Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T07:23:37.516Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

DID in resurgence, not retreat

COMMENTARY ON… Dissociative identity disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 August 2019

Pamela Radcliffe
Affiliation:
A senior barrister, lead editor of Witness Testimony in Sexual Cases: Evidential, Investigative and Scientific Perspectives (Oxford University Press, 2016) and Visiting Research Fellow in the Department of Psychology at the University of Portsmouth, UK. Her academic interest lies in the interdisciplinary nature of law and the nexus between law, medicine and psychology, including the challenges posed to public health and the justice system by controversial psychotherapeutic treatments.
Keith Rix*
Affiliation:
An honorary consultant forensic psychiatrist with Norfolk and Suffolk NHS Foundation Trust, and Visiting Professor of Medical Jurisprudence at the University of Chester, UK. The second edition of his Expert Psychiatric Evidence is due for publication shortly by Cambridge University Press.
*
Correspondence Professor Keith J.B. Rix, The Fermoy Unit, Queen Elizabeth Hospital, Gayton Road, King's Lynn PE30 4ET, UK. Email: keith.rix@nsft.nhs.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Contrary to the assertion of Paris, diverse indicators suggest that the diagnosis and treatment of dissociative identity disorder (DID) are resurgent rather than retreating. This commentary reviews the evidence that justifies the description of this condition as controversial, including research into dissociative amnesia. The potential harm that can result from a diagnosis of DID and risky treatment techniques, including hypnosis and abreaction, are described. It is suggested that this scientifically unproven and potentially harmful treatment model should be confronted and quelled and its diagnosis and treatment subjected to critical clinical review, including randomised controlled trials, as a matter of urgency. A plea is made for the Royal College of Psychiatrists to update its 1997 guidance document and for professional training to incorporate updated psychological and neurobiological research on human memory.

DECLARATION OF INTEREST

None.

Type
Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2019 

‘When ideas go unexamined and unchallenged for a long time […] they become mythological, and they become very, very, powerful’ (E. L. Doctorow)

We endorse Paris's timely article on dissociative identity disorder (DID) (Paris Reference Paris2019, this issue) but contend that the disorder is far from moribund. Although a dearth of reliable research data prevents accurate assessment of its current prevalence, diverse indicators suggest that its diagnosis and treatment are resurgent rather than retreating. As Paris notes, the DID diagnosis treatment model is contentious, endorsing therapeutic methods recognised as potentially harmful (Lilienfeld Reference Lilienfeld2007). Also, the efficacy of the treatment model is unproven and not empirically supported. Significantly, prior to treatment, DID patients typically do not initially present with any ‘alters’ or other ‘selves’ and have no knowledge of previous sexual or other trauma. The unique diagnostic hallmark, namely ‘alters’, typically emerges during treatment, juxtaposed with increased mental distress in the form of dissociative episodes and self-harm. We contend that instead of ‘hop[ing] that the construct will eventually wither from disinterest’ (Paris Reference Paris2019), this scientifically unproven and potentially harmful treatment model should be confronted and quelled and its diagnosis and treatment subjected to critical clinical review, including randomised controlled trials, as a matter of urgency.

Re-emergence and growing acceptance of DID

In the 1990s, numerous malpractice actions against US psychiatrists by their former DID patients – ‘retractors’ – resulted in multi-million-dollar settlements. Freed from therapy, retractors' health improved and they rejected their 'memories' of sexual abuse (Pendergrast Reference Pendergrast2017). Yet, despite the negative exposure, accumulated research data and well-founded academic criticism refuting any evidence for a mechanism capable of causing massive memory repression (dissociative amnesia), DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association 2013) has further embedded and legitimised this controversial diagnosis. In the USA, DID is being promoted via conferences, social media, films and books (Pendergrast Reference Pendergrast2017), a trend echoed in the UK, where the Official Solicitor and a borough council unsuccessfully sought an interim injunction to prevent the broadcast of a film about a woman with an intellectual disability and DID (E v Channel Four Television Corp [2005]). Since 2011, DID advocates in the UK have campaigned for better recognition of DID (Bowlby Reference Bowlby and Briggs2014). There are now four private UK clinics for DID patients and two charities raising awareness via conferences and professional training. National Health Service (NHS) trusts are facilitating and funding often expensive DID treatment. In St Helens Borough Council v Manchester Primary Care Trust [2008], the court bemoaned the fact that two publicly funded bodies had become engaged in expensive litigation to decide which should pay £675 000 per annum for the care of a woman with DID who had been having weekly psychotherapy for over 8 years. However, guidance (a) warning the public of the potentially adverse impact of treatment, (b) cautioning therapists of the attendant therapeutic risks or (c) acknowledging the implications and need for informed consent is absent.

