Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T18:11:13.992Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Monitoring local well-being in environmental interventions: a consideration of practical trade-offs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 October 2015

B. Palmer Fry
Affiliation:
Centre for Environmental Policy and Department of Life Sciences, Silwood Park Campus, Imperial College London, UK
M. Agarwala
Affiliation:
Geography & Environment Department, London School of Economics, UK
G. Atkinson
Affiliation:
Geography & Environment Department, London School of Economics, UK
T. Clements
Affiliation:
Wildlife Conservation Society, Bronx, New York, USA
K. Homewood
Affiliation:
Anthropology Department, University College London, UK
S. Mourato
Affiliation:
Geography & Environment Department, London School of Economics, UK
J.M. Rowcliffe
Affiliation:
Institute of Zoology, Zoological Society of London, UK
G. Wallace
Affiliation:
Frankfurt Zoological Society, Zambia and Tanzania
E.J. Milner-Gulland*
Affiliation:
Centre for Environmental Policy and Department of Life Sciences, Silwood Park Campus, Imperial College London, UK
*
(Corresponding author) E-mail e.j.milner-gulland@imperial.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Within the field of environmental management and conservation, the concept of well-being is starting to gain traction in monitoring the socio-economic and cultural impact of interventions on local people. Here we consider the practical trade-offs policy makers and practitioners must navigate when utilizing the concept of well-being in environmental interventions. We first review current concepts of well-being before considering the need to balance the complexity and practical applicability of the definition used and to consider both positive and negative components of well-being. A key determinant of how well-being is operationalized is the identity of the organization wishing to monitor it. We describe the trade-offs around the external and internal validity of different approaches to measuring well-being and the relative contributions of qualitative and quantitative information to understanding well-being. We explore how these trade-offs may be decided as a result of a power struggle between stakeholders. Well-being is a complex, multi-dimensional, dynamic concept that cannot be easily defined and measured. Local perspectives are often missed during the project design process as a result of the more powerful voices of national governments and international NGOs, so for equity and local relevance it is important to ensure these perspectives are represented at a high level in project design and implementation.

Type
Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Fauna & Flora International 2015

Introduction

Practitioners and policy makers in the field of environmental management are becoming increasingly aware that genuine involvement of local people is centrally important to long-term project success (Brashares & Sam, Reference Brashares and Sam2005; Skutsch et al., Reference Skutsch, Van Laake, Zahabu, Karky, Phartiyal and Angelsen2009; Danielsen et al., Reference Danielsen, Skutsch, Burgess, Jensen, Andrianandrasana and Karky2011). Without free, prior and informed consent, integrated local involvement, clear benefit sharing and community ownership, environmental projects are likely to lack sustainability (TFD, 2012; Harvey & Reed, Reference Harvey and Reed2007; Waylen et al., Reference Waylen, Fischer, McGowan, Thirgood and Milner-Gulland2010). Thus to evaluate the impact of environmental interventions, it is necessary to assess both the human and natural parts: ecological and human dynamics cannot be separated (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Dietz, Carpenter, Alberti, Folke and Moran2007). By definition ‘environmental interventions’ are environmentally focused policy or project actions that lead to localized changes in existing systems but that might additionally have human-centred goals, which may range from positive benefit for the surrounding population to ‘doing no harm’. Intervention ‘success’, therefore, is characterized by the achievement of these ecological and human goals. The complex social, economic and environmental landscapes that frame interventions make effective monitoring of change very difficult (Christie, Reference Christie2004). Monitoring changes in the biophysical environment has received much attention from natural scientists over the years (Millennium Ecosystem Assessment, 2005). These same specialists have also commonly been responsible for monitoring the impacts of interventions on people, an area in which they may not have sufficient experience or training. In response to a deeper understanding of the coupling of social and natural systems, modern conservation science is beginning to draw more readily upon social science expertise and approaches, and thereby is becoming increasingly interdisciplinary and convergent with the field of sustainable development (Roe, Reference Roe2008; Kareiva & Marvier, Reference Kareiva and Marvier2012).

Although the poorest people are often those most directly reliant on functioning biophysical systems (e.g. Bahuguna, Reference Bahuguna2000; Kepe et al., Reference Kepe, Saruchera and Whande2004), policies aimed at conserving these systems frequently marginalize this socio-economic group as a result of factors such as an overemphasis on local rather than global drivers of degradation, and insufficient attention to distributional issues such as elite capture. For example, Lenzen et al. (Reference Lenzen, Moran, Kanemoto, Foran, Lobefaro and Geschko2012) demonstrate that 30% of the threats to Red Listed species are caused by international trade, rather than by locally-originating factors. Sommerville et al. (Reference Sommerville, Jones, Rahajaharison and Milner-Gulland2010) asked local people about their perceptions of the fairness of a Payments for Ecosystem Services scheme in Madagascar. They found that village elites benefited substantially from the intervention whereas more marginalized groups, who were most dependent on the forest resources that the scheme aimed to protect, were not receiving benefits commensurate with the costs imposed by conservation.

