Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T04:18:01.644Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How effective psychological treatments work: mechanisms of change in cognitive behavioural therapy and beyond

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2024

Paul M. Salkovskis*
Affiliation:
University of Oxford Department of Experimental Psychology and Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
Magnus Blondahl Sighvatsson
Affiliation:
Reykjavik University, Reykjavík, Iceland University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland
Jon Fridrik Sigurdsson
Affiliation:
Reykjavik University, Reykjavík, Iceland University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland
*
Corresponding author: Paul M. Salkovskis; paul.salkovskis@hmc.ox.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) has, in the space of 50 years, evolved into the dominant modality in psychological therapy. Mechanism/s of change remain unclear, however.

Aims:

In this paper, we will describe key features of CBT that account for the pace of past and future developments, with a view to identifying candidates for mechanism of change. We also highlight the distinction between ‘common elements’ and ‘mechanisms of change’ in psychological treatment.

Method:

The history of how behaviour therapy and cognitive therapy developed are considered, culminating in the wide range of strategies which now fall under the heading of cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT). We consider how the empirical grounding of CBT has led to the massive proliferation of effective treatment strategies. We then consider the relationship between ‘common factors’ and ‘mechanisms of change’, and propose that a particular type of psychological flexibility is the mechanism of change not only in CBT but also effective psychological therapies in general.

Conclusion:

Good psychological therapies should ultimately involve supporting people experiencing psychological difficulties to understand where and how they have become ‘stuck’ in terms of factors involved in maintaining distress and impairment. A shared understanding is then evaluated and tested with the intention of empowering and enabling them to respond more flexibly and thereby reclaim their life.

Type
Main
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies

Introduction

Cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT), which evolved from behaviour therapy (BT) (Salkovskis, Reference Salkovskis1986) is now a well-established and mature approach to the understanding and treatment of an astonishingly wide range of health problems and beyond. The evidence for the effectiveness of CBT is both broad and compelling, whether considered in terms of common mental health problems such as depression or anxiety disorders in adults (Carpenter et al., Reference Carpenter, Andrews, Witcraft, Powers, Smits and Hofmann2018; Cuijpers et al., Reference Cuijpers, Miguel, Harrer, Plessen, Ciharova, Ebert and Karyotaki2023) or across the widest range of diagnoses and age groups (Fordham et al., Reference Fordham, Sugavanam, Edwards, Stallard, Howard, Nair, Copsey, Lee, Howick, Hemming and Lamb2021).

In this context, the present paper will consider some of the issues that have emerged and evolved during the history of BT and CBT, and what these might imply for future developments. We propose that the central challenge which faces us now is understanding the mechanism of change (MOC), not only in CBT but also more broadly across the widening range of effective psychological therapies. We place such consideration in the context of an understanding of the factors that have shaped the development of CBT as a dominant psychotherapy modality. Phenomenology informed research strategies have played a crucial role in the evolution of CBT as an empirically grounded and evidence-based psychotherapy, and most likely will continue to do so. Finally, we offer a detailed consideration of what we propose as the central MOC which applies not only to CBT but also more broadly to other psychological therapies.

The breadth of CBT

From the early days of behaviour therapy, there has been a bewildering proliferation of strategies and techniques that have given us a class of approaches which fall under the heading of ‘cognitive behavioural therapy’. However, although the range of strategies and interventions currently used to bring about therapeutic change in specific diagnoses is remarkable, there are also data which indicate that the importance of interventions tailored to specific psychiatric problems may not be as great as had been previously thought with transdiagnostic approaches having emerged (Harvey et al., Reference Harvey, Watkins, Mansell and Shafran2004). Helpful and sometimes contrasting interventions in CBT range from applied relaxation to applied tension; from exposure to behavioural experiments; from attention training to cognitive reappraisal, and so on (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Beck and Alford1999; Clark and Beck, Reference Clark and Beck2011). In addition, there are also a number of helpful treatments theoretically or topographically similar to CBT, like mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, compassion-focused treatment, behavioural activation, acceptance and commitment therapy, and dialectical behaviour therapy (Cristea et al., Reference Cristea, Gentili, Cotet, Palomba, Barbui and Cuijpers2017; Millard et al., Reference Millard, Wan, Smith and Wittkowski2023; Piet and Hougaard, Reference Piet and Hougaard2011; Stein et al., Reference Stein, Carl, Cuijpers, Karyotaki and Smits2021). We need to consider what might unify these apparently contrasting and at times contradictory approaches. We consider that typically the contradictions are to be found in the radically different underpinnings (proposed ‘mechanism of change’) of these varied approaches, which are sometimes at very considerable odds with the strategies and techniques they deploy (see, for example, McLoughlin and Roche, Reference McLoughlin and Roche2023). The topographical similarities of such strategies and techniques are easier to understand in terms of ‘common elements’ of these different therapies (e.g. Collard, Reference Collard2019),

Other treatment modalities that are theoretically dissimilar, like psychoanalytic/psychodynamic psychotherapies, have (rather late in the day) begun to seek and in some instances demonstrated evidence for their effectiveness (Ravitz et al., Reference Ravitz, Watson, Lawson, Constantino, Bernecker, Park and Swartz2019; Steinert et al., Reference Steinert, Munder, Rabung, Hoyer and Leichsenring2017). CBT, however, remains by far the most studied psychotherapy to date, and none of these psychotherapies have shown to be systematically superior to CBT; in fact it tends to be the other way around (David et al., Reference David, Cristea and Hofmann2018; Fordham et al., Reference Fordham, Sugavanam, Edwards, Stallard, Howard, Nair, Copsey, Lee, Howick, Hemming and Lamb2021; Hofmann et al., Reference Hofmann, Asnaani, Vonk, Sawyer and Fang2012). The growing range of treatments that have passed the fundamental test to become empirically supported therapies (Chambless and Ollendick, Reference Chambless and Ollendick2001) has inevitably given rise to considerations along the lines of ‘why do they all work?’. Is it because there is a ‘final common pathway’ responsible for the effectiveness of all of these diverse psychological treatments? Or is it because there are different mechanisms working for different treatments for the same mental health problems? Or are there perhaps different mechanisms operating in different treatments for different mental health problems? To answer these questions, researchers and therapists alike would do well to focus on the work of Gordon Paul (Reference Paul1967):

‘In all its complexity, the question towards which all outcome research should ultimately be directed is the following: What treatment, by whom, is most effective for this individual with that specific problem, and under which set of circumstances?’

We propose here that the answer to Gordon Paul’s 56-year-old question may ultimately be found in studying mechanisms of change. We arrive at this conclusion through consideration of the context in which CBT has developed over the last 50 years. Such consideration suggests that nearly all effective developments in CBT have been derived from evolving theoretical understanding of why prevailing treatments were wholly or partially ineffective, and how they could be improved from a better understanding of the mechanisms involved. We highlight a MOC that we suggest is a particularly good fit with the range of approaches which fall under the heading of CBT (Salkovskis, Reference Salkovskis1996; Sighvatsson et al., Reference Sighvatsson, Salkovskis, Sigurdsson, Valdimarsdottir, Thorsdottir and Sigurdsson2021) and beyond.

The evolution of CBT

How did CBT make the transition from a small and ‘upstart’ approach, derided by our psychoanalytic colleagues as at best ineffective and at worst potentially dangerous (Breger and McGaugh, Reference Breger and McGaugh1965; and subsequently Rachman, Reference Rachman2015; Tryon, Reference Tryon2008), to becoming the first- or second-line treatment for almost all mental health problems? Central to the evolution of early BT was the theoretical and empirical work of Ivan Pavlov and B. F. Skinner. Pavlov‘s research on conditioned reflexes is commonly considered to be the precursor of behaviour therapy and consequently CBT (Pavlov, Reference Pavlov2003; Rachman, Reference Rachman2009). In his experiments, Pavlov described associative learning of unconditioned (US) and conditioned stimuli (CS) in animals. Even today his ideas are used as an experimental paradigm to characterise abnormal behaviour, e.g. in the inhibition learning theory (Craske, Reference Craske2015; Craske et al., Reference Craske, Kircanski, Zelikowsky, Mystkowski, Chowdhury and Baker2008). However, it is now understood that the simple formation of a ‘reflex’ connection between US and CS explaining conditioned response (CS = UC => CR) is much more complicated, and the data suggest that perception of causality may underpin how the conditioning process develops (De Houwer, Reference De Houwer2020; Mackintosh, Reference Mackintosh2003; Rescorla, Reference Rescorla1988).

Skinner’s research on operant conditioning, again in animal research, influenced clinical psychologists to use operant procedures to help people suffering from mental health problems. This was based on the view that mental health problems involve ‘abnormal’ behaviours under voluntary control and that such behaviour could be reshaped (Rachman, Reference Rachman2015). An example of how these ideas are applied today is parent management training (Kazdin, Reference Kazdin, Weisz and Kazdin2017). Since the initial publication (Skinner, Reference Skinner1957), Skinner’s ideas have had their fair share of criticism. For example, defining mental health problems in terms of ‘verbal behaviour’ is of limited value in explain how we think, speak and feel (Chomsky, Reference Chomsky1959; Rachman, Reference Rachman1977).

