Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T20:22:14.171Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Improving parenting, child attachment, and externalizing behaviors: Meta-analysis of the first 25 randomized controlled trials on the effects of Video-feedback Intervention to promote Positive Parenting and Sensitive Discipline

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 January 2022

Marinus H. van IJzendoorn*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Education and Child Studies, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, Faculty of Brain Sciences, UCL, University of London, London, UK Leiden Consortium on Individual Development, Leiden University and VU Amsterdam, Leiden, The Netherlands
Carlo Schuengel
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Child and Family Studies and Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Qiang Wang
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Capital Normal University, Beijing, China
Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Child and Family Studies and Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Leiden Consortium on Individual Development, Leiden University and VU Amsterdam, Leiden, The Netherlands
*
Corresponding author: Marinus H. van IJzendoorn, email: marinusvanijzendoorn@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Improving parenting, child attachment, and externalizing behaviors: Meta-analysis of the first 25 randomized controlled trials on the effects of Video-feedback Intervention to promote Positive Parenting and Sensitive Discipline (VIPP-SD). VIPP-SD combines support of parental sensitive responsiveness with coaching parents in sensitive limit setting. Here, we present meta-analyses of 25 RCTs conducted with more than 2,000 parents and caregivers. Parents or children had various risks. We examined its effectiveness in promoting parental cognitions and behavior regarding sensitive parenting and limit setting, in promoting secure child–parent attachment, and reducing externalizing child behavior. Web of Science, MEDLINE, PubMed, and recent reviews were searched for relevant trials (until May 10, 2021). Multilevel meta-analysis with META, METAFOR, and DMETAR in R took account of the 3-level structure of the datasets (studies, participants, measures). The meta-analyses showed substantial combined effect sizes for parenting behavior (r = .18) and attitudes (r = .16), and for child attachment security (r = .23), but not for child externalizing behavior (r = .07). In the subset of studies examining effects on both parenting and attachment, the association between effect sizes for parenting and for attachment amounted to r = .48. We consider the way in which VIPP-SD uses video-feedback an active intervention component. Whether VIPP-SD indeed stimulates secure attachment through enhanced positive parenting remains an outstanding question for further experimental study and individual participant data meta-analysis.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Theory-based parenting interventions are the litmus test of causality and of the translational value of theories about parenting. In the areas of social learning theory and of attachment theory only a handful of parenting interventions have been developed and tested in more than a few RCTs. Prime examples in the social learning tradition are the Incredible Years program (Webster-Stratton, Reference Webster-Stratton, Van Ryzin, Kumpfer, Fosco and Greenberg2015), the parent management training oregon model (Fisher & Stoolmiller, Reference Fisher and Stoolmiller2008; Forgatch & Patterson, Reference Forgatch, Patterson, Weisz and Kazdin2010; Patterson et al., Reference Patterson, Forgatch and Degarmo2010), and parent–child interaction therapy (PCIT; Euser et al., Reference Euser, Alink, Stoltenborgh, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van IJzendoorn2015; Eyberg & Robinson, Reference Eyberg and Robinson1982). Originally, PCIT was strongly inspired by a social learning framework (Eyberg & Robinson, Reference Eyberg and Robinson1982), but it began to include components suggested by attachment theory, such as an emphasis on the child−parent attachment relationship (Allen et al., Reference Allen, Timmer and Urquiza2014). In the tradition of attachment theory, the attachment and biobehavioral catch-up program (Dozier & Bernard, Reference Dozier and Bernard2017), infant–parent psychotherapy (Cicchetti, et al., Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Toth2006), the circle of security (Cassidy et al., Reference Cassidy, Woodhouse, Sherman, Stupica and Lejuez2011; Dehghani et al., Reference Dehghani, Malekpour, Abedi and Amiri2014) and the group attachment-based intervention (Steele et al., Reference Steele, Murphy, Bonuck, Meissner and Steele2019) might be mentioned (see Steele & Steele, Reference Steele and Steele2018, for other attachment-based interventions). Developed and tested in more than 30 years on more than 2,000 families in 25 RCTs, the Video-feedback Intervention to promote Positive Parenting and Sensitive Discipline (VIPP-SD; Juffer et al., Reference Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van IJzendoorn2008) has been the product of integrating social learning and attachment theory (Bosmans et al., Reference Bosmans, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Vervliet, Verhees and Van IJzendoorn2020, Reference Bosmans, Van Vlierberghe, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Kobak, Hermans and Van IJzendoorn2021; Juffer et al., Reference Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2017). VIPP-SD combines support of parental sensitive responsiveness (Ainsworth et al., Reference Ainsworth, Bell, Stayton and Richards1974) with coaching parents to avoid coercive cycles (Patterson, Reference Patterson1982) and promote sensitive limit setting. Here we present a series of meta-analyses of the RCTs conducted with parents and caregivers of a variety of typically and atypically developing children in a broad age range using the suite of video-feedback intervention programs which can be labeled VIPP-SD (Juffer et al., Reference Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2017). The aim is to take stock of the evidence about its effectiveness, and to explore questions that may lend itself for hypothesis driven work through individual participant data meta-analysis (IPD; see Verhage et al., Reference Verhage, Fearon, Schuengel, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Madigan2018, Reference Verhage, Schuengel, Duschinsky, van, Fearon and Madigan2020) and further experimental studies.

Inspired by Ainsworth et al.’s (Reference Ainsworth, Bell, Stayton and Richards1974) concept of sensitive parenting, most variants of VIPP-SD aim at parental sensitivity. In usually four sessions, intervenors focus on (1) learning how to distinguish children’s attachment behaviors from exploratory behaviors, (2) increasing awareness of and attention to subtle child signals by speaking for the child, (3) highlighting sensitive interaction chains consisting of three phases: the child’s signal, the parent’s sensitive response, and the child’s reaction to that response, and (4) sharing emotions and attuning affect (Juffer et al., Reference Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van IJzendoorn2008). To promote gentle but firm control and limit setting, parents are additionally supported in setting limits for and resolving conflicts with their toddler or preschooler. Inspired by Patterson’s (Reference Patterson1982) theory of coercive cycles, VIPP-SD targets the discipline component in parallel to the sensitivity themes as follows: (1) distraction and induction as non-coercive responses to difficult child behavior or conflict-evoking situations, (2) positive reinforcement (praising the child for positive behavior and ignoring negative attention seeking), (3) using a sensitive interaction pause to deescalate conflicts or temper tantrums, and (4) showing empathy for the child while using consistent discipline strategies and clear limit setting (Juffer et al., Reference Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van IJzendoorn2008, Reference Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2017). In two booster sessions, all themes are repeated and integrated. Meta-analytically, the modest number of intervention sessions (“less is more”) and the use of video-feedback were supported by evidence that these intervention modalities were positively associated with effect sizes in a broad range of interventions on parental sensitivity (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn and Juffer2003).

In the original version of VIPP-SD, each video-feedback session starts with videotaping standardized caregiver–child interactions. For example, for addressing parental limit setting, video footage is needed of situations that elicit child challenging behavior and parental disciplinary actions. Mealtime turns out to be useful for this purpose, as well as “don’t touch” and clean-up tasks during play in which the child has to follow unwelcome directions from the parents. This video material is used in the next session, with the feedback being prepared by the intervener in-between the sessions. Sessions last about 1–1.5 hr, and are usually home visits; this facilitates the transfer of trained skills to daily life and underscores the basic VIPP-SD idea of the parent being in charge with a visiting intervener in an empowering role. Three phases can be distinguished during the intervention trajectory. In the first phase (sessions 1 and 2), the intervener builds a working alliance with the parent, and the focus of the video feedback is on child behavior and signals, and on strengths of the parent. In the second phase (sessions 3 and 4), the intervener works actively on improving parenting behaviors by commenting on moments of effective parenting behavior as well as on incidents of ineffective parenting behavior, discussing alternatives for these moments while showing empathy for the parent and watching again video fragments where the parent’s strategy was adequate and effective. In the final phase (sessions 5 and 6) all feedback and information from the previous sessions is repeated. Interveners reinforce positive parent–child interactions and effective parenting strategies, and parents are explicitly acknowledged as the experts on their own child. The intervener supports the parents or caregivers in reflecting on their own interactive images mirrored in the video-records (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, Juffer, Van IJzendoorn and Zeanah2019).

The intervention is thus standardized with respect to structure and themes of the session, but also personalized through ideographic video footage. Because the VIPP-SD is protocoled as well as personalized, it has a degree of plasticity that may allow successful implementation beyond the population and setting for which it was originally developed (see Table 1).

Table 1. Study characteristics and risks of bias in the randomized controlled trials with VIPP-SD

Note. RoB scale = Risk of Bias scale.

