Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T05:39:10.414Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Measuring dissociation across adolescence and adulthood: developing the short-form Černis Felt Sense of Anomaly scale (ČEFSA-14)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 November 2023

Emma Černis*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham, UK Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Institute for Mental Health, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham, UK
Bao S. Loe
Affiliation:
The Psychometrics Centre, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Katie Lofthouse
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK University of East Anglia, Norwich Medical School, Norwich, UK
Polly Waite
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Andrew Molodynski
Affiliation:
Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
Anke Ehlers
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Centre for Anxiety Disorders and Trauma, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Daniel Freeman
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
*
Corresponding author: Emma Černis; Email: e.cernis@bham.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Dissociation may be important across many mental health disorders, but has been variously conceptualised and measured. We introduced a conceptualisation of a common type of dissociative experience, ‘felt sense of anomaly’ (FSA), and developed a corresponding measure, the Černis Felt Sense of Anomaly (ČEFSA) scale.

Aims:

We aimed to develop a short-form version of the ČEFSA that is valid for adolescent and adult respondents.

Method:

Data were collected from 1031 adult NHS patients with psychosis and 932 adult and 1233 adolescent non-clinical online survey respondents. Local structural equation modelling (LSEM) was used to establish measurement invariance of items across the age range. Ant colony optimisation (ACO) was used to produce a 14-item short-form measure. Finally, the expected test score function derived from item response theory modelling guided the establishment of interpretive scoring ranges.

Results:

LSEM indicated 25 items of the original 35-item ČEFSA were age invariant. They were also invariant across gender and clinical status. ACO of these items produced a 14-item short-form (ČEFSA-14) with excellent psychometric properties (CFI=0.992; TLI=0.987; RMSEA=0.034; SRMR=0.017; Cronbach’s alpha=0.92). Score ranges were established based on the expected test scores at approximately 0.7, 1.25 and 2.0 theta (equivalent to standard deviations above the mean). Scores of 29 and above may indicate elevated levels of FSA-dissociation.

Conclusions:

The ČEFSA-14 is a psychometrically valid measure of FSA-dissociation for adolescents and adults. It can be used with clinical and non-clinical respondents. It could be used by clinicians as an initial tool to explore dissociation with their clients.

Type
Main
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies

Introduction

Dissociative symptoms – including highly subjective anomalous experiences of finding one’s own body unreal, memories absent, or surroundings as unexpectedly unfamiliar – are increasingly being considered from a multi-dimensional perspective (e.g. Černis et al., Reference Černis, Beierl, Molodynski, Ehlers and Freeman2021; Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, Brown, Mansell, Fearon, Hunter, Frasquilho and Oakley2005). In contrast to the unidimensional approach which posits that such experiences represent ‘manifestations of a single underlying process’ of dissociation (Briere et al., Reference Briere, Weathers and Runtz2005; p. 222), the multidimensional approach suggests that clusters of dissociative phenomenological experience form separable constructs. Feasibly, these constructs may have different aetiologies and require different treatment approaches (Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, Brown, Mansell, Fearon, Hunter, Frasquilho and Oakley2005), and thus greater precision is required in the measurement of dissociative experiences, if we wish to disentangle the broader construct of ‘dissociation’ into its multiple dimensions.

