Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T05:08:09.646Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Nature, Nurture, and the Meaning of Educational Attainment: Differences by Sex and Socioeconomic Status

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 March 2023

Thalida Em Arpawong*
Affiliation:
Leonard Davis School of Gerontology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, USA
Margaret Gatz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Dornsife School of Letters, Arts and Sciences, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, USA Center for Economic and Social Research, Dornsife School of Letters, Arts and Sciences, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, USA
Catalina Zavala
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Dornsife School of Letters, Arts and Sciences, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, USA
Tara L. Gruenewald
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Chapman University, Orange, California, USA
Ellen E. Walters
Affiliation:
Center for Economic and Social Research, Dornsife School of Letters, Arts and Sciences, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, USA
Carol A. Prescott
Affiliation:
Leonard Davis School of Gerontology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, USA Department of Psychology, Dornsife School of Letters, Arts and Sciences, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Thalida Em Arpawong, Email: arpawong@usc.edu

Abstract

Estimated heritability of educational attainment (EA) varies widely, from 23% to 80%, with growing evidence suggesting the degree to which genetic variation contributes to individual differences in EA is highly dependent upon situational factors. We aimed to decompose EA into influences attributable to genetic propensity and to environmental context and their interplay, while considering influences of rearing household economic status (HES) and sex. We use the Project Talent Twin and Sibling Study, drawn from the population-representative cohort of high school students assessed in 1960 and followed through 2014, to ages 68−72. Data from 3552 twins and siblings from 1741 families were analyzed using multilevel regression and multiple group structural equation models. Individuals from less-advantaged backgrounds had lower EA and less variation. Genetic variance accounted for 51% of the total variance, but within women and men, 40% and 58% of the total variance respectively. Men had stable genetic variance on EA across all HES strata, whereas high HES women showed the same level of genetic influence as men, and lower HES women had constrained genetic influence on EA. Unexpectedly, middle HES women showed the largest constraints in genetic influence on EA. Shared family environment appears to make an outsized contribution to greater variability for women in this middle stratum and whether they pursue more EA. Implications are that without considering early life opportunity, genetic studies on education may mischaracterize sex differences because education reflects different degrees of genetic and environmental influences for women and men.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Society for Twin Studies

Educational attainment (EA) is used by many researchers as a predictor of important life outcomes, including economic success, health, longevity (Hummer & Lariscy, Reference Hummer, Lariscy, Rogers and Crimmins2011; Mirowsky, Reference Mirowsky2017; Montez & Hayward, Reference Montez and Hayward2014; Olshansky et al., Reference Olshansky, Antonucci, Berkman, Binstock, Boersch-Supan, Cacioppo, Carnes, Carstensen, Fried, Goldman, Jackson, Kohl, Rother, Zheng and Goldman2012), and risk for cognitive impairment and dementia (Cook & Fletcher, Reference Cook and Fletcher2015; Ngandu et al., Reference Ngandu, von Strauss, Helkala, Winblad, Nissinen, Tuomilehto, Soininen and Kivipelto2007). These outcomes result from a combination of social, behavioral, and genetic factors (Christensen et al., Reference Christensen, Johnson and Vaupel2006; Reiss & Neiderhiser, Reference Reiss and Neiderhiser2000) that also underlie EA. How these factors facilitate getting more education can operate differently by groups, including ones identified by social resources related to socioeconomics or gender (Buchmann & DiPrete, Reference Buchmann and DiPrete2006; DiPrete & Buchmann, Reference DiPrete, Buchmann and Logan2013). Social resources can also modulate the genetic influences on EA (Branigan et al., Reference Branigan, McCallum and Freese2013; Herd et al., Reference Herd, Freese, Sicinski, Domingue, Mullan Harris, Wei and Hauser2019), yet the magnitude of modulation is uncertain. Deciphering this will help us to better understand how social factors, or nurture, influence the degree to which genetic factors, or nature, differentially predict EA for different people. Using a twin and family design, we shed light on the extent to which an individual’s educational attainment reflects genetic and environmental influences, and whether the balance of these influences on EA varies for men and women, and disparities in the rearing household’s economic resources.

Sources of Genetic and Environmental Variance That Influence Educational Attainment

From the biological and genetic perspectives, higher EA stems from some degree of inherited predisposition (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Wedow, Okbay, Kong, Maghzian, Zacher, Nguyen-Viet, Bowers, Sidorenko, Karlsson Linnér, Fontana, Kundu, Lee, Li, Li, Royer, Timshel, Walters, Willoughby and Cesarini2018; Martin et al., Reference Martin, Medland, Verweij, Lee, Nyholt, Madden, Heath, Montgomery, Wright and Martin2011; Okbay et al., Reference Okbay, Beauchamp, Fontana, Lee, Pers, Rietveld, Turley, Chen, Emilsson, Meddens, Oskarsson, Pickrell, Thom, Timshe, de Vlaming, Abdellaoui, Ahluwalia, Bacelis, Baumbach and Meddens2016). A recent pooled analysis of twin and family studies estimated that 41% of the population variation in EA was due to genetic factors (Silventoinen et al., Reference Silventoinen, Jelenkovic, Sund, Latvala, Honda, Inui, Tomizawa, Watanabe, Sakai, Rebato, Busjahn, Tyler, Hopper, Ordoñana, Sánchez-Romera, Colodro-Conde, Calais-Ferreira, Oliveira, Ferreira and Rebato2020). An additional 31% and 26% of population variability for EA was attributable to shared and unique environmental factors respectively. Arguably, differences in heritability between groups reflect differences in their environmental variance rather than their genetic variance. For example, one twin study of EA found large differences in relative heritability values by sex in a U.S.-based study in Minnesota (18% for women and 38% for men) but not in Finland (45% for women and 48% for men) (Silventoinen et al., Reference Silventoinen, Krueger, Bouchard, Kaprio and McGue2004). The bigger difference in heritability between men and women in Minnesota was due to the fact that shared environmental influences on EA were twice as high for women versus men (50% vs. 25%) while in Finland, estimates by gender were similar (42% vs. 37%). Unique variance was also similar for women and men in each location (Minnesota: 32% and 37%; Finland: 13% and 15%). By looking at the contributions of shared environment by sex, it becomes clear that these estimates do not mean that additive genetic influences differ by sex or country, but rather reflect differences in the amount of raw variance explained by the environment. The differences in relative heritability are consistent with the fact that environments related to EA in Finland (i.e., educational policies that emphasize educational equity, social expectations about academic pursuits) are more uniform for males and females (Ahola et al., Reference Ahola, Hedmo, Thomsen and Vabø2014; Kyrö & Nyyssölä, Reference Kyrö and Nyyssölä2006). For the US, if the amount of raw environmental variance is smaller among men relative to women, and the amount of variance explained by genes is the same in both, then the heritability estimate will be greater for men. This would explain how we might observe an effect where the environment alters the influence of genetics on EA. However, raw variance values are not often provided by studies on EA. Consequently, when proportions of variance (i.e., heritabilities) are compared, it obscures our ability to compare the origins of the group differences.