Adaptations of the program in some of the studies pertained to the content of the videotaped parent−child interactions, such as a tea ceremony as cultural adaptation with Turkish minority families (Yagmur et al., Reference Yagmur, Mesman, Malda, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Ekmekci2014), or the number of sessions, for example one booster session less with the twin families (Euser et al., Reference Euser, Vrijhof, Bulk, Vermeulen and Ijzendoorn2021), or separate, shorter sessions for collecting video material and providing video-feedback for parents with mild intellectual disabilities (Hodes et al., Reference Hodes, Meppelder, Schuengel and Kef2014). Furthermore, parents of infants in their first year of life may not yet require special attention for issues around limit setting (Juffer et al., Reference Juffer, Hoksbergen, Riksen-Walraven and Kohnstamm1997, Reference Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van IJzendoorn2005; Stein et al., Reference Stein, Woolley, Senior, Hertzmann, Lovel, Lee and Fairburn2006) whereas parents of children with neurodevelopmental problems due to (risk of) autism spectrum disorder (Green et al., Reference Green, Pickles, Pasco, Bedford, Wan and Elsabbagh2017; Poslawsky et al., Reference Poslawsky, Naber, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van Daalen, Van Engeland and Van IJzendoorn2015) may benefit from an explicit focus on stimulating joint attention and reducing stereotypical behaviors along with promotion of sensitive parenting. The attachment video-feedback intervention program (Moss et al., Reference Moss, Tarabulsy, Dubois-Comtois, Cyr, Bernier, St-Laurent, Steele and Steele2018) shows considerable overlap with VIPP-SD but its specific target of families with (high risk of) maltreatment required several adaptations (see also Juffer et al., Reference Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2017). Only slightly adapted versions of the program were implemented to enhance caregiver sensitivity in home-based and center group care (Groeneveld et al., Reference Groeneveld, Vermeer, Van IJzendoorn and Linting2011; Werner et al., Reference Werner, Vermeer, Linting and Van IJzendoorn2018). The individualized approach of VIPP-SD implies that parents or caregivers provide their own baseline (“mirror images”) for the intervention sessions being videotaped with their own child, similar to the strange situation procedure, which creates an individualized baseline of child interactive behaviors in the first episodes (Ainsworth et al., Reference Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall1978). Video-feedback may be an active intervention component that works across socioeconomic and cultural settings, age groups, and typical and atypical groups. The potential generic impact of VIPP-SD across a variety of psychological problems may converge with evidence for the existence of a general psychopathology factor (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Moffitt2014; Neumann et al., Reference Neumann, Muetzel, Lahey, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Jaddoe and Tiemeier2020). One of the aims of the current meta-analytic synthesis is to compare the effectiveness across the various samples, although a conclusive test might have to wait for a potentially more powerful IPD meta-analysis.

The VIPP-SD program aims at enhancing positive parenting, with a special emphasis on sensitive responsiveness of the parent to the child’s distress or support seeking signals, and on sensitive discipline or limit setting to avoid coercive cycles. The primary goal is to increase positive parental interactive behavior, because interactive behavior is the final common pathway through which the intervention may benefit the parent−child relationship and children’s development (Van IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg, Reference Van IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg2019). However, changes in parental cognitions about sensitivity and limit setting are important secondary goals, because cognitions such as parenting self-efficacy are theorized to be part of mechanisms of change in which performance feedback, modeling, and verbal persuasion lead to reciprocal changes in efficacy beliefs and performance of target behavior (Bandura, Reference Bandura1977; Schuengel & Oosterman, Reference Schuengel, Oosterman and Bornstein2019).

In several RCTs parental psychopathology symptoms such as depression or anxiety also have been assessed and tested as outcomes of VIPP-SD but these are not part of the mechanism underlying VIPP-SD. Of course, one may have the hope that a focus on parents’ competence during the intervention, and changes in parenting behavior and interactions with the child will also enhance feelings of self-efficacy and result in changes for the better in symptoms of psychopathology, but these effects would be considered side-effects. The VIPP-SD program supports parents in their role as attachment figures and disciplinarians, but the intervention was not developed to make parents happier or better functioning outside the parent–child relationship. Furthermore, the effects on child development are derived from attachment theory and coercion theory, and they pertain specifically to promoting child attachment security and decreasing child externalizing problem behavior. These child effects are supposed to result from the change in parenting behavior and hypothetically, the child effects should be mediated by enhanced parental sensitivity and sensitive limit setting. Again, in several studies a wider net has been thrown on child development, including assessments of internalizing problem behaviors or academic achievement, but these influences may also be regarded as side-effects.

The current meta-analyses are therefore limited to the main goals of the VIPP-SD: promoting sensitive parenting and limit setting in parental cognitions and behavior, promoting secure child–parent attachment, and reducing externalizing child behavior. Four meta-analyses will be conducted to test four central hypotheses: (1) VIPP-SD enhances parental sensitive interactions and sensitive discipline; (2) VIPP-SD leads to parental cognitions favoring sensitive interactions and sensitive discipline; (3) VIPP-SD promotes child attachment security; and (4) VIPP-SD decreases child externalizing problem behaviors. Additionally, we examine whether studies reporting higher effect sizes for sensitive parenting also report higher effect sizes for attachment security.

Methods

Literature search

The preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses guidelines were used in preparing this meta-analytic synthesis. Search terms for finding pertinent RCTs using VIPP type of parenting intervention were the following: (“video-feedback” OR vipp*) AND intervention AND “randomized control* trial” for all databases in Web of Science: WOS, BCI, CCC, DRCI, DIIDW, KJD, MEDLINE, RSCI, SCIELO, ZOOREC. Date of search was May 10, 2021. PubMed was also searched with the following search terms: ((intervention*) AND randomized control* AND trial) AND (videofeedback OR VIPP*). Date of search was May 12, 2021. Three recent reviews (Bergstrom et al., Reference Bergstrom, Gebreslassie, Hedqvist, Lindberg, Sarkadi and Hjern2020; Juffer et al., Reference Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2017; O’Hara et al., Reference O’Hara, Smith, Barlow, Livingstone and Macdonald2019) and the Handbook of Attachment-based Interventions (Steele & Steele, Reference Steele and Steele2018) were also searched for relevant studies. See Figure 1 for a flowchart.

Figure 1. Flowchart of the study selection process.

Coding system

In keeping with the aims of VIPP-SD only effects on parenting (observed sensitive behavior, observed parental limit setting, and parental cognitions) and on specific dimensions of child development (attachment and externalizing behavior) were included in the meta-analysis. Thus, parental mood or psychological problems, and the child domains of cognitive development (e.g., Dubois-Comtois et al., Reference Dubois-Comtois, Cyr, Tarabulsy, St-Laurent, Bernier and Moss2017), executive functions, or internalizing problems were not taken into account.

The coding system covered the type of measure used for intervention outcomes, whether the measure was observational and the reliability for the measure, age of the child at the start of the intervention and at posttest. Type of sample (typical or atypical/clinical), type of control condition (care as usual vs. phone calls), participating parent (mothers, fathers), socioeconomic status of the families, ethnicity, and country were coded. For quality rating of the studies the coding system included how randomization was implemented, whether fidelity checks were used, level of adherence to treatment, percentage of attrition, whether intention-to-treat analyses were performed, the number of intervention sessions, whether the study was preregistered, and whether the developers of the VIPP-SD were involved in the study. An overall quality rating was computed based on the following six indicators: intention-to-treat; fidelity checks; blind coding; attrition; preregistration; and whether the developers were part of the research team (involvement being coded as risk of bias). Average intercoder reliability of the risk of bias indicators was r = .79. Because of the potentially crucial role of involvement of developers of interventions in trials (Munder et al., Reference Munder, Gerger, Trelle and Barth2011) we also separately tested its association with study effect sizes. As the number of studies was limited, only a few moderators could be examined in the various meta-analyses (ideally not more than one continuous moderator is tested per ten effect sizes, and in case of a categorical moderator categories should contain at least four effect sizes (Schwarzer et al., Reference Schwarzer, Carpenter and Rücker2015). We tested the moderating role of typical versus atypical/clinical samples, type of control condition, age of the child at the start of the study, type of outcome variable (i.e., sensitive parenting vs. parental discipline), established attachment measures such as the SSP and AQS (Waters et al., Reference Waters, Vaughn, Waters, Allen and Bakkum2021) versus dyadic proxies such as the child scales of the EAS (Biringen et al., Reference Biringen, Derscheid, Vliegen, Closson and Easterbrooks2014), and the potential influence of study quality. Extraction of effect sizes was done based on consensus between MJB-K and MHvIJ. See Table 1 for an overview of study characteristics and risks of bias.

Meta-analytic procedures

For each RCT the pertinent effect sizes closest to the raw data were extracted, preferably unadjusted estimates, and transformed to the correlation coefficient r and Fisher’s Z r with its variance. The effect sizes were positive when they provided support for the hypotheses of a positive influence on parental sensitivity or sensitive discipline, a positive effect on attachment security, or a decrease of externalizing behavior. Negative effect sizes reflected effects in the direction opposite to the hypotheses. The implication of extracting all effect sizes for each study was that more than one effect size for the same construct (e.g., parenting) within the same sample was available. Multilevel meta-analysis (Assink & Wibbelink, Reference Assink and Wibbelink2016) took account of the 3-level structure of the datasets (participants, measures, samples). This three-level structure was analyzed with META, METAFOR and DMETAR in R using the random effects model (Harrer et al., Reference Harrer, Cuijpers, Furukawa and Ebert2019). The Knapp-Hartung adjustment for confidence intervals (CIs) was applied to reduce the risk of false positives, and the restricted maximum likelihood (REML) method was used to take account of the between-study heterogeneity (Harrer et al., Reference Harrer, Cuijpers, Furukawa and Ebert2021). Besides the overall pooled effect size we also computed the 95% CI to estimate the precision of the pooled effect size (Borenstein, Reference Borenstein2019).

