1-3: Japanese female voters are more likely to prefer female candidates with lower voice pitch more than male voters.

2.2 Societal norm perspective

In contrast to the leadership role perspective, voters may assess political candidates in the same way that they assess ‘ordinary’ women. Japan not only has few female politicians, but it is a much more highly gender-stratified society in general than most other developed democracies. As of 2021, only 12.6% of seats of boards of the largest publicly listed companies in Japan were women, compared to 37.8% in the UK, 36.0% in Germany, or 29.7% in the US (OECD, n.d.). As of 2020, only 13.3% of managers in Japan were women, much lower than the 41.1% in the US, 35.5% in France, or 28.1% in Germany (OECD, n.d.). Given the high degree of gender stratification in society and relative paucity of female candidates in Japan, congruity with societal gender roles may play a larger role in voters' assessments of candidates' voice pitch (see also Eagly and Karau, Reference Eagly and Karau2002, for role congruity theory). From this perspective, voters should prefer female politicians with high voice pitch because Japanese women in general are expected to speak at high pitch. We thus hypothesize that voters are likely to prefer female candidates with higher voice pitch than those with lower pitch. If this is the case, as with Japanese women of other occupations, female politicians who deviate from this expectation by speaking at low pitch may be penalized more than those who speak at high pitch.

Further, if broader societal norms are more important than leadership expectations in shaping voter preferences, in contrast to the leadership role perspective, we expect Japanese voters to be more likely to prefer candidates who are more similar to the ‘average’ Japanese female, i.e., high-pitched and inexperienced in politics. For the same reason, they should least prefer low-pitched female candidates with more experience.

Finally, because male voters are typically more likely to value traditional gender roles, we expect Japanese male voters to prefer female candidates with high voice pitch than female voters.

Hence, we derive the following three hypotheses from the societal norm perspective:

H2-1: Japanese voters are more likely to prefer female candidates with higher voice pitch than those with lower pitch.

H2-2: Japanese voters are likely to most prefer high-pitched female candidates with little political experience and least prefer low-pitched female candidates with more experience.

H2-3: Japanese male voters more than female voters are more likely to prefer female candidates with higher voice pitch.

3. Research design

We conducted an original survey experiment in March 2021 upon approval by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Groningen. The sample was randomly drawn by a Japanese survey firm, Rakuten Insight, from their opt-in online panel, stratified on key demographic variables of age and residential locations.Footnote 5 We obtained responses from 414 respondents. As noted above, studies show voice pitch to be influenced by many factors such as one's height and weight (Fitch and Giedd, Reference Fitch and Giedd1999), age (Pisanski et al., Reference Pisanski, Oleszkiewicz, Plachetka, Gmiterek and Reby2018) as well as socio-cultural factors (Van Bezooijen, Reference Van Bezooijen1995). We thus follow most existing studies on the effects of voice pitch in using an experimental design, which allows us to isolate the effects of different voice pitch levels from other confounding factors. Because the experiment was conducted as part of another study, we over-sampled female respondents, and this will be accounted for in the analyses below. The English-language translation of the question wording for the items is provided in the Appendix, and the original Japanese-language version of the survey is available upon request. Table A.1 in the Appendix provides summary statistics for the respondents.

Because this survey involved listening to a voice recording, at the beginning of the survey, we asked the respondents to complete the survey in a quiet location, and, if they were not, they were given the option of terminating the survey. After asking a series of demographic questions, we randomly split the respondents into one of four treatment groups and asked them to listen to a recording of a speech by a female political candidate. Half of the respondents were informed that the speaker was a 45-year-old candidate who has been elected once, and the other half were told that the speaker was a 45-year-old candidate who has been elected five times. Half of the respondents were given the same speech at a higher pitch, and the remaining half were given the same speech at a lower pitch, resulting in four treatment groups, as shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Four treatment groups