Additionally, twin models typically assume genetic and environmental components are independent (uncorrelated) and additive. Across generations, social stratification in human populations has resulted in genetic-environment correlations, such as when highly educated parents provide their offspring with both genetic endowment and environmental contexts conducive to higher scholastic success. While we do not have access to parental genetic endowment in our study, we can look at the downstream effects in which such stratification can result in more substantial differences for particular subgroups. This form of stratification exemplifies a gene-by-environment interaction (G × E), where average genetic influence expressed across a population, or heritability, varies by social environment.

Heritability Differences Associated with Socioeconomic Status (SES)

Heritability-by-SES interactions have been examined most often in twin studies of intellectual abilities. Some studies have reported heritability for cognitive ability to be higher for twins from higher SES compared to twins from lower SES backgrounds (Rowe et al., Reference Rowe, Jacobson and Van den Oord1999; Scarr-Salapatek, Reference Scarr-Salapatek1971; Tucker-Drob & Bates, Reference Tucker-Drob and Bates2016; Turkheimer et al., Reference Turkheimer, Haley, Waldron, D’Onofrio and Gottesman2003). This is interpreted to indicate that, on average, high SES environments provide opportunities for individuals to achieve their intellectual potential, whereas individuals from lower SES background have less opportunity for full expression of their genetically driven talents.

Only one prior twin study has tested the heritability-by-SES interaction for EA. The study, conducted in a German sample, found some evidence there was higher heritability among twins whose parents had higher (70−78%) vesus lower (33−40%) educational levels, where such educational levels was used as a surrogate for rearing SES (Baier & Lang, Reference Baier and Lang2019). In addition to not having a direct measure of parental SES, the study assessed EA in a sample aged 22−26, before many individuals had the opportunity to complete their education. Although rearing SES does often include parental EA, the study focused exclusively on transmission of EA from one generation to the next.

Implications for Sex or Gender Differences in Genetic and Environmental Influences on EA

Average EA has been shown to be higher for men than women in the current population of older aged individuals who are 70+, but we know less about differences between men and women in genetic and environmental sources of variance in EA by their measured social context. As noted previously, heritability differences can under- or overrepresent the average expression of genetic abilities for subgroups of individuals reared in different social environments (Boardman, Reference Boardman2009; Jencks, Reference Jencks1980; Turkheimer & Horn, Reference Turkheimer, Horn, Finkel and Reynolds2014), including social experiences that differ for men and women. Investigating the basis of sex or gender differences may help explain how these effect relationships between EA and later life health outcomes. For example, if the underlying structure of genetic and environmental influences on EA differs for men and women, such findings could generate hypotheses on why EA predicts cognitive impairment more for men than for women (Gatz et al., Reference Gatz, Svedberg, Pedersen, Mortimer, Berg and Johansson2001; Karp et al., Reference Karp, Kareholt, Qiu, Bellander, Winblad and Fratiglioni2004; Letenneur et al., Reference Letenneur, Launer, Andersen, Dewey, Ott, Copeland, Dartigues, Kragh-Sorensen, Baldereschi, Brayne, Lobo, Martinez-Lage, Stijnen and Hofman2000).

To our knowledge, one study has examined sex × environment × genetic interactions for EA and compared estimates for twins raised in very different settings. This study included the aforementioned twins from Finland and Minnesota (Silventoinen et al., Reference Silventoinen, Krueger, Bouchard, Kaprio and McGue2004). The three-way interaction was also supported by a meta-analysis across 10 countries that reported more of the proportional variance in educational attainment among men was accounted for by genetics, whereas the shared environment accounted for a greater percentage of the variance for women (Branigan et al., Reference Branigan, McCallum and Freese2013). However, in these studies, the reliance on proportions of variance does not facilitate an evaluation of whether nature or nurture is the source of sex differences.

Current Study

Data for the present study come from the Project Talent Twin and Sibling Study (PTTS), a follow-up cohort from Project Talent, a population-representative study of U.S. high school students first studied in 1960 (Flanagan, Reference Flanagan1962), then surveyed again several times via mail and web, between 2014 and 2019 (Prescott et al., Reference Prescott, Achorn, Kaiser, Mitchell, McArdle and Lapham2013; Prescott et al., Reference Prescott, Walters, Arpawong, Zavala, Gruenewald and Gatz2019). Using PTTS for this work provides a rare opportunity to decompose the nature and nurture of EA because measures of family socioeconomic background were collected in 1960 (vs. using retrospective data) from multiple family informants; both twins and siblings are included to provide greater power for decomposing variation (Posthuma et al., Reference Posthuma, Beem, de Geus, van Baal, von Hjelmborg, Iachine and Boomsma2003) in EA; and EA was collected after individuals had completed formal schooling. PTTS is comprised of individuals born in the early 1940s, and thus expectations of EA differs from contemporary cohorts, in which women born after 1960 are expected to attain more education than men (DiPrete & Buchmann, Reference DiPrete, Buchmann and Logan2013). However, PTTS is comprised of a sample of today’s 75-year-olds, and evaluating the effects of early life factors on education is highly relevant to current research with older adults, especially when aimed at evaluating sex effects in the relationships between education and later life health outcomes, including cognition and dementia.