A more nuanced understanding of the social impacts of environmental projects is necessary if interventions are to be sustainably pro-poor (Blomley & Franks, Reference Blomley and Franks2009). One lens through which socio-economic and cultural impact can be discerned is individual well-being, defined by the Oxford English Dictionary as ‘a state characterized by health, happiness and prosperity’ (Agarwala et al., Reference Agarwala, Atkinson, Palmer Fry, Homewood, Mourato and Rowcliffe2014). Scholars and practitioners need a rigorous understanding of the well-being concept to develop and implement frameworks to monitor the intentional or unintentional impacts that environmental interventions have on local people. Agarwala et al. (Reference Agarwala, Atkinson, Palmer Fry, Homewood, Mourato and Rowcliffe2014) provided a review of the different well-being conceptualizations and monitoring frameworks, and Milner-Gulland et al. (Reference Milner-Gulland, McGregor, Agarwala, Atkinson, Bevan and Clements2014) looked at why well-being is of particular interest to conservationists. Here we focus on the implementation of well-being monitoring, specifically on the implications of having multiple stakeholders involved. We focus on interventions in poor rural areas that often have weak systems of governance, and consider specifically the well-being of local beneficiaries or participants in such interventions. Larger scale concepts such as national well-being (as used by the UK Government; ONS, 2011; and the Government of Bhutan; CBS, 2012) are beyond the scope of this paper.

Firstly we briefly review the multifaceted concept of well-being that is being used differently across different fields, from subjective happiness at the national scale (ONS, 2011) to individual empowerment within farming communities (Friis-Hansen & Duveskog, Reference Friis-Hansen and Duveskog2012). We then consider who wants to measure well-being and why, a question that directly influences the conception of well-being and perceptions of success in environmental interventions. It is necessary to develop a greater understanding of the positions, interests and subtle agendas of stakeholders when considering how success is defined for environmental interventions, particularly when that success is measured in human rather than biophysical terms. We do not address how to measure well-being because several methodological reviews already exist (e.g. Schreckenberg et al., Reference Schreckenberg, Camargo, Withnall, Corrigan, Franks and Roe2010; Angelsen et al., Reference Angelsen, Larsen, Lund, Smith-Hall and Wunder2011; Richards & Panfil, Reference Richards and Panfil2011). Furthermore, we do not claim to identify the best approach to using the concept of well-being but instead consider the trade-offs and dilemmas that policy makers and practitioners face when applying the concept within the context of environmental interventions. Two of these trade-offs relate to the concept of well-being, and two to its operationalization. By making these trade-offs explicit, and highlighting the power dynamics that may lie behind particular approaches to monitoring well-being, we aim to promote more informed decision-making about how best to operationalize the concept of well-being in conservation.

What is well-being?

There are numerous definitions of well-being, each of them using a slightly different emphasis, albeit often with significant overlap (see Agarwala et al., Reference Agarwala, Atkinson, Palmer Fry, Homewood, Mourato and Rowcliffe2014 for a review). In this section we briefly outline objective and subjective approaches to well-being, before moving on to discuss hybrid approaches. We use this review as a foundation for exploring our first two trade-offs: single- vs multi-dimensional operationalizations of well-being, and the emphasis on the positive rather than negative aspects of life.

Objective approaches

‘Poverty is pronounced deprivation in well-being’ (World Bank definition; Haughton & Khandker, Reference Haughton and Khandker2009)

Objective changes in this context refer to observable, material changes in the external world surrounding an individual. In high-level, macro-scale development literature, such as the UN Millennium Development Goals and the World Bank resource documents, poverty and well-being are seen as objective concepts that are mutually exclusive, as in the above definition. This broad approach results in a concept that can include a number of different factors such as basic materials for a good standard of living, health, nutrition, security, social relations and freedom of choice. Objective approaches characterize many other policy areas; for example, the health discourse suggests that well-being is determined by good physical health, which can be further improved by engaging in positive behaviours (as exemplified by the mixed content of the UK Government's web page on health and well-being; Government of the UK, 2012). Economists widely use well-being interchangeably with ‘gain’ so that the concept is synonymous with a person's objective access to rights, goods and services (Baldock, Reference Baldock, Baldock, Manning and Vickerstaff2007). Given that well-being by definition is an internal not external state, the objective approach is not so much looking at well-being itself as at the factors that influence well-being. Even when using objective proxies it is far from straightforward to identify correctly the defining factors. Focusing on objectivity we face a conundrum: how, using a purely objective approach, can the genuine sense of well-being commonly described as being felt in extremely poor communities be explained?

