Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T12:27:53.709Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Insight, grey matter and cognitive function in first-onset psychosis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Kevin D. Morgan
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Division of Psychological Medicine, King's College London
Paola Dazzan
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Division of Psychological Medicine, King's College London
Craig Morgan*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Division of Psychological Medicine, King's College London
Julia Lappin
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Division of Psychological Medicine, King's College London
Gerard Hutchinson
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Division of Psychological Medicine, King's College London
John Suckling
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Department of Neurology, King's College London
Paul Fearon
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Division of Psychological Medicine, King's College London
Peter B. Jones
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge, Department of Psychiatry, Addenbrooke's Hospital, Cambridge
Julian Leff
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Division of Psychological Medicine, King's College London, UK
Robin M. Murray
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Division of Psychological Medicine, King's College London, UK
Anthony S. David
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Division of Psychological Medicine, King's College London, UK
*
Kevin D. Morgan, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Westminster, 309 Regent Street, London W1B 2UW, UK. Email: k.d.morgan@westminster.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Several studies have suggested that neuropsychological and structural brain deficits are implicated in poor insight. Few insight studies however have combined neurocognitive and structural neuroanatomical measures.

Aims

Focusing on the ability to relabel psychotic symptoms as pathological, we examined insight, brain structure and neurocognition in first-onset psychosis.

Method

Voxel-based magnetic resonance imaging data were acquired from 82 individuals with psychosis and 91 controls assessed with a brief neuropsychological test battery. Insight was measured using the Schedule for the Assessment of Insight.

Results

The principal analysis showed reduced general neuropsychological function was linked to poor symptom relabelling ability. A subsequent between-psychosis group analysis found those with no symptom relabelling ability had significant global and regional grey matter deficits primarily located at the posterior cingulate gyrus and right precuneus/cuneus.

Conclusions

The cingulate gyrus (as part of a midline cortical system) along with right hemisphere regions may be involved in illness and symptom self-appraisal in first-onset psychosis.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2010 

Studies examining the relationship between insight and cognition in psychosis have shown small–moderate associations of good insight with both intact frontal–executive functioning and general intellectual abilities. Reference Morgan, David, Amador and David1Reference Shad, Muddasani and Keshavan3 In terms of insight and brain structure, findings suggest an association between poor insight and reduced total brain volume, Reference Flashman, McAllister, Andreasen and Saykin4,Reference McEvoy, Johnson, Perkins, Lieberman, Hamer and Keefe5 ventricular enlargement, Reference Takai, Uematsu, Ueki, Sone and Kaiya6 frontal lobe atrophy, Reference Laroi, Fannemel, Ronneberg, Flekkoy, Opjordsmoen and Dullerud7 reduced frontal lobe volume Reference Shad, Muddasani and Keshavan3,Reference Flashman, McAllister, Johnson, Rick, Green and Saykin8,Reference Sapara, Cooke, Fannon, Francis, Buchanan and Anilkumar9 and grey matter deficits in the cingulate gyrus, Reference Sapara, Cooke, Fannon, Francis, Buchanan and Anilkumar9,Reference Haa, Youna, Haa, Rhoc, Leed and Kimd10 temporal lobe, Reference Sapara, Cooke, Fannon, Francis, Buchanan and Anilkumar9Reference Cooke, Fannon, Kulpers, Peters, Williams and Kumari11 parietal lobe Reference Cooke, Fannon, Kulpers, Peters, Williams and Kumari11 and precuneus. Reference Cooke, Fannon, Kulpers, Peters, Williams and Kumari11 There is, however, some inconsistency in these findings including cross-study variation in brain location–insight correlates and in some instances a failure to identify any brain deficits associated with poor insight. Reference Bassitt, Neto, de Castro and Busatto12Reference David, van Os, Jones, Harvey, Foerster and Fahy14 Table 1 summarises brain structure-insight findings. This inconsistency could arise from the complex nature of insight, which is now regarded as a construct with several overlapping dimensions, Reference David15 the analysis of which may be further complicated by the use of varying insight assessment methods. A systematic review Reference Morgan, David, Amador and David1 and a meta-analysis Reference Aleman, Agrawal, Morgan and David2 have found the ability to identify and attribute the symptoms of psychosis as pathological (symptom relabelling) to be the insight component most closely related to neuropsychological function. In this study we conducted a voxel-based magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) examination of individuals with first-onset psychosis combined with an analysis of cognitive function and hypothesised that neurocognitive deficits would be more closely associated with the capacity to relabel psychotic symptoms as psychopathological rather than a global measure of insight. On the basis of evidence from previous structural neuroimaging Reference Laroi, Fannemel, Ronneberg, Flekkoy, Opjordsmoen and Dullerud7,Reference Sapara, Cooke, Fannon, Francis, Buchanan and Anilkumar9 and neuropsychological studies Reference Morgan, David, Amador and David1,Reference Aleman, Agrawal, Morgan and David2 of insight, we further hypothesised that the focal point of those deficits would involve a frontal cortical system.