We chose 45 years old as our experimental condition for several reasons. First, as noted earlier, women's voice pitch levels usually co-vary with age, with women typically speaking at lower pitch levels as they become older, so it would not be credible to provide high-pitched recordings of older women. Secondly, a younger candidate, say, aged 35, would not be credible to Japanese respondents, for legal reasons. Japanese law stipulates that citizens must be at least 25 before they can run for public office.Footnote 6 Although we did not specify whether this candidate was running for national or local office, a five-term candidate aged 35 running for the House of Representatives would have been elected for the first time when they were roughly 21, given that Japanese House of Representatives elections occur on average once every 2.8 years (Kawato, Reference Kawato and Kawasaki2018). This would stretch credulity, since they would not have been legally eligible to run at that age. Japanese House of Councillors (Upper House) and local elections occur even less frequently, every 6 and 4 years, respectively, so a five-term candidate would have had to been elected for the first time when they were in their teens. Lastly, our 45-year-old hypothetical female candidate is not without precedent. In the House of Representatives, Yuko Obuchi, a member of the Liberal Democratic Party (LDP), was first elected to office when she was 26 and won her fifth term when she was 38; similarly, Seiko Noda, also of the LDP, first won office when she was 33 and her fifth term when she was 44. These are highly successful female politicians, but they suggest that our hypothetical scenario is certainly plausible.

We created two voice pitch levels, high and low. We constructed the two pitch levels by digitally manipulating the pitch track of a voice recording using Praat, a software program for linguistic analysis (Boersma and Weenink, Reference Boersma and Weenink2021). We then randomly assigned one of the four conditions in Table 1 to respondents, which allows us to estimate the causal impact of our intervention. Note that as shown in Table A.2 in the Appendix, randomization was not successful for some of the covariates, owing to our small sample size.Footnote 7 Thus, in addition to t-tests, we provide regression results with covariates for all analyses to increase precision of our estimates. Following existing studies, we included the covariates of respondents' age, gender, education (college-educated or not), and income levels. The Male voter variable was coded 1 for male respondents and 0 for female respondents. The College variable was coded 1 for respondents who have a college degree or above and 0 otherwise. The Age variable denotes respondents' age. The Income variable consists of a categorical measure ranging from 0 (household income of below 2 million yen a year) to 5 (household income of more than 10 million yen a year).

The low-pitched recording was the original recording by the speaker, one of the members of the research team. The high-pitched version was 50 Hz higher than the original recording and fell between 180 and 380 Hz. This high-pitched version falls within a typical Japanese woman's normal pitch range (Imaida, Reference Imaida2006). After listening to the 45-second recording, respondents were asked whether or not they would vote for the candidate, assuming that the candidate was running in the respondents' electoral district for the party that the respondent supports.

The text of the speech was taken from an actual female candidate to increase the external validity of our findings but modified for length. The English-language translation of the speech excerpt is shown as follows, and the original Japanese-language text of the speech is available upon request.

The Japanese population, which has steadily increased over 100 years since around 1900, will return to 1900 levels over the next 100 years. What kind of future can we create in an era of depopulation and super-aging that is proceeding now at unprecedented speed? We now stand at a turning point. It is also a turning point in politics. What remains for the next generation should not be a burden or debt but dreams and possibilities. I strongly hope that all children in Japan will feel that there is some hope for the future.

The issues of aging and children, which are the main issues discussed in this text, may be a valence issue, but welfare and child policies are also often considered to be ‘female’ issues (Ono and Yamada, Reference Ono and Yamada2020). This also justifies our focus on only female politicians in this paper. Still, it is also possible that female voters may be more concerned about aging and children's issues and thus prefer the speech more than their male counterparts. This point underscores the importance of examining within-gender variation in the impact of voice pitch on voting intentions.