Subjective approaches

‘Well-being refers to the emotional quality of an individual's everyday experience—the frequency and intensity of experiences of joy, stress, sadness, anger and affection that makes one's life pleasant or unpleasant’ (Kahneman & Deaton, Reference Kahneman and Deaton2010)

Focusing on an individual's experience, subjective definitions such as this capture how a multitude of external, objective factors are translated into internal experiences of well-being. In psychology, where subjective approaches have been adopted most strongly, the eudaimonic approach proposes that happiness is sourced in the experience of life satisfaction and fulfilment, while the hedonic approach engages with the more familiar concept that happiness is sourced in the experience of pleasure and pain (as reviewed by Ryan & Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2001). Both approaches place considerable emphasis on individual personality but do not always correlate because the expectations of people within the same cohort, and of individuals through their lives, can differ significantly. For example, different expectations can cause people with the same emotional well-being and resources to experience very different life satisfaction (Kahneman & Kruger, Reference Kahneman and Kruger2006). Both approaches refer to the term happiness, which is increasingly fashionable with national governments; e.g. the Government of Bhutan's pioneering concept of Gross National Happiness (CBS, 2012) and the UK Government's commissioning of a national well-being study that uses happiness as the cornerstone of its work (ONS, 2011). However, although subjective approaches draw upon relatively direct indicators of individually-experienced well-being, Keyes & Waterman (Reference Keyes, Waterman, Bornstein, Davidson, Keyes and Moore2003) assert that they don't tell enough of the story and so use a slightly hybridized definition that includes internal states as well as external objective capabilities: ‘[well-being] encompasses positive functioning states that include successful accomplishing of social challenges and tasks’.

Hybrid approaches

‘Well-being is a state of being with others, where human needs are met, where one can act meaningfully to pursue one's goals, and where one enjoys a satisfactory quality of life’ (Gough & McGregor, Reference Gough and McGregor2007)

The Wellbeing in Developing Countries research group, a substantial contributor to the well-being literature since 2002, utilizes the above definition, explaining that the blend of the objective and subjective concepts transcends both by recognizing the way each is socially constructed (Gough & McGregor, Reference Gough and McGregor2007). Sen (Reference Sen1999) pioneered this mixed approach by emphasizing that people are likely to subjectively experience their objective deprivation very differently. The hybrid approach has emerged from the social sciences and gained traction in other fields such as health (e.g. the Gallup-Healthways definition: ‘well-being is a state of complete physical, mental and social health’; Gallup, 2009), economics (e.g. the new economics foundation definition: ‘well-being is functioning well, having positive feelings day-to-day and overall, and thinking your life is going well’; NEF, 2012), and government policy (e.g. the Australian Bureau of Statistics definition: ‘well-being is a health or sufficiency in all aspects of life’; ABS, 2001). Although using a hybridized definition promotes a holistic approach to monitoring the impacts of interventions on people and ecosystems, there is a danger that being broadly inclusive can render the well-being concept overly complex and difficult to operationalize. As a result, policy makers and practitioners typically face trade-offs when conceptualizing and measuring the effects of interventions on well-being.

Trade off 1: single vs multi-dimensional definitions

Some definitions of well-being are more complicated than others and this has implications when planning and monitoring environmental interventions while explicitly considering their impact upon human well-being. If a (relatively) one-dimensional definition is used, such as ‘well-being is determined by an individual's experience of life satisfaction and fulfilment’ (the eudaimonic approach; Kahneman & Deaton, Reference Kahneman and Deaton2010), then well-being is readily conceptualized and therefore more easily utilized in monitoring, fulfilling the needs of interventions with very specific interests. The major drawback of such an approach to well-being is that it is narrow: it may at best only partially describe the changes that human subjects experience, in so doing defeating the point of using well-being as a nuanced and holistic approach. Conversely, a multi-dimensional definition such as ‘well-being is determined by all aspects that are important to an individual’ (the Australian Bureau of Statistics approach) is so widely descriptive that any operationalization of the concept is likely to require the inclusion of many subtle elements that contribute to well-being in order to capture the genuine social impacts of an intervention. However, this array of elements may make understanding, monitoring and therefore robust measurement of well-being change an impractically large and complex undertaking. To reconcile this trade-off, policy makers and practitioners may choose to select a manageable set of elements from a pre-defined pool of appropriate indicators, in line with the approach outlined by Bossel (Reference Bossel2001).