We recently delineated one such dimension: ‘felt sense of anomaly’-type dissociation (FSA-dissociation; Černis et al., Reference Černis, Beierl, Molodynski, Ehlers and Freeman2021). Developed de novo using a ‘bottom-up’, phenomenological approach, FSA-dissociation describes dissociative symptoms which have in common a subjective sense of ‘strangeness’ or anomaly, including feelings of (unexpected) unreality, unfamiliarity, automaticity, disconnection, or absence in relation to one’s body, mind, mood, perception, identity, behaviour, or external environment. In this way, FSA-dissociation overlaps phenomenologically with depersonalisation-derealisation (DPDR), as both DPDR and FSA-dissociation encompass ‘strange’ and ‘unreal’ feelings relating to the self and one’s surroundings. Recently, the two have been demonstrated to be highly correlated, but not identical (Lofthouse et al., Reference Lofthouse, Waite and Černis2023). Corresponding measures for FSA-dissociation – the Černis Felt Sense of Anomaly (ČEFSA) scale and the more general Global Felt Sense of Anomaly (GFSA) scale – were developed with a group of 8861 online survey respondents and validated with a group of 1031 patients with psychosis (Černis et al., Reference Černis, Beierl, Molodynski, Ehlers and Freeman2021). This analysis demonstrated that the scales had highly promising psychometric properties (model fit, internal reliability, test–retest reliability, and convergent validity) in both clinical (psychosis) and general population adult groups.

However, as noted in that analysis, further development of the scale is required. In particular, the current version of the scale is not validated for use with respondents below the age of 18. This is important, given that the high-risk period for the first incidence of severe mental illness spans this divide (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Amminger, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Lee and Ustun2007; Uhlhaas et al., Reference Uhlhaas, Davey, Mehta, Shah, Torous, Allen, Avenevoli, Bella-Awusah, Chanen, Chen, Correll, Do, Fisher, Frangou, Hickie, Keshavan, Konrad, Lee, Liu and Wood2023), and is commonly cited as occurring between mid-teens and mid-to-late twenties (e.g. 14–29 years in ultra high risk for psychosis (Waite et al., Reference Waite, Kabir, Johns, Mollison, Tsiachristas, Petit, Černis, Maughan and Freeman2020; Yung et al., Reference Yung, Yung, Pan Yuen, Mcgorry, Phillips, Kelly, Dell’olio, Francey, Cosgrave, Killackey, Stanford, Godfrey and Buckby2005); and 15–24 years for bipolar at-risk (Bechdolf., Reference Bechdolf, Nelson, Cotton, Chanen, Thompson, Kettle, Conus, Amminger, Yung, Berk and McGorry2010). Thus, research and clinical services tailored to groups spanning this nominal divide (e.g. early intervention in psychosis services; and ‘0 to 25 years’ services) require measures that are valid for the age of the client, and using one measure – regardless of age – may be more convenient than adopting a child and adult version of the same measure. This will also permit easier comparison of a client’s scores over time if they move through the age-18 divide during the observation or treatment period.

Therefore, in the present study, we sought to improve the original 35-item ČEFSA scale in three key ways. First, we aimed to expand the utility of the scale beyond its initial validation in adults aged 18 and above by establishing its validity in an age range of 13 years and above. We also assessed its measurement invariance across key demographics. The invariance of the scale across respondent characteristics, such as gender, clinical status, and ethnicity, is important to establish, to ensure it is appropriate for as wide a population as possible. Finally, we also aimed to provide a short-form version of the scale with accompanying scoring guide to improve ease of administration and interpretation. We used three main state-of-the-art statistical approaches to realise these aims.

Measurement invariance: multi-group confirmatory factor analyses and local structural equation modelling (LSEM)

If a scale possesses measurement invariance, it is considered to measure the same construct across specified characteristics (e.g. across ethnicity, gender, or age). This may be reflected in between-group stability of: the scale’s factor structure (configural invariance), the factor loadings of items (metric or ‘weak’ invariance), and/or item intercepts (scalar or ‘strong’ invariance). Measurement invariance is important, as it tests the assumption made when comparing groups that the latent trait has been captured as accurately, and in the same manner, for all groups (Olaru and Jankowsky, Reference Olaru and Jankowsky2022). Therefore, we tested the scalar invariance of the items in the original ČEFSA to determine whether they are interpreted the same way, regardless of age, gender, or clinical status.