Two further caveats are in order. First, with just 414 respondents, our analyses are generally under-powered.Footnote 8 Given the limited sample size, our analyses below focus on the effect of voice pitch and the interaction effect of voice pitch and respondents' gender, controlling for experience and other covariates. Second, because we did not pre-register our survey experiment prior to data collection, the following analyses chiefly focus on our three main variables of interest, i.e., voice pitch, political experience, and respondents' gender. Our analyses of the effects of respondents' education level, age, and income are largely exploratory.

4. Findings

First, we report the distribution of respondents who were willing to vote for the hypothetical candidate, by pitch level. Figure 1 shows that controlling for candidate experience, differences in pitch levels did not lead to systematic differences in voters' willingness to vote for that candidate. In total, 62.8% of respondents who were given the higher-pitched recording and 62.7% of respondents who were given the lower-pitched recording expressed willingness to vote for the candidate.

Figure 1. Percentage of respondents willing to vote for the candidate, by treatment group.

Note: N = 414.

Model 1 of Table 2 confirms the initial exploration. All of the results shown use ordinary least squares models. Controlling for candidate experience, the effect of high voice pitch is negative but fails to reach statistical significance at the 10% level (model 3 includes respondents' covariates).Footnote 9 These results fail to lend support to either H1-1 or H2-1.

Table 2. Regression analysis by treatment groups

Standard errors in parentheses.

*P < 0.10, **P < 0.05, ***P < 0.01.

The results shown in Figure 1 also suggest the possibility of an interaction effect between politicians' voice pitch and political experience. Respondents exhibit the greatest willingness to vote for the low-pitched candidate with more political experience (i.e., five terms), while voters are also more willing to vote for inexperienced candidate with high voice pitch. The low-pitched candidate with one-term experience was the least preferred. These results appear to be more consistent with the leadership role perspective, especially H1-2, than the societal norm perspective, especially H2-2. However, models 2 and 4 of Table 2, which interact the voice treatment with the experience treatment, show that the interaction term between candidate voice pitch and experience fails to reach statistical significance at the 10% level.

We now examine the extent to which respondent characteristics condition the effects of our experimental variables. Table 3 reports the results.

Table 3. Regression analysis by respondents' covariates

Standard errors in parentheses.

*P < 0.10, **P < 0.05, ***P < 0.01.

First, the leadership role perspective had led us to expect female voters to prefer female candidates with low voice pitch more than male voters (H1-3), while the societal norms perspective suggests that male voters prefer high-pitched female candidates more than female voters (H2-3). Table 3 and Figure 2 show that neither hypothesis is supported. However, although the interaction term between voice pitch and respondent gender in model 1 of Table 3 fails to reach statistical significance at the 10% level, Figure 2, which shows the marginal effect of voice pitch by respondents' gender controlling for other covariates and candidates' experience based on model 1 of Table 3, reveals that male voters in fact prefer low-pitched female candidates more than female voters $( X_{MaleLow}{\rm} = 0{\rm .547, \;\ S}{\rm .D} . = 0{\rm .502\ vs\ }X_{FemaleLow}{\rm} = 0{\rm .317, \;\ S}{\rm .D} . = 0{\rm .466) }$.Footnote 10 The figure also shows that Japanese women are largely indifferent to candidate voice pitch levels, whereas male voters prefer lower-pitched female candidates more than higher-pitched. These results suggest that different mechanisms may be at work for male and female voters, with male voters being more influenced by candidate's authoritative qualities.

Figure 2. Marginal effect of candidates' voice pitch by respondents' gender.

Note: The figure is generated using results from model 1 of Table 3.

