Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T05:20:55.140Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of traffic light labelling and increased healthy range on beverage choices from vending machines

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 April 2024

Ryan Calabro*
Affiliation:
Psychology, College of Education, Psychology and Social Work, Flinders University, Adelaide, SA 5001, Australia
Eva Kemps
Affiliation:
Psychology, College of Education, Psychology and Social Work, Flinders University, Adelaide, SA 5001, Australia
Ivanka Prichard
Affiliation:
Health & Exercise Sciences, College of Nursing and Health Sciences, Flinders University, Adelaide, Australia
Marika Tiggemann
Affiliation:
Psychology, College of Education, Psychology and Social Work, Flinders University, Adelaide, SA 5001, Australia
*
*Corresponding author: Email ryan.calabro@flinders.edu.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

To test whether traffic light labels and an increased range of healthy beverages, individually and in combination, can increase healthy beverage choices from vending machines.

Design:

Two studies (n 558, 420) tested whether the provision of traffic light labels (green, amber and red) and an increased range of healthy beverages (from 20 % to 50 % green options), individually and in combination, could increase healthy beverage choices from a digital vending machine display. The studies used a between-subjects experimental design, and a hypothetical beverage choice, a limitation when considering real-world applicability.

Setting:

Both studies utilised an online Qualtrics survey that featured a digital vending machine display.

Participants:

Both studies (n 558, 420) consisted of university students from Flinders University and individuals from a survey recruitment service.

Results:

Featuring traffic lights did not significantly influence beverage choices (P = 0·074), while increasing the healthy range (P = 0·003, OR = 3·27), and the combination of both, did significantly increase healthier beverage choices (P < 0·001, OR = 4·83).

Conclusions:

The results suggest that the traffic light system and increased healthy range are not maximally effective when used on their own, and benefit greatly when combined, to increase healthy beverage choices. It was suggested that the provision of traffic light labels supplied the necessary nutritional information, and the increased healthy range offered greater opportunity to act in accordance with that information. In so doing, the present findings offer a promising pathway for reducing unhealthy beverage consumption.

Type
Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

The consumption of soft drinks (carbonated beverages that are high in sugar) is increasing worldwide, leading to an increased prevalence of health problems related to overconsumption(Reference Tahmassebi and BaniHani1), including diabetes, tooth decay and obesity(Reference Basu, McKee and Galea2,Reference Çetinkaya and Romaniuk3) . Concerningly, one of the largest age groups that consume soft drinks are adolescents and young adults(Reference Han and Powell4), who are particularly sensitive to developing lifelong habits that can negatively impact long-term health. In response to this growing health concern, the WHO(5) made a strong recommendation that people limit their daily intake of free (added) sugar to 10 % of their total energy intake, equating to roughly 50 g or 12 tsp of sugar. With soft drinks being one of the main sources of sugar in many people’s diet(6), this growing health concern has highlighted the importance of reducing soft drink consumption.

Over recent years, several countries (e.g. USA, UK, Chile and NZ) have implemented strategies to reduce soft drink consumption, including taxing soft drinks(Reference Tiffin, Kehlbacher and Salois7), restricting access to soft drinks(Reference Fernandes8,Reference Terry-McElrath, O’Malley and Johnston9) and providing nutritional warning labels(Reference Taillie, Reyes and Colchero10). Each of these strategies has had varying levels of success. For example, while the introduction of a soft drink tax has resulted in reduced soft drink purchases in various countries, it can also shift purchasing behaviour to other high-sugar items(Reference Nakhimovsky, Feigl and Avila11). In addition, while restricting access in schools can reduce soft drink consumption, it has been shown to result in increased soft drink consumption outside of school(Reference Fernandes8,Reference Terry-McElrath, O’Malley and Johnston9) . However, prominent warning labels on products high in sugar together with a tax have reduced soft drink purchases in Chile(Reference Taillie, Reyes and Colchero10). Collectively, current strategies mostly involve government regulation and policy change.

An alternative strategy would be to implement a change within the immediate environment in which the choice is presented. An example is the introduction of a nutritional label at the point of sale rather than on the product itself. This carries the advantage that it still offers the choice of soft drink to those who choose to consume it and does not require manufacturers to change the labels of their products.