Goals of this study are to use a genetically informative design to decipher the etiology and sources of individual differences in educational attainment. We do this by determining the extent to which genetic propensity and shared and unique environmental resources influence educational attainment and whether these factors differ by sex and by family economic resources. Aims are to: (1) investigate how EA differs by sex and socioeconomic background; (2) examine whether there are sex differences in sources of variance underlying EA; and (3) study whether there are sex differences in how genetic influences on EA differ by rearing socioeconomic background. We hypothesize that overall: (h1) men and those of higher socioeconomic background will exhibit more years of EA compared to women and individuals of lower socioeconomic backgrounds; (h2) shared and unique environmental factors will contribute more to individual differences in EA for women compared to men; and (h3) the degree of influence from nature and nurture on EA will differ across groups, with genetic variance being restricted for women and individuals of lower socioeconomic background.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

The PTTS (Prescott et al., Reference Prescott, Achorn, Kaiser, Mitchell, McArdle and Lapham2013; Prescott et al., Reference Prescott, Walters, Arpawong, Zavala, Gruenewald and Gatz2019) is a follow-up study to a subsample of PT, which is a population-representative sample of U.S. high school students in 1960, including 377,015 students first tested in 1960 and followed through 1974 (Wise et al., Reference Wise, McLaughlin and Steel1979). In 2014, all potential twins and siblings of twins were identified in the original PT study, yielding 5003 eligible participants who were contacted for inclusion in PTTS and for additional follow-up in 2019 on their educational, occupational and health outcomes (Prescott et al., Reference Prescott, Walters, Arpawong, Zavala, Gruenewald and Gatz2019). The final phenotypic sample for the current study includes individuals with complete data on educational attainment and other key variables (i.e., sex, household economic status, twin or sibling status) and includes 3552 individuals from 1778 families (93.6% non-Hispanic White, 3.6% African American, 1.3% Hispanic, and 1.5% Other or Mixed Race). Because we allowed up to two twins and one brother and one sister in extended twin biometric analyses, and some families contained triplets or more than two siblings, six individuals were excluded from these families (details provided in the supplementary material) in the final biometric sample. Thus, the biometric analyses included 3546 individuals from 1778 families, in which 3314 individuals were twins, 197 were non-twin brothers, and 215 were non-twin sisters.

Measures

Years of education attained

EA reflects the number of years of formal education completed. Years of education was chosen as the outcome as it represents cumulative achievement and helps to determine finer differences with incremental change from underlying influences that achievement levels or number of degrees cannot. We constructed a harmonized EA variable using all available sources, including self- and sibling reports. Among 2532 respondents to the 2014−2019 surveys, EA was available by self-report (n = 2304), from responses to surveys conducted 5 and 11 years after expected high school graduation (n = 24), or from sibling or cotwin reports in 2014 (n = 24). For the 1200 deceased or nonrespondents to 2014−2019 surveys, EA was available from sibling or cotwin reports (n = 824) or from the 5- and 11-year follow-ups (n = 376). EA is coded as: 9 = 9th grade, 10 = 10th grade, 11 = 11th grade, 12 = GED or high school diploma, 14 = associate’s degree, 16 = 4-year degree, 18 = master’s degree, 20 = doctorate. (Validation of sibling reports and other details are in the supplementary material, including Table S1).

Household economic status (HES)

Family of origin socioeconomic status was indexed by a composite score formed from student responses on five items on the 1960 Project Talent survey. Item content covered household income, home value and ownership, and presence of furnishings, luxury goods, and books in the household (Wise et al., Reference Wise, McLaughlin and Steel1979). (See supplementary material for item wording and response options.) For the 1960 HES composite, first, each student’s response to an item was converted to a z score, with a mean of zero and standard deviation of 1, computed by using the raw mean and standard deviation values from all of the 1960 respondents. Next, an individual-level HES score was constructed by calculating the average of each person’s nonmissing z scores. Finally, the family HES score was calculated as the average of scores from all participating members in a family. Because preliminary analysis showed that the relation between family HES and EA was not linear, and our interest was in comparing the effect of higher versus lower HES on EA, we constructed three discrete strata based on the distribution of HES calculated in the entire 1960 sample: Individuals scoring in the lowest <25% of the 1960 HES range were designated as low HES, those within 25 to <75% of the range were middle HES, and those at 75% or higher were high HES.

Sex and zygosity

We use the term sex to refer to both biological sex and gender. For all individuals participating in PTTS, the sex provided by their high school in 1960 corresponded to their self-reported gender in 2014. Zygosity refers to the determination of whether twin pairs are monozygotic (MZ, or identical) or dizygotic (DZ, or fraternal). Details on zygosity assignments are provided in the supplementary material.

Analyses

We conducted individual-level, or phenotypic, regressions to investigate how educational attainment was predicted by rearing-family socioeconomic indicators and whether this differed by sex (raw data plot shown in Figure S1 in the supplementary material). We then fit a series of biometric variance component models to estimate the relative importance of latent genetic and environmental factors and whether these differ by sex and family HES. Data management and individual-level analysis were conducted in SAS v9.4 (SAS Institute, 2018) and biometric variance components analysis of twin and sibling data were completed in Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2008).

Individual-level analyses

Individual-level regression models were used to evaluate the effects of sex, HES and the interaction of sex × HES on EA (raw data plot, shown in Figure S2 in the supplementary material). Given the family structure of the data, we fit multilevel regression models with individuals nested within families using PROC MIXED in SAS to obtain unbiased estimates of standard errors. We use explained variance (R 2) between nested models to evaluate the importance of added term(s).

Biometric model estimation

We implemented a univariate twin modeling approach to estimate the sources of individual differences in EA, specified by three estimated genetic and environmental variance components: additive genetic (A), shared environment (S), and unique individual environment (E). When using the ‘ASE’ models, several assumptions are made for each component (Eaves et al., Reference Eaves, Last, Young and Martin1978; McArdle & Prescott, Reference McArdle and Prescott2010). Assumptions are that A represents the effects of genetic alleles that combine additively; that MZ pairs share 100% of additive genetic variance, and on average, DZ pairs and ordinary siblings share 50%; S encompasses all aspects of the environment that they share equally and result in twins being similar, including their rearing environment, family social class, other social factors such as religion, race/ethnicity, as well as effects of community-level factors (e.g., school quality, national policies) (Prescott et al., Reference Prescott, Arpawong, McArdle and Wright2015; Prescott & Kendler, Reference Prescott and Kendler1995); and E includes all nonshared factors contributing to differences among twins from the same family, such as events uniquely experienced by the individual as well as measurement error. With this design, the variance and covariance of nontwin siblings are modeled in the same way as for a DZ twin (Blokland et al., Reference Blokland, Mosing, Verweij, Medland and Little2013). Based on the difference in expected genetic resemblance of MZs and DZs, or nontwin siblings, we can construct structural equation models (SEM) to decompose the variance in EA into A, S, and E variance.

Preliminary intrapair correlation calculations (presented in Table S4 in the supplementary material) indicated that EA means, variances and covariances among non-twin sibling pairs (i.e., twin with non-twin sibling) did not differ significantly from those for DZ twin pairs, such that we did not need to include twin environment as a separate component in subsequent model fitting. Calculations for intrapair correlations were conducted with some individuals included more than once, as a co-twin and a sibling of a non-twin sibling. This occurred in 278 families that had a pair of twins and also one non-twin sibling, so that each twin was paired with the co-twin and the sibling; 35 families that had a pair of twins and also two non-twin siblings so that each twin was paired with a co-twin and each sibling; and two families in which there was one twin and two non-twin siblings so the twin was paired with each sibling.