Trade off 2: emphasizing positive well-being vs a focus on negative well-being

In general, the use of the term well-being rather than the term poverty represents a conceptual shift towards a more positive approach to development, focusing on what is good and improves people's lives rather than primarily what is bad or lacking (NEF, 2012). However, there is the potential that this focus on desirable states could neglect to address the negative aspects of people's lives. Well-being is reduced by ‘harms’ such as poverty, malnourishment, social exclusion and more extremely, war, and governments should take steps to reduce these harms (McGregor, Reference McGregor2011). In a conceptual parallel to the differentiation between satisfiers, pseudo-satisfiers and pathogens in Max-Neef's (Reference Max-Neef1991) discourse on human needs, well-being needs to be considered both in relation to the active presence or absence of positive components, and in relation to the presence or absence of harms. Particularly in areas where there is substantial poverty, conflict and land seizure, where environmental interventions can occur, the changing presence of harms may be more relevant to the overall balance of someone's life. For example, an individual may have improving family relationships and increased access to health and education services, but their well-being may remain overwhelmingly affected by social issues such as alcoholism in the community, or ecosystem dis-services such as flooding or drought. Objective approaches should equally cover positive and negative aspects of a person's surrounding environment while subjective information may reveal how these negative and positive factors are internally processed, summed and expressed. Subjective information can also serve to test the relevance of the objective measures: if the objective overview shows a person to be surrounded by more positive well-being factors than negative factors, but they convey poor subjective well-being, the monitoring process may well be missing key elements that shape well-being for that particular person, community or society.

Who wants to monitor well-being and why?

Well-being can be conceptualized by external personnel and used comparatively in different locations and over time, or conceptualized by local people themselves and be as relevant as possible to those affected by an intervention at a particular point in time (e.g. Ferraro & Pattanayak, Reference Ferraro and Pattanayak2006). One key factor that frequently determines the approach taken to well-being in environmental interventions is who is involved in the project and why they are interested in monitoring well-being. Mapping the range of stakeholders involved in environmental interventions is a typical step in planning a monitoring programme. However, practitioners rarely explore the possible underlying agendas of each stakeholder (for examples, see Malan, Reference Malan2008; MacDonald, Reference MacDonald2010), as these more subtle agendas are not easily evidenced and often are only understood after a considerable time working in a locality. In Table 1, we describe a hypothetical round-table discussion for planning and implementing an intervention, incorporating a limited selection of caricatured perspectives that stakeholders may have on why it is important to monitor well-being. Differences in perspectives stem from disparate motivations for involvement in interventions. Table 1 breaks down some typical perspectives, showing the public narrative openly expressed by stakeholders as well as potential underlying strategic interests. These perspectives are generalized from our own experiences and are by no means exhaustive; the aim is to raise common issues relating to well-being. From them, we draw out two key trade-offs relating to operationalizing well-being.

Table 1 A stylized representation of the perspectives of typical stakeholders in environmental interventions and the effect of these perspectives on well-being monitoring.

Trade-off 3: internal vs external validity

With contrasting interests in monitoring well-being (Table 1), subtle power struggles may occur as different stakeholders attempt to have their own needs met. One particularly important outcome of this power struggle is the extent to which monitoring of well-being is focused on external or internal validity. Internal and external validity in this context refers to whom a framework, project or system is primarily serving with respect to investigation and information provision. A well-being monitoring system that is internally valid is well tailored to a specific area or population and represents the local expressions and determinants of well-being. It wouldn't necessarily be as transferable or applicable to other areas or populations, or even to the same population over time, as an externally valid system would be. Both types of validity are important. Internal validity or relevance will often determine the local social sustainability of an intervention, whereas external validity or relevance ensures that, by focusing on spatial or temporal project comparisons, interventions may lead to more wide-reaching benefits and thus potentially better returns on environmental investments. In practice, external validity frequently dominates because of the greater political and economic power of high-level external organizations (for example, see Scheske, Reference Scheske2012). Schmidt & Bullinger (Reference Schmidt, Bullinger, Gough and McGregor2007) describe this trade-off in validity and propose an adaptable cross-cultural approach that includes both external and internal validity.

Trade-off 4: quantitative vs qualitative understanding

Quantitative approaches frequently underpin externally valid interventions and generate numerical data that can be analysed using statistical methods and presented concisely. On the other hand qualitative approaches often provide greater detail about the meaning and experience of well-being and are regularly used in internally valid interventions where issues of local importance are explored within complex social systems (Krauss, Reference Krauss2005; Dominguez Gomez et al., Reference Dominguez Gomez, Roig Merino and Aledo Tur2010). Qualitative information may be preferred by local communities as well as academic social scientists. The two types of understanding are largely complementary because they provide different methods with which to investigate and represent multifaceted well-being (Healy & Perry, Reference Healy and Perry2000). However, quantitative data are often viewed as sufficiently robust for policy formulation, whereas qualitative information may be considered extraneous or preliminary instead of providing essential context (Pullin et al., Reference Pullin, Knight, Stone and Charman2004). This disparity frequently arises from a misconception of what constitutes objective data (numerical data are often perceived as objective; Choi et al., Reference Choi, Pang, Lin, Puska, Sherman and Goddard2005), as well as variation in concepts and terminology, which can lead to misunderstandings about the value of different types of knowledge (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Christian, Nordby, Pergam, Peterson and Pyke2006; Drury et al., Reference Drury, Homewood and Randall2011) and a reduction in the quality or effectiveness of well-being assessments.