There are a variety of nutritional labelling systems in use around the world, such as the nutrient warning, health warning, nutritional score and traffic light system. Of these, a recent review found the traffic light system to be one of the most popular and best performing at nudging consumers towards purchasing healthier food products(Reference Song, Brown and Tan12). This system offers an easy-to-understand visual cue using traffic light colours (red, amber and green) to denote the nutritional value of food and beverages. These colours are universally understood concepts, with green denoting ‘go’ or ‘good’, amber ‘slow down’ or ‘limit’ and red ‘stop’ or ‘avoid’. A review of the traffic light system found that it had a significant influence in promoting healthy food choices from restaurants and cafeterias(Reference Fernandes, Oliveira and Proença13). Because of its success in these food environments, the traffic light system may also prove effective in other settings.

One environment that is a common source of soft drinks is a vending machine, which often feature a disproportionately high number of sugar-sweetened beverages(Reference Rozman, Mis and Kupirovič14Reference Byrd-Bredbenner, Johnson and Quick16). Additionally, vending machines do not typically offer nutritional information at the point of purchase. Research amongst college students has found that an intervention that displays labelling information for healthy options was perceived as the most helpful, compared to price changes, or posters explaining which options were healthier(Reference Kirchoff, Abadia and Palacios17). Thus, implementing the traffic light system for beverages within a vending machine could promote healthier choices much like it has in other consumption environments. Only one study could be identified that investigates specifically the traffic light system (in isolation to other interventions such as price changes and serving size changes). Brown et al. (Reference Brown, Flint and Fuqua18) placed green, amber and red stickers on the shelving below snacks on five high use machines on a university campus, which resulted in a 50 % increase in green product purchases. However, the vending machine only included snack foods.

In addition to the provision of nutritional information, it is also important to consider the opportunity (or lack thereof) for someone to make a healthy choice(Reference Schruff-Lim, Van Loo and van Kleef19). This is particularly pertinent as vending machines have been found to mostly contain unhealthy options(Reference Rozman, Mis and Kupirovič14Reference Byrd-Bredbenner, Johnson and Quick16). A study by Pechey et al. (Reference Pechey, Jenkins and Cartwright20) investigated whether reducing unhealthy or increasing healthy options from vending machines could improve the proportion of energy purchased from healthy options. They found that decreasing the number of positions containing unhealthy drinks resulted in less energy purchased from these drinks but found inconclusive results for increasing the number of positions that contained healthy drinks. However, this study did not feature any additional nutritional information to motivate individuals to choose healthier beverages. The very few studies that have investigated both traffic light labelling in combination with increasing the range of healthy options in vending machines have shown some promising results(Reference Kocken, Eeuwijk and Van Kesteren21,Reference Stamos, Lange and Dewitte22) . However, no studies could be identified that offer the ability to compare the difference in impact between featuring one or the combination of the two interventions (traffic light system and increased healthy range).

Thus, the aim of the present research was to test the impact of the traffic light system, and of increasing the range of healthy beverages, and the combination of both, in a hypothetical choice experiment that utilised a digital vending machine. Study 1 tested the traffic light system on a vending machine featuring beverages representative of the range of drinks (proportion of healthy and unhealthy drinks) typically offered. Study 2 extended this by stocking the vending machine with a greater proportion of healthy options following recent guidelines from the Victorian Healthy Eating Advisory Service(23) (similar to guidelines provided in the USA(24)). Based on previous research(Reference Song, Brown and Tan12,Reference Brown, Flint and Fuqua18) , we predicted that the vending machine with the traffic light system would increase the percentage of healthy (green) choices in both studies. It should be noted that the beverage choice was hypothetical, and the results from the present study were intended to guide the implementation of the traffic light system on real vending machines on campus.

Methods

Participants

Study 1

Participants were 558 young adults (414 women and 143 men) with a mean age of 20·21 years (sd = 2·41). The sample consisted of 317 undergraduate students at Flinders University who took part for course credit and 241 participants from Prolific who received a small monetary reimbursement. Participation was limited to young adults (17–25 years) to capture the core consumers of soft drinks. Mean BMI of the sample was 23·56 kg/ ${{\rm{m}}^2}$ (sd = 5·20).

Study 2

Participants were 420 young adults (284 women and 136 men) with a mean age of 20·67 years (sd = 2·26). The sample consisted of forty-two undergraduate students at Flinders University who took part for course credit and 378 participants from Prolific who received a small monetary reimbursement. Participation was again limited to young adults (17–25 years) to capture the core consumers of soft drinks. Mean BMI of the sample was 24·30 kg/ ${{\rm{m}}^2}$ (sd = 6·40).