Table 1 Previous studies of insight and brain structurea

Authors Participants Main findings Insight measure
Cooke et al (2008) Reference Cooke, Fannon, Kulpers, Peters, Williams and Kumari11 Schizophrenia, schizoaffective, out-patients, chronic (n = 52) Insight impairment associated with reduced grey matter volume in left precuneus and left superior-middle temporal, right superior temporal, right inferior temporal and right lateral parietal gyri. SAI–E/BIS
Sapara et al (2007) Reference Sapara, Cooke, Fannon, Francis, Buchanan and Anilkumar9 Schizophrenia, chronic, out-patients (n = 28) Smaller prefrontal grey matter volume and poor insight association SAI–E
Bassitt et al (2006) Reference Bassitt, Neto, de Castro and Busatto12 Schizophrenia (n = 50) No association between insight impairment and reduced grey or white matter volumes in the prefrontal region or in any other brain areas SUMD
Shad et al (2006) Reference Shad, Muddasani and Keshavan3 Schizophrenia, first episode (n = 14) Reduced symptom awareness and poor symptom attribution associated with reduced right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volume and reduced right orbital prefrontal cortex volume respectively SUMD
McEvoy et al (2006) Reference McEvoy, Johnson, Perkins, Lieberman, Hamer and Keefe5 Schizophrenia, first episode (n = 226) Larger brain volumes associated with more insight ITAQ
Haa et al (2004) Reference Haa, Youna, Haa, Rhoc, Leed and Kimd10 Schizophrenia, out-patients (n = 35) Grey matter reductions in left posterior and right anterior cingulate gyrus and bilateral inferior temporal regions associated with poor insight PANSS
Rossell et al (2003) Reference Rossell, Coakes, Shapleske, Woodruff and David13 Individuals with schizophrenia (males), mixed illness duration (n = 78) No association between insight and whole brain, total white matter and total grey matter volumes SAI–E
Flashman et al (2001) Reference Flashman, McAllister, Johnson, Rick, Green and Saykin8 Schizophrenia (n = 15) Unawareness of illness and symptom misattribution associated with reduced middle frontal gyrus and reduced superior frontal gyrus volume respectively SUMD
Laroi et al (2000) Reference Laroi, Fannemel, Ronneberg, Flekkoy, Opjordsmoen and Dullerud7 Schizophrenia (n = 20) Frontal lobe atrophy associated with poor insight SUMD
Flashman et al (2000) Reference Flashman, McAllister, Andreasen and Saykin4 Schizophrenia spectrum (n = 30) Reduced whole brain volume associated with poor insight SUMD
David et al (1995) Reference David, van Os, Jones, Harvey, Foerster and Fahy14 Mixed psychosis (n = 128) No association between insight and ventricular volume PANSS
Takai et al (1992) Reference Takai, Uematsu, Ueki, Sone and Kaiya6 Schizophrenia, chronic (n = 22) Ventricular enlargement associated with poor insight PANSS

Method

Sample

Participants were recruited from a sample of inner-city South London residents enrolled in an epidemiological study of first-onset psychosis (AESOP: Aetiology and Ethnicity in Schizophrenia and Other Psychoses). Details of the AESOP study and the overall methodology have previously been reported. Reference Morgan, Dazzan, Morgan, Jones, Harrison and Leff16 Insight data were acquired from 137 AESOP study participants. Inclusion criteria for the insight and neurocognition analysis were: age 16–65 years; resident in defined area; presenting consecutively for the first time to local psychiatric services (in-patient and out-patient) between 1997–2000 with symptoms meeting criteria for a functional psychotic illness (ICD–10: 10–19; F20–29; F30–39; psychotic codings) 17 based on the Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN) 18 interview. Exclusion criteria were: head trauma history with >1 h unconsciousness; central nervous system disease; poor English fluency; transient psychotic symptoms resulting from acute intoxication (F1x.0, ICD–10) following consumption of a psychoactive substance.

All 137 participants for whom insight data were acquired were asked to have an MRI scan. Thirty-four people refused and four could not be scanned due to safety/medical reasons (e.g. pregnancy). A total of 99 participants gave informed consent and were subsequently scanned. Scan data for 15 of the participants were unusable as a result of poor image acquisition/motion artefact. A further two scans were excluded after detection of gross brain abnormalities: in one case evidence of significant hydrocephalus and in the other a large frontally located subarachnoid cyst. Thus there were 82 (60%) individuals in the psychosis group who were included in the analysis. These 82 participants had recently presented to psychiatric services for the first time in their lives. Of these, 77 were admitted to hospital at this first presentation, 53 (64.6%) as compulsory patients. Details of their clinical status including medication history are shown in Table 2.

Table 2 Background characteristics of sample

Psychosis group (n = 82) Control group (n = 91) Test statistic P
Age, years: mean (s.d.) 27.15 (7.58) 30.05 (8.65) t = –2.34 0.02
Education, years: mean (s.d.) 12.95 (2.44) 11.80 (16.97) t = 0.60 0.54
Premorbid IQ (National Adult Reading Test), mean (s.d.) 96.4 (16.0) 108.1 (11.5) F = 23.1a <0.001
Total grey matter, ml: mean (s.d.) 574.41 (57.14) 589.55 (56.60) F = 5.64a 0.02
Total ventricular volume, ml: mean (s.d.) 17.20 (8.61) 16.1 (8.47) F = 3.67b 0.06
Parental socioeconomic status, n (%) χ2 = 7.41 0.06
    Managerial/professional 26 (31.7) 34 (37.4)
    Intermediate (e.g. clerical, sales, skilled non-manual) 19 (23.2) 30 (33.0)
    Working (e.g. unskilled, routine occupations) 37 (45.1) 25 (27.5)
Gender, female/male: n (%) 32/50 (39.0/61.0) 37/54 (40.7/59.3) χ2 = 0.48 0.8
Ethnicity, White British/not White British: n (%) 30/52 (36.6/63.4) 42/49 (46.2/53.8) χ2 = 2.58 0.11
Handedness, right/left:c n (%) 76/6 (92.7/7.3) 71/12 (85.5/14.5) χ2 = 2.14 0.14
Diagnosis, n (%)
    Schizophrenia 39 (47.6)
    Schizoaffective 6 (7.3)
    Bipolar disorder 17 (20.7)
    Depressive psychosis 10 (12.2)
    Other psychosis 10 (12.2)
Admission status, n (%)
    Informald 29 (35.4)
    Compulsory 53 (64.6)
Type of antipsychotic, n (%)
    Typical 21 (23.2)
    Atypical 19 (23.2)
    Mixed 29 (35.3)
    None 13 (15.9)
Duration of antipsychotic treatment, weeks: mean (s.d.) 5.70 (6.7)
Interval between first presentation and MRI scan/assessment, weeks: mean (s.d.) 13.06 (13.1)
Total symptom ratings, mean (s.d.) 31.02 (17.7)