From methodological behaviourism to ‘the cognitive revolution’

The further evolution of CBT owes much to the early work of the pioneers of CBT such as Joe Wolpe, Tim Beck and Albert Ellis, amongst others. This second and third generation of clinically grounded theoreticians typically started their training as psychoanalysts or psychodynamic therapists who were dissatisfied with the values and theoretical underpinnings of the psychotherapeutic approaches they had learned, including the rejection of both ‘symptom focused’ therapy and the evaluation of efficacy. These clinical theorists also considered it crucial to experimentally validate the processes and targets underpinning treatment (Beck, Reference Beck2006; Dozois et al., Reference Dozois, Dobson and Rnic2019; Rachman, Reference Rachman2009). Take for example the work of Aaron T. Beck. After graduation from the Philadelphia Psychoanalytic Institute, Beck was interested in validating psychoanalytic concepts and he chose one key concept of the psychoanalytic theory, wish fulfilment (Beck, Reference Beck2006; Hollon, Reference Hollon2022). According to Freudian theory, individuals suffer from depression because of unconscious rage against others, but due to its unacceptable nature, this rage was repressed and turned against the individual himself, causing depression. To validate this construct, Beck chose to look for hostility in the of content of dreams of depressed individuals and compare it with the content of dreams of non-depressed individuals. Beck found that the content of the dreams of depressed individuals was in fact less hostile than that of non-depressed individuals. Instead of rage and hostility, he found that the content of thoughts of depressed individuals during waking hours and dreams was focused on rejection, abandonment and loss (Beck, Reference Beck2006; Hollon, Reference Hollon2022). Beck therefore concluded that how people see themselves is one of the basic processes that explain how mental health problems evolve rather than the proposed psychoanalytic processes he set out to investigate (Beck, Reference Beck2006; Beck, Reference Beck2019; Hollon, Reference Hollon2022).

A second example is Joseph Wolpe, a pioneer in behaviour therapy. As a physician he treated psychological problems of soldiers coming from the battlefield in the Second World War. Trained as a Jungian analyst, he was dissatisfied with dominant schools of psychotherapy and as a consequence he looked for alternatives in treatment (Rachman, Reference Rachman2009; Rachman, Reference Rachman2015). Wolpe’s theoretical formulation stipulated that the MOC in anxiety reduction was the process of ‘reciprocal inhibition’. This was in turn an elaboration of Herrington’s theory of trans-marginal inhibition (Wolpe, Reference Wolpe1958). Although this hypothesised MOC was important in the way it underpinned the practice of systematic desensitisation, these theoretical underpinnings did not hold up under experimental investigation (Paul and Shannon, Reference Paul and Shannon1966). Wolpe’s work and its dissection by Paul and others encouraged the emergence of a wide range of new treatments, most gaining some empirical support but very few advancing theoretical understanding much beyond the circular proposition that effective treatments for anxiety typically involve the ‘common factor’ of exposure to feared situations, so exposure must be the underlying ‘mechanism of change’ (Marks, Reference Marks1973). This led to the adoption of ‘the exposure principle’ stating that thorough exposure to fear-related stimuli was common to all successful treatments for anxiety problems. Exposure was therefore hypothesised to be the MOC on the basis of it being a common factor; fear reduction is therefore due to exposure. Exposure is inferred from the occurrence of fear reduction. Therefore, learning theories (Foa and Kozak, Reference Foa and Kozak1986; Foa and McNally, Reference Foa, McNally and Rapee1996) and later cognitive theories (Abramowitz, Reference Abramowitz2013) provided the justification for the behavioural interventions used, but ultimately failed to identify the MOC involved in effective therapies. This confusion between ‘common factors’ and ‘mechanisms of change’ has in our view held the field back.

A further key factor in the evolution of CBT to its current form were the radical behaviourist model of depression put forward by Ferster and Lewinsohn and the evolutionary model of depression by Seligman (Ferster, Reference Ferster1973; Lewinsohn, Reference Lewinsohn1974; Maier and Seligman, Reference Maier and Seligman2016). According to these behavioural models, the development of depression is due to low levels of response-contingent positive reinforcement and lack of the availability of events that are positive reinforcement for the depressed person. Therefore, increased access to response-contingent reinforcement can be a hypothesised MOC in helpful treatments for depression such as behavioural activation (Dimidjian et al., Reference Dimidjian, Barrera, Martell, Muñoz and Lewinsohn2011; Malik et al., Reference Malik, Ibrahim, Bernstein, Venkatesh, Rai, Chorpita and Patel2021; Martell et al., Reference Martell, Addis and Jacobson2001). Evidence for such a mechanism (beyond treatment outcomes) is, however, extremely weak or even negative (e.g. Janssen et al., Reference Janssen, Hendriks, Baranelli, Lucassen, Oude Voshaar, Spijker and Huibers2021).

How effective psychological therapies evolved: empirically grounded clinical interventions

We will next consider the factors which drove a 50-year period of research leading to evolution in BT then CBT, culminating in the effective dominance of CBT. In the 1950s and 60s, psychotherapy was marked by a degree of complacency and the reliance on the perceived wisdom of authoritative and at times authoritarian leaders of psychotherapy ‘schools’. This state of near stagnation at levels of efficacy comparable to rates of spontaneous remission (Eysenck, Reference Eysenck1952) stalled development in terms of the understanding of helpful treatment processes and was disrupted by the advent of behaviour therapy with its emphasis on evaluation of both outcome and key processes.

Notably, behaviour therapy and cognitive therapy were evidence-based approaches to mental health problems long before the term was coined, but CBT involves much more than the current meaning of the term. Current CBT can best be described as a set of flexibly applied ‘empirically grounded clinical interventions’, carried out by clinicians who seek to operate as scientist-practitioners (Clark, Reference Clark2004; Hayes et al., Reference Hayes, Barlow and Nelson-Gray1999; Salkovskis, Reference Salkovskis2002; Shapiro, Reference Shapiro1961). The continuous and active empirical testing of theory as a way of evolving, developing and refining treatment is central to this framework, and has been the driver not only for new ways of working but also the dissolution and replacement of ineffective or theoretically invalidated approaches. By the time behaviour therapy was widely established 50 years ago, it was clear that central predictions of ‘classical psychoanalytic’ theory had been comprehensively refuted (Tryon, Reference Tryon2008).

The implicit model that has been applied in developing BT and CBT involves a set of interlinked factors as shown in Fig. 1, which is an elaboration of the model first described by Salkovskis (Reference Salkovskis2002), and linked to the process of developing effective CBT as described in detail in Clark (Reference Clark2004).

Figure 1. Development of empirically grounded clinical interventions.

Clinical practice and the phenomenology described by those we seek to help is both the target of our work and a source of information and inspiration which drives other aspects of the process of empirically grounded clinical practice and research. Through careful listening to patients’ experiences of their psychological difficulties, we begin to form an understanding of the key processes involved within and (sometimes) between individuals, including the extent to which these generalise to others with the same or different difficulties. This understanding can be used to derive and apply theory to the specific experiences of our patients. Clinical understanding and theory may also inform the focus of outcome research by suggesting the type of interventions which should be used, and which process and outcome measures are likely to provide the most relevant information. At the level of the individual, the best therapy can be considered to be the experimental investigation of the single case (Shapiro, Reference Shapiro1961; Shapiro, Reference Shapiro1985) and leads to the deployment of single case experimental designs.

Research in psychological and psychiatric treatment has been dogged by a common and recurring logical error. The effectiveness of a treatment is not evidence for the validity of the theory on which it is based with one important exception. Where a treatment demonstrably modifies a factor crucial to a theory of factors involved in a particular problem, but does not impact the hypothesised target of treatment, this failure provides a convincing disconfirmation of the theory. Successful treatment, when theoretically informed, can only reasonably be regarded as consistent with the underpinning theory, which is helpful to know, provided all such studies are reported within the principles of open science (Frankenhuis and Nettle, Reference Frankenhuis and Nettle2018).

So, if theories cannot be built on the effectiveness of treatment, how can the theories which drive novel treatments or enhance existing ones be developed? The answer is through research which allows the identification and evaluation of modifiable processes involved in the maintenance of the problems involved, with appropriate controls in place, and ultimately subject to experimental test. Thus, treatment development in BT and CBT has focused on factors which maintain clinical problems. We note that the past 50 years have seen no significant development of our understanding of causal factors in mental health problems. There has been some progress in identifying risk factors (e.g. Brander et al., Reference Brander, Pérez-Vigil, Larsson and Mataix-Cols2016), but mostly these are not readily modifiable and cannot be regarded as causal. Our inability to identify causality probably stems from the fact that the problems we see are the outcome of a range of convergent processes which have specific outcomes. Most likely, the mental health difficulties people experience are exaggerations of one or more otherwise normal processes and difficulties (e.g. Beck and Haigh, Reference Beck and Haigh2014; Beck, Reference Beck2019; Clark, Reference Clark1999). For example, social anxiety is an almost universal problem early in life, in the form of shyness with greater or lesser impacts. However, for some people such anxiety becomes cripplingly severe and persists over time, where for most people it decreases to manageable levels or dissipates entirely. Similarly, for depression, low mood is universally experienced, for example following bereavement, but for the clinically depressed person their mood problems have become sufficiently severe and persistent as to cause significant impairment of day-to-day life over prolonged periods.