Design

Both studies used a between-subjects design. For Study 1, participants were randomly allocated to one of two vending machine display conditions: traffic light system or no traffic light system (control). The dependent variable was the classification of the beverage chosen (green, amber or red). Study 2 utilised the same design, but included two additional conditions: increased healthy range and increased healthy range combined with traffic lights.

Materials

Vending machine displays (Study 1)

Two vending machine displays were created: traffic light system and control (no traffic light system). As shown in Fig. 1, the traffic light system condition featured labelling under each beverage indicating whether it was classified as either green, amber or red based on nutritional value. Text was also included in the colour classification labels to allow those who are colour blind to participate in the study. A legend in the top right-hand corner provided more information about the colour classifications, with green indicating ‘best choice’, amber ‘choose carefully’ and red ‘limit’. Drink colour classifications and the legend text for the colours were based on the Food Checker classification of the Victorian Healthy Eating Advisory Service(23). This service enables beverages to be searched in an online database to find the colour classification according to set nutritional criteria. The criteria specify that green beverages are low in added sugar, energy or fat, red beverages are high in added sugar, energy or fat, and amber beverages are between the two (see Victorian Healthy Eating Advisory Service(23) for more information). These criteria are similar to those included in the guidelines for healthy vending by the Centre of Disease Control and Prevention in the USA(24). The control vending machine had an identical drink layout and range but did not feature the traffic light system.

Fig. 1 The two vending machine conditions featured in Study 1.

The selection and placement of the drinks in the vending machine were based on Flinders University’s vending machine sales data. This resulted in eight spaces (20 %) in the vending machine layout filled with green beverages, twelve spaces (30 %) with amber beverages and twenty spaces (50 %) with red beverages. The selection of green beverages included popular varieties of water (including lightly flavoured and sparkling waters). The amber beverages included kombucha, aloe vera drinks and sugar-free soft drinks. The red beverages included soft drinks and energy drinks. All serving sizes of beverages were kept as close as possible to a 500-ml offering.

Vending machine displays (Study 2)

For the second study, we aimed to test whether an increased healthy range could promote healthier beverage choices. To design the increased healthy beverage layout for the vending machine, we turned to guidelines that have been developed for this specific purpose, of which there were several available (e.g. USA(24), Canada(25), UK(26) and Australia(23)). We focused on the Australian vending machine layout guidelines(23), as they were the most comprehensive and from the same source used in Study 1 to classify the beverages based on nutritional criteria. These guidelines suggest having at least 50 % green choices and no more than 20 % red choices. Therefore, the newly designed increased healthy range display featured twenty spaces (50 %) filled with green beverages, eight spaces (20 %) with amber beverages and twelve spaces (30 %) with red beverages. Compared to the range featured on the other displays, this offered a 20 % increase in the number of green choices, a 10 % decrease in the number of amber choices and a 10 % decrease in the number of red choices. To achieve these recommended percentages, the increased healthy range featured four additional green beverage choices (one additional water flavour and three additional sparkling water options) and two fewer red beverage choices (soft drinks), removed based on their very low popularity in Study 1. We also created an additional display that featured this increased healthy range in combination with the same traffic light system used in Study 1 (see Fig. 2). These additional vending machine displays enabled us to independently test the main effects of the traffic light system and an increased healthy range, as well as the combination of the two, on beverage choices.

Fig. 2 The four vending machine conditions featured in Study 2.

Beverage choice task

In both studies, participants were asked to imagine that they were standing in front of a vending machine and to choose a beverage that they would like to drink straight away, without consideration of price or serving size. Participants made their selection by clicking on their beverage of choice. They were then asked to describe in a few words why they chose that particular beverage.

Background information

In both studies, participants reported their age and gender, and the last time they drank anything (estimated to the nearest 15 min). They also rated how thirsty they were on a 100-mm visual analogue scale ranging from ‘not at all thirsty’ to ‘extremely thirsty’.

Procedure

Both studies were conducted online using Qualtrics. After providing informed consent, participants provided background information. They then completed the beverage choice task and post-choice question. Most participants completed the study in 10–15 min.