A total of 91 controls were recruited from the same community, using the same exclusion criteria. Evidence of past or present psychosis screened with the Psychosis Screening Questionnaire Reference Bebbington and Nayani19 was an additional exclusion criterion. We excluded eight MRI control scans (motion artefact, n = 7; suspected hydrocephalus, n = 1). As insight–cognition associations cannot be derived from a non-clinical sample, the purpose of including healthy controls was to compare global grey matter volume, ventricular volume and general intellectual functioning (measured with the National Adult Reading Test (NART)) Reference Nelson and Willison20 with the psychosis group. A detailed MRI analysis comparing AESOP patient–control brain structure has been reported earlier. Reference Morgan, Dazzan, Orr, Hutchinson, Chitnis and Suckling21

Assessments

Clinical assessments

Individuals in the psychosis group were interviewed using the SCAN. We made ICD–10 diagnoses in consensus meetings with senior clinicians (R.M.M. or J.L.), using SCAN information and clinical notes. Total symptomatology was scored using an algorithm that summed the SCAN's individual item scores. Reference Wing22

Insight

Insight ratings were made from semi-structured interviews using the Schedule for the Assessment of Insight – Expanded version (SAI–E). Reference Kemp, David and Blackwell23 The SAI–E is an 11-item semi-structured interview that allows for the measure of symptom relabelling, overall illness awareness and treatment adherence. Symptom relabelling ratings are based on two items relating to: awareness of most prominent psychotic symptoms; and explanation of the presence of each prominent symptom. For both items the response for each symptom was scored between 0 (complete lack of awareness) and 4 (full awareness). The individual symptom ratings were averaged yielding a score between 0–4. The two item scores were combined so that the maximum possible (full) symptom relabelling score is 8.

A principal components factor analysis (with varimax rotation) of the insight data yielded three factors that accounted for 66.5% of the variance (lowest eigenvalue 1.05). Reference David, Morgan, Mallett, Leff and Murray24 This finding corresponds directly with the three-component model of insight described above. Ratings of total insight consist of the combined SAI–E scores (items 1–11).

Interrater reliability

Only SCAN-trained researchers administered the SAI–E interviews. Two measures were implemented to ensure good interrater reliability. First, one-to-one SAI–E training sessions were conducted. Second, all SAI–E interviewers scored a battery of 16 completed interviews prepared specifically to assess interrater reliability. These schedules are reproductions of real interviews with a verbatim record of the individuals' responses and the scoring sections left blank. Intraclass correlation coefficients for the total SAI–E scores between the raters ranged from 0.92 to 0.98 (P<0.001).

Cognitive tests

The psychosis group completed a cognitive test battery of selected items to assess premorbid intelligence, current IQ, psychomotor speed, spatial ability, verbal fluency, set shifting, auditory working memory, verbal and non-verbal memory (Table 3). Trained researchers administered the battery and scored the assessments (masked to SAI–E scores). The control group completed the NART. Reference Nelson and Willison20

Table 3 Battery of cognitive tests

Domain Test Author
General intelligence National Adult Reading Test (NART) Nelson && Willison, 1991 Reference Nelson and Willison20
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised (WAIS–R)a Wechsler, 1981 Reference Weschsler25
Ravens Coloured Progressive Matrices (Ravens CPM) Ravens, 1974 Reference Ravens26
Verbal memory Auditory Verbal Learning Test – Immediate recall (AVLT–IR) (Item 1) Rey, 1964 Reference Rey27
Auditory Verbal Learning Test – Verbal learning (AVLT–VL) (Items 1–5)
Non-verbal memory Visual reproduction – Immediate recall (WMS–IR) Wechsler, 1987 Reference Weschsler28
Working memory Letter–Number Span Test – Working memory (LNS–WM) Gold et al, 1997 Reference Gold, Carpenter, Randolph, Goldberg and Weinberger29
Verbal fluency Verbal fluency Controlled Oral Word Association Test (FAS) Yeudall et al, 1986 Reference Yeudall, Fromm, Reddon and Stefanyk30
Set shifting Trails B–A Reitan, 1958 Reference Reitan31
Psychomotor speed Trails A Reitan, 1958 Reference Reitan31

Structural magnetic resonance image acquisition and processing

Scans were acquired with a GE Signa 1.5-T system, at the Maudsley Hospital, London. Contiguous, interleaved proton-density and T 2-weighted 3 mm thick coronal plane dual-echo images were acquired, providing whole brain coverage: repetition time (TR) = 4000 ms and effective echo times of 20 ms and 85 ms were used with 8-echo train length. Matrix size was 256×192, collected from a rectangular field-of-view of 22 cm×16.5 cm, giving an in-plane resolution of 0.859 mm. Total acquisition time was 10 min, 12 s.

Segmentation and registration methods for each fast-spin echo data-set are described in detail elsewhere. Reference Bullmore, Suckling, Overmeyer, Rabe-Hesketh, Taylor and Brammer32,Reference Suckling, Sigmundsson, Greenwood and Bullmore33 Briefly, participant masks were generated to identify neural tissue. Extra-cerebral tissues were removed initially, using an automated algorithm. Manually editing the skull-stripped images was necessary only to remove brainstem and cerebellum from the cerebral hemispheres and diencephalon. The probability of each intracerebral voxel belonging to each of four possible tissue classes (grey matter, white matter, cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) or dura/vasculature) was estimated with a modified fuzzy clustering algorithm. Reference Suckling, Sigmundsson, Greenwood and Bullmore33 This type of segmentation assigns for each voxel a value in the range 0–1 indicating the fraction of the voxel comprised by each tissue type (e.g. a grey matter value of 0.7 means 70% of tissue represented by that voxel is grey matter; therefore the value indicates the proportion of the voxel occupied by grey matter). Total grey tissue volume was calculated at this stage of the analysis.