Research in CBT has made very significant advances in understanding factors involved in the maintenance of psychological problems through a combination of comparisons of key measures across and within groups of people experiencing specific or general problems and experimental studies in which the hypothesised factors are systematically varied to test specific predictions. Such research can inform both theory and clinical practice, allowing the development of novel aspects of treatment which can then be evaluated in randomised controlled trials. Such research can include very precise laboratory investigations which seek to dissect an aspect of psychopathology in ways related to the phenomenology of the problem (Canvin et al., Reference Canvin, Janecka and Clark2016; Clark and Teasdale, Reference Clark and Teasdale1982; Wild et al., Reference Wild, Clark, Ehlers and McManus2008) through to field experiments in which the controls are less precise but ecological validity is built in (Rachman et al., Reference Rachman, De Silva and Roper1976; Salkovskis et al., Reference Salkovskis, Clark, Hackmann, Wells and Gelder1999). This type of strategy has been described in considerable detail elsewhere (Clark, Reference Clark1999). Research methods such as in examination of individual differences, epidemiological studies, qualitative studies and quantitative studies can also make important contributions to improving our understanding of how therapy outcomes can be made more efficient in terms of extent and speed of improvement obtained for comparable or less time invested. There is however a trap, summarised by Jones (Reference Jones2006) as ‘We don’t know how to measure all the environmental effects that impact on our lives, or we would. The danger lies in attributing too much significance to something just because you can measure it’. This is particularly evident in the failure of work on neuroimaging to generate any useful findings contributing to the understanding of mental health problems (Lilienfeld et al., Reference Lilienfeld, Aslinger, Marshall and Satel2018; Nour et al., Reference Nour, Liu and Dolan2022; Satel and Lilienfeld, Reference Satel and Lilienfeld2013; Vitacco et al., Reference Vitacco, Gottfried, Lilienfeld and Batastini2020).

This interplay described above between theory, practice, outcome research and research into the nature of mental health problems is of course neither disorder nor theory specific, but it is the adoption of these connected strategies which has resulted in cognitive and behavioural approaches leading the field in psychological treatments. In terms of the future development and dissemination of effective psychological therapies, we consider that the incorporation of the experience of those we try to help in terms of the choice of intervention is now crucial. To empower those who seek our help, we consider it to be a moral (and practical) imperative to facilitate ‘evidence based patient choice’, a type of shared decision making. Such an approach means helping patients to access a range of information according to their preferences, taking account of their value system. Promotion of evidence-based Clinical Guidelines, such as those produced to be accessible by NICE in the UK are one possible pathway to achieving such shared decision making, but again the broader notion of empirical grounding is also needed where the evidence is scant.

Treatment development

According to Clark (Reference Clark2004) development of effective CBT for particular mental health problems is characterised by six steps:

  1. (1) Talk to people suffering from a particular mental health problem about their experience of their difficulties. The phenomenology can then be expanded through the use of cognitive psychology strategies and clinical interviews to identify core cognitive abnormalities characteristic of that problem.

  2. (2) Develop a theoretical account (which should be capable of informing case conceptualizations) that explains why the cognitive abnormality does not self-correct or even accelerates. Note that the key here is hypothesised maintaining factors rather than causal notions.

  3. (3) Test out these hypothesised maintaining factors (based on the theoretical account in step 2) under controlled experimental conditions.

  4. (4) Develop cognitive treatment that reverses or diminishes the maintaining factors mentioned in steps 2 and 3.

  5. (5) Test the efficacy of the treatment using research strategies which provide some degree of control over threats to validity.

  6. (6) Disseminate this treatment mentioned in step 5.

The development of CBT involves a flexible process involving the interplay between understanding our patients’ phenomenology and our own theory building focused on maintenance factors, then empirically testing this theory in experimental studies which further inform treatment development and refinement. Although not necessarily confined to CBT amongst psychotherapies, this has largely been true over the last 50 years.

Randomised controlled trials are important, but not the only show in town

As can be seen from the discussion above, a variety of methods are used to demonstrate MOCs, with randomised controlled trials (RCTs) being only one of many methods, and in these terms not the gold standard. If RCTs are not king, how does they fit into the range of strategies deployed in psychological treatment research? An overview of possible methods and their interplay in psychological research can be seen in Fig. 2.

Figure 2. Research designs and the development of psychological treatments.

Historically, behavioural, cognitive and cognitive-behavioural approaches were initially attempted with problems that were regarded as difficult or even impossible to treat (‘treating treatment failures’; Emmelkamp, Reference Emmelkamp1987), so trying out new psychological approaches was regarded as a win-win strategy. In the UK this led to early behavioural treatment of problems such as OCD and the deployment of the Token Economy in chronically hospitalised and institutionalised schizophrenics. Developing treatments for ‘treatment failures’ (Otto and Hofmann, Reference Otto and Hofmann2010) became something of a pattern in BT and then CBT which continues to this day. As indicated in Fig. 2, initially, case studies simply describing effective treatment applied to mental health problems previously believed to be untreatable are important. This progresses to single case experimental designs (SCEDs), where more precise and regular measurement allowed valid attribution of improvement to the interventions deployed. Note that this strategy was initially taken from Shapiro’s principle of the intensive investigation of the single case and behaviour analysis (Barlow and Nock, Reference Barlow and Nock2009; Shapiro, Reference Shapiro1961). Having clearly shown effectiveness in a SCED, this progresses to consecutive SCED series. Larger scale ‘open trials’ can follow, leading ultimately to RCTs. RCTs are difficult and expensive to conduct unless they can be embedded in services where routine outcome monitoring is built into the service specification (as in NHS Talking Therapies for Anxiety and Depression). Note that, in the hourglass model used here, it may be that the generalisation to ‘routine clinical populations’ is limited. However, this is not necessarily so, given that one of the commonest reasons for exclusion from trials is low severity and chronicity. Generalisability of treatments from RCTs tends though to be robust (e.g. Lutz et al., Reference Lutz, Schiefele, Wucherpfennig, Rubel and Stulz2016; Wilson, Reference Wilson1998).

Once treatment effectiveness is established, dismantling RCTs can be used to identify active ingredients of treatment. Generalisation studies of various types can then progress things further, to include a wider range of the same population or other populations with similar types of problem. Even in RCTs, attention to individual response is important, not least because it can help identify those who do and do not benefit from treatment. This of course can lead to the systematic identification of treatment failures, who if treated begin the whole process again by the use of single case work.

Does research lead to a better understanding of mechanisms of change?

The history of the development of CBT has focused on at least two strands. Firstly, the development and refinement of treatment strategies, mostly on a pragmatic basis. The second has been investigation of the targets for such therapeutic interventions, varying from disorder specific through to transdiagnostic factors. Thus, the emphasis has been on techniques to bring about change and to understand how best to focus such techniques. These strategies have only tangentially touched on MOC. Although the evidence for the effectiveness of both disorder-specific CBT and transdiagnostic CBT has been established definitively (Fordham et al., Reference Fordham, Sugavanam, Edwards, Stallard, Howard, Nair, Copsey, Lee, Howick, Hemming and Lamb2021; Newby et al., Reference Newby, McKinnon, Kuyken, Gilbody and Dalgleish2015), the mechanisms of change remain poorly understood leaving open the question of which interventions are helpful and which are not (Kazdin, Reference Kazdin2007; Lorenzo-Luaces et al., Reference Lorenzo-Luaces, German and DeRubeis2015; Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Cooper, Hollon and Fairburn2009). The main reasons given for this are:

  1. (1) Emphasis on efficacy studies in the CBT literature.

  2. (2) Lack of statistical procedures to demonstrate the relationship between independent and dependent variables.

  3. (3) Near total absence of studies that establish the timeline of changes where causality of changes is shown.

  4. (4) Failure to identify if CBT specific interventions lead to greater symptom change compared with general interventions like the therapeutic relationship (Kazdin, Reference Kazdin2007; Lemmens et al., Reference Lemmens, Müller, Arntz and Huibers2016; Lorenzo-Luaces et al., Reference Lorenzo-Luaces, German and DeRubeis2015; Lorenzo-Luaces and DeRubeis, Reference Lorenzo-Luaces and DeRubeis2018; Nock, Reference Nock2007).

However, data are accumulating in support of some specific hypothesised mechanisms for individual anxiety disorders and depression (disorder specific) (Donegan and Dugas, Reference Donegan and Dugas2012; Hedman et al., Reference Hedman, Mörtberg, Hesser, Clark, Lekander, Andersson and Ljótsson2013; Hofmann et al., Reference Hofmann, Suvak, Barlow, Shear, Meuret, Rosenfield, Gorman and Woods2007; Kleim et al., Reference Kleim, Grey, Wild, Nussbeck, Stott, Hackmann, Clark and Ehlers2013; Lemmens et al., Reference Lemmens, DeRubeis, Tang, Schulte-Strathaus and Huibers2021; Wilhelm et al., Reference Wilhelm, Berman, Keshaviah, Schwartz and Steketee2015).

Effects of specific interventions versus common factors

Although mechanism of change should necessarily manifest in the occurrence of common factors in therapy, common factors do not necessarily reflect mechanisms of change. For example, therapists typically wear shoes in almost all therapy modalities, making it a common factor but probably not a mechanism of change. There are 16 (and counting) current schools of psychotherapy with different hypothesised MOC, be it treatment specific or general ones (Prochaska and Norcross, Reference Prochaska and Norcross2018). Unfortunately, there is little theoretical or empirical consensus as to whether specific factors, common factors or a combination of both are part of the mechanisms of change in psychological therapy in general (Cuijpers et al., Reference Cuijpers, Reijnders and Huibers2019; DeRubeis et al., Reference DeRubeis, Brotman and Gibbons2005; Hatcher and Barends, Reference Hatcher and Barends2006; Lorenzo-Luaces et al., Reference Lorenzo-Luaces, German and DeRubeis2015). The literature tends to be rather muddled in this respect, with the assumption, for example, that because the therapeutic alliance is commonly considered to be important across psychotherapies, it is a MOC (Ahn and Wampold, Reference Ahn and Wampold2001). It is possible that it could be the other way around, i.e. that common factors (such as therapeutic alliance) are incidental to other MOCs, changing as a consequence of improvement rather than driving it. If someone improves, they are likely to feel more agreeable towards their therapist (and vice versa) than if they do not.