We also conduct several exploratory analyses using respondents' other covariates: college education, age, and income. Model 2 of Table A.2 in the Appendix shows that the interaction between candidates' voice and respondents' college education is statistically significant at the 10% level, but Figure A.2 in the Appendix shows the interaction term exerts limited impact over candidate choice. Models 3 and 4 of Table 3 fail to find support for the view that the interaction term between candidates' voice and respondents' age and income level, respectively, affects voters' preferences. Older voters typically harbor stronger prejudices than younger voters (Von Hippel et al., Reference Von Hippel, Silver and Lynch2000; Gonsalkorale et al., Reference Gonsalkorale, Sherman and Klauer2009), thus making them more likely to support candidates who conform to traditional gender roles. But we find that respondents' age does not affect the likelihood of supporting candidates with different pitch levels. The null finding may be driven by the fact that our sample only includes respondents who are aged between 20 and 50. Future research should explore whether including older respondents would change the results. We also explore the interaction effect between political experience and respondents' gender. Note here again that due to limited sample size, our analyses should be interpreted with caution. But none reached statistical significance at the 10% level. These findings fail to support either the leadership role perspective or the societal norms perspective. Lastly, we note that male respondents are considerably more likely to vote for any of the four candidates compared to female respondents (models 1–4 of Table 3).

In sum, our results offer limited support for either the leadership role perspective or the societal norm perspective. They confirm our broader theoretical expectation of significant interaction effects among the two variables of candidates' voice pitch and respondents' gender, but in a different direction from we had expected in H1-3 and H2-3; males more than females preferred female candidates with lower pitch. Although our results are consistent with previous studies that have been conducted in Japan and other countries that men are more willing to support low-pitched female candidates than high-pitched female candidates, our finding that male voters would be more willing to do so than female voters presents a puzzle that deserves further exploration in future studies.

5. Discussion

Politicians' voice pitch is known to affect voters' assessment of politicians. The case of Japan offers a window into examining the generalizability of the findings of earlier studies, as well as heterogeneity in the effects.

Our findings push the literature forward by expanding the geographical scope to Japan, a society in which traditional gender stereotypes are stronger than in the US, where most studies on the effects of voice pitch have been conducted. We found that in contrast to earlier findings by Banai et al. (Reference Banai, Laustsen, Banai and Bovan2018), Banai et al. (Reference Banai, Banai and Bovan2017) and Okada (Reference Okada2016) that voters, regardless of gender, prefer low-pitched female candidates, Japan's female voters are indifferent to female politicians' voice pitch; only male voters in Japan preferred lower-pitched female politicians than higher-pitched. These findings suggest that different mechanisms may drive women and men's responses to female candidates' voice pitch. Male voters, on the one hand, may look for candidates who sound more professional or authoritative. Japanese women, on the other hand, might care more about substantive issues rather than physical traits such as voice pitch; or, they may not view voice pitch as a proxy for candidate quality at all. Why the mechanisms may differ in Japan, as well as the broader generalizability of our findings, presents a fruitful avenue for future research.

We also found limited evidence that political experience conditions the effect of voice pitch. Voters' preferences for low-pitched or high-pitched female candidates were little affected by their knowledge that the candidate had more or less political experience. Although studies have found voters to view voice pitch as a signal for experience, our findings suggest that at least for Japan's male voters, voice pitch signals something other than experience, possibly authoritativeness and/or leadership capacity. Future work should further disentangle the effects of pitch itself from these other qualities.

Our findings are also revealing because they point to a dilemma for women who aspire to political careers. Heterosexual female candidates may face a trade-off between appealing to male voters and remaining attractive to their partners as they gain in political experience. Studies suggest that men typically find higher-pitched women to be more ‘attractive,’ whether in the US (Feinberg et al., Reference Feinberg, DeBruine, Jones and Perrett2008; Puts et al., Reference Puts, Barndt, Welling, Dawood and Burriss2011), or Japan (Oguchi and Kikuchi, Reference Oguchi and Kikuchi1997). For women, lowering pitch in order to improve electoral prospects may come at the cost of ‘attractiveness.’ If women are aware of this dilemma, they may refrain from running for office in the first place. Our study thus adds to the growing body of work that point to subtler forms of bias, rather than outright discrimination per se, that leads to female under-representation.