The construction of the sample's template image is described elsewhere. Reference Morgan, Dazzan, Orr, Hutchinson, Chitnis and Suckling21 In summary, a template image was constructed using the AFNI (Analysis of Functional Neuroimages) Unix-based program developed at the Medical College of Wisconsin (www.biophysics.mcw.edu), from 6 proton-density images acquired from six healthy, controls and then averaging these images.

Ventricular volume

Additional masks were generated per participant by tracing around the lateral and third ventricles in native space, in every slice in which they were visible. Tracing was performed masked to age, gender and clinical status. Within the masked area, CSF volume was calculated using the data generated from the previously described modified fuzzy clustering algorithm.

Statistical analysis

Psychosis group v. control group

A between-group ANCOVA (controlling for age) comparing the psychosis group and the control group was performed to evaluate whether the whole psychosis group (irrespective of insight levels) was characterised by deficits in total grey matter volume, excess ventricular volume and poor general intellectual functioning.

Insight, global grey matter and ventricular volume

Total tissue volumes were generated prior to the registration of the images into standard space. We calculated correlation coefficients (age partialled) to investigate total grey matter volume and ventricular volume associations (controlling for whole brain volume) with the measures of symptom relabelling and total insight within the whole psychosis group sample.

Insight and regional grey matter volume

Within-group analyses of associations between insight scores and regional grey matter volume were estimated by fitting an ANCOVA model at each intracerebral voxel in standard space covarying for age and total grey matter volume. Permutation testing was used to assess statistical significance and regional relationships were tested at voxel cluster level. Reference Bullmore, Suckling, Overmeyer, Rabe-Hesketh, Taylor and Brammer32 Given that structural brain changes are likely to extend over a number of contiguous voxels, test statistics incorporating spatial information, such as three-dimensional (3D) cluster mass (the sum of suprathreshold voxel statistics), are generally more powerful than other possible test statistics informed only by single voxel data. We set the statistical threshold for cluster significance in all analyses so that the expected number of false-positive clusters (P-value×number of tests) was <1 false positive.

Insight and cognitive function

We calculated correlation coefficients (age partialled) to investigate associations of performance on each of the cognitive takes with the obtained measures of insight within the whole psychosis group sample.

Analysis of symptom relabelling

Additional between-group analyses comparing the psychosis group with no symptom relabelling ability to those with at least partial symptom relabelling ability were performed. These analyses examined between-group differences in total grey matter volume, ventricular volume, regional grey matter volume and cognitive test performance using ANCOVA (age partialled). The procedure for analysis of regional differences in grey matter volume was carried out by fitting an ANCOVA model at each intracerebral voxel in standard space and conducting permutation testing as described above for the within-group analysis.

Linear regression was performed to estimate the extent to which the structural brain abnormalities and/or cognitive differences identified in the series of ANCOVAs described above contributed to the variance in ratings for symptom relabelling. These regression analyses controlled for the potentially confounding factors of age and total grey matter volume.

The SPSS (Version 14.0 for Windows) was used to carry out the statistical tests. All participants gave their informed consent to take part in the study. Ethical approval for the study was granted by the South London and Maudsley NHS Trust.

Results

Psychosis group v. control group

The participants in the psychosis group were significantly older than those in the control group (30.1 (s.d. = 8.7) years v. 27.2 (s.d. = 7.6) years, t = 2.34, d.f. = 171, P = 0.02) and scored significantly lower on the NART (96.4 (s.d. = 16.0) v. 108.1 (s.d. = 11.5), F = 23.1, d.f. = 1155, P<0.001). T-tests and chi-squared analyses showed no significant psychosis group–control group differences for gender, handedness, parental socioeconomic status and ethnicity. Total grey tissue volume in the psychosis group was 2.5% smaller than in the control group (574.81 ml (s.d. = 57.14) v. 589.55 ml (s.d. = 56.60) F = 5.64, d.f. = 169, P = 0.02). Lateral ventricular volume was 6.4% larger in the psychosis group (17.2 ml (s.d. = 8.61) v. 16.1 ml (s.d. = 8.47)), with the psychosis group having a larger lateral ventricle to brain volume ratio than the control group that bordered on statistical significance (F = 3.66, d.f. = 166, P = 0.06) (Table 2).

Within-psychois group analysis: insight, global grey matter, regional grey matter and ventricular volume

There were not significant correlations between symptom relabelling scores and volumetric measures for total grey matter, regional grey matter or lateral ventricular volume. Similarly, there were no significant associations between scores for total insight and the same neuroanatomical measures. There was, however, a correlation between lower total insight ratings and reduced total grey matter volume (age partialled) that reached borderline significance (r = 0.23, P = 0.06).

Insight and cognitive function (correlations – age partialled)

Reduced symptom relabelling correlated significantly with poorer: NART IQ (r = 0.27, P = 0.03); full-scale IQ (FSIQ) (r = 0.33, P = 0.01); performance IQ (PIQ) (r = 0.36, P<0.01); verbal learning (r = 0.30, P = 0.02); and Trails A speed (r = –0.25, P = 0.04). Reduced total insight correlated significantly with poorer: NART IQ (r = 0.32, P = 0.02), FSIQ (r = 0.27, P = 0.02); PIQ (r = 0.35, P<0.01); verbal learning (r = 0.46, P<0.001); and Letter–Number Span (LNS)–Working memory (r = 0.32, P = 0.02). After adjusting with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons only the following correlations remained significant: symptom relabelling with PIQ; total insight with PIQ; and total insight with verbal learning.