In CBT research, this has caused some to doubt the role of reappraisal of negative or threat-based thinking as a MOC in CBT (Craighead et al., Reference Craighead, Sheets and Bjornsson2005; Kazdin, Reference Kazdin2007; Longmore and Worrell, Reference Longmore and Worrell2007). The main arguments against change in negative or threat-based thoughts as a MOC are: (1) component analyses of cognitive therapy vs non-cognitive therapy have failed to show added effects of cognitive interventions in treatment; and (2) symptom reduction starts before cognitive interventions are introduced in cognitive therapy (Ilardi and Craighead, Reference Ilardi and Craighead1994; Jacobson et al., Reference Jacobson, Dobson, Truax, Addis, Koerner, Gollan, Gortner and Prince1996; Kazdin, Reference Kazdin2007). The conclusion is therefore that non-cognitive factors may cause symptom change in CBT. We suggest that this needs to be considered in a more nuanced way, as described below.

Lorenzo-Luaces and colleagues (Reference Lorenzo-Luaces, German and DeRubeis2015) are one of the few research groups that have addressed this complex question. They argue that the question is not only whether cognition leads to diminished symptomology but also how this cognitive change takes place, how fast and in what environment. They proposed four guiding questions when evaluating cognitive change as a MOC in psychological treatment:

  1. (1) Do cognitive change procedures cause more reduction in symptomology of emotional problems compared with other change procedures?

  2. (2) Do cognitive change procedures cause more change in negative and/or threat-based cognition compared with other change procedures?

  3. (3) Does cognitive change (independent of what change procedures are applied) cause diminished symptomology of emotional problems?

  4. (4) Do cognitive change procedures cause cognitive change, but only when these specific procedures are implemented but not others?

Lorenzo-Luaces and colleagues only found three studies that evaluated these four questions but also simultaneously evaluated temporality of changes and other confounding variables (DeRubeis et al., Reference DeRubeis, Evans, Hollon, Garvey, Grove and Tuason1990; Evans et al., Reference Evans, Rohan, Sitnikov, Mahon, Nillni, Lindsey and Vacek2013; Kuyken et al., Reference Kuyken, Watkins, Holden, White, Taylor, Byford, Evans, Radford, Teasdale and Dalgleish2010). According to Lorenzo-Luaces and colleagues the following question: ‘Does cognitive change (independent of what change procedures are applied) cause diminished symptomology of emotional problems?’ is the primary test of whether cognitive change is a mechanism of change in cognitive therapy for various emotional problems. They argue (and provide data) for the view that cognitive change is in fact the mechanism of change in CBT. They also argue that this cognitive change might arise from different mediating pathways, and that it is irrelevant how this cognitive change is acquired, i.e. irrespective of which of the interventions implemented cause this cognitive change. Similar arguments have been put forward by Beck himself (Beck and Haigh, Reference Beck and Haigh2014; Beck and Dozois, Reference Beck and Dozois2011).

Various concepts for different but helpful processes in psychological treatment

Sauer-Zavala et al. (Reference Sauer-Zavala, Gutner, Farchione, Boettcher, Bullis and Barlow2017) published an article to clarify the literature concerning treatment specific processes vs transdiagnostic processes vs common factors. According to them, it is helpful to divide MOC or processes into three categories: universally applied therapeutic principles, modular interventions (in modular treatments), and finally, shared mechanisms in transdiagnostic treatment.

Universally applied therapeutic principles

Some argue that, where differences in efficacy are in some doubt (the so-called ‘Dodo Bird Verdict’) then the answer to more effective and efficient therapy must lie in the identification of ‘common factors’. The ‘contextual model of common factors’ is said to apply to a broad range of emotional problems (Wampold, Reference Wampold2015). Interventions in this group are described as ‘top-down’ procedures that are applied to a diverse range of emotional problems without requiring or even considering whether these emotional problems are maintained by the same or similar processes, and therefore these approaches are ‘one size fits all’. Accordingly, this is an argument for non-technical transdiagnostic approaches.

Modular interventions

A rather different approach in transdiagnostic treatment is the modular approach or the common element treatment approach. Using modular treatment allows the therapist to develop treatment tailored to each participant suffering mental health problems using an ‘intervention bank’ or ‘buffet of interventions’ (Chorpita et al., Reference Chorpita, Taylor, Francis and Moffitt2004). This echoes the older concept of ‘technical eclecticism’ as can be seen in the unified protocol (Barlow et al., Reference Barlow, Farchione, Sauer-Zavala, Murray-Latin, Ellard, Bullis, Bentley, Boettcher and Cassiello-Robbins2017).

Shared mechanisms

Interventions in this category target common underlying mechanisms across a range of mental health problems or shared mechanisms and are therefore guided by theory about the psychopathology of these emotional problems. These kinds of mechanisms were first defined by Harvey et al. (Reference Harvey, Murray, Chandler and Soehner2011), who argued that shared mechanisms in transdiagnostic treatment can be divided into descriptive transdiagnostic processes vs mechanistic transdiagnostic processes. To clarify the difference, descriptive transdiagnostic processes are processes that are seen in many emotional problems without any shared functional role in their development or maintenance. An example of this is low self-esteem, seen in many emotional problems even though it does not share a functional role in these problems. Mechanistic transdiagnostic processes are those that maintain at least two distinct emotional problems such as pre-occupation with shape and weight in different eating disorders (Fairburn, Reference Fairburn2008). Salkovskis and Forrester (Reference Salkovskis and Forrester2002) differentiate between ‘disorder specific’ and ‘disorder relevant’ factors, which of course carries the implication that there are ‘disorder irrelevant’ factors in theory and therapy.

The core mechanism in effective psychological therapy?

Decades of psychological research in mental health problems leads to a very specific conclusion regarding the nature of such problems, which is that people experience mood problems because they interpret their situation as being more negative or threat-based than it really is. The nature and focus of such an interpretation will determine not only its emotional impact, but also behavioural and cognitive changes driven and motivated by the exaggerated negative meanings. Such changes have the effect of maintaining the negative meanings, so that the person becomes ‘stuck’ in a counterproductive way of understanding and reacting to what is happening to them. For understandable and definable reasons, they become inflexible in the way they respond.

If this is so, then it is reasonable to suggest that what any effective therapy does is to help the person identify and consider alternative, less negative explanations of their situation and their reactions to that situation. That is, good therapy is about empowering the person to understand better how the world really works. In doing so, the person is helped to become more psychologically flexible. That is, the person who has attached a particular meaning to their experience is helped to begin to consider other meanings, which may have quite different implications for how they react. Note that the emphasis here is not on the idea that person should think more rationally or logically; just that they should be able to understand and weigh different perspectives on what they are experiencing. It is important to note that, for the person to give proper consideration to helpful alternative explanations, they have to meet at least two criteria. Firstly, they have to fit with the person’s past experience, and secondly, they need to measure up when actively tested; that is, they also have to survive the person’s future experience either incidentally occurring or as exploratory exercises agreed in therapy, such as behavioural experiments.

Well-conducted CBT measures up extremely well to these requirements. Formulation based and driven CBT requires that the formulation (here, the emerging alternative explanation) be derived from a careful and collaborative understanding of the person’s actual experience where the person’s experience and the formulation are perfectly aligned. The CBT therapist working with the person to derive a formulation draws upon their (empirically grounded) understanding of how that person’s particular problem can be maintained, matching this to the specific patient’s lived experience; for example, in panic attacks, through the particular focus of negative meanings, such as catastrophic misinterpretations of bodily sensations in panic attacks and the safety-seeking behaviours which the person uses to deal with their particular feared consequences (see detailed description of case formulation for panic attacks in Clark and Salkovskis, Reference Clark and Salkovskis2009).

The use of eliciting information and discussion of the derived formulation helps the person to begin to evaluate the less threatening explanation alongside the negative beliefs that are driving their distress. Where information the person is able to identify from their past experience is insufficient to support the alternative account, then the person is encouraged and empowered to gather new information, for example by the use of behavioural experiments (‘don’t trust me, find out for yourself’). The results of such behavioural experiments are then fed into the discussion and directly linked to the evolving formulation, and may indeed modify it. In this way, behavioural and cognitive strategies are fully blended in the service of helping the person to understand how their problem works and how it can be resolved or otherwise diminished.

At its simplest, this can be described as contrasting ‘Theory A’ with ‘Theory B’. Theory A describes the negative meanings driving their psychological problems; for example, in OCD, the father who believes that his intrusive thoughts regarding harming his baby son means that he is dangerous to his family, leading to both avoidance and neutralising to both prevent harm occurring and to ensure that he has done all in his power to make sure that this is so. As part of formulation, Theory B is identified, which is that, as a loving father, he is experiencing intrusive thoughts which he understandably has misinterpreted as meaning that he is a danger to his much-loved son, which has then led to safety-seeking behaviour which he erroneously believes has prevented harm. The person can then be helped review the implications of each perspective and how each of these implications fit with his past experience. In the formulation, attempts to rid himself of his thoughts completely (and other safety-seeking behaviours) are identified as possibly involved in maintaining his difficulties. He is then invited to test the impact of thought suppression in the therapist’s office, then at home. He is then asked what can he learn from doing this? What scenario (suppressing a fearful disgusting thought vs letting it go) is most consistent with ‘Theory A’ or ‘Theory B’? (see detailed description of development of ‘Theory A’ vs ‘Theory B’ for OCD in Bream et al. (Reference Bream, Challacombe, Palmer and Salkovskis2017), and how this integrates with exposure and response prevention in OCD).