For categorical demographics (e.g. clinical status), we employed the conventional approach of measurement invariance based on binary grouping categories to evaluate for group differences. However, for age – a continuous variable – we used LSEM to test measurement invariance. Often, continuous variables are forced into arbitrary categories for the purposes of testing for measurement invariance (e.g. 13–18 years, 19–64 years, 65 years and above). This has several limitations; primarily, that a great deal of information can be lost in this method, as data points that are close together on a continuous scale (e.g. 18 years and 19 years) may be treated as very different from each other if separated into different groups (Hildebrandt et al., Reference Hildebrandt, Lüdtke, Robitzsch, Sommer and Wilhelm2016). LSEM instead weights observations around focal points, such that closer points are weighted most heavily, and further points least heavily – following the assumption that points (i.e. ages) that are closer together will be more similar than points (ages) that are further apart (Olaru et al., Reference Olaru, Schroeders, Hartung and Wilhelm2019). Weighting and including observations around the focal points enables an increase in the effective sample size for each focal point. Thus, by determining a narrow ‘step’ between focal points, and repeating the weighting process across the full range of the observed values, LSEM offers more precise estimates and retains far more information about the behaviour of the latent trait across the (age) range.

Development of a short-form scale: ant colony optimisation (ACO)

There is no single accepted way to create a short-form version of an established measure, and all methods suffer from limitations of some kind. Olaru et al. (Reference Olaru, Schroeders, Hartung and Wilhelm2019) outline three challenges: first, that selecting items based on item-level characteristics often sacrifices the quality of the scale-level properties of the measure; second, that scale-level properties change as items are included or removed; and finally, that a suitable balance must be struck between these properties. They therefore argue that the optimal item selection method must be ‘combinatorial’ (as opposed to stepwise), and must take into account multiple scale-level properties simultaneously. Ant colony optimisation (ACO) is a method that iteratively selects combinations of items, and weights items that contribute to the quality of that selection’s scale-level properties such that they are more likely to be re-selected in the next round (Olaru et al., Reference Olaru, Schroeders, Hartung and Wilhelm2019). This method may not have the most optimal variance explained (Olaru et al., Reference Olaru, Schroeders, Hartung and Wilhelm2019), but does produce a strong model fit result and does not suffer from the ‘suppressor effect’ (highly correlated items) that methods such as regularised structural equation modelling suffers from. Therefore, ACO was used to develop a psychometrically valid short-form of the ČEFSA.

Establishing a scoring guide: expected test score function

An interpretive scoring guide for the new short-form version of the scale was produced by consulting the expected test score function derived from an item response theory (IRT) model. IRT allows the calculation of parameters that give further information about the performance of a scale with respect to the latent trait that the scale measures (in this case, FSA-dissociation). The latent trait is represented by values of ‘theta’ along a continuum whereby lower severity of the latent trait is indicated by lower levels of theta, and higher severity by higher theta values. Determining which total score on the scale would be expected to correspond with an ‘average’ level of theta enables the identification of score ranges that would correspond with elevated, moderately severe, or severe theta levels (i.e. FSA-dissociation levels), thus producing an illustrative scoring guide. This method has been used previously (e.g. Bird et al., Reference Bird, Loe, Kirkham, Fergusson, Shearn, Stratford, Teale, Waite and Freeman2020), and is therefore appropriate to the aims of this study.

Method

Participants and procedure

To assess a broad spectrum of responses, data from three groups were combined: non-clinical adolescent, non-clinical adult, and clinical (adults with psychosis diagnoses).

Non-clinical (community) adolescent data participants were recruited via UK schools as well as social media advertising. Consent procedures also took place online (Qualtrics, 2020): participants aged 13 to 15 years provided assent to participate following parental informed consent, and participants aged 16 to 18 years provided informed consent. Inclusion criteria were: aged 13 to 18 years, and resident in the UK. Data were collected between 5 and 25 November 2021. Participants were not asked about their mental health history or status. Full details are available in Lofthouse et al. (Reference Lofthouse, Waite and Černis2023).

