Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T01:40:27.495Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Brain–body dysconnectivity: deficient autonomic regulation of cortical function in first-episode schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2025

Kaia Sargent*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Emily Martinez
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Alexandra Reed
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Anika Guha
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Morgan Bartholomew
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Caroline Diehl
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Christine Chang
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Sarah Salama
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Kenneth Subotnik
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Joseph Ventura
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Keith Nuechterlein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Gregory Miller
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Cindy Yee
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Kaia Sargent; Email: kaiasargent@ucla.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

An accumulating body of evidence indicates that peripheral physiological rhythms help regulate and organize large-scale brain activity. Given that schizophrenia (SZ) is characterized by marked abnormalities in oscillatory cortical activity as well as changes in autonomic function, the present study aimed to identify mechanisms by which central and autonomic nervous system deficits may be related. We evaluated phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) as a physiological mechanism through which autonomic nervous system (ANS) and central nervous system (CNS) activity are integrated and that may be disrupted in SZ.

Methods

PAC was measured between high-frequency heart rate variability (HF-HRV) as an index of parasympathetic activity and electroencephalography (EEG) oscillations in 36 individuals with first-episode SZ and 38 healthy comparison participants at rest.

Results

HRV-EEG coupling was lower in SZ in the alpha and theta bands, and HRV-EEG coupling uniquely predicted group membership, whereas HRV and EEG power alone did not. HRV-EEG coupling in the alpha band correlated with measures of sustained attention in SZ. Granger causality analyses indicated a stronger heart-to-brain effect than brain-to-heart effect, consistent across groups.

Conclusions

Lower HRV-EEG coupling provides evidence of deficient autonomic regulation of cortical activity in SZ, suggesting that patterns of dysconnectivity observed in brain networks extend to brain–body interactions. Deficient ANS–CNS integration in SZ may foster a breakdown in the spatiotemporal organization of cortical activity, which may contribute to core cognitive impairments in SZ such as dysregulated attention. These findings encourage pursuit of therapies targeting autonomic function for the treatment of SZ.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Schizophrenia (SZ) is characterized by widespread changes in the organization of brain networks, leading the disorder to be described as a syndrome of dysconnectivity (Friston & Frith, Reference Friston and Frith1995). In a recent study involving healthy individuals, Raut et al. (Reference Raut, Snyder, Mitra, Yellin, Fujii, Malach and Raichle2021) demonstrated that autonomic nervous system (ANS) activity plays a critical role in spatiotemporally organizing large-scale central nervous system (CNS) activity. Given prominent changes in ANS functioning in SZ, we hypothesized that autonomic dysfunction reflected in parasympathetic activity contributes to aberrant cortical activity and organization. The present study examined ANS modulation of CNS activity in SZ by measuring coupling between heart rate fluctuations and electroencephalography (EEG) oscillations. We sought to evaluate ANS–CNS integration to determine whether ‘dysconnectivity’, typically a description of impaired communication within or between brain networks, may also manifest in disrupted brain–body interactions.

Reports of autonomic changes in SZ date to Kraepelin, who described alterations in heart rate, skin conductance, and pupillary function as prominent characteristics of the disorder (Kraepelin, Reference Kraepelin1899; Stogios et al., Reference Stogios, Gdanski, Gerretsen, Chintoh, Graff-Guerrero, Rajji, Remington, Hahn and Agarwal2021). These observed differences imply higher sympathetic activity and lower parasympathetic activity in SZ. More recent evidence indicates that autonomic disruption can lead to severe health complications, as individuals with SZ have a 15–20 year reduced life expectancy that is largely attributable to a threefold increase in risk for cardiovascular disease (Hennekens, Hennekens, Hollar, & Casey, Reference Hennekens, Hennekens, Hollar and Casey2005).

In addition to physical health consequences of autonomic dysfunction, there is speculation that ANS changes precipitate the development of psychotic symptoms. Studies of electrodermal activity indicate that autonomic changes temporally predict fluctuations in clinical symptoms associated with SZ (Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Schell, Rissling, Ventura, Subotnik and Nuechterlein2010; Dawson & Nuechterlein, Reference Dawson and Nuechterlein1984; Dawson & Schell, Reference Dawson and Schell2002; Schell et al., Reference Schell, Dawson, Rissling, Ventura, Subotnik, Gitlin and Nuechterlein2005), suggesting that ANS changes contribute to symptom course. Autonomic dysfunction may also result in psychological changes that eventually lead to psychotic symptoms. For example, it has been suggested that chronically elevated arousal may be misinterpreted as fear signals and misattributed to (nonexistent) threats in the environment, leading to paranoid cognitions (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Das, Harris, Liddell, Brammer and Olivieri2004). Misattribution of visceral signals may also relate to interoceptive abnormalities and a disrupted sense of self (Yao & Thakkar, Reference Yao and Thakkar2022), a core phenomenological feature of schizophrenia. Although psychological mechanisms linking autonomic dysfunction to psychotic symptoms have some empirical support and intuitive appeal, further work is needed to clarify these processes and the physiological mechanisms through which autonomic function relates to clinical symptoms in SZ.

In particular, the parasympathetic branch of the ANS has been linked to functioning across cognitive, affective, and social domains that are notably disrupted in SZ (Bär et al., Reference Bär, Letzsch, Jochum, Wagner, Greiner and Sauer2005; Chung et al., Reference Chung, Yang, Lin, Lin, Chang, Shen, Ouyang, Loh and Chiu2013; Clamor, Lincoln, Thayer, & Koenig, Reference Clamor, Lincoln, Thayer and Koenig2016; Jáuregui et al., Reference Jáuregui, Costanzo, de Achával, Villarreal, Chu, Mora, Vigo, Castro, Leiguarda, Bär and Guinjoan2011). Parasympathetic function can be indexed reliably by measuring cyclical fluctuations in heart rate associated with respiration (0.15–0.4 Hz), known as high-frequency heart rate variability (HF-HRV). HF-HRV is widely theorized to index an individual’s ability to respond flexibly and adaptively to environmental demands (Porges, Reference Porges2007; Thayer & Lane, Reference Thayer and Lane2000) and is consistently found to be lower in SZ (Clamor, Lincoln, Thayer, & Koenig, Reference Clamor, Lincoln, Thayer and Koenig2016). In clinical and non-clinical populations, higher HF-HRV is associated with better psychological function across various domains, including emotion regulation, ability to cope with stress, and overall mental well-being (Beauchaine & Thayer, Reference Beauchaine and Thayer2015). Parasympathetic control is also thought to be highly relevant for social behaviors (Porges, Reference Porges2001), and individuals with SZ have been observed to show decreased HF-HRV during social cognition tasks relative to resting baseline (Jáuregui et al., Reference Jáuregui, Costanzo, de Achával, Villarreal, Chu, Mora, Vigo, Castro, Leiguarda, Bär and Guinjoan2011).