To accommodate the variety of family compositions in number and sex of siblings in our sample, we implemented an expanded twin family modeling approach to allow family structures with up to two twins plus one brother and one sister. Coding details for each family structure are provided in the supplementary methods. We evaluated our hypotheses concerning differences in EA variance sources by fitting a series of sex- and HES-moderated variance components models estimating additive genetic, shared and unique environmental factors using multiple-group SEM. We used a series of five-group SEMs to initially test for sex differences in means and variance structures. We then used a series of 15-group SEMs to partial HES from EA and evaluate models for HES × sex differences, by allowing variance structures to differ by sex and HES levels. Individuals were first assigned to five groups based on sex and zygosity type of the twin pair. Assignment details for family structures with more than two twins or siblings is provided in the supplementary methods. Families in the five groups were further assigned to one of 15 groups, based on rearing family HES (3 strata: low, middle, high) to evaluate HES and HES × sex differences. Models were estimated with full information maximum likelihood (FIML) to minimize the -2 log likelihood (-2LL). When comparing alternative models, we used the difference between their -2LL, which is distributed as a χ 2 statistic. Details of using SEM to fit models for twin data are available in greater detail elsewhere (McArdle & Prescott, Reference McArdle and Prescott2005).

A nonstandard aspect of our analysis approach is the use of HES as a continuous predictor of EA and HES-strata as a categorical moderator of variance components. When a hypothesized moderator is not independent of the outcome, stratifying on it reduces the total variance by removing some of the moderator-outcome covariance. The usual approach for testing moderation of variance components in twin/family models is to estimate simultaneously the biometric structure of the dependent variable, the moderator and their covariance (Neale et al., Reference Neale, Roysamb and Jacobson2006; Purcell, Reference Purcell2002). This decomposition of the moderator is not necessary in the present case, as HES is by definition a family-level variable and contributes only to shared environmental variance. Variation in EA due to a linear effect of HES is part of the shared (family) variance; any remaining nonlinear effects are included in the estimated latent common variance for the HES strata.

Results

Table 1 summarizes key variables for the 1876 females (53%) and 1676 males (47%) in the analysis sample with EA quantified in years. Average age at the 2014 follow-up was 69.7 years. Men had significantly higher EA than women (14.2 vs. 13.5 years) and were more likely to graduate college (36.9% vs. 25.9%). HES measured in 1960 did not differ between men and women.

Table 1. Descriptive information by sex on 3552 twins and siblings from 1778 families

Note: HES variables were scaled in z scores for analysis though converted to raw scores in this table to be interpreted as the range of 0 (low) to 5 (high), with high HES indicating more household resources. Range for age was 67 to 72 and for educational attainment was 9 to 20 years. M, mean; SD, standard deviation; CI, confidence interval. *p < .05; ***p < .001.

Differences in Years of Education by Sex and Socioeconomic Background

First, to begin addressing aim 1, we investigate how EA differ by sex and socioeconomic background. In all three strata of HES, we calculated that men had higher mean and greater variability in EA compared to women: low (male M = 13.2, SD = 2.2; female M = 12.6, SD = 1.8), middle (male M = 14.0, SD = 2.5; female M = 13.3, SD = 2.2), and high (male M = 15.6, SD = 2.8; female M = 14.9, SD = 2.5).

Using multilevel regression, we construct a model with EA as the dependent variable that accounts for family relationships, includes fixed effects of sex and 1960 family HES, and adjusts for other indicators of socioeconomic status including father’s education, mother’s education, and father’s occupation. The full model accounted for 20.9% of the variation in EA (details in Table S3 in the supplementary material). Results confirmed hypothesis 1: Compared to females, being male was associated with 0.60 years higher EA. Overall, a 1 SD higher HES score predicted 0.82 years more in EA. Adding a sex-by-HES interaction to the model did not increase the explained variance in EA (R 2 < .1%) thereby indicating that the observed relationship between EA and HES was the same for men and women.

Sex Differences in the Underlying Sources of Variance on Education Attained

Second, we address aim 2, whether there are sex differences in the sources of variances underlying EA. First, we calculate pair resemblance by pair sex and relationship type (presented in Table S4 in the supplementary material). We verified that intrapair correlations for EA were higher for MZ (248 female pairs r = .69, 196 male pairs r = .74) than for DZ pairs (273 female pairs r = .56, 240 male pairs r = .39), which is evidence for the presence of genetic contributions to individual differences in EA.

Next, we fit a series of five-group structural equation models (SEMs), in which individuals were assigned to the groups based on sex and zygosity type of the twin pair (MZ females, MZ males, DZ females and female siblings, DZ males and male siblings, opposite-sex DZ twins and siblings) and conducted a general test of equality of variance and components of variance. Model fitting results are presented in Table S5 in the supplementary material. Model-fitting results reject that there is equality across sex (χ2 = 33.59, df = 3, model 1 in Table S5 in the supplementary material). The effect of HES contributed equally to EA for men and women, as a test for whether the effect of HES differed across sex showed no difference in model fit (χ2 = 1.35, df = 1, model 2 in Table S5 in the supplementary material). Given these results, in all subsequent analyses, variance estimates were allowed to differ by sex, and the HES intercept was equated across sex. Figure 1 depicts how the sexes differ in total variance and magnitudes of genetic and environmental variance for EA, including HES (estimates are provided in Table S6 in the supplementary material). Men had a greater amount of variance overall, and more genetic variance than women. In relative amounts, genetic variance accounted for 51% of the total variance, but within women and men, heritability was calculated as 40% and 58% respectively. Women had more shared environmental variance contributing to EA than men.

Fig. 1. Total variance and components of variance in years of education attained estimated from twin pair relationships. Variance components (y-axis) are divided into sources, from household economic status (HES), shared environment, genetic factors, and unique environment. Columns depict variance sources estimated separately for women (left) and men (right) using an expanded sex-moderation model (sex-specific variance estimates are provided in Supplementary Material, Table S6).

Moderation of Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Education Attained by Sex and HES

To address aim 3 and the fundamental question of whether education reflects the same influences across different rearing contexts, we evaluated the degree that genetic and environmental influences differ by sex and HES strata. To this end, we fit a series of biometric models using 15-group SEMs. Comparison models were evaluated relative to a baseline model equating variance components across sex and HES strata. All models allowed different intercepts for each sex × HES level. Model-fitting results are presented in Table S7 in the supplementary material. Comparing models, we found that a model allowing variance sources for EA to differ by HES strata as well as sex fit substantially better than the HES-invariance model (χ2 = 70.11, df = 12; model 2 in Table S7 in the supplementary material). As shown in Table 2, the total variance in EA increased as HES stratum increased and was greater for men than for women at each stratum.