Results

Study 1

Beverage choice

Overall participants mostly chose red beverages (57 %), followed by green beverages (23 %) and amber beverages (20 %). The most commonly reported reason for choosing a particular beverage was because it was liked (18·8 %), or because it was considered healthy (9·2 %), a favourite (8·8 %), refreshing (8·7 %) or tasty (8·7 %).

Effect of vending machine condition on beverage choice

From Table 1, it can be seen that there was a slight trend towards healthier beverage choices for the vending machine that featured the traffic light system, with green beverages chosen 6·1 % more frequently and red beverages chosen 6·3 % less frequently relative to the control machine. However, in both conditions most participants still chose unhealthy (red) beverages. A binary regression was conducted to test whether the traffic light condition predicted healthy (green) beverage choices, while controlling for thirst. The overall model was not significant, X 2 (2, n 558) = 5·347, P = 0·069, indicating that featuring the traffic light system did not significantly influence healthy beverage choices.

Table 1 Study 1 percentage of beverage colour classifications chosen in each condition

Study 2

Beverage choice

As in Study 1, overall participants again mostly chose red beverages (53 %), followed by amber beverages (25 %) and green beverages (22 %). The most commonly reported reason for choosing a particular beverage was because it was liked (18·1 %), or because it was considered healthy (12·7 %), a favourite (9·9 %), tasty (8·7 %) or refreshing (7·4 %).

Effect of vending machine condition on beverage choice

Figure 3 shows the beverage choices in each condition. It can be seen that red beverages were clearly the most frequently chosen in the first three conditions. In the traffic light system and increased healthy range combined condition, however, green and amber beverages were selected slightly more often than red beverages.

Fig. 3 Study 2 percentage of beverage colour classifications chosen in each condition.

A binary regression was conducted to test of the effect of condition (traffic light system, increased healthy range and the combination) on the likelihood that participants chose a healthy (green) beverage while controlling for thirst. The overall model was significant, X 2 (4, n 420) = 25·691, P < 0·001. The model explained 9·1 % (Nagelkerke R2) of the variance in healthy beverage choices and correctly classified 78·1 % of cases. As can be seen in Table 2, participants in the traffic light condition were not significantly more likely to choose a healthy beverage (P = 0·074), while those in the increased healthy range condition were 3·2 times more likely to choose a healthy beverage, and those in the traffic light system and increased healthy range combined condition were 4·8 times more likely to choose a healthy beverage, compared to the no traffic light system and standard range (control) condition.

Table 2 Coefficients for the variables entered in the binary regression predicting a healthy (green) beverage choice

* P < 0·05.

** P < 0·001.

Discussion

The present studies examined the effects of a traffic light system and an increased healthy range, individually and in combination, on beverage choices from a vending machine. Featuring the traffic light system on the vending machine resulted in a non-significant trend towards more healthy beverage choices (Studies 1 and 2). However, increasing the range of healthy options did produce significantly more healthy beverage choices (Study 2). In addition, the combination of the traffic light system and an increased healthy range resulted in the largest proportion of healthy beverage choices.

It was predicted that featuring the traffic light system on the vending machine would result in significantly more healthy beverage choices. Although there was a trend towards healthier (green) beverages and fewer unhealthy (red) beverages being chosen when featuring the traffic light system, the effect of the traffic light system was not significant. This parallels the result of Brown et al. (Reference Brown, Flint and Fuqua18), although their reported increase (50 %) was much larger. These findings suggests that in settings where mostly unhealthy items are on offer (such as vending machines(Reference Rozman, Mis and Kupirovič14Reference Byrd-Bredbenner, Johnson and Quick16)), using traffic lights alone may not be enough to produce a substantial shift towards healthier choices. Traffic lights have been shown to be successful in increasing healthy choices in settings that typically offer a greater variety of healthy items (e.g. restaurants and cafeterias)(Reference Fernandes, Oliveira and Proença13), increasing the likelihood of an appealing alternative. Consequently, a stronger nudge than the traffic light system may be required to produce healthier beverage choices from a vending machine environment that typically features predominantly unhealthy beverages.