Discussion

‘[there is a] more widely accepted view, associated with Sen (Reference Sen1999), which is that human well-being depends on a range of functions and capabilities that enable people to lead a good life, each of which needs to be directly and objectively measured and which cannot, in general, be aggregated into a single summary measure’ (Deaton, 2008)

Well-being is a holistic concept that can be used to promote a more comprehensive understanding of the human part of an ecosystem. Natural scientists and development workers involved in environmental interventions have traditionally tended towards concise, quantitative results to evaluate the impact of their projects and demonstrate success to donors and colleagues. It therefore follows that these practitioners will, for the most part, look for similar externally-orientated outputs when considering well-being. However, significant local input is required to provide a nuanced understanding of the local situation within which an intervention takes place, a necessary requirement if the intervention is aiming to improve (or not harm) the human condition (Scott, Reference Scott1998).

The power struggle to gain control or influence over an intervention is centrally important because those who dominate will determine which conceptualization and approach to well-being is used and therefore what is monitored during the ongoing project. In conservation, local voices are often overpowered by more influential bodies such as local elites, investors and the government (Brockington, Reference Brockington2003; Scheske, Reference Scheske2012). To achieve greater equity and intervention sustainability, policy makers and practitioners should endeavour to give local people influence over project design as well as facilitating the genuine airing of local perspectives at all levels (e.g. in national and international-level project meetings and not just community-level meetings).

Using the well-being concept in developing a monitoring programme for the social impacts of an intervention may improve understanding of local context while raising the profile of local contributions to the planning process. Navigating this process is only possible when the subtle as well as the obvious motivations and agendas of the various stakeholders are explicitly understood. Promoting internal validity may lead to the further integration of qualitative approaches and human narratives with the classically dominant quantitative methods, shaping the more holistic methodology advocated by Thomas (Reference Thomas2008). However, there are two generally important cautions here: (1) the project team will consequently need to be bigger, more inter-disciplinary and may need longer to decide upon a monitoring plan, as a result of the different disciplinary thinking, language and traditions (Pooley et al., Reference Pooley, Mendelsohn and Milner-Gulland2014); and (2) careful consideration should be given to whether enhancing participation of local people is both appropriate and beneficial; assuming more participation is good is a value-statement and is not universally applicable (Bishop & Davis, Reference Bishop and Davis2002).

A dilemma emerges as well-being measurement shifts towards more internal validity and a stronger focus on subjective well-being, as we advocate. The concept of well-being is itself constructed by individuals in a relational context (McGregor, Reference McGregor2008). As such the entire conceptual framework for an individual can change radically as the relational situation changes. As a complex concept with a potentially shifting baseline, measuring well-being over time to determine the success of an intervention becomes extremely challenging. For example, Batwa Pygmies in East and Central Africa were displaced from their forest homes as a result of gazetting national parks (Lewis & Richmond, Reference Lewis and Richmond2000). Their well-being was previously centred on traditional craft and hunter-gathering strategies but is now increasingly influenced by access to education and justice as they seek to flourish while living in close contact with surrounding societies (Lewis & Richmond, Reference Lewis and Richmond2000). Constructing an a priori framework to track changes in the components of well-being through these changing circumstances would be extremely difficult.

To negotiate the trade-offs we have identified there is firstly a requirement to understand the needs and perspectives of each stakeholder and determine the degree of external and internal validity that is needed from the monitoring (Table 1, trade-off 3). Subsequently, agreement can be reached on a definition and conceptual framework for well-being that includes a range of subjective and objective dimensions (trade-off 1) and the balance of positive and negative factors to be monitored (trade-off 2). Finally, the quantitative and qualitative contributions can be decided when developing actual measures (trade-off 4).

The issues examined here lead to three main conclusions. Firstly, individual well-being is not a simple concept that can be easily defined and measured to see how an intervention is affecting people. It is multifaceted and contains a mixture of positive and negative elements, which are likely to be affected by environmental interventions in complex, often indirect ways. Secondly, policy makers and practitioners should be aware of the need to give local perspectives on well-being more attention when designing and implementing environmental interventions. In doing this the local relevance of indicators may be improved, stakeholder equity may be enhanced, and more revealing methodologies may shape monitoring systems. Thirdly, this is only possible when the underlying motivations of each of the stakeholder groups are explicitly understood. Finding a fair, feasible and fitting balance of trade-offs in monitoring well-being will help shape more successful environmental interventions in the future.