Table 2. Genetic and environmental sources of variation in educational attainment by family rearing Household Economic Status (HES) level, separately for females and males

Note: Var, variance; SE, standard error; %, percent of total variance.

HES levels are based on quartiles from the full 1960 Project Talent sample: Low HES includes 0 to 25 percentile; Middle is 25 to 75 percentile; High is above 75 percentile.

Variance estimates are from a model with intercepts and variances allowed to vary by sex and each level of HES (Model 2 in Supplement Table S7). Family HES is estimated across all strata and equated over sex.

What we did not expect, and has not been reported previously, is that HES moderation of genetic variance was present only for women. This is depicted in Figure 2 to emphasize the pattern of variance estimates across HES. A model equating genetic variance across HES for males did not show worse fit than allowing HES-specific genetic estimates (χ2 = .98, df = 2). However, in women, the test for equality of genetic variance across HES was rejected (χ2 = 14.14, df = 2). A series of model comparisons showed that underlying differences across HES are due to differences in genetic variance (depicted in Figure 2). Although we hypothesized that genetic variance in EA would be greater with higher strata of HES, this was true for women only, with about double the absolute variance for lowest compared to highest HES. This finding would not have been detected if relying on variance proportions (shown in Table 2) to compare genetic contributions to EA by HES stratum for women. The effect of HES was not linear. For women, genetic factors contribute the most in variance for the high HES stratum and least in variance for the middle HES stratum, with both low and middle HES strata having less variance than the high stratum and less variance compared to men in all strata.

Fig. 2. Total variance and magnitude for sources of variance (y-axis) underlying years of educational attainment estimated separately for (a) women and (b) men, by household economic status (HES) strata (x-axis). Columns depict variance sources — from HES, shared environment, genetic factors, and unique environment — estimated separately for each HES strata (model 2 in Supplementary Material, Table S7). Estimates show that HES moderation of genetic variance was present only for women.

The nonlinear pattern of variance estimates across HES raised the question of how dependent the results were on the cut-points used to define HES strata. We examined this by estimating the sex differences model with seven overlapping subsamples, each comprising 25% of the full sample, defined by a moving window of HES percentile (i.e., 0 to 25, 12.5 to 37.5, 25 to 50, 37.5 to 62.5, 50 to 75, 62.5 to 87.5, and 75 to 100). These estimates, shown in Figure S3 in the supplementary material, illustrate that there were more distinct differences in genetic variance for EA at HES percentile points of 25% and 75% for women, whereas for men differences were more moderate across HES percentiles. This supports a pattern of difference in genetic variance across HES for women and the importance of making comparisons between low, middle and high strata to test for differential contributions of genetic variance.

Discussion

In this article, we addressed a long-standing question on the importance of nature, via genetic endowment, and nurture, via shared and unique environmental influences, for EA. We found that the balance of nature and nurture underlying EA is not uniform between sexes. First, men and women who were raised in homes with higher household economic status had more years of EA and greater variability in EA than those raised in homes with lower household economic status. Second, we found that overall, there was larger genetic and total variance underlying EA for men than women. Third, nature makes the largest contribution for individuals from the highest family-of-origin economic backgrounds for both men and women. When sex and household economic stratum are both considered, absolute genetic variance contributes similar amounts for men across the economic strata, as well as for women from only the highest economic strata. For women in the lowest and middle economic strata, genetic variance contributes much less to the variability in EA compared to women in the highest economic stratum and to male counterparts across all economic strata. Unexpectedly, for women from the middle economic stratum, it appears that family rearing environment, which may in part reflect parents’ own genetic endowments, may take on an outsized role in contributing to EA. These results confirm that critical interrelationships exist, with nurture moderating effects of nature to alter the range of influence possible on EA. Greater total variance and expression of genetic potential for EA is afforded differently by degree of household economic resources when growing up and in combination with sex or gender.

Findings from this study help us understand the etiology of EA and that EA does not mean the same thing across people, especially for older women today, who were born in the early 1940s. Differences in household economic resources contribute to a disparity in total years of education attained, evidenced by lower overall means and less variability in years of EA for both men and women in the lowest household economic brackets. The finding on differing sources of variance for EA for women by socioeconomic strata was not detectable when examining phenotypic HES × sex effects in predicting EA. This points out that null results in phenotypic models that test sex interactions do not preclude there being differences in etiologies for men and women, particularly with regard to outcomes like EA that are likely influenced by complex processes related to gender socialization and expectations.

Overall, genetic variance accounted for more variation in men’s EA than women’s, at 58% and 40% of the total variance respectively. This is consistent with the ranges for sex-specific calculations reported in prior literature (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Treloar, Reynolds, Heath and Martin1996; Branigan et al., Reference Branigan, McCallum and Freese2013; Heath et al., Reference Heath, Berg, Eaves, Solaas, Corey, Sundet, Magnus and Nance1985; Nielsen & Roos, Reference Nielsen and Roos2015). In turn, the role of nurture was greater for women than for men. This finding supports the interpretation that socio-cultural factors and opportunities shape different trajectories of expression of genetic endowment for men and women (Allan, Reference Allan2011; Klein et al., Reference Klein, Ortman, Campbell, Greenberg, Hollingsworth, Jacobs, Kachuck, McClelland, Pollard, Sadker, Sadker, Schmuck, Scott and Wiggins1994). In this cohort, men were able to pursue genetically driven talents for EA irrespective of socioeconomic strata of their families of origin, but women did not have the same benefit unless in the highest HES group. These findings are in line with prior research that has not detected SES-by-sex differences in the heritability of EA (Branigan et al., Reference Branigan, McCallum and Freese2013; Silventoinen et al., Reference Silventoinen, Krueger, Bouchard, Kaprio and McGue2004) in countries that implement social policies to promote equity in access to educational opportunities (Ahola et al., Reference Ahola, Hedmo, Thomsen and Vabø2014; Gorard & Smith, Reference Gorard and Smith2004; Kyrö & Nyyssölä, Reference Kyrö and Nyyssölä2006). Our findings also support evidence thus far on sex differences by country and birth cohort that show EA likely reflects accumulated genetic sensitivities to the environment (gene-by-SES and gene-by-sex) that are different depending on environmental circumstances (Heath et al., Reference Heath, Berg, Eaves, Solaas, Corey, Sundet, Magnus and Nance1985; Silventoinen et al., Reference Silventoinen, Krueger, Bouchard, Kaprio and McGue2004), and therefore support for G × E effects, by sex and socio-economic group. This points to the results reflecting an opportunity structure and differences in men’s and women’s lived experiences, not biological sex differences.