In Study 2, the effect of the increased range of healthy beverages was significant and was stronger than that of the traffic light system for increasing healthy beverage choices. The range of healthy beverages offered was based on the guidelines by the Victorian Healthy Eating Advisory Service(23), which mainly involved increasing the range of green beverage options and correspondingly reducing the range of ‘red’ options. The present findings show that following these or similar guidelines for the stocking of beverage vending machines could potentially lead to an increase in the percentage of healthy beverage choices and a decrease in the percentage of unhealthy beverage choices. One reason for the stronger effect compared to the traffic light system may simply be that the increased healthy range offered a greater number of appealing healthy options. It is also possible that stocking the vending machine with a majority of spaces (50 %) featuring healthy beverages may have acted as a normative cue. This result is in line with previous research that found reducing the number of positions that featured unhealthy drinks in vending machines resulted in a reduced proportion of energy purchased from these unhealthy drinks(Reference Pechey, Jenkins and Cartwright20). In the present study, although the number of spots occupied by unhealthy beverages was reduced, there was minimal reduction in the range of unhealthy options to ensure that the choice of those beverages was still available.

Importantly, the combination of featuring the traffic light system and an increased healthy range resulted in the largest increase in the percentage of healthy choices. It is possible that either alone addresses only one part of the process, whereas together they offer both easy to understand nutritional information though the traffic light system, together with sufficient opportunity to act on this information through the increased healthy range. A similar finding was reported by Kocken et al. (Reference Kocken, Eeuwijk and Van Kesteren21) who found that their healthy vending machine intervention worked best when including the combination of lower calorie products and labelling (however, their lower calorie products were also lower priced). It should be noted that the combination condition here resulted in an increase in both green and amber beverage choices, the latter possibly representing people who would have originally chosen an unhealthy (red) beverage changing their choice to a healthier option (amber), for example, by choosing a sugar-free soft drink (amber) over a standard soft drink (red). A similar finding of an increase in both green and amber choices in response to labelling and stock changes in vending machines was reported by Stamos et al. (Reference Stamos, Lange and Dewitte22). In terms of public support for interventions of this nature, there is evidence that increasing the healthy range of products in vending machines(Reference Carrad, Louie and Milosavljevic27) and featuring nutritional information(Reference Bos, van der Lans and van Kleef28) are considered favourably. Importantly, research has shown that having healthier options in vending machines does not necessarily decrease revenue(Reference Hua, Kimmel and Van Emmenes29Reference Viana, Leonard and Kitay31). In fact, Hua et al. (Reference Hua, Kimmel and Van Emmenes29) note the importance of highlighting healthy options in the machine as a strategy to mitigate any potential profit loss from removing any unhealthy options.

The present studies show the importance of both increasing the healthy range and including the traffic light system to maximise the percentage of healthy beverage choices from vending machines. While increasing the healthy range offered in the vending machine resulted in a significant increase in healthy choices, this effect was less than half of that obtained by combining it with the traffic light system. Indeed, the combined condition was the only condition where the percentage of healthy (green) choices exceeded that of unhealthy (red) choices. Such an increase in the percentage of healthy beverage choices is a potentially significant step towards reducing negative health outcomes associated with the overconsumption of soft drink and other sugar-sweetened beverages. For example, on each occasion an individual chooses a sugar-free soft drink over a standard soft drink, they will consume approximately twelve teaspoons less of sugar, in turn making them more likely to meet the WHO recommendations for daily free sugar intake and avoid the health issues related to the overconsumption of soft drinks.

Although the effect of an increased healthy range in combination with traffic lights was demonstrated for vending machines, these colour coding and layout changes could also be implemented in other environments (such as drink fridges in cafes, schools and supermarket shelves) and may result in a similar increase in the percentage of healthy beverage choices. Supermarkets, in particular, have been shown to feature a disproportionately high number of unhealthy (ultra-processed) packaged foods(Reference Eyles, Luiten and Ni Mhurchu32,Reference Vandevijvere, Van Dam and Inaç33) . Existing research on this intervention combination outside of vending machines is limited, with one study reporting(Reference Lowe, Tappe and Butryn34) that calories and macronutrient content labelling in combination with introducing lower energy-dense foods resulted in healthier eating behaviour in a workplace cafeteria. A systematic review by Schruff-Lim et al. (Reference Schruff-Lim, Van Loo and van Kleef19) also highlights the need for more research investigating the combination of nutritional labels (such as the traffic light system) and an increased healthy range. The present study offers promising evidence in support of such combined interventions in the vending machine environment. Future research could test the present studies’ findings in other settings.