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the support of the Natural Environment Research Council's Valuing Nature Network (VNN) project and the ESRC/DFID project Measuring complex outcomes of environment and development interventions. BPF would like to acknowledge the support of the Economic and Social Research Council. We thank all the members of the VNN project team and the attendees at the VNN well-being project workshops for their inputs and insights to this work.

Biographical sketches

Ben Palmer Fry has advised on the design of a community-based REDD+ MRV protocol. His experience of community engagement comes mostly from East Africa, through Kulika Uganda and Tearfund Southern Sudan, and of forest monitoring through work in Argentina. This paper is the result of an interdisciplinary collaboration of a team of natural scientists, economists, and anthropologists, brought together to study this topic through the NERC Valuing Nature Network (http://www.iccs.org.uk/well-being).

References

Agarwala, M., Atkinson, G., Palmer Fry, B., Homewood, K., Mourato, S., Rowcliffe, J.M. et al. (2014) Assessing the relationship between human well-being and ecosystem services: a review of frameworks. Conservation and Society, 12, 437449.Google Scholar
Angelsen, A., Larsen, H.O., Lund, J.F., Smith-Hall, C. & Wunder, S. (eds) (2011) Measuring Livelihoods and Environmental Independence: Methods for Research and Fieldwork. Earthscan, Edinburgh, UK.Google Scholar
ABS (Australian Bureau of Statistics) (2001) Australian Bureau of Statistics: Measuring Well-being. Http://www.abs.gov.au/AUSSTATS/ABS@.NSF/0/B176042438EE2331CA2571B7000A43A7?opendocument [accessed 2 September 2015].Google Scholar
Bahuguna, V. (2000) Forests in the economy of the rural poor: an estimate of dependency. Ambio, 29, 126129.Google Scholar
Baldock, J. (2007) Social policy, social welfare, and the welfare state. In Social Policy, 3rd edition (eds Baldock, J., Manning, N. & Vickerstaff, S.), pp. 726. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
Biddle, N. (2011) Definitions of Well-being and their Applicability to Indigenous Policy in Australia. 2011 CAEPR lecture series: Measures of indigenous well-being and their determinants across the life-course. Centre for Aboriginal Economic Policy Research, Australian National University, Canberra, Australia.Google Scholar
Bishop, P. & Davis, G. (2002) Mapping public participation in policy choices. Australian Journal of Public Administration, 61, 1429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blomley, T. & Franks, P. (2009) Making Participatory Forest Management Work for the Poor. CARE Denmark, Copenhagen, Denmark.Google Scholar
Bossel, H. (2001) Assessing viability and sustainability: a systems-based approach for deriving comprehensive indicator sets. Ecology and Society, 5(2), 12.Google Scholar
Brashares, J.S. & Sam, M.K. (2005) How much is enough? Estimating the minimum sampling required for effective monitoring of African reserves. Biodiversity and Conservation, 14, 27092722.Google Scholar
Brockington, D. (2003) Injustice and conservation: is local support necessary for sustainable protected areas? Policy Matters, 12, 2230.Google Scholar
CBS (Centre for Bhutan Studies) (2012) Gross National Happiness. Http://www.grossnationalhappiness.com/ [accessed 2 September 2015].Google Scholar
Choi, B., Pang, T., Lin, V., Puska, P., Sherman, G. & Goddard, M. (2005) Can scientists and policy makers work together? Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 59, 632637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christie, P. (2004) Marine Protected Areas as biological successes and social failures in Southeast Asia. American Fisheries Society Symposium, 42, 155164.Google Scholar
Cooke, M., Mitrou, F., Lawrence, D., Guimond, E. & Beavon, D. (2007) Indigenous well-being in four countries: an application of the UNDP's Human Development Index to indigenous peoples in Australia, Canada, New Zealand and the United States. BMC International Health and Human Rights, 7(9).Google Scholar
Coulthard, S., Johnson, D. & McGregor, J.A. (2011) Poverty, sustainability and human well-being: a social well-being approach to the global fisheries crisis. Global Environmental Change, 21, 453463.Google Scholar
Danielsen, F., Skutsch, M.M., Burgess, N.D., Jensen, P.M., Andrianandrasana, H., Karky, B.S. et al. (2011) At the heart of REDD+: a role for local people in monitoring forests? Conservation Letters, 4, 158167.Google Scholar
De Rond, M. & Miller, A. (2005) Publish or perish: bane or boon of academic life? Journal of Management Inquiry, 14, 321329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dominguez Gomez, J., Roig Merino, B. & Aledo Tur, A. (2010) The identity of sociology or what to do when the universe is unknown: qualitative solutions against quantitative obsession. Spatial and Organizational Dynamics, 5.Google Scholar