Publication bias and biases resulting from the (improper) use of too many researcher degrees of freedom such as p-hacking were estimated using funnel plots, Egger’s tests (Egger et al., Reference Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider and Minder1997), the trim-and-fill approach (Duval & Tweedie, Reference Duval and Tweedie2000), and p-curve analysis (Simonsohn et al., Reference Simonsohn, Nelson and Simmons2014). The meta-analytic study builds on a previous one (Juffer et al., Reference Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2017). Since then, the number of trials has more than doubled, enabling the testing of additional moderators. The current update and extension has not been preregistered. To optimize transparency and facilitate replicability, papers included in the meta-analyses, datasets, and R codes of the meta-analyses can be found in the publicly available Supplemental materials stored at the OSF website

Results

Quality of the studies

See Table 1 for an overview of the risks of bias in the 25 studies. Fidelity was checked in the large majority of the studies (84%) as it is part of the guidelines in the VIPP-SD protocol (Juffer et al., Reference Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van IJzendoorn2008). Blind coding of videotaped pre- and posttest assessments was done in all RCTs. More than one third of the studies –mostly older papers − did not use intention-to-treat analyses (40%) although this is highly recommended in CONSORT and other guidelines for the statistical analysis of RCTs. Attrition might have played a role in almost half of the trials which might make generalizability problematic and without intention-to-treat may also jeopardize the internal validity of the studies. Preregistration is important from the perspective of reproducibility as it limits, although not eradicates, Researcher Degrees of Freedom during data-analysis and (selective) reporting (National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, 2019; Stoll et al., Reference Stoll, Mancini, Hubenschmid, Dreimüller, König, Cuijpers and Lieb2020), and 32% of the studies did use preregistration in a trial register. Again, more recent studies used preregistration more often. Note, however, that preregistrations differ in their levels of specification of, for example, the analytic approach, and some preregistrations leave room for (maybe too) many researcher degrees of freedom. Involvement of the VIPP-SD developers was noted in 48% of the studies and was counted as a risk of bias. Overall risk of bias was estimated to be low if fewer than two potential biases were rated high, whereas the risk was considered high if more than three potential biases were rated high. Two studies (8%) were considered to be at high risk of bias, nine studies (36%) were at low risk, and 14 studies (56%) raised some concerns as to risk of bias. In each of the four outcome domains, we tested whether effect size of the studies was associated with risk of bias and with developer involvement separately because of its potentially crucial role in the trial.

VIPP-SD effects on sensitive parenting and discipline

The structure of the dataset consisted of three levels: (1) participants, (2) measures within studies (e.g., sensitivity and sensitive discipline as outcome variables, or sensitivity at posttest and at follow-up assessments), and (3) variation between studies. A multilevel meta-analysis was performed because it takes this structure of potential dependence into account. No outlying effect sizes (deviating more than 3.29 SD from the mean) for parental sensitivity and discipline in k = 24 studies with 63 effect sizes (N = 1,905) were identified. The effect sizes combined within studies are presented in a forest plot, see Figure 2.

Figure 2. Forest plot of effect sizes for parental sensitivity and discipline.

The correlation r and its 95% CI, and the weight of each study have been included. The random model using a REML method and a Knapp−Hartung approach showed a pooled effect size for parental sensitivity and discipline of r = 0.18 (95% CI 0.12, 0.23; p < .0001). Overall heterogeneity amounted to I 2 = 54.7%. The model without the within-study level showed an equivalent fit compared to the full model (LRT = 2.29, p = .13), but the level representing the variation between studies could not be omitted (LRT 12.15, p < .001).

A contour-enhanced funnel plot (Harrer et al., Reference Harrer, Cuijpers, Furukawa and Ebert2021) showed some asymmetry, and the Egger’s linear regression test of funnel plot asymmetry (Egger et al., Reference Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider and Minder1997) confirmed a potential publication bias (t [22] = 3.47, p = .002). Duval and Tweedie’s (Reference Duval and Tweedie2000) trim-and-fill compensation for potential publication bias resulted in 8 added effect sizes to reach symmetry and a corrected combined effect size of r = .11 (95% CI 0.04, 0.18) that remained significant (p = .003). The p-curve analysis (Simonsohn et al., Reference Simonsohn, Nelson and Simmons2014) included 8 significant study effects of which 5 effect sizes were significant at a p < .025 level. The right-skewness test was significant (zHalf = −1.87, p = .031), and the flatness test was not significant (zHalf = 2.73, p = .997). Thus, evidential value was present, and there was no lack of evidence for the absence of p-hacking or selective reporting. Overall risk of bias of the studies was not associated with effectiveness of the intervention on sensitive parenting and discipline, F(1, 22) = 1.30, p = .27.

We tested whether the effect sizes of studies on typical samples versus atypical/clinical samples were significantly different, but the multilevel meta-analysis with this moderator was not significant, F(1, 61) = 0.65, p = .42. Nor did age of the child moderate the effectiveness of the intervention, F(1, 61) = 0.66, p = .42. The contrast between sensitivity and discipline outcomes was not statistically significant either, F(1, 61) = 0.49, p = .49. No statistical differences were found between effect sizes in studies with and without involvement of developers in the trial, F(1, 61) = 2.15, p = 0.15, or with variation in treatment of the control group, F(1, 61) = 3.16, p = 0.08.

VIPP-SD effects on parenting attitudes

The pooled effect size for parental attitudes about sensitive parenting and sensitive discipline was based on 13 effect sizes in 9 studies (N = 961) and was r = 0.16 (95% CI 0.09, 0.23; p < .001), see Figure 3.

Figure 3. Forest plot of effect sizes for parental attitudes about sensitivity and discipline.

The two-level models showed a good fit to the data compared to the three-level model, with either level 2 restricted to zero (LRT = 0.272, p = .60) or level 3 restricted to zero (LRT = 0.0007, p = .98). Overall heterogeneity amounted to I 2 = 23.4%.

Testing for publication biases, we found that the funnel plot showed no asymmetry, and Egger’s test showed no publication bias (t [7] = 1.48, p = .18). The p-curve analysis included four significant effects of which three effect sizes were significant at a p < .025 level. The flatness test was not significant (zHalf = 1.41, p = .92), but the right-skewness test was not significant either (zHalf = 0.72, p = .77). This implies that there was no indication for p-hacking, but according to the right-skewness test the evidential value was insufficient as there were only few studies with very small p-values. More risk of bias was associated with larger effect sizes on parental attitudes, F(1, 7) = 9.72, p = .017. In the multilevel meta-analysis, the effect sizes of studies in typical samples and atypical/clinical samples were not significantly different, F(1, 11) = 0.27, p = 0.61. Involvement of developers in the trial did not make a statistically significant difference for study effect size, F(1, 11) = 0.01, p = 0.91), nor did treatment of the control group, F(1, 11) = 3.51, p = 0.09.

VIPP-SD effects on child attachment

No outlying effect sizes for child attachment of the pertinent k = 11 studies with 16 effect sizes (N = 788) were identified. The effect sizes combined within studies are presented in a forest plot, see Figure 4.

Figure 4. Forest plot of effect sizes for child attachment security.

The overall combined effect size amounted to r = .23 (95% CI 0.11, 0.34; p = .001). The two-level model showed a good fit to the data compared to the three-level model with either level 3 restricted to zero (LRT = 3.13, p = .08) or level 2 restricted to zero (LRT = 0.04, p = .84). Overall heterogeneity amounted to I 2 = 63.1%.

The contour-enhanced funnel plot (Harrer et al., Reference Harrer, Cuijpers, Furukawa and Ebert2021) showed no asymmetry or p-hacking. Egger’s test (Egger et al., Reference Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider and Minder1997) did not show a potential publication bias (t (9) = 0.81, p = .44). Duval and Tweedie’s (Reference Duval and Tweedie2000) trim-and-fill test did not show publication bias either, and no trim-and-fill compensation for potential publication bias was indicated. The p-curve analysis (Simonsohn et al., Reference Simonsohn, Nelson and Simmons2014) included 5 significant study effects in the analysis of which 4 effect sizes were significant at a p < .025 level. The right-skewness test was significant (zHalf = −2.24, p = .013, whereas the flatness test was not significant (zHalf = 3.03, p > .99) Thus, the evidential value was present, and there was no indication for the absence or inadequacy of the evidential value. There was no reason to suspect p-hacking. Risk of bias of the studies was not associated with intervention effect size, F(1, 9) = 1.30, p = .27.