Between-psychosis group analysis

An analysis of the obtained insight scores revealed that 20/82 (24.4%) of the psychosis group (the first quartile) scored zero on the symptom relabelling component of insight (Fig. 1). We considered that neurocognitive deficts may be most evident in those individuals where there is a total absence of any capacity to question the presence of psychotic symptoms as pathological. Therefore, a between-group analysis of the 20 participants with no symptom relabelling ability and the 62 participants with at least some symptom relabelling ability was performed to compare global and regional grey matter volume, ventricular volume and cognitive performance. Chi-squared tests and independent samples t-tests revealed no significant between-group differences for gender, total symptoms, diagnosis (schizophrenia v. other psychosis), years of education and handedness. The mean age of the no symptom relabelling ability subgroup was higher than that of the subgroup with at least some symptom relabelling scores (30.6 v. 26.0 years, t = –2.41, d.f. = 80, P = 0.02). The distribution of scores for total insight was not comparable with that of symptom relabelling, i.e. no participants scored <1 for total insight, therefore a similar post hoc analysis for total insight was not performed.

Fig. 1 Distribution of symptom relabelling scores (n = 82). SAI–E, Schedule for the Assessment of Insight – Expanded version.

Symptom relabelling between-group analysis: global grey matter, regional grey matter and ventricular volume

Total grey tissue volume in the no symptom relabelling subgroup was 6.7% lower than in the subgroup with at least some symptom relabelling ablity (ANCOVA controlling for age: F = 6.77, d.f. = 78, P = 0.01). A significant reduction in grey tissue volume was identified in the no symptom relabelling subgroup in the following five spatially extensive 3D voxel clusters:

  1. (a) left hemisphere cluster centred on the insula;

  2. (b) right hemisphere cluster extending from the putamen to the superior temporal gyrus and precentral gyrus;

  3. (c) left hemisphere cluster extending from the precentral gyrus to the superior temporal gyrus;

  4. (d) bilateral cluster extending posteriorly from the posterior cingulate gyrus to the superior parietal lobe/precuneus;

  5. (e) right hemisphere cluster extending posteriorly from the precuneus through the cuneus to the medial occipital gyrus.

There were no significant between-group differences in ventricular volumes (Tables 4 and 5 and online Fig. DS1). To examine the potential effect of IQ on the between-group differences in regional grey matter volumes, a further ANCOVA was conducted controlling for FSIQ. In this additional analysis, the between-group differences in all five regions described above remained significant (P<0.001).

Table 4 Comparison of brain structure and cognitive function between patients with low or moderate-high insight levels

Symptom relabelling ability
No symptom relabelling Some symptom relabelling
n Mean (s.d.) n Mean (s.d.) B β P
Brain structure
Volume, ml
    Total grey 20 545.0 (5.5) 62 583.9 (57.1) 0.01
    Lateral ventriclesa 20 16.9 (6.3) 62 18.0 (13.8) NS
Cluster volume, ml
    Posterior cingulate cyrus (bilateral) 20 1.4 (0.2) 62 1.7 (0.2) 0.002
    Insula (left hemisphere) 20 0.9 (0.1) 62 1.1 (0.9) 0.002
    Superior temporal gyrus (right hemisphere) 20 1.6 (0.2) 62 1.8 (0.1) 0.002
    Superior temporal gyrus (left hemisphere) 20 0.8 (0.1) 62 1.0 (0.1) 0.002
    Cuneus (right hemisphere) 20 0.5 (0.1) 62 0.6 (0.1) 0.002
Cognitive testb
    National Adult Reading Test 19 90.7 (14.5) 60 98.2 (15.9) 0.07
    Performance IQ 19 77.6 (11.6) 62 91.7 (20.3) <0.01
    Verbal IQ 18 82.9 (10.8) 60 93.2 (17.4) <0.05
    Ravens Coloured Progressive Matrices 18 28.9 (4.1) 56 31.6 (4.2) <0.05
    Auditory Verbal Learning Test – Immediate recall 18 5.4 (1.2) 58 6.50 (2.19) 0.05
    Auditory Verbal Learning Test – Verbal learning 18 42.28 (9.1) 58 47.7 (10.5) 0.05
    Wechsler Memory Scale – Visual reproduction 17 9.2 (2.7) 52 10.5 (1.4) NS
    Verbal Fluency Controlled Oral Word Association Test 18 9.6 (14.7) 59 62.0 (18.8) <0.05
    Letter–Number Span Test 17 11.4 (2.9) 58 13.2 (3.7) 0.07
    Trails Ab 18 40.6 (18.0) 59 66.0 (43.4) 0.001
    Trails B–Ab 18 55.7 (42.1) 58 79.8 (46.9) <0.05
Regression analysisc
    Predictor
    Cuneus (right hemisphere) 12.9 0.4 <0.001
    Performance IQ 0.1 0.3 <0.01

Table 5 Location of grey matter deficits in relation to absent or at least partial symptom relabelling (n = 82)

Anatomical areaa Number of voxels in cluster Location of cluster centre x, y, z
Centroid Anterior limit Posterior limit
Left insula (43) Insula (45) Insula (43) 390 –38, 4, 7
Right superior temporal gyrus Putamen (n/a) Superior temporal gyrus (22) 273 23, 7, 2
(22)/precentral gyrus (4)
Left superior temporal gyrus (41) Precentral gyrus (4) Superior temporal gyrus (41) 300 –43, 28, 15
Bilaterial posterior cingulate gyrus (31) Posterior cingulate gyrus (31) Superior parietal lobe/precuneus (7) 533 0, –48, 40
Right cuneus (18) Precuneus (7) Medial/lateral occipital gyrus (19) 273 7, –79, 24

Symptom relabelling between-group analysis: cognition

The ANCOVA (controlling for age) showed that the subgrop with no symptom relabelling ability scored significantly lower than those with some symptom relabelling ability on FSIQ (F = 8.75, d.f. = 77, P<0.01); verbal IQ (VIQ) (F = 5.90, d.f. = 77, P = 0.02), PIQ (F = 8.30, d.f. = 79, P<0.01); verbal fluency (F = 6.60, d.f. = 75, P<0.05); Trails A (F = 11.59, d.f. = 75, P<0.01); and verbal learning (F = 3.83, d.f. = 74, P = 0.05). There were no other significant differences for cognitive performance (Table 4).