Multiple studies of SZ have also found that low HF-HRV is associated with more severe positive and negative symptoms of psychosis (Bär et al., Reference Bär, Letzsch, Jochum, Wagner, Greiner and Sauer2005; Chung et al., Reference Chung, Yang, Lin, Lin, Chang, Shen, Ouyang, Loh and Chiu2013; Kim, Ann, & Lee, Reference Kim, Ann and Lee2011). Additionally, factor analytic approaches reveal a strong inverse relationship between HF-HRV and a cognitive/disorganized factor (Kim, Ann, & Lee, Reference Kim, Ann and Lee2011). Associations between HF-HRV and cognitive symptoms in SZ are consistent with a large body of evidence obtained in non-clinical populations, supporting a relationship between higher HF-HRV and better cognitive performance, particularly involving executive function (Forte, Favieri, & Casagrande, Reference Forte, Favieri and Casagrande2019).

Neuroimaging studies of healthy individuals show HF-HRV to be associated with brain activity in prefrontal regions involved in attention, working memory, executive function, and social cognition (see Thayer, Hansen, Saus-Rose, & Johnsen, Reference Thayer, Hansen, Saus-Rose and Johnsen2009). SZ is characterized by marked deficits in all of these domains as well as in prefrontal activity and connectivity (Perlstein, Carter, Noll, & Cohen, Reference Perlstein, Carter, Noll and Cohen2001; Smucny, Dienel, Lewis, & Carter, Reference Smucny, Dienel, Lewis and Carter2022). Additionally, evidence suggests that impairments in executive functioning and social cognition are associated with lower HF-HRV in SZ (Hamilton et al., Reference Hamilton, Sun, Green, Kee, Lee, Sergi, Sholty, Mathis, Jetton, Williams, Kern, Horan, Fiske, Subotnik, Ventura, Hellemann, Nuechterlein and Yee2014; Jáuregui et al., Reference Jáuregui, Costanzo, de Achával, Villarreal, Chu, Mora, Vigo, Castro, Leiguarda, Bär and Guinjoan2011; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kwon, Min, Kim, Jin and Lee2019; Mathewson, Jetha, Goldberg, & Schmidt, Reference Mathewson, Jetha, Goldberg and Schmidt2012).

Observing that the same brain structures and networks are involved in autonomic and behavioral regulation, Thayer and Lane (Reference Thayer and Lane2000) proposed the neurovisceral integration model to describe a single integrated system of ANS and CNS feedback loops that accounts for robust relationships between HRV and psychological function. This system, termed the ‘central autonomic network’ (CAN), is comprised of prefrontal, cingulate, and subcortical regions that collectively support autonomic, cognitive, and emotional control. Within this framework, a few studies have investigated connections between ANS and CNS activity in SZ using measures of HRV (Schulz, Bolz, Bär, & Voss, Reference Schulz, Bolz, Bär and Voss2016; Schulz, Haueisen, Bär, & Voss, Reference Schulz, Haueisen, Bär and Voss2019). These investigations relied on various linear (e.g. partial directed coherence) and nonlinear (e.g. transfer entropy) measures to investigate coupling between EEG and heart rate, respiration, and blood pressure time series. They observed altered CNS influence on autonomic function in SZ, though patterns were largely inconsistent even within studies; by some measures, SZ showed stronger EEG influence over HRV than did healthy comparison participants, and by other measures they showed weaker EEG to heart rate effects. These discrepancies may be due to the complex nature of ANS–CNS interactions; as the systems are comprised of multiple embedded feedback loops, some measures of coupling may reflect information transfer within specific circuits and timescales, while other measures reflect different (perhaps reciprocal) processes. Furthermore, the influence of factors such as attention may affect the direction of information transfer, leading to mixed results.

Whereas the above studies largely emphasized the role of top-down CNS control over peripheral physiology, recent work indicates that bottom-up ANS regulation of brain function may also contribute to the robust relationships between HRV and psychological function. Raut et al. (Reference Raut, Snyder, Mitra, Yellin, Fujii, Malach and Raichle2021) demonstrated that autonomic fluctuations serve a fundamental role in organizing large-scale cortical activity. They showed that fMRI signal fluctuations in humans correspond to the phase of autonomic cycles indexed by respiratory volume, HRV, and pupil size. Moreover, these signal fluctuations map onto the structure of functional neural networks, suggesting that waves of sympathetic and parasympathetic activity help spatiotemporally structure brain activity into segregated networks by producing coherent activity in disparate brain regions that varies systematically as a function of autonomic cycle phase. Consistent with these findings, we recently demonstrated that HF-HRV oscillations can modulate EEG oscillations through phase-amplitude coupling (PAC; Sargent et al., Reference Sargent, Martinez, Reed, Guha, Bartholomew, Diehl, Chang, Salama, Popov, Thayer, Miller and Yee2024).

PAC is a fundamental mechanism of organization and communication typically studied in the brain whereby the phase of a slower oscillation (e.g. theta) modulates the amplitude of a faster oscillation (e.g. gamma), fostering the integration of neural assemblies across spatiotemporal scales. Although PAC has primarily been examined among neural oscillations, our findings add to a growing body of evidence that slower peripheral physiological cycles such as respiration and gastric rhythms also modulate the amplitude of EEG oscillations (Herrero et al., Reference Herrero, Khuvis, Yeagle, Cerf and Mehta2018; Richter, Babo-Rebelo, Schwartz, & Tallon-Baudry, Reference Richter, Babo-Rebelo, Schwartz and Tallon-Baudry2017; Zelano et al., Reference Zelano, Jiang, Zhou, Arora, Schuele, Rosenow and Gottfried2016). Unlike other measures of coupling in complex systems, PAC reflects a specific physiological mechanism such that slow oscillations affect faster oscillations by modulating the excitability of neural populations. Evidence that PAC extends to brain–body interactions suggests that autonomic activity plays a critical role in regulating cortical and psychological function.