A flawed memory mechanism, treatment model and the potential for serious harm

Paris recognises that ‘[o]ne of the central ideas behind DID, the repression and/or dissociation of traumatic memories, has never been accepted by memory researchers’. Memory researchers and clinicians have unsuccessfully searched for evidence in support of this mechanism. Moreover, decades of psychological research contraindicate dissociative amnesia as a mechanism (Brandon Reference Brandon, Boakes and Glaser1998; Piper Reference Piper, Lillevik and Kritzer2008). An empirical study specifically using verified childhood sexual abuse also contradicts it (Alexander Reference Alexander, Quas and Goodman2005). Dissociative amnesia/DID sceptics agree that trauma may cause depersonalisation and memory errors but contend that there is no evidence for dissociative amnesia – even though it is now embedded in the pantheon of diagnoses in DSM-5 (Patihis Reference Patihis, Otgaar and Merckelbach2019). They observe that dissociative amnesia requires a traumatic event to be: (a) accurately encoded and stored, (b) blocked from awareness – owing to the traumatic event – and (c) accurately retrieved in pristine form. No case or study has been found replicating this.

Guidelines from the International Society for the Study of Trauma and Dissociation (2011) encourage therapists actively to search for ‘alters’ and evidence of sexual abuse using risky techniques, including hypnosis and abreaction. Treatment requires a minimum of 5 years and encourages extensive self-rumination. Typically, ‘alters’ proliferate alongside marked clinical deterioration in the form of florid post-traumatic stress disorder and ‘[h]allucinations, increasing discomfort, and severe dysphoria’, often leading to ‘states of chronic crisis for long periods of time’ (Piper Reference Piper and Merskey2004). Other potential serious harm includes multiple suicide attempts, pseudo-memories of horrific ritual abuse and the fragmentation of family life.

DID in forensic settings

There are few reported cases of DID in the UK justice system. In R v Baker [2009], the defendant claimed one of his two alters, possessing a narcissistic quality, contributed to his fraudulent conduct. Expert evidence identified DID but concluded that he was capable of forming dishonest intent. In R v Cowan [2016], the defendant attributed the criminal act – sending distressing social media messages – to her ‘alter ego’. Expert opinion concluded that she had an emotionally unstable personality disorder rather than DID. How the belief/diagnosis of DID emerged in the above cases is unknown.

The resurgence of DID has serious implications in other forensic contexts, notably:

  1. (a) local authority care proceedings

  2. (b) private law family proceedings, and

  3. (c) criminal proceedings relating to childhood sexual abuse.

In category (a), a mother's DID rendered her unable safely to care for her child (Re M (A Child: Care Order: Mother with Dissociative Identity Disorder) [2015]). Her diagnosis arose during 2 years' treatment with an unspecified ‘therapist’. Symptom presentation pretreatment is unknown. The court-accepted expert opined that the mother's DID was the product of severe recurrent childhood trauma, but was it? If the mother's condition was or may have been iatrogenically caused, this poses profound implications for public health and the integrity of the justice system. No category (b) or (c) case has been officially reported in the UK courts of which we are aware. However, a recent Australian case of a witness harbouring 2,500 alters (Moore Reference Moore2019) who gave witness testimony against her abusive father (who pleaded guilty mid-trial), indicates similar cases may soon present within the UK criminal justice system. Evaluating the testimonial admissibility and if admitted in evidence, the reliability of disputed memories of childhood sexual abuse acquired during therapy will require expert evidence at trial and justice professionals' understanding of the relevant scientific and clinical issues, as well as full disclosure of the therapeutic modality and treatment records. The consequences of serious sexual allegations arising from genuinely held but potentially false or pseudo-memories of sexual abuse may lead to profound miscarriages of justice if they are not detected before trial.