A stepwise linear regression controlling for diagnostic status revealed significant effects for right hemisphere cuneus grey matter deficits and PIQ scores accounting for 31% of the variance in symptom relabelling (R 2 = 0.30, P<0.001). Reduced grey matter in those clusters and lower PIQ scores were associated with lower symptom relabelling scores. No other factors were associated with symptom relabelling. The beta (standardised) coefficients indicate right hemisphere grey matter cuneus deficits (β = 0.413, P<0.001) are more predictive of poor symptom relabelling than lower PIQ scores (β = 0.274, P<0.01) (Tables 4 and 5 and online Fig. DS1).

Discussion

Psychosis group v. control group

The psychosis group–control group comparisons indicated global grey matter deficits, ventricular enlargement and lower general intellectual functioning in the group with psychosis. These structural and functional abnormalities are consistent with the findings of several cognitive and MRI studies of individuals with first-episode, intermediate and chronic psychosis. Reference Shenton, Dickey, Frumin and McCarley34,Reference Heinrichs and Zakzanis35

Within-psychosis group analysis of insight

Contrary to our predictions, the main analyses of symptom relabelling across the whole range of scores found no significant associations with structural brain measures, although a correlation between global grey matter volume and total insight scores that approached statistical significance (P = 0.06) was identified. As expected and also in agreement with previous findings, Reference Morgan, David, Amador and David1,Reference Aleman, Agrawal, Morgan and David2 the analysis of cognitive performance provided evidence that both symptom relabelling and insight in general are at least partly dependent on good overall cognitive function.

Between-psychosis group analysis of insight

Scrutiny of the insight scale revealed that there was a bimodal distribution of scores for symptom relabelling. Twenty people in the psychosis group (24.4%) scored zero for insight relabelling, i.e. showing no capacity to identify psychotic symptoms as pathological (Fig. 1). Further to the planned analysis, we considered that in terms of symptom relabelling ability, this subgroup of participants may more closely resemble people with neurological conditions experiencing anosognosia and that consequently more evidence of neuroanatomical and neuropsychological deficits would be found in this group. This prediction was based on the rationale that neurocognitive deficits would be most evident in indivdiuals where there appears to be no capacity to hold in mind, question and consider alternative (non-psychopathological) explanations for symptoms such as hallucinations and delusions. Thus we compared those participants with no symptom relabelling ability with those with at least some relabelling ability. The findings from the analyses of cognitive function supported this rationale. Whereas deficits were seen on tests of working memory, processing speed and verbal fluency, there was evidence of a reduction in general intellectual abilities in both the verbal but also non-verbal domains, the latter traditionally linked to right hemisphere functions.

In terms of brain structure we found significantly reduced global grey matter volume in the subgroup with no symptom relabelling ability. In the regional analysis we found reduced grey matter in the left posterior cingulate cyrus, the right precuneus and the cuneus, with the latter region emerging as a significant predictor of poor symptom relabelling in the regression analysis. The finding in relation to the posterior cingulate gyrus is in line with a previous voxel-based morphometry study Reference Haa, Youna, Haa, Rhoc, Leed and Kimd10 that found a correlation between severity of lack of insight in schizophrenia and reduced left posterior cingulate gyrus volume at the same location (Brodmann 31). In another voxel-based morphometry analysis of people with schizphrenia, Reference Cooke, Fannon, Kulpers, Peters, Williams and Kumari11 left precuneus grey matter reductions were also shown to be associated with poor insight. Elsewhere, however, comparatively few studies have focused on the function and dysfunction of posterior medial cortices in general or in schizophrenia. However, of possible relevance to developing an understanding of the neuroanatomical substrates of insight, the precuneus has been shown to be involved in the representation (monitoring) of the world around us. Reference Gusnard, Raichle and Raichle36 Dysfunction of the right parietal lobe has also been highlighted as underlying delusions of alien control through faulty self-monitoring Reference Spence, Brooks, Hirsch, Liddle, Meehan and Grasby37 and the condition of anosognosia. Reference Berti, Bottini, Gandola, Pia, Smania and Stracciari38 Recent functional imaging studies have shown a consistent localisation of self-appraisal processes to the midline cortical structures (including the posterior cingulate gyrus) in healthy volunteers. Reference Northoff, Heinzel, de Greck, Bermpohl, Dobrowolny and Panksepp39 Consistent with this, the posterior cingulate gyrus was activated during self-appraisal in a study of elderly people with cognitive impairments. Reference Ries, Jabbar, Schmitz, Trivedi, Gleason and Carlsson40