We note that purely technical implementation and development of ‘Theory A’ vs ‘Theory B’ is not enough, however. A crucial component of such an approach (and we think crucial to being able to activate this MOC) is the atmosphere of therapy, which emphasises the idea of a collaboration of two experts; that is, the ‘expert’ therapist as someone who has a good empirical understanding of psychological problems (but who knows very little about their patient’s experience), and the patient, who has an excellent understanding of their life and the problems they are experiencing. Good therapy therefore involves the collaboration of these two experts in understanding how the problem (and the world) works and therefore the kind of things which might alleviate and/or eliminate it and allow the person to reclaim areas of their life which have been previously impaired by their psychological difficulties. We therefore believe that therapeutic alliance is highly important, but only when combined with implementation, development and testing of ‘Theory A’ vs ‘Theory B’ and other CBT specialists concur with our assertion (Kazantzis et al., Reference Kazantzis, Dattilio, McGinn, Newman, Persons and Radomsky2018).

We also assert that from a theoretical standpoint this MOC applies to all effective psychological therapies (see below), but that formulation-based CBT is particularly powerful in tapping into it. An important factor is the extent to which the theory or theories guiding a therapist in their efforts to bring about psychological flexibility are well founded and have implications for ways of evaluating the alternative understanding. Purely ‘insight’-focused therapies meet part of the requirements set out, presuming that they draw on the person’s experiences (which they do not always do), but typically do not address the issue of actively evaluating the validity of the formulation which constitutes Theory B. We take the view that it is important to avoid ‘Trust me, I’m your therapist’ and instead adopt the more flexible approach of ‘Don’t trust me on this, instead work with me to find out how things work for you’.

The primary focus in CBT is therefore on building and evaluating Theory B. Sometimes it is also possible to bring about a disconfirmation of Theory A (as in several anxiety disorders). Note, however, that identifying the formulation prior to disconfirmation is helpful both in terms of making it possible for the person to engage in disconfirming behavioural experiments and then to integrate what they have learned from disconfirmation experiences and thereby generalise them. Interested readers are directed to Bennett-Levy et al. (Reference Bennett-Levy, Butler, Fennell, Hackmann, Mueller and Westbook2004), Bream et al. (Reference Bream, Challacombe, Palmer and Salkovskis2017); Clark and Salkovskis (Reference Clark and Salkovskis2009), Salkovskis (Reference Salkovskis1996) and Sighvatsson et al. (Reference Sighvatsson, Salkovskis, Sigurdsson, Valdimarsdottir, Thorsdottir and Sigurdsson2021) for a more detailed description of this process.

It seems likely that effective therapies that do not have such a focus implicitly develop alternatives, and it is the development of more flexible and less negative meanings which accounts for therapeutic change. How about explicitly behavioural approaches, such as exposure as deployed so effectively in phobias? The proposal here is that, although not explicitly targeted in therapy, the success of such approaches requires the patient to discover and shift to an alternative, less threatening understanding of their problem. If an agoraphobic is simply supported to repeatedly confront the situation where panic attacks typically occur, prolonged repetition can result in them realising that the things they fear don’t happen, and that their problem is in fact one of anxiety and avoidance rather than being rooted in the catastrophic consequences they fear. Repeated experiences allow them to shift to an implicit Theory B. Note that the development of the cognitive model of panic occurred in the context of the observation that habituation to avoided situations was typically dissipated by the occurrence of panic attacks (Matthews et al., Reference Matthews, Gelder, Johnston and Johnston1981) and such experiences adversely affected expectations and negative thinking (Rachman, Reference Rachman1991). Even more persuasively, it has been found in both agoraphobia (Salkovskis et al., 1991; Salkovskis et al., Reference Salkovskis, Clark, Hackmann, Wells and Gelder1999) and in social phobia (Wells et al., Reference Wells, Clark, Salkovskis, Ludgate, Hackmann and Gelder1995) that exposure presented as behavioural experiments designed to disconfirm threat beliefs was very markedly superior to exposure alone. Similar principles can be considered with respect to approaches such as behavioural activation; the key question in cognitive behavioural therapies should be ‘what is it that the person learns when they undertake psychotherapy?’. The fact that increasing activities gives the person a sense of mastery and pleasure is likely to shift them from the Theory A perspective shift (e.g. ‘My life is meaningless and not worth living’ to the alternative ‘I feel hopeless because I am not doing enough to give my life meaning’ can be generated by behavioural changes and result in a shift from a ‘stuck’ negative perspective to a more flexible perspective on the reality of the person’s world.

For other forms of psychotherapy, we propose that similar processes apply. Pretty much by definition, a person seeking help for mental health difficulties presents as being stuck in a problematic view of their situation, typically convinced that they are unable to manage what is happening to them and/or who they are. The act of seeking help from mental health services means that the person already has an implicit and often ill-defined Theory B in play (‘Maybe this is a psychological problem?’). In this context the account offered by a health professional who at least appears to be listening has the potential to enhance the person’s understanding (‘I feel this way because … of my childhood experiences … my inner conflicts … trauma I experienced … a biochemical imbalance …’). All psychological therapies are underpinned by a more or less explicit Theory B, and it is in the nature of the evolution of psychotherapies that those available to each will be likely to have some approximation to the person’s past experience, so meeting one of the criteria for a credible Theory B. The extent to which they then stand the test of experience will vary, and some psychotherapies explicitly indicate that for example the person’s symptoms will not be helped by therapy, but that some more profound change may take place over a protracted period of time. Each psychotherapy will incorporate more or less impactful elements, strategies and techniques aimed at achieving psychological and/or behavioural change; the perceived success of such strategies will tend to reinforce (or undermine) the believability of the alternative explanation which the particular therapy deploys. Note that the more empirically grounded a particular therapy is (that is, effective in achieving its stated aims and fulfilling its predictions), the more the person will be convinced by the alternative explanation.

A voyage of discovery: understanding the role of cognitive change, behavioural experiments and therapeutic alliance

Clinical practice is both the target of our work and a rich source of information and inspiration which drives other aspects of the process of empirically grounded clinical approaches. By seeking to understand individuals’ personal stories and their experiences of psychological difficulties, the clinician and researcher alike should be able to form an understanding of the key processes. Sometimes, this understanding may generalise to others with the same or different difficulties. This understanding can be used to generate and apply theory to problems where common processes cut across different presentations. Clinical understanding and theory may also inform the focus of outcome research by suggesting the type of interventions which should be used and which process and outcome measures are likely provide the best information. Taken together, CBT therapists’ main goal should be to alleviate our patients suffering, therefore no matter what CBT therapists do, be it as a scientist or practitioner, your job is to devise an individually tailored treatment that helps your patient to understand how their world really works. We propose that the core mechanism of change involves supporting this understanding through the explicit identification of the problematic negative meaning (‘Theory A’) and its contrast to the shared understanding collaboratively developed as part of formulation (‘Theory B’). The therapeutic alliance is necessary insofar as it facilitates the development of a shared understanding about how the world really works, but it is not sufficient to engender the necessary psychological flexibility in terms of meaning making. Ultimately, the aim of psychological therapy is then to enable the change in perspective on themselves and their problems to lead to the person being able to respond (behave) more flexibly in ways which help them to reclaim key aspects of their self and life. Good psychological therapy helps people to make the transition from vicious circles which trap them to adapted self-maintaining patterns of meaning making and flexible behavioural responding.

We have a problem!

Evaluation of MOC in psychological treatment can be both fruitful (if mechanisms are found), but also at the same time demanding, time-consuming and difficult (Doss, Reference Doss2004; Kazdin, Reference Kazdin2007; Kraemer et al., Reference Kraemer, Wilson, Fairburn and Agras2002; Lorenzo-Luaces, Reference Lorenzo-Luaces2023; Lorenzo-Luaces et al., Reference Lorenzo-Luaces, German and DeRubeis2015). Why is this so? Earlier in this paper, four reasons were provided which offer an agenda for doing things better (Lorenzo-Luaces, Reference Lorenzo-Luaces2023; Lorenzo-Luaces et al., Reference Lorenzo-Luaces, German and DeRubeis2015; Lutz et al., Reference Lutz, de Jong, Rubel and Delgadillo2021; Nock, Reference Nock2007; Tolin et al., Reference Tolin, McKay, Olatunji, Abramowitz and Otto2023):

  1. (1) We should promote the measurement of MOC alongside the evaluation of treatments efficacy (e.g. Kazdin, Reference Kazdin2007; Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Cooper, Hollon and Fairburn2009).

  2. (2) Apply statistical procedures to demonstrate the relationship between independent and dependent variables (e.g. Kazdin and Nock, Reference Kazdin and Nock2003; MacKinnon et al., Reference MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West and Sheets2002), taking into account improvements in methodological and statistical procedures in MOC research (Baldwin and Goldberg, Reference Baldwin, Goldberg, Barkham, Lutz and Castonguay2021; Lutz et al., Reference Lutz, de Jong, Rubel and Delgadillo2021).

  3. (3) To identify if CBT-specific interventions lead to greater symptom change compared with general interventions like the therapeutic relationship as mentioned above (Kazdin, Reference Kazdin2007; Lemmens et al., Reference Lemmens, Müller, Arntz and Huibers2016; Lorenzo-Luaces et al., Reference Lorenzo-Luaces, German and DeRubeis2015; Lorenzo-Luaces and DeRubeis, Reference Lorenzo-Luaces and DeRubeis2018; Nock, Reference Nock2007).