Given evidence that autonomic cycles help regulate and organize CNS activity, we sought to determine whether dysfunction involving the parasympathetic system contributes to disorganized cortical activity in SZ. SZ is characterized by changes in oscillatory activity and synchrony across multiple EEG frequency bands (Uhlhaas & Singer, Reference Uhlhaas and Singer2010). As a general principle, slower EEG rhythms cover larger areas of cortex and thus bring distant brain regions into communication, whereas high-frequency activity within these regions performs faster local computations and ‘binds’ stimulus features into a unified percept (Tallon-Baudry & Bertrand, Reference Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand1999). Oscillatory abnormalities, particularly in the gamma frequency band, have been shown to correlate strongly with positive symptoms (Spencer et al., Reference Spencer, Nestor, Perlmutter, Niznikiewicz, Klump, Frumin, Shenton and McCarley2004). As coherent oscillatory activity is thought to enable information sharing and perceptual binding (Fries, Reference Fries2015), aberrant oscillatory activity may disrupt communication between neural assemblies and contribute to a wide range of abnormalities in perception and thinking (e.g. hallucinations and delusions) due to a failure to effectively integrate information across brain regions and networks. Furthermore, individuals with SZ show global alterations in fMRI network connectivity (Pettersson-Yeo et al., Reference Pettersson-Yeo, Allen, Benetti, McGuire and Mechelli2011). These changes affect within- and between-network connectivity to such an extent that unifying pathophysiological mechanisms may be necessary to explain such a generalized breakdown in organization.

Building upon evidence that autonomic fluctuations help spatiotemporally segregate and organize functional brain networks in healthy individuals (Raut et al., Reference Raut, Snyder, Mitra, Yellin, Fujii, Malach and Raichle2021), the present study examined HRV-EEG coupling in SZ for evidence that ANS dysfunction contributes to aberrant CNS activity. We suggest that autonomic oscillations indexed by HF-HRV help regulate cortical excitability and coordinate oscillatory communication in the brain and that disrupted HF-HRV oscillations in SZ may lead to ineffective communication between neural assemblies and breakdowns in spatiotemporal organization. Specifically, we hypothesized that PAC between HF-HRV and EEG oscillations is lower in individuals with SZ than in healthy comparison (HC) participants and that Granger causality analyses would reveal decreased directional effects from cardiac to cortical activity. We further hypothesized that deficits in HRV-EEG coupling would be associated with symptom severity and cognitive dysfunction, particularly measures of executive function and social cognition, given known relationships between HRV and these domains (Jáuregui et al., Reference Jáuregui, Costanzo, de Achával, Villarreal, Chu, Mora, Vigo, Castro, Leiguarda, Bär and Guinjoan2011; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kwon, Min, Kim, Jin and Lee2019; Mathewson, Jetha, Goldberg, & Schmidt, Reference Mathewson, Jetha, Goldberg and Schmidt2012). Evidence of deficient autonomic regulation of neural activity in SZ would indicate that patterns of dysconnectivity extend to brain–body interactions and could partially account for global breakdowns in communication within and between brain networks.

Methods and materials

Participants

The study included 36 outpatients in the first-episode phase of schizophrenia (n = 35) or schizoaffective disorder (n = 1) and 38 healthy comparison participants (HC) recruited from the greater Los Angeles community. The HC sample was selected to be demographically comparable overall to the patient sample in age, gender, race/ethnicity, and parental educational level. SZ participants were being treated at the UCLA Aftercare Research Program and were recruited as part of a clinical trial of cognitive training and aerobic exercise (Nuechterlein et al., Reference Nuechterlein, McEwen, Ventura, Subotnik, Turner, Boucher, Casaus, Distler and Hayata2023). All physiological recordings for the present study were obtained after initial clinical stabilization of patients with antipsychotic medications and prior to randomization to a study intervention protocol.

Study inclusion criteria for the patients were: (1) an onset of psychotic illness within the last 2 years; (2) a DSM-5 diagnosis of schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder (depressed type), or schizophreniform disorder; (3) 18 to 45 years of age; and (4) sufficient fluency in English to avoid invalidating research measures. Exclusion criteria for the patients were: (1) a known neurological disorder or significant CNS traumatic injury; (2) moderate or severe alcohol or substance use disorder within the 6 months prior to the first episode or evidence that substance abuse makes the schizophrenia diagnosis ambiguous; and (3) premorbid IQ below 70. The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5, Research Version (SCID-5-RV; First, Williams, Karg, & Spitzer, Reference First, Williams, Karg and Spitzer2015) was used to determine diagnosis for SZ participants and eligibility for HC participants. HC participants were recruited through flyers distributed across Los Angeles area community sites and excluded if they had a history of any SZ spectrum or other psychotic disorders, current or recurrent major depression, bipolar disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, neurological disorders, significant head injury, or moderate to severe alcohol/substance use disorder history. Data from a subset of the HC group (n = 37) were analyzed in Sargent et al. (Reference Sargent, Martinez, Reed, Guha, Bartholomew, Diehl, Chang, Salama, Popov, Thayer, Miller and Yee2024).

Participants provided written informed consent, and the UCLA Institutional Review Board approved all procedures. Participants were asked to refrain from drug or alcohol use within 24 hours of the laboratory visit and to refrain from smoking cigarettes or consuming caffeine at least 1 hour prior to the visit.