We tested whether the effect sizes of studies on typical samples versus studies on atypical/clinical samples were significantly different, but the multilevel meta-analysis with this moderator did not show a significant F-test: F(1, 14) = 1.47, p = .24. The contrast between studies using the standard attachment measures SSP or AQS versus other dyadic measures was not significant either, F(1, 14) = 0.84, p = .38. However, age was a significant moderator, revealing stronger effects on attachment in studies with older children, F(1, 14) = 7.48, p = .016. No statistical difference was found between study effect sizes for involvement of developers in the trial, F(1, 14) = 3.20, p = .10, or for treatment of the control group, F(1, 14) = 0.49, p = .49.

VIPP-SD effects on externalizing behaviors

In the multilevel meta-analysis the pooled effect size of 13 effect sizes in 9 studies (N = 1,030) was r = .07 (95% CI − 0.04, 0.19; p = .20), see Figure 5.

Figure 5. Forest plot of effect sizes for child externalizing behavior.

The two-level model showed a good fit to the data compared to the three-level model with level 3 restricted to zero (LRT = 1.60, p = .21) or level 2 restricted to zero (LRT = 0.00, p = .99). Overall heterogeneity amounted to I 2 = 63.83%. Because the combined effect size was not significant, we did not test for publication bias or p-hacking.

Risk of bias was not associated with size of the effects of VIPP-SD on child externalizing, F(1, 7) = 0.05, p = .83. We tested whether the effect sizes of studies on typical samples versus studies on atypical/clinical samples were different, but the multilevel meta-analysis with this moderator did not show a significant F-test: F(1, 11) = 0.39, p = .54. Age was a significant moderator of the effect size, F(1, 11) = 9.50, p = .01. The intervention was more effective in reducing externalizing child behavior in studies with younger children. Again, no statistical difference was found between study effect sizes for involvement of developers in the trial, F(1, 11) = 0.44, p = .52), nor for treatment of the control group, F(1, 11) = 0.002, p = .96. It should be noted that the moderator tests were based on small numbers of study outcomes.

Parental sensitivity and child attachment

In eleven studies both parental sensitivity and discipline and attachment were assessed. The association between the eleven effect sizes for parenting (combined within study when more than one parenting assessment was available) and the effect sizes for child attachment amounted to r = .48, p = .14). When the effect sizes for specifically parental sensitivity were selected, the association with effect sizes for attachment was r = .50 (k = 11, p = .11). One study (Kalinauskiene et al., Reference Kalinauskiene, Cekuoliene, Van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Juffer and Kusakovskaja2009) emerged as an outlier in the scatterplot, with a strong positive effect of VIPP-SD on parenting (Fisher’s Z r = 0.39) and a negligible effect on attachment security (Fisher’s Z r = 0.003). In general, however, stronger effect sizes for parenting tended to be accompanied by stronger effect sizes for attachment.

Discussion

The meta-analyses of 25 randomized controlled trials testing interventions with the VIPP-SD method in more than 2,000 families showed substantial combined effect sizes on parenting behavior (r = .18) and attitudes (r = .16), and on child attachment security (r = .23), but not on child externalizing behavior problems (r = .07). The primary aim of the VIPP-SD intervention is the promotion of parental sensitivity and limit setting, and reaching this aim is a robust and replicated result. This effect did not statistically depend on (a-)typical status of the parents or children involved, which supports the potential generic impact of VIPP-SD across a variety of psychological problems. The VIPP-SD also was remarkably effective in enhancing child attachment security, which is assumed to be the consequence of higher levels of parental sensitivity and sensitive discipline. Within the small set of available studies the association between effect sizes for attachment and parenting was substantial (r = .48) but not statistically significant, which makes the hypothesis of VIPP-SD enhanced sensitivity and sensitive discipline mediating the increase in child attachment security a candidate for further study, in particular for a more powerful IPD (see, e.g., Verhage et al., Reference Verhage, Schuengel, Duschinsky, van, Fearon and Madigan2020). It should however be noted that even a RCT may not guarantee causal mediation free from confounder biases (Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Mulder and van IJzendoorn2020).

It is somewhat disappointing that despite the effects on parental sensitivity and sensitive discipline, we did not find a meta-analytic effect on child externalizing behavior problems. One study showed a delayed effect of VIPP-SD on externalizing problems (Van Zeijl et al., Reference Van Zeijl, Mesman, Van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Juffer, Stolk and Alink2006), which suggests that such effects may need more time to be established. Effects on child behavior are more distal to the intervention than effects on parenting behavior. Moreover, some interventions did not include the discipline variant, in particular in studies of families with infants in their first year of life or with children struggling with neurodevelopmental risks. In one study (Van der Asdonk et al., Reference Van der Asdonk, de Haan, Van Berkel, Van IJzendoorn, Rippe, Schuengel and Alink2020) the intervention even seemed to increase externalizing behaviors, but the authors noticed that in this sample of maltreating mothers in residential treatment child behavior problems might have been overreported by the mothers to legitimize their harsh parenting (see also Reid et al., Reference Reid, Kavanagh and Baldwin1987). It is promising that the largest RCT with the lowest risk of bias to date targeting families with children at risk for externalizing behavior problems showed a robust effect size for independently rated externalizing behaviors (r = .15; O’Farrelly et al., Reference O’Farrelly, Watt, Babalis, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Ramchandani2021).

Limitations

Before continuing with a discussion of the implications, we address some of the limitations of the current meta-analytic synthesis. The quality of the studies included in the meta-analyses varied. On an aggregated rating of six quality indicators (intent-to-treat analysis, fidelity, blindness, attrition, preregistration, and participation of developers) three studies showed high risk of bias, whereas nine studies were considered at low risk. Risk of bias was however not associated with study effect sizes. For effects on parental sensitivity and limit setting, publication bias seemed possible, with a significant Egger’s test (Egger et al., Reference Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider and Minder1997) and after the Duval and Tweedie’s (Reference Duval and Tweedie2000) trim-and-fill compensation for potential publication bias the combined effect size shrank but remained significant. Evidential value for the meta-analyses was established with the p-curve approach (Simonsohn et al., Reference Simonsohn, Nelson and Simmons2014), and no evidence for p-hacking could be detected for any of the outcomes. However, the statistical power to find an effect size of r = .16 (or d =0 .32, for parenting) was sufficient in only seven out of the 25 studies. A repeated measures ANOVA with a time by group interaction in a pretest posttest control group design, with alpha = .05 andpower = .80 requires a minimum sample size of N = 80 (G * Power 3.1; https://www.psychologie.hhu.de/arbeitsgruppen/allgemeine-psychologie-und-arbeitspsychologie/gpower).

Other limitations of the meta-analyses pertain to the relatively modest number of studies involved, with accompanying limited possibilities of moderator analyses. Despite inclusion of randomized trials using only interventions of the suite of VIPP-SD parenting programs the heterogeneity of study outcomes was substantial. The focus on core outcome variables related to parenting and child attachment and externalizing behaviors in four separate meta-analyses decreased heterogeneity somewhat but the use of random effects models was still required. The inclusion of randomized controlled trials may have had a mitigating effect on the estimated pooled effect sizes, as a previous meta-analysis showed inflated effect sizes of non-RCTs (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn and Juffer2003). This should be kept in mind when comparing the current pooled effect sizes with the results of a meta-analysis of the Circle of Security interventions (Yaholkoski et al., Reference Yaholkoski, Hurl and Theule2016) that included mostly quasi-experiments without proper control groups or randomization. Since meta-analyzing RCTs enhances the validity of the causal conclusions to be derived, we think there are good reasons to limit the inclusion to RCTs.

An important issue to be mentioned is that two of the developers of VIPP-SD (MJB-K and MHvIJ) had a major contribution to the current meta-analyses and they also coauthored somewhat less than half of the trials. It should be noted that the developers never received a financial compensation for the VIPP-SD program. Nevertheless, it cannot be ruled out that their contribution to the current meta-analyses might have led to (implicit) biases around the many choices meta-analysists must make. Parallel to misuse of researcher degrees of freedom (Simmons et al., Reference Simmons, Nelson and Simonsohn2011), we propose that meta-analyst degrees of freedom also exist. This is the reason why we have advocated the independent replication of meta-analyses (Van IJzendoorn, Reference Van IJzendoorn, van der Veer, Van IJzendoorn and Valsiner1994; Van IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg, Reference Van IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg2021). Here we took two actions against misuse of meta-analyst degrees of freedom. First, independent RCTs (no developers involved) were assigned a higher quality rating, and we also separately tested the influence of involvement of the developers in the trials on the effect sizes. Second, we documented each step of the meta-analyses on OSF in the service of transparency and reproducibility, see https://osf.io/4x2m7/. The absence of formal pre-registration of the meta-analytic synthesis might be partly compensated by the achieved transparency and the publicly available “raw data” in the published papers. We welcome others to examine whether they can reproduce our findings.

How strong are the VIPP-SD effects?