Conclusions

That DID is experiencing a revival seems to us beyond doubt. Merskey's comment remains apposite: ‘[T]he simple, strong theory of repression of unpleasant material into the unconscious mind is no longer an acceptable version of what happens when people develop hysterical or dissociative symptoms’ (Merskey Reference Merskey1998). We suggest that the Royal College of Psychiatrists publishes an updated version of its 1997 guidance document (Royal College of Psychiatrists 1997). We also suggest that professional training incorporates updated psychological and neurobiological research on human memory. The diagnosis and treatment of DID has important implications for public health and justice settings; we ignore DID at our peril.

Footnotes

See this issue.

References

References

Alexander, KW, Quas, JA, Goodman, GS, et al. (2005) Trauma impact predicts long-term memory for documented child sexual abuse. Psychological Science, 16: 3340.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th edn) (DSM-5). American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Bowlby, X, Briggs, D (eds) (2014) Living with the Reality of Dissociative Identity Disorder: Campaigning Voices. Karnac Books.Google Scholar
Brandon, S, Boakes, J, Glaser, D, et al. (1998) Recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse: implications for clinical practice. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172: 296307.Google Scholar
International Society for the Study of Trauma and Dissociation (2011) Guidelines for treating dissociative identity disorder in adults, third revision. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 12: 115–87.Google Scholar
Lilienfeld, SO (2007) Psychological treatments that cause harm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2: 5370.Google Scholar
Merskey, H (1998) Prevention and management of false memory syndrome. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 4: 253–62.Google Scholar
Moore, C (2019) ‘“Dad has destroyed my life”: Incest father SMIRKS as his brave daughter who was left with 2,500 personalities because of his sickening abuse faces him in court’. Daily Mail Australia, 31 May. Available at: https://www.dailymail.co.uk/news/article-7089389/Woman-2-500-personalities-testifies-court-fathers-monstrous-acts.html (Accessed 15 July 2019).Google Scholar
Paris, J (2019) Dissociative identity disorder: validity and use in the criminal justice system. BJPsych Advances, 25.Google Scholar
Patihis, L, Otgaar, H, Merckelbach, H (2019) Expert witnesses, dissociative amnesia and extraordinary remembering: response to Brand et al. Psychological Injury and Law, Feb 9: doi.org/10.1007/s12207-019-09348-8 [Epub ahead of print].Google Scholar
Pendergrast, M (2017) Memory Warp: How the Myth of Repressed Memory Arose and Refuses to Die. Upper Access Books.Google Scholar
Piper, A, Merskey, H (2004) The persistence of folly: critical examination of dissociative identity disorder. Part II. The defence and decline of multiple personality or dissociative identity disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 49: 678–83.Google Scholar
Piper, A, Lillevik, L, Kritzer, R (2008) What's wrong with believing in repression? A review for legal professionals. Psychology, Public Policy and Law, 14: 223–42.Google Scholar
Royal College of Psychiatrists (1997) Reported recovered memories of child sexual abuse: recommendations for good practice and implications for training, continuing professional development and research. Psychiatric Bulletin, 21: 663–5.Google Scholar

Cases

E v Channel Four Television Corp [2005] EWHC 1144 (Fam).Google Scholar
R v Baker [2009] EWCA Crim 89.Google Scholar
R v Cowan [2016] EWCA Crim 2094.Google Scholar
Re M (A Child: Care Order: Mother with Dissociative Identity Disorder) [2015] NI Fam 13.Google Scholar
St Helens Borough Council v Manchester Primary Care Trust [2008] EWCA Civ 931.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.