Psychophysiological recording

Participants completed a 5-minute eyes-open resting state EEG subsequent to an auditory task (not analyzed in the present study). Participants were instructed to keep their eyes open and maintain their gaze on a white cross at the center of a black screen. EEG data were recorded at 2000 Hz using a Brain Products actiCHamp active electrode system with a 10–5 distribution of 90 scalp electrodes (including mastoids) and 6 EOG sites. Vertical and horizontal electrooculograms (EOGs) were recorded from electrodes placed above and below each pupil and adjacent to the outer canthus of each eye. EOG and EEG sites were referenced online to the left mastoid and re-referenced offline to average mastoids (Miller, Lutzenberger, & Elbert, Reference Miller, Lutzenberger and Elbert1991). Electrode impedances were kept below 25 kΩ per vendor guidance. EKG was recorded concurrently at 2000 Hz with electrodes placed symmetrically on the right and left lower ribs and a ground electrode located approximately two inches below and medial to the electrode on the side corresponding to the participant’s non-dominant hand.

Symptom evaluation and cognitive testing

For all SZ participants, symptom severity was evaluated by trained clinicians using the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS; Andreasen, Reference Andreasen1984) and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS; Andreasen, Reference Andreasen1989). Functional outcome was assessed using the Role Functioning Scale, which contains four subscales measuring work/school productivity, independent living/self-care, relationships with family, and relationships with friends (Goodman, Sewell, Cooley, & Leavitt, Reference Goodman, Sewell, Cooley and Leavitt1993). A summary score was computed as the average of the four scores. SZ and HC participants completed the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB; Nuechterlein et al., Reference Nuechterlein, Green, Kern, Baade, Barch, Cohen, Essock, Fenton, Frese, Gold, Goldberg, Heaton, Keefe, Kraemer, Mesholam-Gately, Seidman, Stover, Weinberger, Young, Zalcman and Marder2008). The MCCB was designed specifically to assess cognitive domains that are impaired in SZ and includes measures of processing speed, attention/vigilance, working memory, verbal learning, visual learning, reasoning and problem solving, and social cognition. Given evidence of particularly strong relationships between HRV and social and executive functioning, analyses focused on the attention/vigilance, working memory, and social cognition domains of the MCCB.

EKG processing

EKG data were initially processed using QRSTool (Allen, Chambers, & Towers, Reference Allen, Chambers and Towers2007). Automated beat detection was used to identify each R-wave, and trained research assistants visually inspected and corrected each timeseries for missed beats. The corrected interbeat interval (IBI) timeseries was transformed from cardiac time to real time to provide temporal alignment between the IBI and EEG timeseries. This was performed by calculating a vector of IBI values, one per millisecond, with each entry being the duration of the IBI during which that time point occurred. Each IBI timeseries was then subjected to autoregressive modeling using Kubios software (Niskanen, Tarvainen, Ranta-Aho, & Karjalainen, Reference Niskanen, Tarvainen, Ranta-Aho and Karjalainen2004) to identify the peak in the power spectrum in the HF-HRV range (0.15–0.4 Hz). Each participant’s HF-HRV peak-power frequency was used to extract sinusoidal variation in the participant’s heart rate (HF-HRV) at that frequency.

EEG processing

Trained research assistants visually inspected EEG data for myogenic artifact and other noise. Artifactual segments and noisy channels were removed from the data, and a 1 Hz high-pass filter was applied, after which independent component analysis (ICA) was conducted using the Extended Infomax algorithm implemented in EEGLAB (DeLorme & Makeig, Reference Delorme and Makeig2004) with the principal component decomposition limited to the first 30 components. All components were visually inspected by the first author. Artifactual components corresponding to eye blink, eye movement, and cardiac activity were identified and removed. The resulting ICA weights excluding artifactual components were then applied to the original dataset (without artifactual segments removed and without the 1 Hz high-pass filter) to preserve the temporal structure of the data. The data were reconstructed and down-sampled to 1000 Hz.

Phase-amplitude coupling

Each participant’s real-time IBI timeseries was filtered around their HF-HRV peak frequency (±0.05 Hz), foregrounding the HF-HRV oscillation. A finite impulse response (FIR) filter was designed (Delorme & Makeig, Reference Delorme and Makeig2004) with its order set to capture 3 cycles of the lower bound frequency for each participant. The filter was applied with a Hamming window point-by-point over the length of the timeseries. The filtered timeseries was then Hilbert-transformed to compute a timeseries of phase angles for the HF-HRV oscillation. The EEG timeseries was averaged over select frontal channels (Fz, FCz, F1, F2, and AFz) into a single timeseries, which was then filtered for each frequency band (delta: 1–4 Hz; theta: 4–8 Hz; alpha: 8–12 Hz; beta: 12–30 Hz; gamma: 30–50 Hz). A FIR filter with a Hamming window was applied for each frequency band, with the filter order set to capture 3 cycles of the lower bound frequency for each band. Filtered timeseries were then Hilbert-transformed to obtain the amplitude envelope for each EEG frequency band.

For each participant and each EEG band, the HF-HRV phase timeseries and the EEG amplitude timeseries were used to compute a modulation index (MI) based on methods developed by Tort, Komorowski, Eichenbaum, and Kopell (Reference Tort, Komorowski, Eichenbaum and Kopell2010). HF-HRV phase angles were sorted into 18 bins spanning -pi to pi, and the average EEG amplitude in each frequency band was calculated for each HF-HRV phase bin. If there is no PAC between frequencies, then there will be a uniform distribution of average EEG amplitude across HF-HRV phase bins. Deviations from a uniform distribution were quantified with the MI, with higher values (ranging between 0 and 1) indicating systematic variance in EEG amplitude over HF-HRV phase bins and, therefore, reflecting PAC. A Group × EEG band MANOVA evaluated whether SZ and HC differed in PAC.

Granger Causality analyses were performed to assess the direction of PAC relationships using methods similar to those described by Munia and Aviyente (Reference Munia and Aviyente2021). Granger Causality assesses the extent to which a timeseries X provides information that is useful in predicting future values of timeseries Y. Variable X is said to Granger-cause variable Y if past values of X and Y together provide better predictions of future Y values than do past values of Y alone. The same HF-HRV phase and EEG amplitude timeseries for each frequency band were used for Granger analyses in each direction (i.e. with HF-HRV phase and EEG amplitude as X and Y respectively, and then a second time with X and Y reversed). This resulted in a heart-to-brain F-statistic and a brain-to-heart F-statistic for each participant and each band. To assess whether SZ and HC groups differed in the directionality of effects, Granger F-statistics were compared with an ANOVA for each EEG frequency band with group as the between-subjects factor and direction (heart-to-brain vs. brain-to-heart) as the within-subjects factor.