Non-clinical adult participants were recruited via Facebook advertisements to participate in an online cross-sectional self-report questionnaire study (Černis et al., Reference Černis, Ehlers and Freeman2022a). Informed consent and assessment were both carried out online using Qualtrics (Qualtrics, 2019). Surveys were accessible on desktop and mobile web browsers. Inclusion criteria were: age 18 or above, and usually resident in the UK. Data were collected between 30 January 2019 and 25 February 2019. Note that this group is distinct from that used for the original measure development by Černis et al. (Reference Černis, Beierl, Molodynski, Ehlers and Freeman2021). In the present study, only data from respondents who reported no current or previous mental health difficulties were retained.

Clinical participants were recruited by Clinical Research Network (CRN) research assistants and clinical studies officers embedded in clinical and research teams across 36 NHS trusts in England to participate in a cross-sectional self-report questionnaire study (Černis et al., Reference Černis, Molodynski, Ehlers and Freeman2022b). Informed consent was obtained by CRN staff. Inclusion criteria were: age 16 years or over, currently under the care of an NHS mental health service, with a diagnosis of non-affective psychosis, and willing and able to give informed consent to participate. Exclusion criteria were: insufficient English language to complete the questionnaires even with support, or an affective psychosis diagnosis (e.g. psychotic depression, bipolar disorder). Data were collected between 18 October 2019 and 19 March 2020. Note that this is the same clinical group whose data were used for scale validation by Černis et al. (Reference Černis, Beierl, Molodynski, Ehlers and Freeman2021).

Selecting participants with less than or equal to 20% missing data on the ČEFSA scale resulted in data being excluded from 211 adolescent, 30 non-clinical adult, and seven clinical participants. Thus, the final dataset included from 1233 adolescent online survey respondents, 932 adult online survey respondents, and 1031 NHS patients with non-affective psychosis diagnoses, resulting in a sample of 3196 responses for analysis. This exceeds the criteria required for structural equation modelling (Bentler and Chou, Reference Bentler and Chou1987; Wolf et al., Reference Wolf, Harrington, Clark and Miller2013). Whilst guidelines regarding adequate sample size for ACO do not currently exist, this sample size was determined to be more than sufficient, given that this method has been demonstrated to cope well with major mis-specifications at a sixth of the sample size (Koğar, Reference Koğar2022).

Measures

The Černis Felt Sense of Anomaly scale (ČEFSA; Černis et al., Reference Černis, Beierl, Molodynski, Ehlers and Freeman2021) measures dissociative experiences sharing a core phenomenological experience of a felt sense of anomaly (FSA) using 35 items. The 35 items form seven factors, each of five items. Factors are: Anomalous Experience of the Self (e.g. ‘I feel like a stranger to myself’), Anomalous Experience of the Body (e.g. ‘My body feels numb’), Anomalous Experiences of Emotion (‘I don’t fully experience emotions’), Altered Sense of Familiarity (‘Places that I know seem unfamiliar’), Altered Sense of Connection (‘I feel detached from what I’m doing’), Altered Sense of Agency (‘I don’t notice how much time passes’), and Altered Sense of Reality (‘I feel like other people aren’t real’). Items are rated for the past 2 weeks on a Likert scale from 0 (‘never’) to 4 (‘always’). Higher scores indicate higher levels of FSA-dissociation.

The ČEFSA has good convergent validity with the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES-II; Carlson and Putnam, Reference Carlson and Putnam1993) (r=0.802, p<0.001; Černis et al., Reference Černis, Beierl, Molodynski, Ehlers and Freeman2021), and high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha of 0.98 in the current participant group).

Statistical analysis

All analyses were carried out in R version 4.2.1 (R Core Team, 2022) using the following packages: sirt (v.3.12-66; Robitzsch, Reference Robitzsch2019), lavaan (v.0.6-14; Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012), psych (v.2.2.9; Revelle, Reference Revelle2022), mirt (v.1.37.1; Chalmers, Reference Chalmers2012). Levels of missing data were low (0.13%) and were replaced via multiple imputation using the mice package (v.3.15.0; van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Reference van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn2011).