  4. (4) Prioritise studies that establish the timeline of changes where causality of changes is shown.

This is a major problem for psychotherapy process studies (see for example in Feeley et al., Reference Feeley, DeRubeis and Gelfand1999; Lemmens et al., Reference Lemmens, Müller, Arntz and Huibers2016; Lorenzo-Luaces et al., Reference Lorenzo-Luaces, Keefe and DeRubeis2016; Webb et al., Reference Webb, Miles and Sheeran2012); when both process and symptom measures are evaluated typically correlation between these variables rather than temporal precedence is assessed. Kazdin (Reference Kazdin2007) proposed helpful guidelines to demonstrate temporality of changes in psychological treatment, viz:

  1. (1) Data have to show that the treatment causes the proposed mediator variable to change, which consequently causes the outcome (diminished symptomology), but not the other way around. The only way to achieve this is to evaluate process and symptom measures multiple times during treatment (e.g. Collins and Graham, Reference Collins and Graham2002; Johansson and Høglend, Reference Johansson and Høglend2007; Kazdin and Nock, Reference Kazdin and Nock2003; Kraemer et al., Reference Kraemer, Wilson, Fairburn and Agras2002; Lemmens et al., Reference Lemmens, Müller, Arntz and Huibers2016; Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Cooper, Hollon and Fairburn2009).

  2. (2) Demonstration of specificity of the mediator–outcome relations under study, but not others.

  3. (3) Rule out other mediator–outcome relations. This can only be done in strict experimental conditions.

  4. (4) Divergent and multiple evaluations of mediator–outcome relations (consistency).

  5. (5) Demonstration of gradient effect (greater activation of the hypothesised process causes greater change in symptom measures).

  6. (6) Data collection has to be performed from multiple sources.

Where should we go from here?

We strongly encourage researchers to follow the guidelines put forward by Kazdin (Reference Kazdin2007). As can be clearly be seen in Lorenzo-Luaces (Reference Lorenzo-Luaces2023) and Lemmens et al. (Reference Lemmens, Müller, Arntz and Huibers2016), the literature is far from reaching that goal. It is no longer sufficient to exclusively evaluate symptoms and process measures pre–post treatment. Alternative technologies such as sudden gains research are also promising (Tang and DeRubeis, Reference Tang and DeRubeis1999). This is a matter of behaviour change in the research community and if one thing is what we should be good at, it is gaining cognitive flexibility and changing behaviour. We have all kinds of methods and statistical procedures to evaluate this like multi-trait-multi-method approaches, item response theory approach, bi–factor model approach in factor analysis or network analysis to name a few (Lutz et al., Reference Lutz, de Jong, Rubel and Delgadillo2021). To follow this requirement also calls for a changed behaviour in when we evaluate process and symptom measures. New technology like ecological momentary assessment might be used more often instead of the traditional method of having our patients answering with paper and pencil at the treatment site. If we look at the second and third requirement (demonstration of specificity of the mediator–outcome relations and rule out other mediator–outcome relations) the only sensible way to evaluate this is under strict experimental conditions. This has been done and replicated in emotion regulation studies (Gross, Reference Gross1998; Gross and John, Reference Gross and John2003; Webb et al., Reference Webb, Miles and Sheeran2012); why not CBT? If we look at the final requirement (data collection from multiple sources), a number of methodological procedures are available as a substitute of RCTs; to name a few: qualitative methods (Levitt et al., Reference Levitt, McLeod, Stiles, Barkham, Lutz and Castonguay2021), experimental, randomised, non-randomised and mixed methods (Baldwin and Goldberg, Reference Baldwin, Goldberg, Barkham, Lutz and Castonguay2021) and finally single case experimental designs (Barlow and Nock, Reference Barlow and Nock2009).

Conclusion

The effectiveness of psychological therapies for a range of psychological issues has been established beyond doubt. This has been achieved in the context of an applied science, where the science is theoretically driven and its application, although harder to define, is empirically grounded and person-centred. That is, good psychological therapy involves a skilful blend of clinical art and clinical science. The aim of such good therapy is, as far as possible, to work collaboratively with the person we seek to empower them to examine their situation in a flexible way in order to understand how their world works. It is not about fostering positive thinking regardless, and is often about how to manage best in difficult circumstances through identifying where psychological flexibility has been lost and how some flexibility might be restored. CBT and other therapies need to build such considerations into their evolution in order to serve better those who seek our help.

Data availability statement

Data availability is not applicable to this article as no new data were created or analysed in this study.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to the many colleagues and service users who have contributed to our understanding of the matters described here. We particularly appreciate the inspiring work of Professor A.T. Beck, who has helped so many to understand how the world really works to the benefit of all.

Author contributions

Paul Salkovskis: Conceptualization (lead), Project administration (lead), Writing – original draft (lead), Writing – review & editing (lead); Magnús Sighvatsson: Writing – original draft (supporting), Writing – review & editing (supporting); Jón Sigurðsson: Writing – original draft (supporting), Writing – review & editing (supporting).

Financial support

None.

Competing interests

Paul Salkovskis is Editor of Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy. He was not involved in the review or editorial process for this paper, on which he is listed as an author.

Ethical standards

The authors have abided by the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct as set out by the BABCP and BPS.