Data from one SZ participant was excluded from analyses due to the use of beta blockers. All analyses were repeated controlling statistically for antipsychotic (AP) medications (chlorpromazine equivalents), which did not alter the results.

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics of study participants are presented in Table 1. The HC sample was well matched to the SZ group and differed only in years of education and MCCB scores.

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics

Note: MCCB, MATRICS consensus cognitive battery; RFS, role functioning Scale; SANS, scales for the assessment of negative symptoms; SAPS, scale for the assessment of positive symptoms.

* p < .05;

** p < 0.1.

Phase-amplitude coupling

Figure 1 shows HF-HRV phase modulation of the EEG alpha rhythm in a representative HC participant (a) and SZ participant (b), who were selected as their MI values were close to the group average for HC and SZ, respectively. Sinusoidal fluctuations in alpha amplitude are visible in the HC plot and, though still apparent, are somewhat less orderly in the SZ plot. Figure 2 shows the average MI for each EEG frequency band in SZ and HC groups, with theta MI and alpha MI significantly lower in SZ.

Figure 1. Average alpha amplitude in each HRV phase bin for (a) a representative HC participant (modulation index = 0.76e-3) and (b) SZ participant (modulation index = 0.50e-3).

Figure 2. Average modulation index (MI) for each EEG frequency band for SZ and HC. Error bars reflect standard deviation. Theta MI and alpha MI were significantly lower in SZ as indicated by asterisks.

A Group × EEG band MANOVA, evaluating whether SZ and HC differed in PAC, detected an effect of band (F(4,69) = 21.87, p = <.001, partial η 2 = .56) with no main effect of group (F(1,72) < .01, p = .99, partial η 2 < .001). Following up a Group × Band interaction (F(4, 69) = 2.62, p = .04, partial η 2 = .13), simple effects tests revealed group effects for theta (F = 4.95, p = .03) and alpha (F(1,72) = 7.08, p = .01) but not for other frequency bands (delta: F = .23, p = .64; beta: F = 1.46, p = .23; gamma: F = .56, p = .46). In both theta and alpha bands, MI was higher in HC than SZ.

Prediction of group membership

To determine whether HRV-EEG coupling contributed variance in diagnosis (HC vs. SZ) beyond that contributed by HF-HRV and EEG power, a binary logistic regression was performed predicting group membership from HF-HRV power, EEG power, and MI for each band that showed a group difference in HRV-EEG coupling (theta and alpha). Models were run separately for each band due to high collinearity between theta power and alpha power and between theta MI and alpha MI. The full models predicted considerable variance in diagnostic group (theta: chi-square (3) = 8.67, p = .03, Nagelkerke R2 = .15; alpha: chi-square (3) = 10.49, p = .01, Nagelkerke R2 = .18). For theta, MI contributed unique variance (∆R2 = .09, p = .04), whereas theta power and HF-HRV power did not (theta power: ∆R2 = .02, p = .29; HF-HRV: ∆R2 = .02, p = .25). For alpha, MI contributed unique variance (∆R2 = .11, p = .02), whereas alpha power and HF-HRV power did not (alpha power: ∆R2 = .02, p = .26; HF-HRV: ∆R2 = .04, p = .13).

Relationships with clinical and cognitive functioning

Table 2 shows correlations between alpha and theta MI and scores on clinical measures, cognitive tests, and the Role Functioning Scale for SZ. Although MI was not associated with any of the clinical or role functioning measures, alpha MI and sustained attention were positively correlated. (Bonferroni correction would be overly conservative, as the four cognitive functioning correlations were not independent but would still provide a critical alpha (.05/4 = .0125) above the alpha/attention correlation p-value). When including CPZ equivalents as a covariate, the correlation between alpha MI and sustained attention remained significant (r = .39, p = .02) and no other correlations between symptom severity or cognitive functions changed substantially.

Table 2. Correlations between theta and alpha modulation index and cognitive, symptom, and functioning scores

Note: MCCB, MATRICS consensus cognitive Battery; RFS, role functioning scale; SANS, scales for the assessment of negative symptoms; SAPS, scale for the assessment of positive symptoms.

Granger causality

To assess directional relationships between HF-HRV and EEG oscillations and to determine whether SZ and HC groups differed in those relationships, Granger causality analyses were performed for each EEG frequency band using the same phase and amplitude timeseries that were used to compute MI. Table 3 presents the Group × Direction ANOVAs, which showed a stronger heart-to-brain effect than brain-to-heart effect in every band (as evidenced by estimated marginal means, not shown). There was no effect of Group and no Group × Direction interaction in any frequency band, indicating that both groups showed a predominant heart-to-brain effect.

Table 3. Results of ANOVAs evaluating effects of group (HC vs. SZ), direction (heart-to brain vs. brain-to-heart), and Group × Direction interactions on Gr

Discussion

ANS and CNS dysfunctions have been reported extensively – though largely separately – in SZ. Present findings indicate that disrupted integration of ANS and CNS function may be an important physiological feature of SZ. Specifically, individuals with SZ showed less coupling between cardiac rhythms, as measured by HF-HRV, and neural oscillations in theta and alpha bands than HC participants. Furthermore, lower coupling in the alpha band correlated with the extent of deficit in attention/vigilance. In binary regression models, only lower HRV-EEG coupling, not HRV or EEG power, contributed unique variance as a predictor of group status. Although groups differed in the magnitude of coupling, the directions of effects were consistent, with both HC and SZ showing a predominant heart-to-brain effect.

Accordingly, dysconnectivity in SZ appears to extend beyond the brain to encompass peripheral physiology and aligns with recent evidence that autonomic cycles play a critical role in regulating and organizing CNS activity (Raut et al., Reference Raut, Snyder, Mitra, Yellin, Fujii, Malach and Raichle2021). We propose that deficient ANS–CNS integration in SZ fosters a breakdown in the spatiotemporal organization of cortical activity, which may contribute to core impairment in sustained attention in SZ.