Evaluating measurement invariance across participant characteristics

To test for measurement invariance across categorical participant characteristics (e.g. gender and clinical status), multi-group confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) were carried out and different levels of measurement invariance were compared. First, we estimated the factor structure without parameter constraints across groups (the configural model). Next, we estimated the weak (or metric) model by constraining the factor loadings to be equal across groups. Finally, we estimated the strong (scalar) model where both the factor loadings and item intercepts were constrained to be equal across groups. To determine whether there was a significant difference between the nested models, we applied the criteria outlined by Chen (Reference Chen2007) for large sample sizes. Following these, invariance cannot be assumed if there is a difference of ≥–0.01 in CFI, ≥0.015 in RMSEA, or ≥0.030 in SRMR between models.

For the continuous variable of age, we used local structural equation modelling (LSEM). Following Olaru et al. (Reference Olaru, Schroeders, Hartung and Wilhelm2019), the R package sirt was used to apply LSEM to the data with each age in the observed range (13–74 years) set as a focal point, with a bandwidth parameter (h) of 2. As Olaru et al. (Reference Olaru, Schroeders, Hartung and Wilhelm2019) describe, the bandwidth parameter artificially inflates the sample size as the weighted samples include the observations around each focal point. This creates an ‘effective’ sample size (N eff) that is larger than the observed sample size. Hildebrandt et al. (Reference Hildebrandt, Lüdtke, Robitzsch, Sommer and Wilhelm2016) showed that a recommended bandwidth of 2 is sufficiently accurate to detect differences in the model while reducing the effects of noise.

In this analysis, the data were treated as continuous given that items contained five response categories (Rhemtulla et al., Reference Rhemtulla, Brosseau-Liard and Savalei2012). A maximum likelihood estimator was used, and the standard errors were estimated using a bootstrap approach. As LSEM does not allow assessment of the effect of the continuous variable (age) on model parameters, a permutation test using 1000 permutations was carried out to test the null hypothesis that the parameters do not differ significantly across the age range (Allemand et al., Reference Allemand, Olaru and Hill2021; Hildebrandt et al., Reference Hildebrandt, Lüdtke, Robitzsch, Sommer and Wilhelm2016).

Developing a short-form version of the ČEFSA

After the analysis of measurement invariance, the ant colony optimisation (ACO) was used to select the resulting items for a short-form version of the ČEFSA scale. The method used in this study follows the tutorial outlined by Olaru et al. (Reference Olaru, Schroeders, Hartung and Wilhelm2019), including the advice to compare results ‘across several runs of the algorithm’ ‘in order to approach optimal solutions’ (p. 403). The method outlined in this tutorial requires manual specification of the number of items to be retained by the ACO analysis. A priori discussion between E.Č., B.S.L. and D.F. reached the conclusion that, if feasible following rejection of non-invariant items, then a 14-item version of the scale (two items in each of the seven factors) would be a good compromise between scale length (aiming for fewer than 20 items) and phenomenological breadth (encompassing the fullest possible range of FSA-dissociative experiences).

To ensure that the findings are robust, ACO was performed 10 times with 30 iterations and 50 ants per run, and the best fitting result across multiple runs were assessed against the optimal model fit criteria of CFI>0.95 and RMSEA<0.06 (Hu and Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999). We randomly split the dataset equally into two subsamples (training and validation) to avoid overfitting and to ensure the robustness of the item selection procedure. In the first step, we employed the ACO algorithm on the training sample to identify the best solution. In the second step, we conducted further analysis on the recommended solution using the validation sample. Specifically, we estimated the correlated factor structure of the best solution using CFA on the validation sample to evaluate the robustness of the model fit. We then performed multi-group CFA across the two subsamples to ensure parameter equivalence of the model.