A previous meta-analysis on the effects of video-feedback on sensitivity, attachment security, parental stress, and anxiety (O’Hara et al., Reference O’Hara, Smith, Barlow, Livingstone and Macdonald2019) included 22 randomized and quasi-randomized trials with N = 1,889 families. The mixed nature of the set of included studies, with both RCTs and quasi-experiments without proper randomization, marks one of the differences with the current study. Furthermore, of the 22 studies included in the O’Hara et al. (Reference O’Hara, Smith, Barlow, Livingstone and Macdonald2019) meta-analysis, ten trials were studies using VIPP-SD. The other trials were based on various other theoretical models, for example, Video Interaction Guidance (VIG, e.g., Barlow et al., Reference Barlow, Sembi and Underdown2016), which is rooted in communication theory and not in social learning or attachment theory. In this mix of studies, they found a combined effect size of d =0 .34 (95% CI: 0.20–0.49) for parental sensitivity, which is convergent with our finding of a combined effect size of r = .18 (comparable to d = 0.37). In the four included studies on attachment security, however, the authors found little evidence for an impact of the interventions. In contrast, in our meta-analysis of eleven RCTs with VIPP-SD effects on attachment security we found a combined effect size of r = .23 (comparable to d =0 .47).

Should the combined effect size for parental sensitivity and sensitive discipline be considered small, moderate, or strong? The conventional Cohen’s d criteria for a small (d =0 .20), moderate (d =0 .50), and strong (d =0 .80) effect sizes are often used incorrectly, in particular when the question of translational value is addressed (Cuijpers et al., Reference Cuijpers, Turner, Koole, Dijke and Smit2014; Götz et al., Reference Götz, Gosling and Rentfrow2021; Kraft, Reference Kraft2020). Cohen (Reference Cohen1988) argued that effect sizes should be evaluated in the specific context and domain under investigation. Based on meta-analyses in the field of attachment more valid benchmarks might be the following: small: r = .10, medium: r = .20, and large: r = .30 (Schuengel et al., Reference Schuengel, Verhage and Duschinsky2021), but even these benchmarks may put the bar higher than necessary because they are mostly based on correlational studies instead of intervention experiments with immediate translational potential (Götz et al., Reference Götz, Gosling and Rentfrow2021; Kraft, Reference Kraft2020). Comparison with RCTs in other, related areas might serve as a better yardstick. For example, the overall combined effect size of parent training programs in the USA reported by HOMVEE, is d =0 .10 (Sama-Miller et al., Reference Sama-Miller, Lugo-Gil, Harding, Akers and Coughlin2020). This estimate converges with the recent Michalopoulos et al. (Reference Michalopoulos, Faucetta, Hill, Portilla, Burrell, Lee and Knox2019) report on four popular home-visiting programs stimulating positive parenting that found a combined effect size of d =0 .11. In educational intervention research, Kraft (Reference Kraft2020) argued that even effect sizes d lower than 0.05 should be considered small, 0.05–0.20 medium, and 0.20 or more should be called large. Thus, it is critical to weight the effect size of d =0 .37 found in the current meta-analysis for VIPP-SD effects on parenting against similar interventions in similar domains instead of using abstract yardsticks (see also Funder & Ozer, Reference Funder and Ozer2019). In common language an effect size of d =0 .37 would mean that providing the intervention to five families leads to one family experiencing a substantial improvement in parenting as compared to no intervention, the so-called number needed to treat (computed in dmetar with the Kraemer & Kupfer method, see Harrer et al., Reference Harrer, Cuijpers, Furukawa and Ebert2021, chapter 17.8). It is important to keep in mind that seemingly small effects of interventions that are rolled out in large populations, for example in a universal prevention program for parents visiting a well-baby clinic, would have large impact population-wide.

Future Directions

From a global perspective, large parts of the world are missing in the overview of countries with a VIPP-SD trial (see Table 1). In particular the LMICs or non-WEIRD countries (Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010) are largely absent with the notable exceptions of Turkey and Colombia, but work is in progress in China, Vietnam, and Zambia. The development of an online version of training and of the intervention protocol accelerated by the COVID-19 pandemic (Virtual VIPP; Stevens et al., Reference Stevens, Van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Steele, Struis, Fearon and Tsefos2020), awaiting firm evidence of its effectiveness, might make VIPP-SD programs more readily accessible in Asian and African countries. Second, the use of VIPP-SD to support decision-making about out-of-home placement of children at high risk for maltreatment is being examined (Forslund et al., Reference Forslund, Granqvist, van IJzendoorn, Sagi-Schwartz, Glaser, Steele and Psouni2021). The idea is that a brief, evidence-based intervention can elicit a positive response in a maltreating parent, enhancing mutual confidence in working towards family preservation or restoration (Cyr et al., Reference Cyr, Dubois-Comtois, Paquette, Lopez and Bigras2020; Van der Asdonk et al., Reference Van der Asdonk, de Haan, Van Berkel, Van IJzendoorn, Rippe, Schuengel and Alink2020). Such brief interventions also make it easier to try out alternatives if a parent does not immediately respond to one such intervention. A third development is a focus on fathers. In most studies to date, mothers have participated as the primary caregivers of the children. Although fathers are increasingly considered important parental figures shaping their children’s development in substantial ways (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, Juffer, Van IJzendoorn and Zeanah2019), and attachment relationships with fathers and mothers jointly affect developmental outcomes (Dagan et al., Reference Dagan, Schuengel, Verhage, van IJzendoorn, Sagi-Schwartz, Madigan and Cummings2021; Van IJzendoorn et al., Reference Van IJzendoorn, Sagi and Lambermon1992), fathers have not been targeted in most RCTs. The VIPP-SD protocol suggests involvement of partners of the primary caregivers in the final two booster sessions to make partners aware of the intervention process and content, and to solidify the intervention effects through the understanding and support that mothers might receive from their partner. In a pilot study focusing only on fathers the feasibility was rated high, but the pilot was too small (N = 5) for effectiveness evaluation (Lawrence et al., Reference Lawrence, Davies and Ramchandani2013). In the large Healthy Start, Happy Start trial the involvement of fathers seemed not to add to the effectiveness of the intervention (O’Farrelly et al., Reference O’Farrelly, Watt, Babalis, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Ramchandani2021). Another study showed the feasibility of prenatal video-feedback using ultrasound (VIPP-PRE) with fathers (Alyousefi-van Dijk et al., Reference Alyousefi-van Dijk, De Waal., Van IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg2021). Last, it is somewhat remarkable that RCTs with VIPP-SD have not been conducted in the USA, and this may change in the future. Explanations for the absence of RCTs with VIPP-SD in the current set of studies may be that funding for long-term trials might have been difficult to get as the intervention had to compete with other social learning or attachment-based parenting interventions originating from the USA, and that in the USA group-based programs might be more acceptable.

The combined effect sizes for VIPP-SD do not account for differential susceptibility of parents and children to changes in the environment due to the intervention. In the current meta-analyses, we focused on main effects of the interventions, and we limited moderator analyses to a minimum to preserve statistical power. Moreover, in most published reports of the RCTs with VIPP-SD, moderator analyses with markers of differential susceptibility (temperamental reactivity; stress reactivity; neural connectivity; and dopamine-system related genotypes; Crone et al., Reference Crone, Achterberg, Dobbelaar, Euser and Van IJzendoorn2020; Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van IJzendoorn2011) have not been performed (but see Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn, Pijlman, Mesman and Juffer2008; Barone, Ozturk, & Lionetti, Reference Barone, Ozturk and Lionetti2019) , making a meta-analytic synthesis impossible. Having said that, it is promising that a previous meta-analysis on a large number of RCTs in various domains of development showed impressive differences in intervention effects between more versus less susceptible participants depending on their genotype as a marker of differential susceptibility (Bakermans-Kranenburg & Van IJzendoorn, 2015). In a single RCT the overall effect size of an intervention might be an underestimation of its effectiveness for the a priori designated sub-group of more susceptible parents or children (e.g., Cassidy et al., Reference Cassidy, Woodhouse, Sherman, Stupica and Lejuez2011; Klein Veldermanet al., Reference Klein Velderman, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Juffer and Van IJzendoorn2006a), and this differential effect might be exacerbated in a meta-analytic synthesis of such studies. A coordinated effort across several study sites to conduct RCTs with a similar design and assessing one or more markers of differential susceptibility might solve the problem of low statistical power observed in most studies, not only for finding main effects of the VIPP-SD but also for moderator effects. Moderator effects indicating differential susceptibility require even more power than main intervention effects, but they are crucially important to answer the question “what works for whom?.” An IPD (see e.g., Verhage et al., Reference Verhage, Fearon, Schuengel, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Madigan2018, Reference Verhage, Schuengel, Duschinsky, van, Fearon and Madigan2020) meta-analysis would also enable examining the role of such susceptibility markers with more statistical power than in single RCTs.

In search of the mechanisms of change

We consider the way in which VIPP-SD implements video feedback a specific active component of the intervention. The persuasiveness of video material cannot be overestimated. Parents are given the opportunity to see their own child from a bystander perspective. They need not act in the moment but are given the time to observe their child and reflect on what they see. The intervener is present to help them observe and reflect, to pause the video on a still of a specific facial expression, or to repeat a significant fragment. Video fragments of the child’s behavior, emotional expressions, and body language paired with the provision of “subtitles” as suggested and elicited by the intervener may stimulate parents to take the child’s perspective. This in turn may lead to a more accurate perception of the child’s needs (Juffer et al., Reference Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van IJzendoorn2008, Reference Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2017). Figure 6 shows the specific active components of VIPP-SD, and the hypothetical mediators that these may initiate or support in the parent and that may result in more sensitive interaction and limit setting.