Mechanisms of ANS–CNS dysconnectivity in SZ

PAC supports the coordination of oscillatory activity within the brain. Slower oscillations (e.g. theta) modulate the excitability of neural assemblies that generate faster local rhythms (Canolty & Knight, Reference Canolty and Knight2010), fostering the integration of neural populations across spatiotemporal scales. Autonomic fluctuations may similarly help regulate cortical excitability and coordinate neural oscillations. Abnormal neural oscillations are widely believed to contribute to a central pathophysiological mechanism in SZ, as deficient oscillatory activity is closely correlated with cognitive dysfunction and other core symptoms of the disorder (Uhlhaas & Singer, Reference Uhlhaas and Singer2010). A failure of autonomic regulation of cortical excitability may contribute to abnormalities in oscillatory activity and synchrony, resulting in compromised organization and communication in the brain that contributes to psychosis and cognitive dysfunction.

In addition to oscillatory abnormalities, SZ is characterized by altered patterns of connectivity in functional networks, which may also be related to autonomic dysregulation. Emerging evidence supports a role for autonomic activity in fostering connectivity within functional networks. Raut et al. (Reference Raut, Snyder, Mitra, Yellin, Fujii, Malach and Raichle2021) demonstrated in nonpatients that traveling waves of autonomic activity produce coherent fMRI activity in disparate brain regions that varies systematically as a function of autonomic oscillatory phase and that patterns of activation map onto the structure of functional neural networks. We reported MI findings in the present HC sample (Sargent et al., Reference Sargent, Martinez, Reed, Guha, Bartholomew, Diehl, Chang, Salama, Popov, Thayer, Miller and Yee2024) that support the proposal by Mather and Thayer (Mather & Thayer, Reference Mather and Thayer2018) that heart rate oscillations induce or modulate oscillatory activity in the brain which, in turn, may enhance functional connectivity in networks that support cognition. Present data indicate less impact of heart rate oscillations on neural oscillatory activity in SZ. Further work is needed to determine whether HRV-EEG coupling tracks and perhaps drives functional connectivity and whether abnormal connectivity in SZ may be casually related to autonomic dysregulation.

Autonomic and attentional regulation

The present finding that HRV-EEG coupling in the alpha band correlates with attention/vigilance is aligned with prominent models of ANS and CNS integration, such as the neurovisceral integration model (Thayer & Lane, Reference Thayer and Lane2000) and polyvagal theory (Porges, Reference Porges1995). Both models emphasize relationships between autonomic function and attention, as regulation in both domains depends on shared anatomical structures and functional networks. Porges has reported extensively on reductions in HRV during sustained attention tasks (Porges, Reference Porges1972, Reference Porges1986; Porges & Raskin, Reference Porges and Raskin1969), with individuals with higher baseline HRV showing more HRV suppression to stimuli and better performance. Effective autonomic regulation may facilitate allocation of resources appropriate to environmental demands and conservation of resources when demands subside, leading to an improved ability to regulate and maintain attention.

Disrupted attention is a prominent feature of SZ that may have diverse consequences for stimulus processing and other cognitive functions (Braff, Reference Braff1993; Nuechterlein & Dawson, Reference Nuechterlein and Dawson1984). Attentional dysfunction has been found to relate to deficient modulation of alpha rhythms in SZ (Kustermann et al., Reference Kustermann, Rockstroh, Kienle, Miller and Popov2016), and although yet to be examined, it is possible that inefficient alpha modulation is related to disrupted autonomic regulation. As attentional deployment is critical for the selection of task-relevant inputs, attentional dysfunction in SZ may lead to disrupted stimulus filtering and other processing abnormalities across perceptual and cognitive domains (Kustermann et al., Reference Kustermann, Rockstroh, Kienle, Miller and Popov2016).

Limitations and future directions

Disruptions associated with the COVID-19 pandemic limited our ability to recruit a greater number of participants, which would have yielded large enough samples to examine within-group heterogeneity, especially among SZ. Given that all patients were receiving psychiatric treatment for an initial episode of illness from the same outpatient clinic, duration of illness was relatively constrained (median duration = 7 months) along with medication status and level of clinical care. Future work would benefit from recruitment of larger samples that would allow for exploration of any effects associated with different antipsychotic medications. Patients who participated in the present study were also relatively low in rated symptom levels, which might explain why HRV-EEG PAC was not related to positive or negative symptoms.

Due to the fact that SZ was the only diagnosis assessed, it remains unclear whether deficient ANS–CNS integration is specific to SZ or whether it may be a transdiagnostic phenomenon. Given that HRV changes are prevalent across the diagnostic spectrum, it is possible that lower HRV-EEG coupling contributes to other psychiatric disorders. As attentional dysfunction is also a common transdiagnostic feature across psychiatric diagnoses, it is possible that ANS–CNS dysconnectivity is more closely related to cognitive dysfunction than it is to specific psychiatric symptoms. Additional research is also needed to examine relationships between HRV-EEG coupling and cognitive, perceptual, and affective processes in diverse clinical and non-clinical populations to further clarify these relationships. Furthermore, as attention may fluctuate over the course of a session, it would be beneficial to assess state-level attentional effects in addition to trait-level effects as they relate to HRV-EEG coupling. Despite the noted limitations, findings from the present study provide a deeper understanding of ANS–CNS integration and its relevance for mental health and cognitive function which, in turn, may support and encourage pursuit of therapies targeting autonomic function in the treatment of SZ and potentially other disorders.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH110544 and R01 MH110544-S1) and by the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship Program (DGE-2034835).

Competing interest

K.S., E.M., A.R., A.G., M.B., C.D., C.C., S.S., G.M., and C.Y. have no conflicts of interest to report. For other research projects, K.S. has received research funding from Alkermes and lecture honoraria from Janssen. J.V. has received funding from Brain Plasticity, Inc., and Janssen Scientific Affairs, LLC, and has served as a consultant to Boehringer-Ingelheim, GmbH, and Brain Plasticity, Inc. For other projects, K.N. has received research grant support from Janssen Scientific Affairs, LLC, and Boehringer Ingelheim. He has served as a consultant to Astellas, Boehringer Ingelheim, Janssen, and Recognify. He is an officer in the nonprofit company, MATRICS Assessment, Inc., but receives no financial compensation.