Developing a scoring guide for the short-form version of the ČEFSA

Finally, the expected test score function was calculated, derived from the higher order factor of a second-order IRT model. This type of IRT model is mathematically equivalent to the Testlet IRT model and is a restricted version of the bi-factor model, which allows for the examination of the higher order factor based on the shared variance among multiple primary constructs (Rijmen, Reference Rijmen2009).

Using IRT modelling to obtain the expected test function enables interpretative score guides to be established at different points across the severity spectrum (e.g. Bird et al., Reference Bird, Loe, Kirkham, Fergusson, Shearn, Stratford, Teale, Waite and Freeman2020). To establish our scoring guide, the theta range between the minimum and maximum possible test score was considered, and expected test scores at various theta values above average were inspected. The aim was to create four approximately even scoring ranges with boundaries reflective of relative severity on the basis of the spread of expected test scores (i.e. standard deviations above average).

Results

Participant characteristics

The mean age of the whole group was 34.9 years (range=13–74; SD=18.6). The group consisted mainly of female (57.73%) and White (82.57%) participants (Table 1). However, there was significant heterogeneity between participant group types, with significant differences between all groups for age, gender distribution, and ČEFSA mean scores, and a significant difference between clinical and non-clinical participants with respect to ethnicity distribution.

Table 1. Demographic details and difference tests for the final group (n=3196)

ČEFSA, Černis Felt Sense of Anomaly scale. Tukey’s HSD test results: *all three groups differ significantly from each other; **the clinical group differs significantly from the two non-clinical groups, but the non-clinical groups do not differ significantly from each other.

Measurement invariance

Due to relatively low numbers of participants in ethnic groups other than White, it was not possible to carry out a test of measurement invariance across ethnicity. Therefore, measurement invariance (MI) was tested across gender, clinical status and age. Within the category of gender, too few participants identified as ‘other’ [n=163 (5.10%)] and therefore MI was tested only between female and male respondents.

Categorical participant characteristics (gender, participant group)

Indicating that CFA was appropriate in this dataset, the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin value was 0.98, and Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (χ2=5608.85, p<0.001, d.f.=595).

Model fit statistics for a correlated 7-factor structure of the scale data for all participants were adequate given the large sample size (χ2=5041.58, d.f.=553, p<0.001, CFI=0.929, TLI=0.923, RMSEA=0.051, SRMR=0.043).

Invariance across participant groups

The configural model (see Table 2 for all model fit statistics) showed good model fit, indicating the factor structure is invariant across patients with psychosis diagnoses, and adult and adolescent online respondents.

Table 2. Showing the model fit indices for all invariance models for the demographics variables

*These models are based on retaining only 25 of the original 35 ČEFSA items. K, number of parameters; d.f., degrees of freedom; AIC, Akaike Information Criteria; BIC, Bayesian Information Criteria: CFI, comparative fit index; RMSEA, root mean square error of approximation; SRMR, standardised root mean square residual.

Next, weak (metric) and strong (scalar) models were estimated (Table 2). However, 10 items were found to have sufficiently large item means that they were affecting model fit comparison results. As there is no accepted convention for dealing with invariant items (Putnick and Bornstein, Reference Putnick and Bornstein2016), we opted to discard these items in order to achieve MI at the scalar level. Therefore, these models are based on a 25-item version of the ČEFSA (see Supplementary material). (Note that, when tested, the model fit for these 25 items was found to be superior to the full 35-item scale: χ2=1815.992, d.f.=300, p<0.001, CFI=0.964, TLI=0.957, RMSEA=0.0439, SRMR=0.0272.)

Comparison of model fit statistics against the aforementioned criteria (see ‘Statistical analysis’ section) indicated that the differences between the weak and strong models were sufficiently small to indicate item invariance across participant groups. Furthermore, the strong model had the lowest BIC value of the three models, indicating that scalar invariance can be assumed.