Shared family estimates from this study are substantially smaller than what is reported in the most recent meta-analysis of proportional variance, yet closer to what is expected given prior knowledge of twin and family studies of other traits (Turkheimer, Reference Turkheimer and DiLalla2004). Shared environmental variance encompasses family-level resources, including the measured component of household economic status and nonmeasured components, such as family activities and behaviors modeled at home to facilitate exposure to scholarly interests or success in academic pursuits, an living in social and built environments that promote EA. These are not entirely distinguishable from the larger community-level environmental factors that members from the same household share, such as better school quality, or access to healthcare services that promote mental and emotional health. Among women in the middle socioeconomic stratum, the shared family environment appears to make a particularly weighty contribution to greater variability in whether these women pursue higher educational attainment. Conceptually, the family-level resources can have genetic components (e.g., through genetic-environment correlation, effects of assortative mating), but given that twin correlations for both MZ and DZ women were similar and large, this implies that twin and sibling members of the family experience them as a part of their shared, social environment.

Environmental factors on EA that are resources unique to the individual could include parent expectations placed on individual children, peer encouragement, varied learning opportunities offered by teachers, or experiences after school that reinforce scholarly pursuits. When comparing estimates by sex and HES strata, differences in unique, individual-specific experiences are relatively small. Although it is possible these factors have profound influence on particular individuals, findings suggest that adolescents who grew up with more influences from both unique factors and socioeconomic resources in the family are more variable in whether they pursue higher educational opportunities.

The balance of nature and nurture components holds implications for use of EA to predict later life outcomes for different groups. While there are robust and consistent correlations in the literature between education and cognitive function (Opdebeeck et al., Reference Opdebeeck, Martyr and Clare2016; Ritchie & Tucker-Drob, Reference Ritchie and Tucker-Drob2018), our prior work showed that genetic variance underlying earlier life cognitive ability overlaps only 11% with genetic variance sources for EA (Arpawong et al., Reference Arpawong, Zavala, Gatz, Gruenewald and Prescott2018). This finding suggests that the strong relationship between education and cognition is predominantly driven by overlapping nurture components, including life experiences and resources. Relatedly, while education has shown strong predictive ability for cognitive impairment and dementia (Caamaño-Isorna et al., Reference Caamaño-Isorna, Corral, Montes-Martínez and Takkouche2006), it has also shown differential ability to predict dementia risk by sex. In particular, education has the lowest predictive value for risk of dementia among women in more impoverished countries (Sharp & Gatz, Reference Sharp and Gatz2011). Given our findings, we speculate that constrained potential in lower resourced countries and for women means that irrespective of genetic potential, there is less opportunity for these women to attain education; hence, this reduces the overlapping genetic variance between education and cognition. If access to education is driven by within-country environmental factors (e.g., access to resources to pay for education, social prioritization of academic achievement for boys vs. girls), this likely reduces the genetic correlations for EA and cognitive status. In contrast, in higher resourced countries and for men, genetic endowment has more opportunity for expression and thus greater overlap.

In this Project Talent sample, we had limited power to test effects of other social constraints, such as racial/ethnic inequalities. Additionally, we are unable to assess mechanisms by which socioeconomic bracket influences educational differences beyond the variance components quantified, or for those who would not have attended high school given the recruitment design for Project Talent and compulsory schooling laws. Furthermore, we cannot conclude causal associations. For instance, common concerns about causal inference in observational studies center on issues of reverse causation and confounding (McGue et al., Reference McGue, Osler and Christensen2010). With the present study, we use a longitudinal design where genetics and household economic status precede educational attainment, thus alleviating the first concern. With the second concern, invoking the twin design enables us to control for genetics and shared family environment, and thereby account for the degree of influences from unmeasured environmental factors, or potential confounders (McGue et al., Reference McGue, Osler and Christensen2010). Thus, although we are not able to establish causality with this study, we are able to make inferences for the direction of effects. A limitation to the design is our inability to make general inferences about siblings because those included are all siblings of twins, and siblings within the same age range of twins, and thus are not representative of the experience of all siblings within families. Lastly, results are likely cohort specific because our estimates align well with prior research evaluating variance sources in education in individuals born between 1940 and 1961 (Heath et al., Reference Heath, Berg, Eaves, Solaas, Corey, Sundet, Magnus and Nance1985). Follow-up analyses in younger cohorts will be important to compare differences in findings.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2023.6.

Data availability

Data used for this study is available for approved researchers: https://www.air.org/project/project-talent

Acknowledgments

We thank all Project Talent participants for sticking with the studies for up to six decades. We thank Ken Schultz and Jimi Huh for their reading of manuscript drafts, Susan J. Lapham, Kelly Peters, and Deanna Achorn-Lyter at American Institutes for Research (AIR) for their work with Project Talent studies.

Author contributions

TEA and CAP designed the study; TEA, CAP, EEW, CZ analyzed data; TEA, CAP and MG drafted the manuscript; TEA, MG, TLG, EEW, CAP contributed to data development, interpretation, and critical revisions.

Financial support

This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health, Grant Numbers for the Project Talent Twin and Sibling Study (NIA AG-043656, PI: Prescott) and Project Talent Aging Study (NIA AG-056163, PI: Carol A. Prescott; RF1 AG-056164, PI: Manly). Contributions by C. Zavala were supported by a pilot project (NIA P30 AG-017265, PI: Eileen M. Crimmins).