Finally, we point to at least two directions for future research. First, our results are likely to be conditioned by the information environment. Studies show that subtle cues such as voice pitch are more effective in changing voters' perceptions when information about politicians is not available (Klofstad, Reference Klofstad2017). Accordingly, the effect of voice pitch may be more important in low information environments when voters have only limited information on political candidates. The combined effects of greater information on the candidates, including their policy positions or personal backgrounds, and voice pitch, present a fruitful avenue for further inquiry.

Second, even if male voters prefer female politicians with low voice pitch, there is likely to be a limit. Studies show that when individuals' voice pitch is perceived as being unnaturally low or unnaturally high, there is a backlash effect (Fraccaro et al., Reference Fraccaro, O'Connor, Re, Jones, DeBruine and Feinberg2013; Levon and Ye, Reference Levon and Ye2020), and the listener ceases to view the speaker in a positive light. These findings are consistent with the societal norm perspective. In the present study, we used the 130–330 Hz pitch range for our low-pitched treatment, yet whether, and at which point, low pitch provokes voter backlash against political candidates in Japan and other countries also deserves exploration in future studies.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1468109922000354 and https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataset.xhtmlpersistentId=doi:10.7910/DVN/0V8H6K.

Acknowledgement

Rieko Kage thanks Susan Pharr for all of her support and encouragement over the years and wishes her a healthy and happy retirement. An early version of the paper was presented at ‘Japan in the World: A Symposium in Honor of Susan Pharr,’ Harvard University, 25 May 2021. The human subject protocol of the research was evaluated and approved by the Institutional Review Board, University of Groningen, and the research was conducted following the approval. The authors are listed in alphabetical order. We thank Christina Davis, Andy Eggers, Junko Kato, Robert Pekkanen, Susan Pharr, and three anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions on earlier versions of this paper. Rio Tsuchiya provided excellent research assistance. Please note that one of the co-authors, Frances Rosenbluth, sadly passed away in November 2021. But she contributed fully to the design of the survey experiment as well as the writing of the manuscript before she passed away.

Footnotes

1 See also Asano and Patterson (Reference Asano and Patterson2018) for more nuanced findings.

3 Some studies find that individuals with exaggerated pitch levels may be found to be unattractive (Fraccaro et al., Reference Fraccaro, O'Connor, Re, Jones, DeBruine and Feinberg2013; Levon and Ye Reference Levon and Ye2020).

4 According to Kage et al. (Reference Kage, Rosenbluth and Tanaka2019), Japanese voters actually prefer female over male candidates and they do not expect them to stress traditionally ‘female’ issue-areas such as social policy. The study also reveals that female politicians are still under-represented for other reasons such as gate-keeper effects. Other studies point to persistent gender bias against female politicians in Japan (Ono and Yamada, Reference Ono and Yamada2020).

5 Because our study sought to examine the attitudes of the working-age population, we surveyed respondents between the ages of 20 and 50.

6 Public Offices Elections Act, Article 10.

7 The use of block randomization may mitigate potential imbalances in covariates and increase efficiency for a small-N study (Horiuchi et al., Reference Horiuchi, Imai and Taniguchi2007; Gerber and Green, Reference Gerber and Green2012).

8 When alpha is set at 0.05 and beta is at 0.8, a comparison between inexperienced and experienced politicians with lower voice is well powered. But other experimental comparisons are under-powered with the same specifications, and future research should use our study as a reference when calculating sufficient sample size.

9 In addition to simple regression analyses without covariates, as a robustness check, we also report results controlling for respondents' gender, education, income, and age.

10 Figure A.1 in the Appendix also reports means and standard deviations by the four groups. Adjusting for multiple comparisons (using Bonferroni methods) does not affect the main results. The use of a triple interaction model (i.e., voice pitch × experience × respondents' gender) also yields similar results (Table A.3 in the Appendix).