We also found grey matter insula deficits to be associated with an absence of symptom relabelling. Although no previous insight–brain structure studies have reported insula abnormalities, findings from functional imaging studies suggest that the insula is involved in self-monitoring, Reference Shergill, Bullmore, Brammer, Williams, Murray and McGuire41 the ability to discriminate between other-person generated actions and self-generated actions Reference Blakemore, Rees and Frith42 and in the mental representation of internal states, Reference Blakemore, Rees and Frith42 all of which appear relevant to insight. In respect of temporal lobe-insight associations, reduced grey matter reductions have previously been shown to correlate with poor symptom relabelling (right superior temporal gyrus) and poor illness awareness (left–middle temporal region). Reference Cooke, Fannon, Kulpers, Peters, Williams and Kumari11 In our study, superior temporal gyrus grey matter deficits (left and right hemisphere) were also found in the subgroup with no symptom relabelling ability. Based on structural imaging findings the superior temporal gyrus has long been implicated in key psychotic symptoms, notably auditory hallucinations, Reference Barta, Pearlson, Powers, Richards and Tune43 a phenomenon thought to involve defective monitoring and self-attribution of inner-speech. Reference Shergill, Cameron, Brammer, Williams, Murray and McGuire44 This pattern of MRI abnormalities leads us to speculate that insight into psychosis involves midline cortical and right posterior hemisphere regions underlying illness awareness and self-appraisal processes in general, plus lateralised symptom-specific regions connected more directly to the origin of the symptoms themselves, i.e. superior temporal gyrus in relation to auditory hallucinations and parietal cortex in passivity. Such a model is similar to that advanced for anosognosia with general and symptom specific modules of awareness. Reference McGlynn and Schacter45

Main findings

In this study of people with first-onset psychosis, performance on a range of cognitive tasks was impaired in those with poor symptom relabelling ability and poor global insight suggesting that a good level of general cognitive functioning is necessary for good insight into psychosis rather than a specific system such as one drawing on frontal–executive resources. In those with the lowest levels of symptom relabelling ability there was evidence of both global and regional cortical deficits. Our data point to a midline and right hemisphere cortical system, normally involved in self-appraisal, being implicated in symptom appraisal, with temporal and parietal regions also involved, perhaps reflecting symptom-specific elements of psychiatric insight.

Study limitations

The inclusion of additional neuropsychological tests more sensitive to disturbances in executive function (e.g. the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task) or those aimed at dissecting the cognitive processes underpinning self-appraisal would have been desirable and could potentially have shed more light on the complex relationship between neurocognition and the self-appraisal of symptoms. The study's findings are also limited by the fact that the association with symptom relabelling only emerged strongly when the phenomenon was considered categorically rather than as a dimension. From a diagnostic perspective it was not our primary concern to investigate whether mechanisms for insight into psychotic symptoms differ across conditions. Diagnostic status was however examined in the linear regression analysis and did not emerge as a significant factor in the associations found between symptom relabelling and the measures of regional grey matter and cognition. It would have been informative to also consider diagnostic status in the between-group analyses; however, the size of the group numbers do not provide sufficient statistical power to perform separate analyses based on diagnostic status. Our findings should therefore be regarded as tentative and as providing a basis for future investigations of insight in the psychoses. In this respect, a longitudinal analysis of both insight and neurocognitive status over time would be revealing given the cross-sectional nature of our study. Further evidence that neurocognitive deficits underpin the most severe incapacity to identifying psychotic states as pathological may be of importance clinically in setting a limit upon expectations for recovery.