References

Allen, J. J. B., Chambers, A. S., & Towers, D. N. (2007). The many metrics of cardiac chronotropy: A pragmatic primer and a brief comparison of metrics. Biological Psychology, 74, 243262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.08.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreasen, N. C. (1984). Scale for the assessment of positive symptoms. Psychiatrie & Psychobiologie.Google Scholar
Andreasen, N. C. (1989). The Scale for the assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS): Conceptual and theoretical foundations. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 155(S7), 4952.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bär, K. J., Letzsch, A., Jochum, T., Wagner, G., Greiner, W., & Sauer, H. (2005). Loss of efferent vagal activity in acute schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 39(5), 519527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P., & Thayer, J. F. (2015). Heart rate variability as a transdiagnostic biomarker of psychopathology. International Journal of Psychophysiology: Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, 98(2 Pt 2), 338350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braff, D. L. (1993). Information processing and attention dysfunctions in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 19(2), 233259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canolty, R. T., & Knight, R. T. (2010). The functional role of cross-frequency coupling. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(11), 506515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chung, M.-S., Yang, A. C., Lin, Y.-C., Lin, C.-N., Chang, F.-R., Shen, S.-H., Ouyang, W. C., Loh, W., & Chiu, H.-J. (2013). Association of altered cardiac autonomic function with psychopathology and metabolic profiles in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 210(3), 710715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clamor, A., Lincoln, T. M., Thayer, J. F., & Koenig, J. (2016). Resting vagal activity in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis of heart rate variability as a potential endophenotype. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 208(1), 916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, M. E., & Nuechterlein, K. H. (1984). Psychophysiological dysfunctions in the developmental course of schizophrenic disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 10(2), 204232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, M. E., & Schell, A. M. (2002). What does electrodermal activity tell us about prognosis in the schizophrenia spectrum? Schizophrenia Research, 54(1–2), 8793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., Rissling, A., Ventura, J., Subotnik, K. L., & Nuechterlein, K. H. (2010). Psychophysiological prodromal signs of schizophrenic relapse: A pilot study. Schizophrenia Research, 123(1), 6467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M. B., Williams, J. B. W., Karg, R. S., & Spitzer, R. L. (2015). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5, Research Version. American Psychiatric Association Publishing.Google Scholar
Forte, G., Favieri, F., & Casagrande, M. (2019). Heart rate variability and cognitive function: A systematic review. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, 710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fries, P. (2015). Rhythms for cognition: Communication through coherence. Neuron, 88(1), 220235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. J., & Frith, C. D. (1995). Schizophrenia: A disconnection syndrome? Clinical Neuroscience (New York, N.Y.), 3(2), 8997.Google ScholarPubMed
Goodman, S. H., Sewell, D. R., Cooley, E. L., & Leavitt, N. (1993). Assessing levels of adaptive functioning: The Role Functioning Scale. Community Mental Health Journal, 29(2), 119131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, H. K., Sun, J. C., Green, M. F., Kee, K. S., Lee, J., Sergi, M., Sholty, G. L., Mathis, K. I., Jetton, C., Williams, T. J., Kern, R., Horan, W., Fiske, A., Subotnik, K. L., Ventura, J., Hellemann, G., Nuechterlein, K. H., & Yee, C. M. (2014). Social cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: The moderating role of cardiac vagal tone. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(4), 764770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hennekens, C. H., Hennekens, A. R., Hollar, D., & Casey, D. E. (2005). Schizophrenia and increased risks of cardiovascular disease. American Heart Journal, 150(6), 11151121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrero, J. L., Khuvis, S., Yeagle, E., Cerf, M., & Mehta, A. D. (2018). Breathing above the brain stem: Volitional control and attentional modulation in humans. Journal of Neurophysiology, 119(1), 145159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jáuregui, O. I., Costanzo, E. Y., de Achával, D., Villarreal, M. F., Chu, E., Mora, M. C., Vigo, D. E., Castro, M. N., Leiguarda, R. C., Bär, K. J., & Guinjoan, S. M. (2011). Autonomic nervous system activation during social cognition tasks in patients with schizophrenia and their unaffected relatives. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology: Official Journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology, 24(4), 194203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J.-H., Ann, J.-H., & Lee, J. (2011). Relationship between heart rate variability and the severity of psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 23(4), 161166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, Y., Kwon, A., Min, D., Kim, S., Jin, M. J., & Lee, S.-H. (2019). Neurophysiological and psychological predictors of social functioning in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Investigation, 16(10), 718727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraepelin, E. (1899). Psychiatry: A Textbook for Students and Physicians. Publishing International.Google Scholar
Kustermann, T., Rockstroh, B., Kienle, J., Miller, G. A., & Popov, T. (2016). Deficient attention modulation of lateralized alpha power in schizophrenia. Psychophysiology, 53(6), 776785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M., & Thayer, J. F. (2018). How heart rate variability affects emotion regulation brain networks. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 19, 98104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathewson, K. J., Jetha, M. K., Goldberg, J. O., & Schmidt, L. A. (2012). Autonomic regulation predicts performance on Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) in adults with schizophrenia. Biological Psychology, 91(3), 389399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G. A., Lutzenberger, W., & Elbert, T. (1991). The linked-reference issue in EEG and ERP recording. Journal of Psychophysiology, 5(3), 273276.Google Scholar