References

Armstrong, MM, Lee, AJ and Feinberg, DR (2019) A house of cards: bias in perception of body size mediates the relationship between voice pitch and perceptions of dominance. Animal Behaviour 147, 4351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asano, M and Patterson, DP (2018) Smiles, turnout, candidates, and the winning of district seats: evidence from the 2015 local elections in Japan. Politics and the Life Sciences 37, 1631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aung, T, Rosenfield, KA and Puts, D (2021) Male voice pitch mediates the relationship between objective and perceived formidability. Evolution and Human Behavior 42, 121129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banai, IP, Banai, B and Bovan, K (2017) Vocal characteristics of presidential candidates can predict the outcome of actual elections. Evolution and Human Behavior 38, 309314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banai, B, Laustsen, L, Banai, IP and Bovan, K (2018) Presidential, but not prime minister, candidates with lower pitched voices stand a better chance of winning the election in conservative countries. Evolutionary Psychology 16, 1474704918758736.Google Scholar
Bauer, NM and Carpinella, C (2021) Visual information and candidate evaluations: the influence of feminine and masculine images on support for female candidates. Political Research Quarterly 71, 395407.Google Scholar
Boersma, P and Weenink, D (2021) Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 6.1.49, retrieved 12 June 2021. Available at http://www.praat.org/.Google Scholar
Borkowska, B and Pawlowski, B (2011) Female voice frequency in the context of dominance and attractiveness perception. Animal Behaviour 82, 5559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boussalis, C, Coan, TG, Holman, M and Müller, S (2021) Gender, candidate emotional expression, and voter reactions during televised debates. American Political Science Review 115, 12421257.Google Scholar
Carpinella, CM, Hehman, E, Freeman, JB and Johnson, KL (2016) The gendered face of partisan politics: consequences of facial sex typicality for vote choice. Political Communication 33, 2138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, JT, Tracy, JL, Ho, S and Henrich, J (2016) Listen, follow me: dynamic vocal signals of dominance predict emergent social rank in humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 145, 536.Google ScholarPubMed
Dietrich, BJ, Hayes, M and O'Brien, DZ (2019) Pitch perfect: vocal pitch and the emotional intensity of congressional speech. American Political Science Review 113, 941962.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eagly, AH and Karau, SJ (2002) Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review 109, 573598.Google ScholarPubMed
Feinberg, DR, DeBruine, LM, Jones, BC and Perrett, DI (2008) The role of femininity and averageness of voice pitch in aesthetic judgments of women's voices. Perception 37, 615623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitch, WT and Giedd, J (1999) Morphology and development of the human vocal tract: a study using magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 106, 15111522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraccaro, PJ, O'Connor, JJ, Re, DE, Jones, BC, DeBruine, LM and Feinberg, DR (2013) Faking it: deliberately altered voice pitch and vocal attractiveness. Animal Behaviour 85, 127136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerber, AS and Green, DP (2012) Field Experiments: Design, Analysis, and Interpretation. New York, NY: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Gonsalkorale, K, Sherman, JW and Klauer, KC (2009) Aging and prejudice: diminished regulation of automatic race bias among older adults. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45, 410414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollien, H (2014) Vocal fold dynamics for frequency change. Journal of Voice 28, 395405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horiuchi, Y, Imai, K and Taniguchi, N (2007) Designing and analyzing randomized experiments: application to a Japanese election survey experiment. American Journal of Political Science 51, 669687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horiuchi, Y, Komatsu, T and Nakaya, F (2012) Should candidates smile to win elections? An application of automated face recognition technology. Political Psychology 33, 925933.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imaida, A (2006) Wakai Nihonjin Joseino Pitch Henkani Miru Bunkateki Kihanno Eikyo [The impact of cultural norms on young Japanese women's voice pitch]. Gengo Bunka Ronshu 27, 1326.Google Scholar