Figure 6. Hypothetical model of active components and mediators of change induced by VIPP-SD.

First, repeating a video fragment provides the opportunity to recognize child signals that went unnoticed in live interaction, because the signals were subtle, or because the parent was focusing on something else. Recognizing child signals is a prerequisite for sensitivity and can be actively trained with video feedback. Second, speaking for the child stimulates the parent to take the child’s perspective and gives the child’s perspective an explicit role in the interaction. Third, highlighting chains of sensitive interaction (i.e., a child signal, parent’s sensitive response, and the child’s reaction to that response) makes the parent aware that feedback is given in the interaction itself. VIPP-SD interveners aim to make themselves superfluous: Enabling the parent to see the child’s feedback is expected to start a positive feedback loop where they need no third person to comment on the interaction. This is one of the reasons why we think that even a short-term intervention may be effective in changing parent−child interactions in the long-term (Van IJzendoorn et al., Reference Van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Juffer, Berlin, Ziv, Amaya-Jackson and Greenberg2005). Fourth, a focus on child efforts to make contact or to comply with difficult tasks or demands on the videotape, as is done in the sensitive discipline part, may stimulate parental empathy, which paves the way for an understanding, sensitive response to a child who tries hard but only partly succeeds. Fifth, watching video fragments of insensitive or inadequate parenting and discussing alternatives for such parenting behaviors is expected to help parents reframe their thoughts and beliefs and recognize their own role in the interaction with their child (Juffer & Steele, Reference Juffer and Steele2014). This may lay the foundation for an increased capacity to reflect on the self and the other, in the sense of Reflective Functioning (Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Steele, Steele, Moran and Higgitt1991). Importantly, such “corrective discussions” are embedded in an atmosphere of recognizing the parent’s competence and expertise of their own child, which is established by, sixth, showing and repeating positive fragments. Thus, even an interaction-focused brief intervention like VIPP-SD assumes multiple processes involved that need to be considered hypotheses for further experimental work. Such experimental studies may help to distill and match the intervention components so as to optimize the mechanism of change for a larger proportion of families. However, increasing access to interventions, enhancing the skills of intervenors, and increasing retention of families in intervention are similarly important for further boosting the program’s effectiveness.

In conclusion, the combination of attachment-based and social learning guidelines in Video-feedback Interventions to promote Positive Parenting and Sensitive Discipline is shown to be effective in enhancing parenting and attachment. The intervention is effective in a broad spectrum of families ranging from poverty-stricken to struggling with parental psychopathology or child neurodevelopmental issues. The COVID-19 pandemic accelerated the development of online training and implementation of a Virtual VIPP version and stringent effectiveness tests in RCTs of Virtual VIPP is outstanding. More work inspired by differential susceptibility theory is badly needed to examine temperamental, genetic, neural, and stress-related moderators and to find out what works best for whom. Because VIPP-SD is a personalized intervention starting with videotaped recordings of the unique interactions between the specific parent and their own child, it may have a role as a universal preventive intervention, as a diagnostic tool (Cyr et al., Reference Cyr, Dubois-Comtois, Paquette, Lopez and Bigras2020) or a generic adjunct to a more specialized parent or child psychiatric treatment.

Author contributions

Marinus H. van IJzendoorn and Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg equally contributed to this work. MHvIJ: conceptualization, project administration, supervision, investigation, data curation, formal analysis, methodology, validation, writing – original draft preparation, visualization, writing – review and editing. MJB-K: conceptualization, project administration, formal analysis, methodology, validation, visualization, writing – review and editing. QW: conceptualization, data curation, writing – review and editing. CS: methodology, writing – review and editing.

Funding statement

The work of MHVIJ is supported by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research, Spinoza Award 2004. MJB-K is supported by the European Research Council (ERC AdG). MHVIJ and MJB-K are additionally supported by the Gravitation award of the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO grant number 024.001.003). CS was supported by an Academic Collaborative Center grant from ZonMW-The Netherlands Organisation for Health Research and Development, grant no 641001103.

Conflicts of interest

Together with Femmie Juffer, MHVIJ and MJB-K are the developers of the VIPP-SD program but they developed and evaluated the intervention on a nonprofit basis.