Munia, T. T. K., & Aviyente, S. (2021, June 6). Granger causality based directional phase-amplitude coupling measure. ICASSP 2021–2021 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing (ICASSP). Presented at the ICASSP 2021–2021 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing (ICASSP), Toronto, ON, Canada. https://doi.org/10.1109/icassp39728.2021.9414004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niskanen, J.-P., Tarvainen, M. P., Ranta-Aho, P. O., & Karjalainen, P. A. (2004). Software for advanced HRV analysis. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine, 76(1), 7381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nuechterlein, K. H., & Dawson, M. E. (1984). Information processing and attentional functioning in the developmental course of schizophrenic disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 10(2), 160203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nuechterlein, K. H., Green, M. F., Kern, R. S., Baade, L. E., Barch, D. M., Cohen, J. D., Essock, S., Fenton, W. S., Frese, F. J. 3rd, Gold, J. M., Goldberg, T., Heaton, R. K., Keefe, R. S., Kraemer, H., Mesholam-Gately, R., Seidman, L. J., Stover, E., Weinberger, D. R., Young, A. S., Zalcman, S., … Marder, S. R. (2008). The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(2), 203213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nuechterlein, K. H., McEwen, S. C., Ventura, J., Subotnik, K. L., Turner, L. R., Boucher, M., Casaus, L. R., Distler, M. G., & Hayata, J. N. (2023). Aerobic exercise enhances cognitive training effects in first-episode schizophrenia: Randomized clinical trial demonstrates cognitive and functional gains. Psychological Medicine, 53(10), 47514761.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perlstein, W. M., Carter, C. S., Noll, D. C., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Relation of prefrontal cortex dysfunction to working memory and symptoms in schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(7), 11051113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettersson-Yeo, W., Allen, P., Benetti, S., McGuire, P., & Mechelli, A. (2011). Dysconnectivity in schizophrenia: where are we now? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(5), 11101124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porges, S. W. (1972). Heart rate variability and deceleration as indexes of reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 92(1), 103110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porges, S. W. (1986). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Physiological basis, quantitative methods, and clinical implications. In Cardiorespiratory and Cardiosomatic Psychophysiology (pp. 101115). Springer US.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porges, S. W. (1995). Orienting in a defensive world: Mammalian modifications of our evolutionary heritage. A Polyvagal Theory. Psychophysiology, 32(4), 301318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porges, S. W. (2001). The polyvagal theory: phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system. International Journal of Psychophysiology: Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, 42(2), 123146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 116143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porges, S. W., & Raskin, D. C. (1969). Respiratory and heart rate components of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(3), 497503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raut, R. V., Snyder, A. Z., Mitra, A., Yellin, D., Fujii, N., Malach, R., & Raichle, M. E. (2021). Global waves synchronize the brain’s functional systems with fluctuating arousal. Science Advances, 7(30), eabf2709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richter, C. G., Babo-Rebelo, M., Schwartz, D., & Tallon-Baudry, C. (2017). Phase-amplitude coupling at the organism level: The amplitude of spontaneous alpha rhythm fluctuations varies with the phase of the infra-slow gastric basal rhythm. NeuroImage, 146, 951958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sargent, K. S., Martinez, E. L., Reed, A. C., Guha, A., Bartholomew, M. E., Diehl, C. K., Chang, C. S., Salama, S., Popov, T., Thayer, J. F., Miller, G. A., & Yee, C. M. (2024). Oscillatory coupling between neural and cardiac rhythms. Psychological Science, 35, 517528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schell, A. M., Dawson, M. E., Rissling, A., Ventura, J., Subotnik, K. L., Gitlin, M. J., & Nuechterlein, K. H. (2005). Electrodermal predictors of functional outcome and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Psychophysiology, 42(4), 483492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, S., Bolz, M., Bär, K.-J., & Voss, A. (2016). Central- and autonomic nervous system coupling in schizophrenia. Philosophical Transactions. Series A, Mathematical, Physical, and Engineering Sciences, 374(2067), 20150178.Google ScholarPubMed
Schulz, S., Haueisen, J., Bär, K.-J., & Voss, A. (2019). Altered causal coupling pathways within the central-autonomic-network in patients suffering from schizophrenia. Entropy (Basel, Switzerland), 21(8), 733.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smucny, J., Dienel, S. J., Lewis, D. A., & Carter, C. S. (2022). Mechanisms underlying dorsolateral prefrontal cortex contributions to cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 47(1), 292308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spencer, K. M., Nestor, P. G., Perlmutter, R., Niznikiewicz, M. A., Klump, M. C., Frumin, M., Shenton, M. E., & McCarley, R. W. (2004). Neural synchrony indexes disordered perception and cognition in schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(49), 1728817293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stogios, N., Gdanski, A., Gerretsen, P., Chintoh, A. F., Graff-Guerrero, A., Rajji, T. K., Remington, G., Hahn, M. K., & Agarwal, S. M. (2021). Autonomic nervous system dysfunction in schizophrenia: impact on cognitive and metabolic health. Npj Schizophrenia, 7(1), 22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallon-Baudry, C., & Bertrand, O. (1999). Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(4), 151162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thayer, J. F., Hansen, A. L., Saus-Rose, E., & Johnsen, B. H. (2009). Heart rate variability, prefrontal neural function, and cognitive performance: The neurovisceral integration perspective on self-regulation, adaptation, and health. Annals of Behavioral Medicine: A Publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine, 37(2), 141153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thayer, J. F., & Lane, R. D. (2000). A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61(3), 201216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tort, A. B. L., Komorowski, R., Eichenbaum, H., & Kopell, N. (2010). Measuring phase-amplitude coupling between neuronal oscillations of different frequencies. Journal of Neurophysiology, 104(2), 11951210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uhlhaas, P. J., & Singer, W. (2010). Abnormal neural oscillations and synchrony in schizophrenia. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 11(2), 100113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, L. M., Das, P., Harris, A. W., Liddell, B. B., Brammer, M. J., Olivieri, G., … & Gordon, E. (2004). Dysregulation of arousal and amygdala-prefrontal systems in paranoid schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(3), 480489.Google Scholar
Yao, B., & Thakkar, K. (2022). Interoception abnormalities in schizophrenia: A review of preliminary evidence and an integration with Bayesian accounts of psychosis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 132, 757773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelano, C., Jiang, H., Zhou, G., Arora, N., Schuele, S., Rosenow, J., & Gottfried, J. A. (2016). Nasal respiration entrains human limbic oscillations and modulates cognitive function. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 36(49), 1244812467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics

Figure 1

Figure 1. Average alpha amplitude in each HRV phase bin for (a) a representative HC participant (modulation index = 0.76e-3) and (b) SZ participant (modulation index = 0.50e-3).

Figure 2

Figure 2. Average modulation index (MI) for each EEG frequency band for SZ and HC. Error bars reflect standard deviation. Theta MI and alpha MI were significantly lower in SZ as indicated by asterisks.

Figure 3

Table 2. Correlations between theta and alpha modulation index and cognitive, symptom, and functioning scores

Figure 4

Table 3. Results of ANOVAs evaluating effects of group (HC vs. SZ), direction (heart-to brain vs. brain-to-heart), and Group × Direction interactions on Gr