Inter-Parliamentary Union (n.d.) Monthly ranking of women in national parliaments. Available at https://data.ipu.org/women-ranking?month=11&year=2021 (Accessed 23 December 2021).Google Scholar
Jones, JJ (2016) Talk ‘like a Man’: the linguistic styles of Hillary Clinton, 1992–2013. Perspectives on Politics 14, 625642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kage, R, Rosenbluth, F and Tanaka, S (2019) What explains low female political representation? Evidence from survey experiments in Japan. Politics & Gender 15, 285309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawato, S (2018) Nihonni okeru Seiken Uneito Kaisan Sosenyo [Cabinet management, parliamentary dissolution, and general elections in Japan]. In Kawasaki, T (ed.), Nihonto Yoropano Senkyoto Seiji. Tokyo: Gyosei, pp. 3147.Google Scholar
Klofstad, CA (2016) Candidate voice pitch influences election outcomes. Political Psychology 37, 725738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klofstad, CA (2017) Looks and sounds like a winner: perceptions of competence in candidates’ faces and voices influences vote choice. Journal of Experimental Political Science 4, 229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klofstad, CA, Anderson, RC and Peters, S (2012) Sounds like a winner: voice pitch influences perception of leadership capacity in both men and women. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 279, 26982704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klofstad, CA, Anderson, RC and Nowicki, S (2015) Perceptions of competence, strength, and age influence voters to select leaders with lower-pitched voices. PLoS ONE 10, e0133779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klofstad, CA, Nowicki, S and Anderson, RC (2016) How voice pitch influences our choice of leaders: when candidates speak, their vocal characteristics – as well as their words – influence voters’ attitudes toward them. American Scientist 104, 282288.Google Scholar
Ko, SJ, Sadler, MS and Galinsky, AD (2015) The sound of power: conveying and detecting hierarchical rank through voice. Psychological Science 26, 314.Google ScholarPubMed
Lawless, JL and Pearson, K (2008) The primary reason for women's underrepresentation? Reevaluating the conventional wisdom. The Journal of Politics 70, 6782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leongómez, JD, Binter, J, Kubicová, L, Stolařová, P, Klapilová, K, Havlíček, J and Craig Roberts, S (2014) Vocal modulation during courtship increases proceptivity even in naive listeners. Evolution and Human Behavior 35, 489496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levon, E and Ye, Y (2020) Language, indexicality and gender ideologies: contextual effects on the perceived credibility of women. Gender and Language 14, 123151.Google Scholar
McCann, SJ (2001) Height, societal threat, and the victory margin in presidential elections (1824–1992). Psychological reports 88, 741742.Google ScholarPubMed
Montano, KJ, Tigue, CC, Isenstein, SG, Barclay, P and Feinberg, DR (2017) Men's voice pitch influences women's trusting behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior 38, 293297.Google Scholar
O'Connor, JJ and Barclay, P (2018) High voice pitch mitigates the aversiveness of antisocial cues in men's speech. British Journal of Psychology 109, 812829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
OECD (n.d.) OECD Gender Data Portal. Available at https://www.oecd.org/gender/data/ (Accessed 29 June 2022).Google Scholar
Oguchi, T and Kikuchi, H (1997) Voice and interpersonal attraction. Japanese Psychological Research 39, 5661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohara, Y (1999) Performing gender through voice pitch: a cross-cultural analysis of Japanese and American English. In Wahrnehmung und Herstellung von Geschlecht. Opladen/Wiesbaden: Springer, pp. 105116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Okada, Y (2016) Seijikano Insho Keiseini okeru Koeno Koteino Eikyo [The effect of voice pitch on evaluation of politician]. Oyo Shakaigaku Kenkyu 58, 5366.Google Scholar
Okada, Y (2017) Koeno Koteiga Seito Toshuno Insho Keiseini Ataeru Eikyo [The effect of voice pitch on impression formation of party leaders]. Kodo Keiryogaku 44, 1725.Google Scholar
Ono, Y and Yamada, M (2020) Do voters prefer gender stereotypic candidates? Evidence from a conjoint survey experiment in Japan. Political Science Research and Methods 8, 477492.Google Scholar