Funding

The AESOP study was funded by the Medical Research Council (UK) and the Stanley Medical Research Institute (USA). We would also like to thank the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health for their support.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Morgan, KD, David, A. Neuropsychological studies of insight in patients with psychotic disorders. In Insight and Psychosis (eds Amador, X, David, A): 177196. Oxford University Press, 2004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2 Aleman, A, Agrawal, N, Morgan, KD, David, AS. Insight in psychosis and neuropsychological function. Meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 2006; 189: 204–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3 Shad, MU, Muddasani, S, Keshavan, MS. Prefrontal subregions and dimensions of insight in first-episode schizophrenia – a pilot study. Psychiatry Res 2006; 146: 3542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 Flashman, LA, McAllister, TW, Andreasen, NC, Saykin, AJ. Smaller brain size associated with unawareness of illness in patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157: 1167–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 McEvoy, JP, Johnson, J, Perkins, D, Lieberman, JA, Hamer, RM, Keefe, RS, et al. Insight in first-episode psychosis. Psychol Med 2006; 36: 1385–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 Takai, A, Uematsu, M, Ueki, H, Sone, K, Kaiya, H. Insight and its related factors in chronic-schizophrenic patients – a preliminary study. Euro J Psychiatry 1992; 6: 159–70.Google Scholar
7 Laroi, F, Fannemel, M, Ronneberg, U, Flekkoy, K, Opjordsmoen, S, Dullerud, R, et al. Unawareness of illness in chronic schizophrenia and its relationship to structural brain measures and neuropsychological tests. Psychiatry Res 2000; 100: 4958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Flashman, LA, McAllister, TW, Johnson, SC, Rick, JH, Green, RL, Saykin, AJ. Specific frontal lobe subregions correlated with unawareness of illness in schizophrenia: a preliminary study. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2001; 13: 255–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9 Sapara, A, Cooke, M, Fannon, D, Francis, A, Buchanan, RW, Anilkumar, AP, et al. Prefrontal cortex and insight in schizophrenia: a volumetric MRI study. Schizophr Res 2007; 89: 2234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10 Haa, T, Youna, T, Haa, K, Rhoc, K, Leed, J, Kimd, I, et al. Grey matter abnormalities in paranoid schizophrenia and their clinical correlations. Psychiatry Res: Neuroimaging 2004; 132: 251–60.Google Scholar
11 Cooke, MA, Fannon, D, Kulpers, E, Peters, E, Williams, SC, Kumari, V. Neurological basis of poor insight in psychosis: a voxel-based MRI study. Schizophr Res 2008; 103: 4051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12 Bassitt, D, Neto, M, de Castro, C, Busatto, G. Insight and regional brain volumes in schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2006; 257: 5862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
13 Rossell, SL, Coakes, J, Shapleske, J, Woodruff, PW, David, AS. Insight: its relationship with cognitive function, brain volume and symptoms in schizophrenia. Psychol Med 2003; 33: 111–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14 David, A, van Os, J, Jones, P, Harvey, I, Foerster, A, Fahy, T. Insight and psychotic illness. Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations. Br J Psychiatry 1995; 167: 621–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15 David, AS. Insight and psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 1990; 156: 798808.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16 Morgan, C, Dazzan, P, Morgan, K, Jones, P, Harrison, G, Leff, J, et al. First episode psychosis and ethnicity: initial findings from the AESOP study. World Psychiatry 2006; 5: 40–6.Google ScholarPubMed
17 World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. WHO, 1992.Google Scholar
18 World Health Organization. Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN 2.0). WHO, 1994.Google Scholar
19 Bebbington, P, Nayani, T. The Psychosis Screening Questionnaire. Int J Methods Psychiatric Res 1995; 5: 11–9.Google Scholar
20 Nelson, H, Willison, J. National Adult Reading Test (Part II) Test Manual. nferNelson, 1991.Google Scholar
21 Morgan, KD, Dazzan, P, Orr, KG, Hutchinson, G, Chitnis, X, Suckling, J, et al. Grey matter abnormalities in first-episode schizophrenia and affective psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 2007; 191 (suppl 51): s1116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
22 Wing, JK SE. The PSE-ID-CATEGO System: Supplementary Manual. Medical Research Council, 1978.Google Scholar
23 Kemp, R, David, A. Insight and compliance. In Treatment Compliance and the Therapeutic Alliance (ed Blackwell, B): 6184. Wisconsin, 1997.Google Scholar
24 David, AS, Morgan, KD, Mallett, RM, Leff, J, Murray, R. Insight: unitary or multidimensional phenomenon? Schizophr Res 2003; 60: 14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
25 Weschsler, D. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised. The Psychological Corporation, 1981.Google Scholar
26 Ravens, J. Coloured Progressive Matrices: Sets A, Ab, B. Oxford Psychologists Press, 1947.Google Scholar
27 Rey, A. L'examen Clinique en Psychologie. [Clinical Examination in Psychology.] Presses Universitaires de France, 1964.Google Scholar
28 Weschsler, D. Wechsler Memory Scale Revised. Psychological Corporation, 1987.Google Scholar
29 Gold, J, Carpenter, C, Randolph, C, Goldberg, T, Weinberger, D. Auditory working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia. Gen Psychiatry 1997; 54: 159–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30 Yeudall, L, Fromm, D, Reddon, J, Stefanyk, W. Normative data stratified by age and sex for 12 neuropsychological tests. J Clin Psychol 1986; 42: 918–46.3.0.CO;2-Y>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
31 Reitan, R. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of brain damage. Percept Mot Skills 1958; 8: 271–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
32 Bullmore, ET, Suckling, J, Overmeyer, S, Rabe-Hesketh, S, Taylor, E, Brammer, MJ. Global, voxel, and cluster tests, by theory and permutation, for a difference between two groups of structural MR images of the brain. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 1999; 18: 3242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33 Suckling, J, Sigmundsson, T, Greenwood, K, Bullmore, ET. A modified fuzzy clustering algorithm for operator independent brain tissue classification of dual echo MR images. Magn Reson Imaging 1999; 17: 1065–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34 Shenton, ME, Dickey, CC, Frumin, M, McCarley, RW. A review of MRI findings in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2001; 49: 152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35 Heinrichs, RW, Zakzanis, KK. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychol 1998; 12: 426–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36 Gusnard, DA, Raichle, ME, Raichle, ME. Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2001; 2: 685–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37 Spence, SA, Brooks, DJ, Hirsch, SR, Liddle, PF, Meehan, J, Grasby, PM. A PET study of voluntary movement in schizophrenic patients experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control). Brain 1997; 120 (Pt 11): 19972011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38 Berti, A, Bottini, G, Gandola, M, Pia, L, Smania, N, Stracciari, A, et al. Shared cortical anatomy for motor awareness and motor control. Science 2005; 309: 488–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39 Northoff, G, Heinzel, A, de Greck, M, Bermpohl, F, Dobrowolny, H, Panksepp, J. Self-referential processing in our brain–a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage 2006; 31: 440–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40 Ries, ML, Jabbar, BM, Schmitz, TW, Trivedi, MA, Gleason, CE, Carlsson, CM, et al. Anosognosia in mild cognitive impairment: relationship to activation of cortical midline structures involved in self-appraisal. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007; 13: 450–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41 Shergill, SS, Bullmore, ET, Brammer, MJ, Williams, SC, Murray, RM, McGuire, PK. A functional study of auditory verbal imagery. Psychol Med 2001; 31: 241–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42 Blakemore, SJ, Rees, G, Frith, CD. How do we predict the consequences of our actions? A functional imaging study. Neuropsychologia 1998; 36: 521–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43 Barta, PE, Pearlson, GD, Powers, RE, Richards, SS, Tune, LE. Auditory hallucinations and smaller superior temporal gyral volume in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1990; 147: 1457–62.Google ScholarPubMed
44 Shergill, SS, Cameron, LA, Brammer, MJ, Williams, SC, Murray, RM, McGuire, PK. Modality specific neural correlates of auditory and somatic hallucinations. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001; 71: 688–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45 McGlynn, SM, Schacter, DL. Unawareness of deficits in neuropsychological syndromes. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1989; 11: 143205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Previous studies of insight and brain structurea

Figure 1

Table 2 Background characteristics of sample

Figure 2

Table 3 Battery of cognitive tests

Figure 3

Fig. 1 Distribution of symptom relabelling scores (n = 82). SAI–E, Schedule for the Assessment of Insight – Expanded version.

Figure 4

Table 4 Comparison of brain structure and cognitive function between patients with low or moderate-high insight levels

Figure 5

Table 5 Location of grey matter deficits in relation to absent or at least partial symptom relabelling (n = 82)

Supplementary material: PDF

Morgan et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Figure S1

Download Morgan et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 105.8 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.