Like all research, the present studies have some limitations warranting acknowledgement. First, participants were presented with only a hypothetical beverage choice and never had the opportunity to consume their chosen drink. Nevertheless, hypothetical choices have been found to engage comparable brain systems as real choices(Reference Kang, Rangel and Camus35) and thus can be considered a valid predictor of actual choices. Second, it is not possible to rule out the effect of demand characteristics in the present studies. Specifically, in the conditions where beverages were labelled by the traffic light system, participants may have deduced the purpose of the research and chosen beverages accordingly. This may have magnified the effect observed compared to an intervention featured on real vending machines.

In conclusion, the present studies offer evidence supporting healthy vending machine guidelines, such as the ones available in Australia(23), for stocking and providing nutritional labelling on vending machines. The resulting interventions provide a promising pathway for guiding people’s choices away from soft drinks and other unhealthy drink options towards something healthier with less or no sugar and thereby reducing sugar consumption.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to Sarah Hollitt for assistance with Qualtrics survey construction.

Financial support

This research was supported under the Australian Research Council’s Discovery Project funding scheme (project number DP180100545). The ARC had no role in the study design; the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; and in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Conflicts of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.

Authorship

All authors contributed to the design of the study and writing of the manuscript. Ryan Calabro was responsible for collecting data under supervision of Eva Kemps. Ryan Calabro conducted the data analysis and wrote the first draft of the manuscript, with all other authors editing subsequent drafts of the manuscript. All authors have approved the final manuscript.

Ethics of human subject participation

This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving research study participants were approved by the Flinders University Human Research Ethics Committee. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects.