References

Abramowitz, J. S. (2013). The practice of exposure therapy: relevance of cognitive-behavioral theory and extinction theory. Behavior Therapy, 44, 548558. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2013.03.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahn, H., & Wampold, B. (2001). Where oh where are the specific ingredients? A meta-analysis of component studies in counselling and psychotherapy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 48, 251257. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.48.3.251 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, S., & Goldberg, S. (2021). Methodological foundations and innovations in quantitative psychotherapy research. In Barkham, M., Lutz, W., & Castonguay, L. G. (eds), Bergin and Garfield’s Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change (pp. 1951). Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Barlow, D. H., Farchione, T. J., Sauer-Zavala, S., Murray-Latin, H., Ellard, K., Bullis, J. R., Bentley, K. H., Boettcher, H. T., & Cassiello-Robbins, C. (2017). Unified Protocol for Transdiagnostic Treatment of Emotional Disorders: Therapist Guide (2nd edn). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barlow, D. H., & Nock, M. K. (2009). Why can’t we be more idiographic in our research? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 1921. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01088.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T. (2006). How an anomalous finding led to a new system of psychotherapy. Nature Medicine, 12, 11391141. https://doi.org/10.1038/nm1006-1139 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T. (2019). A 60-year evolution of cognitive theory and therapy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14, 1620. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691618804187 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T., & Dozois, D. J. A. (2011). Cognitive therapy: current status and future directions. Annual Review of Medicine, 62, 397409. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-med-052209-100032 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A., & Haigh, E. (2014). Advances in cognitive theory and therapy: the generic cognitive model. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153734 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett-Levy, J., Butler, G., Fennell, M., Hackmann, A., Mueller, M., & Westbook, D. (2004). Oxford Guide to Behavioral Experiments in Cognitive Therapy. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brander, G., Pérez-Vigil, A., Larsson, H., & Mataix-Cols, D. (2016). Systematic review of environmental risk factors for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a proposed roadmap from association to causation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 65, 3662. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bream, V., Challacombe, F., Palmer, A., & Salkovskis, P. M. (2017). Cognitive Behaviour Therapy for Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breger, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (1965). Critque and reformulation of ‘learning-theory’ approaches to psychotherapy and neurosis. Psychlogical Bulletin, 63, 338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canvin, L. K., Janecka, M., & Clark, D. M. (2016). Focussing attention on oneself increases the perception of being observed by others. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 7, 160171. https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.047315 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, J. K., Andrews, L. A., Witcraft, S. M., Powers, M. B., Smits, J. A. J., & Hofmann, S. G. (2018). Cognitive behavioral therapy for anxiety and related disorders: a meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials. Depression and Anxiety, 35, 502514. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22728 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chambless, D. L., & Ollendick, T. H. (2001). Empirically supported psychological interventions: controversies and evidence. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 685716. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.685 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of B.F. Skinner’s verbal behavior. Language, 35, 2658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chorpita, B. E., Taylor, A. A., Francis, S. E., & Moffitt, C. (2004). Efficacy of modular cognitive behavior therapy for childhood anxiety disorders. Behavior Therapy, 35, 263287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (2011). Cognitive Therapy of Anxiety Disorders: Science and Practice (updated edition). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Clark, D. A., Beck, A., & Alford, B. (1999). Scientific Foundations of Cognitive Theory and Therapy of Depression. Wiley.Google Scholar
Clark, D. M. (1999). Anxiety disorders: why they persist and how to treat them. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, S5S27. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00048-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, D. M. (2004). Developing new treatments: on the interplay between theories, experimental science and clinical innovation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 10891104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.05.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, D. M., & Salkovskis, P. M. (2009). Panic Disorder: Manual for Improving Access to Psychological Therapy (IAPT) High Intensity CBT Therapists. Google Scholar
Clark, D. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (1982). Diurnal variation in clinical depression and accessibility of memories of positive and negative experiences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 8795. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.91.2.87 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collard, J. J. (2019). ACT vs CBT: an exercise in idiosyncratic language. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 12, 126145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, L. M., & Graham, J. W. (2002). The effect of the timing and spacing of observations in longitudinal studies of tobacco and other drug use: temporal design considerations. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 68, S8596. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0376-8716(02)00217-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craighead, W. E., Sheets, E. S., & Bjornsson, A. S. (2005). Specificity and nonspecificity in psychotherapy. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 12, 189193. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bpi024 Google Scholar
Craske, M. (2015). Optimizing exposure therapy for anxiety disorders: an inhibitory learning and inhibitory regulation approach. Verhaltenstherapie, 25, 134143. https://doi.org/10.1159/000381574 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craske, M. G., Kircanski, K., Zelikowsky, M., Mystkowski, J., Chowdhury, N., & Baker, A. (2008). Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 527. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cristea, I. A., Gentili, C., Cotet, C. D., Palomba, D., Barbui, C., & Cuijpers, P. (2017). Efficacy of psychotherapies for borderline personality disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 74, 319. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.4287 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuijpers, P., Miguel, C., Harrer, M., Plessen, C. Y., Ciharova, M., Ebert, D., & Karyotaki, E. (2023). Cognitive behavior therapy vs. control conditions, other psychotherapies, pharmacotherapies and combined treatment for depression: a comprehensive meta-analysis including 409 trials with 52,702 patients. World Psychiatry, 22, 105115. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.21069 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuijpers, P., Reijnders, M., & Huibers, M. J. H. (2019). The role of common factors in psychotherapy outcomes. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 15, 207231. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095424 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
David, D., Cristea, I., & Hofmann, S. G. (2018). Why cognitive behavioral therapy is the current gold standard of psychotherapy. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Houwer, J. (2020). Revisiting classical conditioning as a model for anxiety disorders: a conceptual analysis and brief review. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 127, 103558. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2020.103558 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeRubeis, R. J., Brotman, M. A., & Gibbons, C. J. (2005). A conceptual and methodological analysis of the nonspecifics argument. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 12, 174183. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bpi022 Google Scholar
DeRubeis, R. J., Evans, M. D., Hollon, S. D., Garvey, M. J., Grove, W. M., & Tuason, V. B. (1990). How does cognitive therapy work? Cognitive change and symptom change in cognitive therapy and pharmacotherapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 862869. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006x.58.6.862 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dimidjian, S., Barrera, M., Martell, C., Muñoz, R. F., & Lewinsohn, P. M. (2011). The origins and current status of behavioral activation treatments for depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 7, 138. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104535 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donegan, E., & Dugas, M. (2012). Generalized anxiety disorder: a comparison of symptom change in adults receiving cognitive-behavioral therapy or applied relaxation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80, 490496. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028132 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doss, B. D. (2004). Changing the way we study change in psychotherapy. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 368386. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bph094 Google Scholar
Dozois, D., Dobson, K., & Rnic, K. (2019). Historical and philosophical bases of the cognitive-behavioral therapies. In Handbook of Cognitive-Behavioral Therapies (4th edn, pp. 332). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Emmelkamp, P. M. (1987). Failures in Behavior Therapy. Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Evans, M., Rohan, K. J., Sitnikov, L., Mahon, J. N., Nillni, Y. I., Lindsey, K. T., & Vacek, P. M. (2013). Cognitive change across cognitive-behavioral and light therapy treatments for seasonal affective disorder: what accounts for clinical status the next winter? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-013-9561-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, H. (1952). The Effects of Psychotherapy: An Evaluation. PsycNET. https://doi.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Fh0063633 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fairburn, C. G. (2008). Cognitive Behavior Therapy and Eating Disorders (pp. xii, 324). Guilford Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Feeley, M., DeRubeis, R. J., & Gelfand, L. A. (1999). The temporal relation of adherence and alliance to symptom change in cognitive therapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 578582. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.67.4.578 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferster, C. B. (1973). A functional analysis of depression. American Psychologist, 28, 857870. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035605 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 2035. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.20 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, E. B., & McNally, R. (1996). Mechanism of change in exposure therapy. In Rapee, R. M. (ed), Current Controversies in the Anxiety Disorders (pp. 329342). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Fordham, B., Sugavanam, T., Edwards, K., Stallard, P., Howard, R., Nair, R. das, Copsey, B., Lee, H., Howick, J., Hemming, K., & Lamb, S. E. (2021). The evidence for cognitive behavioural therapy in any condition, population or context: a meta-review of systematic reviews and panoramic meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 51, 2129. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720005292 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankenhuis, W. E., & Nettle, D. (2018). Open science is liberating and can foster creativity. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13, 439447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271299. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348362. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, A. G., Murray, G., Chandler, R. A., & Soehner, A. (2011). Sleep disturbance as transdiagnostic: consideration of neurobiological mechanisms. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 225235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, A., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive behavioural processes across psychological disorders: a transdiagnostic approach to research and treatment. In Cognitive Behavioural Processes across Psychological Disorders. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatcher, R., & Barends, A. (2006). How a return to theory could help alliance research. Psychotherapy, 43, 292299. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-3204.43.3.292 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, S. C., Barlow, D. H., & Nelson-Gray, R. O. (1999). The Scientist Practitioner: Research and Accountability in the Age of Managed Care (2nd edn, pp. x, 438). Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Hedman, E., Mörtberg, E., Hesser, H., Clark, D. M., Lekander, M., Andersson, E., & Ljótsson, B. (2013). Mediators in psychological treatment of social anxiety disorder: Individual cognitive therapy compared to cognitive behavioral group therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51, 696705. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2013.07.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, S. G., Asnaani, A., Vonk, I. J. J., Sawyer, A. T., & Fang, A. (2012). The efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy: a review of meta-analyses. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36, 427440. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-012-9476-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, S. G., Suvak, M. K., Barlow, D. H., Shear, M. K., Meuret, A. E., Rosenfield, D., Gorman, J. M., & Woods, S. W. (2007). Preliminary evidence for cognitive mediation during cognitive-behavioral therapy of panic disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 374379. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.75.3.374 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollon, S. D. (2022). Depression. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 29, 507510. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2022.02.016 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ilardi, S. S., & Craighead, W. E. (1994). The role of nonspecific factors in cognitive-behavior therapy for depression. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 1, 138155. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2850.1994.tb00016.x Google Scholar