Drury, R., Homewood, K. & Randall, S. (2011) Less is more: the potential of qualitative approaches in conservation research. Animal Conservation, 14, 1824.Google Scholar
Ferraro, P.J. & Pattanayak, S.K. (2006) Money for nothing? A call for empirical evaluation of biodiversity conservation investments. PLoS Biology, 4, 482488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, H., Christian, C., Nordby, C., Pergam, O., Peterson, G. & Pyke, C. (2006) Perceived barriers to integrating social science and conservation. Conservation Biology, 20, 18171820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friis-Hansen, E. & Duveskog, D. (2012) The empowerment route to well-being: an analysis of Farmer Field Schools in East Africa. World Development, 40, 414427.Google Scholar
Gallup (2009) Gallup–Healthways Well-being Index: Methodology Report for Indexes. Http://wbi.meyouhealth.com/files/GallupHealthwaysWBI-Methodology.pdf [accessed 2 September 2015].Google Scholar
Gjoksi, N. (2010) International Approaches to Measure Wealth and Well-being in the Context of Sustainable Development. ESDN Case Study No. 3. European Sustainable Development Network. H ttp:// www.sd-network.eu/pdf/case%20studies/03_ESDN%20Case%20Study%203_FINAL.pdf [accessed 2 September 2015].Google Scholar
Gough, I. & McGregor, J.A. (2007) Well-being in Developing Countries: From Theory to Research. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harvey, P. & Reed, R. (2007) Community-managed water supplies in Africa: sustainable or dispensable? Community Development Journal, 42, 365378.Google Scholar
Haughton, J. & Khandker, S.R. (2009) Handbook on Poverty and Inequality. World Bank, Washington, DC, USA.Google Scholar
Healy, M. & Perry, C. (2000) Comprehensive criteria to judge validity and reliability of qualitative research within the realism paradigm. Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal, 3, 118126.Google Scholar
Jordan, S.J., Hayes, S.E., Yoskowitz, D., Smith, L.M., Summers, J.K., Russel, M. & Benson, W.H. (2010) Accounting for natural resources and environmental sustainability: linking ecosystem services to human well-being. Environmental Science and Technology, 44, 15301536.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. & Deaton, A. (2010) High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 1648916493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. & Kruger, A.B. (2006) Developments in the measurement of subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20, 324.Google Scholar
Kareiva, P. & Marvier, M. (2012) What is conservation science? BioScience, 62, 962969.Google Scholar
Kepe, T., Saruchera, M. & Whande, W. (2004) Poverty alleviation and biodiversity conservation: a South African perspective. Oryx, 38, 143145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keyes, C. & Waterman, M. (2003) Dimensions of well-being and mental health in adulthood. In Well-being: Positive Development Across the Life Course (eds Bornstein, N., Davidson, L., Keyes, C. & Moore, K.), pp. 477497. Psychology Press, New Jersey, USA.Google Scholar
Khumalo, I.P., Temane, Q.M. & Wissing, M.P. (2012) Socio-demographic variables, general psychological well-being and the mental health continuum in an African context. Social Indicators Research, 105, 419442.Google Scholar
Krauss, S. (2005) Research paradigms and meaning making: a primer. The Qualitative Report, 10, 758770.Google Scholar
Lenzen, M., Moran, D., Kanemoto, K., Foran, B., Lobefaro, L. & Geschko, A. (2012) International trade drives biodiversity threats in developing nations. Nature, 486, 109112.Google Scholar
Lewis, J. & Richmond, S. (eds) (2000) The Batwa Pygmies of the Great Lakes Region. Minority Rights Group International, London, UK.Google Scholar
Liu, J., Dietz, T., Carpenter, S., Alberti, M., Folke, C., Moran, E. et al. (2007) Complexity of coupled human and natural systems. Science, 317, 15131516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, K.I. (2010) Business, biology, and new ‘fields’ of conservation: The World Conservation Congress and the renegotiation of organizational order. Conservation and Society, 8, 256275.Google Scholar
Malan, L.C. (2008) Beyond the debate: exploring the underlying values and assumptions of biodiversity conservation in protected areas. PhD thesis. Antioch University, New England, USA.Google Scholar
Max-Neef, M. (1991) Human Scale Development: Conception, Application and Further Reflections. The Apex Press, New York, USA.Google Scholar