Conflict of interest

None

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008

References

Ahola, S., Hedmo, T., Thomsen, J.-P., & Vabø, A. (2014). Organisational features of higher education; Denmark, Finland, Norway and Sweden. University of Copenhagen. http://www.nifu.no/publications/1162368/ Google Scholar
Allan, E. J. (2011). Women’s status in higher education ¾ Equity matters. ASHE Higher Education Report, 37, 1163.Google Scholar
Arpawong, T., Zavala, C., Gatz, M., Gruenewald, T., & Prescott, C. (2018). Genetics matter less for women and people of lower socioeconomic status in educational attainment. Innovation in Aging, 2, 936937. doi: 10.1093/geroni/igy031.3480 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baier, T., & Lang, V. (2019). The social stratification of environmental and genetic influences on education: New evidence using a register-based twin sample. Sociological Science, 6, 143171. doi: 10.15195/v6.a6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, L. A., Treloar, S. A., Reynolds, C. A., Heath, A. C., & Martin, N. G. (1996). Genetics of educational attainment in Australian twins: Sex differences and secular changes. Behavior Genetics, 26, 89102. doi: 10.1007/BF02359887 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blokland, G. A., Mosing, M. A., Verweij, K. H., & Medland, S. E. (2013). Twin studies and behavior genetics. In Little, T. D. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of quantitative methods: Statistical analysis, (pp. 198218). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Boardman, J. D. (2009). State-level moderation of genetic tendencies to smoke. American Journal of Public Health, 99, 480486. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.134932 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branigan, A. R., McCallum, K. J., & Freese, J. (2013). Variation in the heritability of educational attainment: An international meta-analysis. Social Forces, 92, 109140. doi: 10.1093/sf/sot076 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchmann, C., & DiPrete, T. A. (2006). The growing female advantage in college completion: The role of family background and academic achievement. American Sociological Review, 71, 515541. https://doi.org/10.1177/000312240607100401 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caamaño-Isorna, F., Corral, M., Montes-Martínez, A., & Takkouche, B. (2006). Education and dementia: A meta-analytic study. Neuroepidemiology, 26, 226232. doi: 10.1159/000093378 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christensen, K., Johnson, T. E., & Vaupel, J. W. (2006). The quest for genetic determinants of human longevity: Challenges and insights. Nature Reviews Genetics, 7, 436448. doi: 10.1038/nrg1871 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, C. J., & Fletcher, J. M. (2015). Can education rescue genetic liability for cognitive decline? Social Science & Medicine, 127, 159170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.06.049 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DiPrete, T. A., & Buchmann, C. (2013). Gender disparities in educational attainment in the new century: Trends, causes and consequences. In Logan, J. (Ed.), Diversity & disparities: America enters a new century (pp. 375414). Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Eaves, L. J., Last, K. A., Young, P. A., & Martin, N. G. (1978). Model-fitting approaches to the analysis of human behaviour. Heredity, 41, 249320. doi: 10.1038/hdy.1978.101 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flanagan, J. C. (1962). Project Talent. Applied Psychology, 11, 314. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.1962.tb00440.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gatz, M., Svedberg, P., Pedersen, N. L., Mortimer, J. A., Berg, S., & Johansson, B. (2001). Education and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease: Findings from the study of dementia in Swedish twins. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 56, P292P300. DOI: 10.1093/geronb/56.5.p292 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorard, S., & Smith, E. (2004). An international comparison of equity in education systems. Comparative Education, 40, 1528. https://doi.org/10.1080/0305006042000184863 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heath, A. C., Berg, K., Eaves, L. J., Solaas, M. H., Corey, L. A., Sundet, J., Magnus, P., & Nance, W. E. (1985). Education policy and the heritability of educational attainment. Nature, 314, 734736. doi: 10.1038/314734a0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herd, P., Freese, J., Sicinski, K., Domingue, B. W., Mullan Harris, K., Wei, C., & Hauser, R. M. (2019). Genes, gender inequality, and educational attainment. American Sociological Review, 84, 10691098. https://doi.org/10.1177/0003122419886550 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hummer, R. A., & Lariscy, J. T. (2011). Educational attainment and adult mortality. In Rogers, R. G. & Crimmins, E. M. (Eds.), International handbook of adult mortality (pp. 241261). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jencks, C. (1980). Heredity, environment, and public policy reconsidered. American Sociological Review, 45, 723736. https://doi.org/10.2307/2094892 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karp, A., Kareholt, I., Qiu, C., Bellander, T., Winblad, B., & Fratiglioni, L. (2004). Relation of education and occupation-based socioeconomic status to incident Alzheimer’s disease. American Journal of Epidemiology, 159, 175183. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwh018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, S. S., Ortman, P. E., Campbell, P., Greenberg, S., Hollingsworth, S., Jacobs, J., Kachuck, B., McClelland, A., Pollard, D., Sadker, D., Sadker, M., Schmuck, P., Scott, E., & Wiggins, J. (1994). Continuing the journey toward gender equity. Educational Researcher, 23, 1321. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X023008013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyrö, M., & Nyyssölä, K. (2006). Attitudes towards education in Finland and other Nordic countries. European Journal of Education, 41, 5970. doi: 10.1111/j.1465-3435.2006.00246.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, J. J., Wedow, R., Okbay, A., Kong, E., Maghzian, O., Zacher, M., Nguyen-Viet, T. A., Bowers, P., Sidorenko, J., Karlsson Linnér, R., Fontana, M. A., Kundu, T., Lee, C., Li, H., Li, R., Royer, R., Timshel, P. N., Walters, R. K., Willoughby, E. A., ¼ Cesarini, D. (2018). Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nature Genetics, 50, 11121121. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0147-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Letenneur, L., Launer, L. J., Andersen, K., Dewey, M. E., Ott, A., Copeland, J. R., Dartigues, J. F., Kragh-Sorensen, P., Baldereschi, M., Brayne, C., Lobo, A., Martinez-Lage, J. M., Stijnen, T., & Hofman, A. (2000). Education and the risk for Alzheimer’s disease: Sex makes a difference. EURODEM pooled analyses. EURODEM Incidence Research Group. American Journal of Epidemiology, 151, 10641071. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a010149 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, N. W., Medland, S. E., Verweij, K. J., Lee, S. H., Nyholt, D. R., Madden, P. A., Heath, A. C., Montgomery, G. W., Wright, M. J., & Martin, N. G. (2011). Educational attainment: A genome wide association study in 9538 Australians. PloS One, 6(6), e20128. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020128 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArdle, J. J., & Prescott, C. A. (2005). Mixed-effects variance components models for biometric family analyses. Behavior Genetics, 35, 631652. doi: 10.1007/s10519-005-2868-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArdle, J. J., & Prescott, C. A. (2010). Contemporary modeling of gene-by-environment effects in randomized multivariate longitudinal studies. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 606621. doi: 10.1177/1745691610383510 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGue, M., Osler, M., & Christensen, K. (2010). Causal inference and observational research: The utility of twins. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 546556. doi: 10.1177/1745691610383511 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mirowsky, J. (2017). Education, social status, and health. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montez, J. K., & Hayward, M. D. (2014). cumulative childhood adversity, educational attainment, and active life expectancy among US Adults. Demography, 51, 413435. doi: 10.1007/s13524-013-0261-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2008). Mplus (Version 5.1).Google Scholar