Jacobson, N. S., Dobson, K. S., Truax, P. A., Addis, M. E., Koerner, K., Gollan, J. K., Gortner, E., & Prince, S. E. (1996). A component analysis of cognitive-behavioral treatment for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 295304. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006x.64.2.295 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janssen, N. P., Hendriks, G.-J., Baranelli, C. T., Lucassen, P., Oude Voshaar, R., Spijker, J., & Huibers, M. J. H. (2021). How does behavioural activation work? A systematic review of the evidence on potential mediators. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 90, 8593. https://doi.org/10.1159/000509820 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johansson, P., & Høglend, P. (2007). Identifying mechanisms of change in psychotherapy: mediators of treatment outcome. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 14, 19. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.514 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, D. (2006). All about me. New Scientist, 191, 2836. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0262-4079(06)60170-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kazantzis, N., Dattilio, F. M., McGinn, L. K., Newman, C. F., Persons, J. B., & Radomsky, A. S. (2018). Defining the role and function of the therapeutic relationship in cognitive behavioral therapy: a modified Delphi panel. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 11, 158183. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41811-018-0014-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kazdin, A. E. (2007). Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 127. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kazdin, A. E. (2017). Parent management training and problem-solving skills training for child and adolescent conduct problems. In Weisz, J. R., & Kazdin, A. E. (eds), Evidence-Based Psychotherapies for Children and Adolescents (3rd edn, pp. 142158). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kazdin, A. E., & Nock, M. K. (2003). Delineating mechanisms of change in child and adolescent therapy: methodological issues and research recommendations. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 11161129. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00195 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleim, B., Grey, N., Wild, J., Nussbeck, F. W., Stott, R., Hackmann, A., Clark, D. M., & Ehlers, A. (2013). Cognitive change predicts symptom reduction with cognitive therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 81, 383393. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031290 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraemer, H. C., Wilson, G. T., Fairburn, C. G., & Agras, W. S. (2002). Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 877883. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.59.10.877 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuyken, W., Watkins, E., Holden, E., White, K., Taylor, R. S., Byford, S., Evans, A., Radford, S., Teasdale, J. D., & Dalgleish, T. (2010). How does mindfulness-based cognitive therapy work? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 11051112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.08.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemmens, L. H. J. M., DeRubeis, R. J., Tang, T. Z., Schulte-Strathaus, J. C. C., & Huibers, M. J. H. (2021). Therapy processes associated with sudden gains in cognitive therapy for depression: exploring therapeutic changes in the sessions surrounding the gains. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 143. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.576432 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemmens, L. H. J. M., Müller, V. N. L. S., Arntz, A., & Huibers, M. J. H. (2016). Mechanisms of change in psychotherapy for depression: an empirical update and evaluation of research aimed at identifying psychological mediators. Clinical Psychology Review, 50, 95107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.09.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levitt, H., McLeod, J., & Stiles, W. (2021). The conceptualization, design, and evaluation of qualitative methods in research in psychotherapy. In Barkham, M., Lutz, W., & Castonguay, L. G. (eds), Bergin and Garfield’s Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change (pp. 5187). Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Lewinsohn, P. M. (1974). A behavioral approach to depression. In The Psychology of Depression: Contemporary Theory and Research (pp. xvii, 318xvii, 318). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Lilienfeld, S. O., Aslinger, E., Marshall, J., & Satel, S. (2018). Neurohype: a field guide to exaggerated brain-based claims. In The Routledge Handbook of Neuroethics (pp. 241261). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315708652-18 Google Scholar
Longmore, R. J., & Worrell, M. (2007). Do we need to challenge thoughts in cognitive behavior therapy? Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 173187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.08.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorenzo-Luaces, L. (2023). Identifying active ingredients in cognitive-behavioral therapies: What if we didn’t? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 168, 104365. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2023.104365 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorenzo-Luaces, L., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2018). Miles to go before we sleep: advancing the understanding of psychotherapy by modeling complex processes. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 42, 212217. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-018-9893-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenzo-Luaces, L., German, R. E., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2015). It’s complicated: the relation between cognitive change procedures, cognitive change, and symptom change in cognitive therapy for depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 41, 315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.12.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorenzo-Luaces, L., Keefe, J. R., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2016). Cognitive-behavioral therapy: nature and relation to non-cognitive behavioral therapy. Behavior Therapy, 47, 785803. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.02.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lutz, W., de Jong, K., Rubel, J., & Delgadillo, J. (2021). Measuring, predicting, and tracking change in psychotherapy. In Bergin and Garfield’s Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change (pp. 89135). Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Lutz, W., Schiefele, A.-K., Wucherpfennig, F., Rubel, J., & Stulz, N. (2016). Clinical effectiveness of cognitive behavioral therapy for depression in routine care: a propensity score based comparison between randomized controlled trials and clinical practice. Journal of Affective Disorders, 189, 150158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.08.072 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83104. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.83 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackintosh, N. (2003). Pavlov and associationism. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 6, 177184. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1138741600005321 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maier, S. F., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2016). Learned helplessness at fifty: insights from neuroscience. Psychological Review, 123, 349367. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000033 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malik, K., Ibrahim, M., Bernstein, A., Venkatesh, R. K., Rai, T., Chorpita, B., & Patel, V. (2021). Behavioral activation as an ‘active ingredient’ of interventions addressing depression and anxiety among young people: a systematic review and evidence synthesis. BMC Psychology, 9, 150. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-021-00655-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marks, I. M. (1973). Reduction of fear: towards a unifying theory. Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal, 18, 912. https://doi.org/10.1177/070674377301800103 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martell, C. R., Addis, M. E., & Jacobson, N. S. (2001). Depression in Context: Strategies for Guided Action (pp. xxx, 223). W W Norton & Co.Google Scholar
Matthews, A. M., Gelder, M. G., Johnston, D. W., & Johnston, D. (1981). Agoraphobia: Nature and Treatment. Tavistock.Google Scholar
McLoughlin, S., & Roche, B. T. (2023). ACT: a process-based therapy in search of a process. Behavior Therapy, 54, 939955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Millard, L. A., Wan, M. W., Smith, D. M., & Wittkowski, A. (2023). The effectiveness of compassion focused therapy with clinical populations: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 326, 168192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2023.01.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, R., Cooper, Z., Hollon, S. D., & Fairburn, C. G. (2009). How do psychological treatments work? Investigating mediators of change. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newby, J. M., McKinnon, A., Kuyken, W., Gilbody, S., & Dalgleish, T. (2015). Systematic review and meta-analysis of transdiagnostic psychological treatments for anxiety and depressive disorders in adulthood. Clinical Psychology Review, 40, 91110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2015.06.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nock, M. K. (2007). Conceptual and design essentials for evaluating mechanisms of change. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 31, 4s12s. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00488.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nour, M. M., Liu, Y., & Dolan, R. J. (2022). Functional neuroimaging in psychiatry and case for failing better. Neuron, 110, 25242544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Otto, M., & Hofmann, S. G. (2010). Avoiding Treatment Failures in the Anxiety Disorders (Series in Anxiety and Related Disorders). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paul, G. L. (1967). Strategy of outcome research in psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 31, 109118. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0024436 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, G. L., & Shannon, D. T. (1966). Treatment of anxiety through systematic desensitization in therapy groups. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 71, 124135. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0023172 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavlov, I. (2003). Conditioned Reflexes. Dover Publications.Google Scholar
Piet, J., & Hougaard, E. (2011). The effect of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for prevention of relapse in recurrent major depressive disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 10321040. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.05.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prochaska, J. O., & Norcross, J. C. (2018). Systems of Psychotherapy: A Transtheoretical Analysis (9th edn). Oxford University Press, USA.Google Scholar
Rachman, S. (1977). The conditioning theory of fear acquistion: a critical examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 375387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rachman, S. (1991). The consequences of panic. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 5, 187197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rachman, S. (2009). Psychological treatment of anxiety: the evalution of behavior therapy and cognitive behavior therapy. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5, 97119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rachman, S. (2015). The evolution of behavior therapy and cognitive behaviour therapy. Behavior Research and Therapy, 64, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachman, S., De Silva, P., & Roper, G. (1976). The spontaneous decay of compulsive urges. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 14, 445453. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(76)90091-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ravitz, P., Watson, P., Lawson, A., Constantino, M. J., Bernecker, S., Park, J., & Swartz, H. A. (2019). Interpersonal psychotherapy: a scoping review and historical perspective (1974–2017). Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 27, 165180. https://doi.org/10.1097/HRP.0000000000000219 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, R. A. (1988). It’s not what you think it is. American Psychologist, 15.Google Scholar
Salkovskis, P. M. (1986). The cognitive revolution: new way forward, backward somersault or full circle? Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 14, 278282. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0141347300014890 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salkovskis, P. M., Clark, D. M., & Hackmann, A. (1991). Treatment of panic attacks using cognitive therapy without exposure or breathing retraining. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29, 161166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salkovskis, P. M. (1996). The cognitive approach to anxiety: threat beliefs, safety seeking behaviour, and the special case of health anxiety and obsessions. In Frontiers of Cognitive Therapy (pp. 4875). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Salkovskis, P. M. (2002). Emprically grounded clinical interventions: cognitive-behavioural therapy progress through a multi-dimenstional approach to clinical science. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 30, 39. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465802001029 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salkovskis, P. M., Clark, D. M., Hackmann, A., Wells, A., & Gelder, M. G. (1999). An experimental investigation of the role of safety-seeking behaviours in the maintenance of panic disorder with agoraphobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 559574. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00153-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salkovskis, P.M., & Forrester, E. (2002). Responsibility. In Cognitive Approaches to Obsessions and Compulsions (pp. 4561).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Satel, S., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2013). Brainwashed: The Seductive Appeal of Mindless Neuroscience. https://www.amazon.com/Brainwashed-Seductive-Appeal-Mindless-Neuroscience/dp/0465062911 Google Scholar
Sauer-Zavala, S., Gutner, C. A., Farchione, T. J., Boettcher, H. T., Bullis, J. R., & Barlow, D. H. (2017). Current definitions of ‘transdiagnostic’ in treatment development: a search for consensus. Behavior Therapy, 48, 128138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.09.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapiro, M. B. (1961). The single case in fundamental clinical psychological research. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 34, 255262. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1961.tb00949.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapiro, M. B. (1985). A reassessment of clinical psychology as an applied science. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 24, 111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1985.tb01308.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sighvatsson, M. B., Salkovskis, P. M., Sigurdsson, E., Valdimarsdottir, H. B., Thorsdottir, F., & Sigurdsson, J. F. (2021). Evaluation of mechanism of change in transdiagnostic cognitive behaviour therapy using single case experimental design. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 71, 101634. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2020.101634 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior (pp. xi, 478). Appleton-Century-Crofts. https://doi.org/10.1037/11256-000 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, A. T., Carl, E., Cuijpers, P., Karyotaki, E., & Smits, J. A. J. (2021). Looking beyond depression: a meta-analysis of the effect of behavioral activation on depression, anxiety, and activation. Psychological Medicine, 51, 14911504. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720000239 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinert, C., Munder, T., Rabung, S., Hoyer, J., & Leichsenring, F. (2017). Psychodynamic therapy: as efficacious as other empirically supported treatments? A meta-analysis testing equivalence of outcomes. American Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 943953. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17010057 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tang, T. Z., & DeRubeis, R. J. (1999). Sudden gains and critical sessions in cognitive-behavioral therapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 894904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolin, D. F., McKay, D., Olatunji, B. O., Abramowitz, J. S., & Otto, M. W. (2023). On the importance of identifying mechanisms and active ingredients of psychological treatments. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 170, 104425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2023.104425 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tryon, W. W. (2008). Whatever happened to symptom substitution? Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 963968. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2008.02.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vitacco, M. J., Gottfried, E., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Batastini, A. (2020). The limited relevance of neuroimaging in insanity evaluations. Neuroethics, 13, 249260. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-019-09421-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wampold, B. E. (2015). How important are the common factors in psychotherapy? An update. World Psychiatry, 14, 270277. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20238 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webb, T. L., Miles, E., & Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: a meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 775808. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027600 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, A., Clark, D. M., Salkovskis, P. M., Ludgate, J., Hackmann, A., & Gelder, M. (1995). Social phobia: the role of in-situation safety behaviours in maintaining anxiety and negative beliefs. Behavior Therapy, 26, 153161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wild, J., Clark, D. M., Ehlers, A., & McManus, F. (2008). Perception of arousal in social anxiety: effects of false feedback during a social interaction. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39, 102116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.11.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilhelm, S., Berman, N. C., Keshaviah, A., Schwartz, R. A., & Steketee, G. (2015). Mechanisms of change in cognitive therapy for obsessive compulsive disorder: role of maladaptive beliefs and schemas. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 65, 510. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2014.12.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, G. T. (1998). The clinical utility of randomized controlled trials. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 24, 1329.3.0.CO;2-3>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by Reciprocal Inhibition (pp. xiv, 239). Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Development of empirically grounded clinical interventions.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Research designs and the development of psychological treatments.

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.