References

Tahmassebi, JF & BaniHani, A (2019) Impact of soft drinks to health and economy: a critical review. Eur Arch Paediatric Dent 21, 109117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basu, S, McKee, M, Galea, G et al. (2013) Relationship of soft drink consumption to global overweight, obesity, and diabetes: a cross-national analysis of 75 countries. Am J Public Health 103, 20712077.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Çetinkaya, H & Romaniuk, P (2020) Relationship between consumption of soft and alcoholic drinks and oral health problems. Cent Eur J Public Health 28, 94102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, EP & Powell, LMP (2013) Consumption patterns of sugar-sweetened beverages in the United States. J Academy Nutr Diet 113, 4353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO (2015) Guideline: Sugars Intake for Adults and Children. http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/10665/149782/1/9789241549028_eng.pdf (accessed 01 September 2023).Google Scholar
Australian Bureau of Statistics (2020) Apparent Consumption of Selected Foodstuffs, Australia. https://www.abs.gov.au/statistics/health/health-conditions-and-risks/apparent-consumption-selected-foodstuffs-australia/latest-release#added-and-free-sugars (accessed 01 September 2023).Google Scholar
Tiffin, R, Kehlbacher, A & Salois, M (2015) The effects of a soft drink tax in the UK. Health Econ 24, 583600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandes, MMM (2008) The effect of soft drink availability in elementary schools on consumption. J Am Dietetic Assoc 108, 14451452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terry-McElrath, YMMSA, O’Malley, PMP & Johnston, LDP (2013) School soft drink availability and consumption among U.S. secondary students. Am J Prev Med 44, 573582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taillie, LS, Reyes, M, Colchero, MA et al. (2020) An evaluation of Chile’s Law of Food Labeling and Advertising on sugar-sweetened beverage purchases from 2015 to 2017: a before-and-after study. PLoS Med 17, e1003015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakhimovsky, SS, Feigl, AB, Avila, C et al. (2016) Taxes on sugar-sweetened beverages to reduce overweight and obesity in middle-income countries: a systematic review. PLOS One 11, e0163358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, J, Brown, MK, Tan, M et al. (2021) Impact of color-coded and warning nutrition labelling schemes: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. PLoS Med 18, e1003765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandes, AC, Oliveira, RC, Proença, RPC et al. (2016) Influence of menu labeling on food choices in real-life settings: a systematic review. Nutr Rev 74, 534548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozman, U, Mis, NF, Kupirovič, UP et al. (2021) Nutritional quality of beverages available in vending machines in health and social care institutions: do we really want such offers? J Health Popul Nutr 40, 29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawrence, S, Boyle, M, Craypo, L et al. (2009) The food and beverage vending environment in health care facilities participating in the healthy eating, active communities program. Pediatrics 123, S287S292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrd-Bredbenner, C, Johnson, M, Quick, VM et al. (2012) Sweet and salty. An assessment of the snacks and beverages sold in vending machines on US post-secondary institution campuses. Appetite 58, 11431151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirchoff, C, Abadia, K & Palacios, C (2022) Perception about the effectiveness of different vending machine interventions to promote healthier snack selection among college students. J Am College Health 72, 17.Google ScholarPubMed
Brown, MV, Flint, M & Fuqua, J (2014) The effects of a nutrition education intervention on vending machine sales on a university campus. J Am Coll Health 62, 512516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schruff-Lim, E-M, Van Loo, EJ, van Kleef, E et al. (2023) Turning FOP nutrition labels into action: a systematic review of label+ interventions. Food Policy 120, 102479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pechey, R, Jenkins, H, Cartwright, E et al. (2019) Altering the availability of healthier v. less healthy items in UK hospital vending machines: a multiple treatment reversal design. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 16, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kocken, PL, Eeuwijk, J, Van Kesteren, NMC et al. (2012) Promoting the purchase of low-calorie foods from school vending machines: a cluster-randomized controlled study. J Sch Health 82, 115122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stamos, A, Lange, F & Dewitte, S (2019) Promoting healthy drink choices at school by means of assortment changes and traffic light coding: a field study. Food Qual Preference 71, 415421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Victorian Healthy Eating Advisory Service (2023) Food Checker. https://foodchecker.au (accessed 01 September 2023).Google Scholar
Centre for Disease Control and Prevention (2022) Behavioral Design Strategies for Vending Machine Operation. https://www.cdc.gov/nutrition/food-service-guidelines/behavioral-design/vending-machines.html (accessed 28 December 2023).Google Scholar
Alberta Health Services (2023) Healthier Vending Toolkit. https://www.albertahealthservices.ca/nutrition/Page13884.aspx (accessed 29 December 2023).Google Scholar
Live Well Greenwich (2023) Healthy Vending in Greenwich. https://livewellgreenwich.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2020/03/RBG_Healthy-Vending_8pp_A4_v4-FINAL.pdf (accessed 29 December 2023).Google Scholar
Carrad, AM, Louie, JC-Y, Milosavljevic, M et al. (2015) Consumer support for healthy food and drink vending machines in public places. Aust New Zealand J Public Health 39, 355357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bos, C, van der Lans, IA, van Kleef, E et al. (2018) Promoting healthy choices from vending machines: effectiveness and consumer evaluations of four types of interventions. Food Policy 79, 247255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hua, SV, Kimmel, L, Van Emmenes, M et al. (2017) Health promotion and healthier products increase vending purchases: a randomized factorial trial. J Acad Nutr Diet 117, 10571065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grivois-Shah, R, Gonzalez, JR, Khandekar, SP et al. (2017) Impact of healthy vending machine options in a large community health organization. Am J Health Promot 32, 14251430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viana, J, Leonard, SA, Kitay, B et al. (2018) Healthier vending machines in a university setting: effective and financially sustainable. Appetite 121, 263267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eyles, H, Luiten, CM, Ni Mhurchu, C et al. (2016) Ultra-processed foods have the worst nutrient profile, yet they are the most available packaged products in a sample of New Zealand supermarkets. Public Health Nutr 19, 530538.Google Scholar
Vandevijvere, S, Van Dam, I, Inaç, Y et al. (2023) Unhealthy food availability, prominence and promotion in a representative sample of supermarkets in Flanders (Belgium): a detailed assessment. Arch Public Health 81, 154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowe, MR, Tappe, KA, Butryn, ML et al. (2010) An intervention study targeting energy and nutrient intake in worksite cafeterias. Eating Behav 11, 144151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kang, MJ, Rangel, A, Camus, M et al. (2011) Hypothetical and real choice differentially activate common valuation areas. J Neurosci 31, 461468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 The two vending machine conditions featured in Study 1.

Figure 1

Fig. 2 The four vending machine conditions featured in Study 2.

Figure 2

Table 1 Study 1 percentage of beverage colour classifications chosen in each condition

Figure 3

Fig. 3 Study 2 percentage of beverage colour classifications chosen in each condition.

Figure 4

Table 2 Coefficients for the variables entered in the binary regression predicting a healthy (green) beverage choice