McGregor, A. (2011) The promotion of global well-being can drive the development agenda. Http://www.guardian.co.uk/global-development/poverty-matters/2011/aug/08/global-wellbeing-drive-development-agenda [accessed 16 February 2012].Google Scholar
McGregor, J.A. (2008) Well-being, Poverty and Conflict. Report number: 1/08. WeD Group, Bath, UK.Google Scholar
Millennium Ecosystem Assessment (2005) Ecosystems and Human Well-being: Synthesis. Island Press, Washington, DC, USA.Google Scholar
Milner-Gulland, E.J., McGregor, J.A., Agarwala, M., Atkinson, G., Bevan, P., Clements, T. et al. (2014) Accounting for the impact of conservation on human well-being. Conservation Biology, 28, 11601166.Google Scholar
NEF (New Economics Foundation) (2012) Measuring Well-being: A Guide for Practitioners. New Economics Foundation, London, UK.Google Scholar
ONS (Office for National Statistics) (2011) Measuring what Matters. National Statistician's Reflections on the National Debate on Measuring National Well-being. Office for National Statistics, Newport, UK. Http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/guide-method/user-guidance/well-being/publications/measuring-what-matters-national-statistician-s-reflections-on-the-national-debate-on-measuring-national-well-being.pdf [accessed 2 September 2015].Google Scholar
Pooley, S., Mendelsohn, A. & Milner-Gulland, E.J. (2014) Hunting down the chimera of multiple disciplinarity in conservation science. Conservation Biology, 28, 2232.Google Scholar
Pullin, A.S., Knight, T.M., Stone, D.A. & Charman, K. (2004) Do conservation managers use scientific evidence to support their decision-making? Biological Conservation, 119, 245252.Google Scholar
Rangan, V.K., Chu, M. & Petkoski, D. (2011) Segmenting the base of the pyramid. Harvard Business Review. June 2011. Http://hbr.org/2011/06/the-globe-segmenting-the-base-of-the-pyramid [accessed 2 September 2015].Google Scholar
Ribot, J., Agrawal, A. & Larson, A. (2006) Recentralizing while decentralizing: how national governments re-appropriate forest resources. World Development, 34, 18641886.Google Scholar
Richards, M. & Panfil, S.N. (2011) Social and Biodiversity Impact Assessment (SBIA) Manual for REDD+ Projects: Part 1 – Core Guidance for Project Proponents. Climate, Community & Biodiversity Alliance, Forest Trends, Fauna & Flora International, and Rainforest Alliance, Washington, DC, USA.Google Scholar
Roe, D. (2008) The origins and evolution of the conservation–poverty debate: a review of key literature, events and policy processes. Oryx, 42, 491503.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. & Deci, E. (2001) On happiness and human potential: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141166.Google Scholar
Scheske, C. (2012) A critical analysis of socio-economic monitoring in the context of community-based marine conservation. MSc thesis. Imperial College London, London, UK. Http://www.iccs.org.uk/wp-content/thesis/consci/2012/Scheske.pdf [accessed 2 September 2015].Google Scholar
Schmidt, S. & Bullinger, M. (2007) Cross-cultural quality of life assessment approaches and experiences from the health care field. In Well-being in Developing Countries: from Theory to Research (eds Gough, I. & McGregor, A.), pp. 219241. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Schreckenberg, K., Camargo, I., Withnall, K., Corrigan, C., Franks, P., Roe, D. et al. (2010) Social Assessment of Conservation Initiatives: A Review of Rapid Methodologies. Natural Resource Issues No. 22, IIED, London, UK.Google Scholar
Scott, J. (1998) Seeing Like a State: How Certain Schemes to Improve the Human Condition have Failed. Yale University Press, New Haven, USA.Google Scholar
Sen, A. (1999) Development as Freedom. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
Skutsch, M.M., Van Laake, P.E., Zahabu, E., Karky, B.S. & Phartiyal, P. (2009) Community monitoring in REDD+. In Realizing REDD+: National Strategy and Policy Options (ed. Angelsen, A.), pp. 101112. CIFOR, Bogor, Indonesia.Google Scholar
Sommerville, M., Jones, J.P.G., Rahajaharison, M. & Milner-Gulland, E.J. (2010) The role of fairness and benefit distribution in community-based payment for environmental service interventions: a case study from Menabe, Madagascar. Ecological Economics, 69, 12621271.Google Scholar
Thomas, B.K. (2008) Methodological debate in poverty studies: towards ‘participatory qual-quant’? Development in Practice, 18, 280288.Google Scholar
Waylen, K., Fischer, A., McGowan, P., Thirgood, S. & Milner-Gulland, E.J. (2010) The effect of cultural context on the success of community-based conservation interventions. Conservation Biology, 24, 11191129.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 A stylized representation of the perspectives of typical stakeholders in environmental interventions and the effect of these perspectives on well-being monitoring.