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. J. (1974). Infant-mother attachment and social development: Socialization as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In Richards, M. P. M. (Ed.), The integration of a child into a social world (99135). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Allen, B., Timmer, S. G., & Urquiza, A. J. (2014). Parent-child interaction therapy as an attachment-based intervention: Theoretical rationale and pilot data with adopted children. Children and Youth Services Review, 47, 334341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alsancak-Akbulut, C., Sahin-Acar, B., & Sumer, N. (2020). Effect of video-feedback intervention on Turkish mothers’ sensitivity and physical intrusiveness: A randomized control trial. Attachment & Human Development, 1, 119. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2020.1753085 Google Scholar
Alyousefi-van Dijk, K., De Waal., N., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2021). Development and feasibility of the prenatal video-feedback intervention to promote positive parenting for expectant fathers. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 114. https://doi.org/10.1080/02646838.2021.1886258 Google ScholarPubMed
Assink, M., & Wibbelink, C. (2016). Fitting three-level meta-analytic models in R: A step-by-step tutorial. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 12(3), 154174. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.12.3.p154 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Juffer, F., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2019). Reflections on the mirror: On video-feedback to promote positive parenting and infant mental health. In Zeanah, C. H. (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health (4th ed., pp. 527542) Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Lotz, A. M., Alyousefi-van Dijk, K., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2019). The birth of a father: Fathering in the first 1,000 days. Child Development Perspectives, 13, 247253. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12347 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Oosterman, M. (2021). Attachment-based interventions to promote secure attachment. In Thompson, R. A., Simpson, J. A., & Berlin, L. J. (Eds.), Attachment. The fundamental questions (pp. 299306) Guilford.Google Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Pijlman, F., Mesman, J., & Juffer, F. (2008). Experimental evidence for differential susceptibility: Dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates intervention effects on toddlers’ externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial. Developmental Psychology, 44(1), 293300. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.293 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more: Meta-Analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 195215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.195 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191215. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.84.2.191 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barlow, J., Sembi, S., & Underdown, A. (2016). Pilot RCT of the use of Video Interaction Guidance with preterm babies. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology. ISSN: 0264-6838 (Print) 1469-672X.Google Scholar
Barone, L., Barone, V., Dellagiulia, A., & Lionetti, F. (2018). Testing an attachment-based parenting intervention-VIPP-FC/A in adoptive families with post-institutionalized children: Do maternal sensitivity and genetic markers count? Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 156. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2018.00156 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barone, L., Carone, N., Salazar, J. G., & Jenny, A. (2020). Enhancing food habits via sensitivity in rural low-SES mothers of children aged 1-3 living in Colombia: A randomized controlled trial using video-feedback intervention. Attachment & Human Development, 1, 122. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2020.1784243 Google Scholar
Barone, L., Ozturk, Y., & Lionetti, F. (2019). The key role of positive parenting and children’s temperament in post-institutionalized children’s socio-emotional adjustment after adoption placement. A RCT study. Social Development, 28(1), 136151. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12329 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baudry, C & Tarabulsy, G.M. (2013, April). A critical examination of the link between the quality of mother-infant interactions and infant cognitive development. Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, WAGoogle Scholar
Bergstrom, M., Gebreslassie, M., Hedqvist, M., Lindberg, L., Sarkadi, A., & Hjern, A. (2020). Narrative review of interventions suitable for well-baby clinics to promote infant attachment security and parents’ sensitivity. Acta Paediatrica, 109, 17451757. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.15212 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biringen, Z., Derscheid, D., Vliegen, N., Closson, L., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (2014). Emotional availability (EA): Theoretical background, empirical research using the EA scales, and clinical applications. Developmental Review, 34(2), 114167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borenstein, M. (2019). Common mistakes in meta-analysis and how to avoid them. Biostat Inc.Google Scholar
Bosmans, G., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Vervliet, B., Verhees, M. W. F. T., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2020). A learning theory of attachment: Unraveling the black box of attachment development. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 113, 287298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.03.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosmans, G., Van Vlierberghe, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Kobak, R., Hermans, D., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2021). Clinical translations of the learning theory of attachment: Attachment-focused intervention strategies for early and middle childhood (Manuscript submitted for publication).Google Scholar
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Goldman-Mellor, S. J., Harrington, H., Israel, S.Moffitt, T. E. (2014). The p factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 119137. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702613497473 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassidy, J., Woodhouse, S. S., Sherman, L. J., Stupica, B., & Lejuez, C. W. (2011). Enhancing infant attachment security: An examination of treatment efficacy and differential susceptibility. Development & Psychopathology, 23(1), 131148. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000696 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (2006). Fostering secure attachment in infants in maltreating families through preventive interventions. Development & Psychopathology, 18, 623649. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579406060329 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Crone, E. A., Achterberg, M., Dobbelaar, S., Euser, S., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2020). Neural and behavioral signatures of social evaluation and adaptation in childhood and adolescence: The Leiden Consortium on Individual Development (L-CID). Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 45(6), 100805. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100805 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuijpers, P., Turner, E. H., Koole, S. L., Dijke, A. V., & Smit, F. (2014). What is the threshold for a clinically relevant effect? The case of major depressive disorders. Depression & Anxiety, 31(5), 374378. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22249 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cyr, C., Dubois-Comtois, K., Paquette, D., Lopez, L., & Bigras, M. (2020). An attachment-based parental capacity assessment to orient decision-making in child protection cases: A randomized control trial. Child Maltreatment, Article first published online: October 28, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077559520967995 Google Scholar
Dagan, O., Schuengel, C., Verhage, M., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Sagi-Schwartz, A., Madigan, S.Cummings, E. M. (2021). Configurations of mother-child and father-child attachment as predictors of internalizing and externalizing symptoms: An individual participant data (IPD) meta-analysis. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/x4td2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Haan, W. D., Van der Asdonk, S., Van Berkel, S. R., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Forder, C. J., Rippe, R. C. A.Alink, L. R. A. (2021). Effectiveness of an attachment-based intervention on parental sensitivity, child attachment, and physiological stress in maltreating families (Manuscript submitted for publication).Google Scholar
Dehghani, A., Malekpour, M., Abedi, A., & Amiri, S. (2014). The efficacy of circle of security on attachment and well-being in preschool children. Management Science Letters, 4(3), 607612. https://doi.org/10.5267/j.msl.2013.12.031 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dozier, M., & Bernard, K. (2017). Attachment and biobehavioral catch-up: Addressing the needs of infants and toddlers exposed to inadequate or problematic caregiving. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 111117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubois-Comtois, K, Cyr, C, Moss, E, St-André, M, & Carignan, M (2011, April). Is attachment-based intervention with foster parents effective in enhancing sensitivity and reducing parental stress? Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Montréal, Canada.Google Scholar
Dubois-Comtois, K., Cyr, C., Tarabulsy, G., St-Laurent, D., Bernier, A., & Moss, E. (2017). Testing the limits: Extending attachment-based intervention effects to infant cognitive outcome and parental stress. Development and Psychopathology, 29(2), 565574. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417000189 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56(2), 455463. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ, 315(7109), 629634. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary-neurodevelopmental theory. Development & Psychopathology, 23(1), 728. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000611 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Euser, S., Alink, L. R. A., Stoltenborgh, M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2015). A gloomy picture: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials revealed disappointing effectiveness of programs aiming at preventing child maltreatment. BMC Public Health, 15, 10681081. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-015-2387-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Euser, S., Vrijhof, C. I., Bulk, B., Vermeulen, R., & Ijzendoorn, M. (2021). Video-feedback promotes sensitive limit-setting in parents of twin preschoolers: A randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychology, 9(1), 46. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-021-00548-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eyberg, S. M., & Robinson, E. A. (1982). Parent-child interaction training: Effect on family functioning. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 11(2), 130137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, P. A., & Stoolmiller, M. (2008). Intervention effects on foster parent stress: Associations with child cortisol levels. Development & Psychopathology, 20(3), 10031021. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000473 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Moran, G. S., & Higgitt, A. C. (1991). The capacity for understanding mental states: The reflective self in parent and child and its significance for security of attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 12, 201218.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forgatch, M. S., & Patterson, G. R. (2010). Parent management training-Oregon model: An intervention for antisocial behavior in children and adolescents. Evidence-Based Psychotherapies for Children and Adolescents, In Weisz, J. R., & Kazdin, A. E. (Eds.), Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and adolescents (pp. 159–177). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Forslund, T., Granqvist, P., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Sagi-Schwartz, A., Glaser, D., Steele, M.Psouni, E. (2021). Attachment goes to court: Child protection and custody issues. Attachment & Human Development. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2020.1840762 Google ScholarPubMed
Funder, D. C., & Ozer, D. J. (2019). Evaluating effect size in psychological research: Sense and nonsense. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 2(2), 156168. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245919847202 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Götz, F. M., Gosling, S. D., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2021). Small effects: The indispensable foundation for a cumulative psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620984483 Google ScholarPubMed
Green, J., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Wan, M. W., Elsabbagh, M., Slonims, V.British Autism Study of Infant Siblings (BASIS) Team (2015). Parent-mediated intervention versus no intervention for infants at high risk of autism: A parallel, single-blind, randomised trial. The Lancet Psychiatry, 2(2), 133140. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00091-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, J., Pickles, A., Pasco, G., Bedford, R., Wan, M. W., Elsabbagh, M.The British Autism Study of Infant Siblings (BASIS) Team (2017). Randomised trial of a parent-mediated intervention for infants at high risk for autism: Longitudinal outcomes to age 3 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 58(12), 13301340. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12728 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groeneveld, M. G., Vermeer, H. J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Linting, M. (2011). Enhancing home-based child care quality through video-feedback intervention: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Family Psychology, 25(1), 8696. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022451 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamaker, E. L., Mulder, J. D., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2020). Description, prediction and causation: Methodological challenges of studying child and adolescent development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 46, Article 100867. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100867 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrer, M., Cuijpers, P., Furukawa, T., & Ebert, D.. DMETAR: Companion R package for the guide doing meta-analysis in R 2019. R Package Version 0.0. 9000.Google Scholar
Harrer, M., Cuijpers, P., Furukawa, T. A., & Ebert, D. D. (2021). Doing meta-analysis with R: A hands-on guide. Chapmann & Hall/CRC Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature, 466, 29. https://doi.org/10.1038/466029a CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodes, M. W., Meppelder, H. M., Schuengel, C., & Kef, S. (2014). Tailoring a video-feedback intervention for sensitive discipline to parents with intellectual disabilities: A process evaluation. Attachment & Human Development, 16, 387401. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2014.912490 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodes, M. W., Meppelder, M., de Moor, M., Kef, S., & Schuengel, C. (2017). Alleviating parenting stress in parents with intellectual disabilities: A randomized controlled trial of a video-feedback intervention to promote positive parenting. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 30(3), 423432. https://doi.org/10.1111/jar.12302 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodes, M. W., Meppelder, M., de Moor, M., Kef, S., & Schuengel, C. (2018). Effects of video-feedback intervention on harmonious parent-child interaction and sensitive discipline of parents with intellectual disabilities: A randomized controlled trial. Child Care Health & Development, 44(4), 304311. https://doi.org/10.1111/cch.12506 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juffer, F. (personal communication September 3, 2018) archived on OSF 2021 by Marinus van IJzendoorn.Google Scholar
Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2017). Pairing attachment theory and social learning theory in video-feedback intervention to promote positive parenting. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 189194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2005). The importance of parenting in the development of disorganized attachment: Evidence from a preventive intervention study in adoptive families. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(3), 263274. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469–7610.2004.00353.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2008). Promoting positive parenting: An attachment-based intervention. Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Juffer, F., Hoksbergen, R. A. C., Riksen-Walraven, J. M. A., & Kohnstamm, G. A. (1997). Early intervention in adoptive families: Supporting maternal sensitive responsiveness, infant-mother attachment, and infant competence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(8), 10391050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juffer, F., & Steele, M. (2014). What words cannot say: The telling story of video in attachment-based interventions. Attachment and Human Development, 16, 307314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalinauskiene, L., Cekuoliene, D., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Juffer, F., & Kusakovskaja, I. (2009). Supporting insensitive mothers: The Vilnius randomized control trial of video-feedback intervention to promote maternal sensitivity and infant attachment security. Child Care Health & Development, 35(5), 613623. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2214.2009.00962.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein Velderman, M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Juffer, F., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2006a). Effects of attachment-based interventions on maternal sensitivity and infant attachment: Differential susceptibility of highly reactive infants. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 266274. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.20.2.266 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein Velderman, M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Juffer, F., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Mangelsdorf, S. C., & Zevalkink, J. (2006b). Preventing preschool externalizing behavior problems through video-feedback intervention in infancy. Infant Mental Health Journal, 27(5), 466493. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20104 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraft, M. A. (2020). Interpreting effect sizes of education interventions. Educational Researcher, 49(4), 241253. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X20912798 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, P. J., Davies, B., & Ramchandani, P. G. (2013). Using video feedback to improve early father-infant interaction: A pilot study. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18(1), 6171. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104512437210 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michalopoulos, C., Faucetta, K., Hill, C. J., Portilla, X. A., Burrell, L., Lee, H.Knox, V. (2019). Impacts on family outcomes of evidence-based early childhood home visiting: Results from the mother and infant home visiting program evaluation. Office of Planning, Research, and Evaluation, Administration for Children and Families, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Washington, DC, OPRE Report 2019-07.Google Scholar
Moss, E., Dubois-Comtois, K., Cyr, C., Tarabulsy, G. M., St-Laurent, D., & Bernier, A. (2011). Efficacy of a home-visiting intervention aimed at improving maternal sensitivity, child attachment, and behavioral outcomes for maltreated children: A randomized control trial. Development and Psychopathology, 23(1), 195210. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000738 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moss, E., Tarabulsy, G. M., Dubois-Comtois, K., Cyr, C., Bernier, A., & St-Laurent, D. (2018). The attachment video-feedback intervention program. Development and validation. In Steele, H., & Steele, M. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment-based interventions. Guilford Press. Google Scholar
Munder, T., Gerger, H., Trelle, S., & Barth, J. (2011). Testing the allegiance bias hypothesis: A meta-analysis. Psychotherapy Research, 21(6), 670684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine 2019). Reproducibility and replicability in science. The National Academies Press, Washington, DC, https://doi.org/10.17226/25303 Google Scholar
Negrão, M., Pereira, M., Soares, I., & Mesman, J. (2014). Enhancing positive parent-child interactions and family functioning in a poverty sample: A randomized control trial. Attachment & Human Development, 16(4), 315328. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2014.912485 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neumann, A., Muetzel, R. L., Lahey, B. B., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Jaddoe, V. W.Tiemeier, H. (2020). White matter microstructure and the general psychopathology factor in children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 59, 1285–1296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.12.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Farrelly, C., Watt, H., Babalis, D., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Ramchandani, P. G. (2021). A brief home-based parenting intervention to reduce behavior problems in young children: A pragmatic randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatrics, 175(5), 567576. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.6834 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Hara, L., Smith, E. R., Barlow, J., Livingstone, N., & Macdonald, G. (2019). Video feedback for parental sensitivity and attachment security in children under five years. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 59, 12851296. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD012348.pub2 Google ScholarPubMed
Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process: A social learning approach. Castalia.Google Scholar
Patterson, G. R., Forgatch, M. S., & Degarmo, D. S. (2010). Cascading effects following intervention. Development & Psychopathology, 22(4), 949970. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000568 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pereira, M., Negro, M., Soares, I., & Mesman, J. (2014). Decreasing harsh discipline in mothers at risk for maltreatment: A randomized control trial. Infant Mental Health Journal, 35(6), 604613. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21464 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Platje, E., Sterkenburg, P., Overbeek, M., Kef, S., & Schuengel, C. (2018). The efficacy of VIPP-V parenting training for parents of young children with a visual or visual-and-intellectual disability: A randomized controlled trial. Attachment & Human Development, 20(5), 455472. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2018.1428997 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poslawsky, I. E., Naber, F. B. A., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van Daalen, E., Van Engeland, H., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2015). Video-feedback intervention to promote positive parenting adapted to autism (VIPP-AUTI): A randomized controlled trial. Autism, 19(5), 588603. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361314537124 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reid, J. B., Kavanagh, K., & Baldwin, D. V. (1987). Abusive parents’ perceptions of child problem behaviors: An example of parental bias. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15(3), 457466. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00916461 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sama-Miller, E., Lugo-Gil, , Harding, J., Akers, L., & Coughlin, R. (2020). Home visiting evidence of effectiveness (HomeVEE) systematic review. OPRE, Washington, DC, OPRE Report 2020-151,Google Scholar
Schoemaker, N. K., Juffer, F., Rippe, R. C. A., Vermeer, H. J., Stoltenborgh, M., Jagersma, G. J.Alink, L. R. A. (2020). Positive parenting in foster care: Testing the effectiveness of a video-feedback intervention program on foster parents’ behavior and attitudes. Children & Youth Services Review, 110, 104779. https://doi.org/10477910.1016/j.childyouth.2020.104779 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuengel, C., & Oosterman, M. (2019). Parenting self-efficacy. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Being and becoming a parent (654680). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429433214-19 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuengel, C., Verhage, M. L., & Duschinsky, R. (2021). Prospecting the attachment research field: A move to the level of engagement. Attachment & Human Development, 23(4), 375395. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2021.1918449 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarzer, G., Carpenter, J. R., & Rücker, G. (2015). Meta-analysis with R. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22(11), 13591366. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417632 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonsohn, U., Nelson, L. D., & Simmons, J. P. (2014). P-curve: A key to the file-drawer. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 143(2), 534547. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033242 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steele, H., Murphy, A., Bonuck, K., Meissner, P., & Steele, M. (2019). Randomized control trial report on the effectiveness of Group Attachment-Based Intervention (GABI©): Improvements in the parent-child relationship not seen in the control group. Development and Psychopathology, 31(1), 203217. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418001621 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steele, H., & Steele, M. Eds (2018). Handbook of attachment-based interventions. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Stein, A., Netsi, E., Lawrence, P. J., Granger, C., Kempton, C., Craske, M. G.Cooper, P. J. (2018). Mitigating the effect of persistent postnatal depression on child outcomes through an intervention to treat depression and improve parenting: A randomised controlled trial. The Lancet Psychiatry, 5(2), 134144. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30006-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, A., Woolley, H., Senior, R., Hertzmann, L., Lovel, M., Lee, J.Fairburn, C. G. (2006). Treating disturbances in the relationship between mothers with bulimic eating disorders and their infants: A randomized, controlled trial of video feedback. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(5), 899906. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.2006.163.5.899 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, E., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Steele, M., Struis, E. Fearon, R. P.… et al. Tsefos, A. (2020). Virtual VIPP protocol. Virtual delivery of video-feedback intervention to promote positive parenting (VIPP). Anna Freud Centre.Google Scholar
Stoll, M., Mancini, A., Hubenschmid, L., Dreimüller, N., König, J., Cuijpers, P.Lieb, K. (2020). Discrepancies from registered protocols and spin occurred frequently in randomized psychotherapy trials: A meta-epidemiologic study. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 128, 4956. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2020.08.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Asdonk, S., de Haan, W. D., Van Berkel, S. R., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Rippe, R. C. A., Schuengel, C.Alink, L. R. A. (2020). Effectiveness of an attachment-based intervention for the assessment of parenting capacities in maltreating families: A randomized controlled trial. Infant Mental Health Journal, 41(6), 821835. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21874 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1994). Process model of replication studies: On the relations between different types of replication. In van der Veer, R., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.), Reconstructing the mind: Replicability in research on human development (pp. 5770) Ablex Norwood.Google Scholar
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2019). Bridges across the intergenerational transmission of attachment gap. Current Opinion in Psychology, 25, 3136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2021). Replication crisis lost in translation? On translational caution and premature applications of attachment theory. Attachment & Human Development, 23(4), 422437. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2021.1918453 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Juffer, F. (2005). Why less is more. From the Dodo Bird Verdict to evidence-based interventions on sensitivity and early attachments. In Berlin, L., Ziv, Y., Amaya-Jackson, L., & Greenberg, M. T. (Eds.), Enhancing early attachment: Theory, research, intervention, and policy. (pp. 297312) Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Sagi, A., & Lambermon, M. W. E. (1992). The multiple caretaker paradox: Data from Holland and Israel. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1992(57), 524. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.23219925703 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Zeijl, J., Mesman, J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Juffer, F., Stolk, M. N.Alink, L. R. A. (2006). Attachment-based intervention for enhancing sensitive discipline in mothers of 1- to 3-year-old children at risk for externalizing behavior problems: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(6), 9941005. https://doi.org/https://doi.org10.1037/0022-006X.74.6.994 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhage, M. L., Fearon, R. M. P., Schuengel, C., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Madigan, S.Collaboration on Attachment Transmission Synthesis (2018). Constraints on the intergenerational transmission of attachment via individual participant data meta-analysis. Child Development, 89, 20232037. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13085 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhage, M. L., Schuengel, C., Duschinsky, R., van, I. M. H., Fearon, R. M. P., Madigan, S.the Collaboration on Attachment Transmission Synthesis (2020). A move to the level of individual-participant-data meta-analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(2), 199206. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721420904967 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waters, E., Vaughn, B. E., Waters, H. S., Allen, J. P., & Bakkum, L. (2021). Measuring attachment developmental assessment across the Lifespan. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Webster-Stratton, C. (2015). The Incredible Years® series: A developmental approach. In Van Ryzin, M. J., Kumpfer, K. L., Fosco, G. M., & Greenberg, M. T. (Eds.), Family-based prevention programs for children and adolescents. (pp. 5479) Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Werner, C. D., Vermeer, H. J., Linting, M., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2018). Video-feedback intervention in center-based child care: A randomized controlled trial. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 42, 93104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.07.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yagmur, S., Mesman, J., Malda, M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Ekmekci, H. (2014). Video-feedback intervention increases sensitive parenting in ethnic minority mothers: A randomized control trial. Attachment & Human Development, 16(4), 371386. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2014.912489 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yaholkoski, A., Hurl, K., & Theule, J. (2016). Efficacy of the circle of security intervention: A meta-analysis. Journal of Infant, Child, and Adolescent Psychotherapy, 15(2), 95103. https://doi.org/10.1080/15289168.2016.1163161 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Study characteristics and risks of bias in the randomized controlled trials with VIPP-SD

Figure 1

Figure 1. Flowchart of the study selection process.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Forest plot of effect sizes for parental sensitivity and discipline.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Forest plot of effect sizes for parental attitudes about sensitivity and discipline.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Forest plot of effect sizes for child attachment security.

Figure 5

Figure 5. Forest plot of effect sizes for child externalizing behavior.

Figure 6

Figure 6. Hypothetical model of active components and mediators of change induced by VIPP-SD.