Neale, M. C., Roysamb, E., & Jacobson, K. (2006). Multivariate genetic analysis of sex limitation and G x E interaction. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 481489. doi: 10.1375/183242706778024937 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ngandu, T., von Strauss, E., Helkala, E. L., Winblad, B., Nissinen, A., Tuomilehto, J., Soininen, H., & Kivipelto, M. (2007). Education and dementia: What lies behind the association? Neurology, 69, 14421450. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000277456.29440.16 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, F., & Roos, J. M. (2015). Genetics of educational attainment and the persistence of privilege at the turn of the 21st century. Social Forces, 94, 535561. doi: 10.1093/sf/sov080 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Okbay, A., Beauchamp, J. P., Fontana, M. A., Lee, J. J., Pers, T. H., Rietveld, C. A., Turley, P., Chen, G. B., Emilsson, V., Meddens, S. F., Oskarsson, S., Pickrell, J. K., Thom, K., Timshe, l P., de Vlaming, R., Abdellaoui, A., Ahluwalia, T. S., Bacelis, J., Baumbach,. C., ¼ Meddens, S. F. W. (2016). Genome-wide association study identifies 74 loci associated with educational attainment. Nature, 533, 539542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olshansky, S. J., Antonucci, T., Berkman, L., Binstock, R. H., Boersch-Supan, A., Cacioppo, J. T., Carnes, B. A., Carstensen, L..L, Fried, L. P., Goldman, D. P., Jackson, J., Kohl, i M., Rother, J., Zheng, Y., & Goldman, D. P. (2012). Differences in life expectancy due to race and educational differences are widening, and many may not catch up. Health Affairs, 31, 18031813. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2011.0746 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Opdebeeck, C., Martyr, A., & Clare, L. (2016). Cognitive reserve and cognitive function in healthy older people: A meta-analysis. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 23, 4060. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posthuma, D., Beem, A. L., de Geus, E. J., van Baal, G. C., von Hjelmborg, J. B., Iachine, I., & Boomsma, D. I. (2003). Theory and practice in quantitative genetics. Twin Research, 6, 361376. doi: 10.1375/136905203770326367 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prescott, C. A., Achorn, D. L., Kaiser, A., Mitchell, L., McArdle, J. J., & Lapham, S. J. (2013). The Project TALENT Twin and Sibling Study. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 437448. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.71 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prescott, C. A., Arpawong, T. E., & McArdle, J. J. (2015). Genetic studies of behavior: Methodology. In In Wright, J. D. (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Elsevier.Google Scholar
Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (1995). Twin study design. Alcohol Research, 19, 200205.Google ScholarPubMed
Prescott, C. A., Walters, E. E., Arpawong, T. E., Zavala, C., Gruenewald, T. L., & Gatz, M. (2019). The Project Talent Twin and Sibling Study: Zygosity and new data collection. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 22, 769778. doi: 10.1017/thg.2019.117 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purcell, S. (2002). Variance components models for gene-environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Research, 5, 554571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reiss, D., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2000). The interplay of genetic influences and social processes in developmental theory: Specific mechanisms are coming into view. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 357374. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400003060 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritchie, S. J., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2018). How much does education improve intelligence? A meta-analysis. Psychological Science, 29, 13581369. doi: 10.1177/0956797618774253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, D. C., Jacobson, K. C., & Van den Oord, E. J. (1999). Genetic and environmental influences on vocabulary IQ: Parental education level as moderator. Child Development, 70, 11511162. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00084 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
SAS Institute. (2018). SAS 9.4 language reference: Concepts. https://documentation.sas.com/api/docsets/lrcon/9.4/content/lrcon.pdf Google Scholar
Scarr-Salapatek, S. (1971). Race, social class, and IQ. Science, 174, 12851295. doi: 10.1126/science.174.4016.1285 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharp, E. S., & Gatz, M. (2011). The relationship between education and dementia: An updated systematic review. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 25, 289304. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e318211c83c CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silventoinen, K., Jelenkovic, A., Sund, R., Latvala, A., Honda, C., Inui, F., Tomizawa, R., Watanabe, M., Sakai, N., Rebato, E., Busjahn, A., Tyler, J., Hopper, J..L, Ordoñana, J. R., Sánchez-Romera, J. F., Colodro-Conde, L., Calais-Ferreira, L., Oliveira, V. C., Ferreira, P. H., ¼ Rebato, E. (2020). Genetic and environmental variation in educational attainment: An individual-based analysis of 28 twin cohorts. Scientific Reports, 10, 111. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-69526-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silventoinen, K., Krueger, R. F., Bouchard, T. J., Kaprio, J., & McGue, M. (2004). Heritability of body height and educational attainment in an international context: Comparison of adult twins in Minnesota and Finland. American Journal of Human Biology, 16, 544555. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.20060 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tucker-Drob, E. M., & Bates, T. C. (2016). Large cross-national differences in gene x socioeconomic status interaction on intelligence. Psychological Science, 27, 138149. doi: 10.1177/0956797615612727 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turkheimer, E. (2004). Spinach and ice cream: Why social science is so difficult. In DiLalla, L. F. (Ed.), Behavior genetics principles: Perspectives in development, personality, and psychopathology (pp. 161189). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10684-011 Google Scholar
Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D’Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science, 14, 623628. doi: 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turkheimer, E., & Horn, E. E. (2014). Interactions between socioeconomic status and components of variation in cognitive ability. In Finkel, D. & Reynolds, C. A. (Eds.), Behavior genetics of cognition across the lifespan (pp. 4168). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wise, L., McLaughlin, D., & Steel, L. (1979). The Project TALENT data handbook (rev.). American Institutes for Research.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive information by sex on 3552 twins and siblings from 1778 families

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Total variance and components of variance in years of education attained estimated from twin pair relationships. Variance components (y-axis) are divided into sources, from household economic status (HES), shared environment, genetic factors, and unique environment. Columns depict variance sources estimated separately for women (left) and men (right) using an expanded sex-moderation model (sex-specific variance estimates are provided in Supplementary Material, Table S6).

Figure 2

Table 2. Genetic and environmental sources of variation in educational attainment by family rearing Household Economic Status (HES) level, separately for females and males

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Total variance and magnitude for sources of variance (y-axis) underlying years of educational attainment estimated separately for (a) women and (b) men, by household economic status (HES) strata (x-axis). Columns depict variance sources — from HES, shared environment, genetic factors, and unique environment — estimated separately for each HES strata (model 2 in Supplementary Material, Table S7). Estimates show that HES moderation of genetic variance was present only for women.

Supplementary material: PDF

Arpawong et al. supplementary material

Arpawong et al. supplementary material 1

Download Arpawong et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 797.3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Arpawong et al. supplementary material

Arpawong et al. supplementary material 2

Download Arpawong et al. supplementary material(File)
File 6.8 MB