Palmer, B and Simon, D (2010) Breaking the Political Glass Ceiling: Women and Congressional Elections. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pisanski, K, Mishra, S and Rendall, D (2012) The evolved psychology of voice: evaluating interrelationships in listeners’ assessments of the size, masculinity, and attractiveness of unseen speakers. Evolution and Human Behavior 33, 509519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pisanski, K, Oleszkiewicz, A, Plachetka, J, Gmiterek, M and Reby, D (2018) Voice pitch modulation in human mate choice. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 285, 20181634.Google ScholarPubMed
Puts, DA, Barndt, JL, Welling, LL, Dawood, K and Burriss, RP (2011) Intrasexual competition among women: vocal femininity affects perceptions of attractiveness and flirtatiousness. Personality and Individual Differences 50, 111115.Google Scholar
Puts, DA, Apicella, CL and Cárdenas, RA (2012) Masculine voices signal men's threat potential in forager and industrial societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 279, 601609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, SW, Kahn, S and Tran, T (1991) Creating a political image: shaping appearance and manipulating the vote. Political Behavior 13, 345367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, S and Coppock, A (2021) What have we learned about gender from candidate choice experiments? A meta-analysis of 67 factorial survey experiments. Journal of Politics 84, 655668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sell, A, Bryant, GA, Cosmides, L, Tooby, J, Sznycer, D, Von Rueden, C, Krauss, A and Gurven, M (2010) Adaptations in humans for assessing physical strength from the voice. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277, 35093518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigelman, L, Sigelman, CK and Fowler, C (1987) A bird of a different feather? An experimental investigation of physical attractiveness and the electability of female candidates. Social Psychology Quarterly 50, 3243.Google Scholar
Sorokowski, P (2010) Politicians’ estimated height as an indicator of their popularity. European Journal of Social Psychology 40, 13021309.Google Scholar
Sorokowski, P, Puts, D, Johnson, J, Żółkiewicz, O, Oleszkiewicz, A, Sorokowska, A, Kowal, M, Borkowska, B and Pisanski, K (2019) Voice of authority: professionals lower their vocal frequencies when giving expert advice. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 43, 257269.Google Scholar
Stockemer, D and Praino, R (2015) Blinded by beauty? Physical attractiveness and candidate selection in the US House of Representatives. Social Science Quarterly 96, 430443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stulp, G, Buunk, AP, Verhulst, S and Pollet, TV (2013) Tall claims? Sense and nonsense about the importance of height of US presidents. The Leadership Quarterly 24, 159171.Google Scholar
Teele, D, Kalla, J and Rosenbluth, FM (2018) The ties that double bind: social roles and women's underrepresentation in politics. American Political Science Review 112, 525541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tigue, CC, Borak, DJ, O'Connor, JJ, Schandl, C and Feinberg, DR (2012) Voice pitch influences voting behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior 33, 210216.Google Scholar
Van Bezooijen, R (1995) Sociocultural aspects of pitch differences between Japanese and Dutch women. Language and Speech 38, 253265.Google ScholarPubMed
Verhulst, B, Lodge, M and Lavine, H (2010) The attractiveness halo: why some candidates are perceived more favorably than others. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 34, 111117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Von Hippel, W, Silver, LA and Lynch, ME (2000) Stereotyping against your will: the role of inhibitory ability in stereotyping and prejudice among the elderly. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 26, 523532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamazawa, H and Hollien, H (1992) Speaking fundamental frequency patterns of Japanese women. Phonetica 49, 128140.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Four treatment groups

Figure 1

Figure 1. Percentage of respondents willing to vote for the candidate, by treatment group.Note: N = 414.

Figure 2

Table 2. Regression analysis by treatment groups

Figure 3

Table 3. Regression analysis by respondents' covariates

Figure 4

Figure 2. Marginal effect of candidates' voice pitch by respondents' gender.Note: The figure is generated using results from model 1 of Table 3.

Supplementary material: PDF

Bowern et al. supplementary material

Appendix

Download Bowern et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 277.1 KB