Invariance across gender

The same reduced subset of 25 items used for the analysis of participant groups was used to assess configural, weak and strong levels of MI across male and female gender (Table 2). Again, the magnitude of the differences between weak and strong model indices indicate invariance across gender groups, as does the small BIC value for the strong model relative to the other nested models.

Invariance across age

The average weighted sample size across the LSEMs estimated at each age in the configural model was n eff =811.108 [min=216.258 (age 74); max=1401.994 (age 17)].

Measurement invariance of the 25 items across the age range was assessed at configural (CFI=0.956, RMSEA=0.054, SRMR=0.032), weak (CFI=0.956, RMSEA=0.052, SRMR=0.037), and strong (CFI=0.955, RMSEA=0.052, SRMR=0.037) levels (Table 2). Again, the magnitude of differences in the fit indices between models indicates measurement invariance at the strong level: i.e. item intercepts can be considered invariant across age. (For graphs of individual item intercepts over the age range, see Supplementary material.)

As the scale is invariant at the item level, factor means were calculated to identify any trends across the age range. This analysis revealed that factor means did not remain stable over age (Fig. 1). Rather, there was a steep decline in mean scores as age increased. Between mid- to late-teens and approximately 30 years of age, factor means dropped by approximately one standard deviation over the 15 years. This trend continued at a shallower rate from 30 years of age to the end of the sampled range: factor means dropped by approximately half a standard deviation over this 30-year span. This pattern was true for all seven factors, but particularly pronounced in the ‘Altered Sense of Reality’ (‘Unreal’) factor (Fig. 1). (Visual inspection of the mean raw scores for each factor across age (see Supplementary material) confirmed this pattern was not an artefact of LSEM sampling.)

Figure 1. Factor means of the 25-item version of the ČEFSA scale. The continuous black line shows point estimates at each age point. The dashed grey lines represent the 95% confidence interval for each point estimate. Blue lines show the linear approximation.

Key: ‘Self’, Anomalous Experience of the Self factor; ‘Body’, Anomalous Experience of the Body factor; ‘Unfamiliar’, Altered Sense of Familiarity factor; ‘Emotion’, Anomalous Experience of Emotion factor; ‘Detached’, Altered Sense of Connection factor; ‘Passive’, Altered Sense of Agency factor; ‘Unreal’, Altered Sense of Reality factor.

Note, however, that caution is required when interpreting results around 65 years and above (and at the lowest end of the observed age-range), as the lack of sample size availability reduces the fitness of the model at these extremes. Illustrating this, plotting the model fit indices showed a stable trend as age increased, until approximately age 65 years, when drop-off of fitness rapidly declined (see Supplementary material).

Confirming the pattern of each factor showing a decline in factor mean score as age increased, the results of the permutation test demonstrated a significant difference between observed and permuted trends, i.e. that the null hypothesis must be rejected (Table 3). The permutation test included 62 focal points for age.

Table 3. Permutation test for LSEM (conducted with h=2)

M, weighted average of the parameter function; SD, test statistic of the permutation test; p (SD), p-value of the permutation test.

Development of the short-form ČEFSA (ČEFSA-14)

The final item set selected by ACO is shown in the Appendix (see Supplementary material). Two items were selected from each of the seven factors to produce a multifactorial 14-item scale.

The final 14-item scale achieved the optimal fit criteria. The results showed excellent model fit in both the training (χ2=6050.17; d.f.=56; p=0.003; CFI=0.995; TLI=0.992; RMSEA=0.026; SRMR=0.013) and validation (χ2=6334.76; d.f.=91; p<0.001; CFI=0.992; TLI=0.987; RMSEA=0.034; SRMR=0.017) sample. Cronbach’s alpha was excellent (0.92) in this sample. Inspection of model fit statistics for the configural, metric and scalar models of the short-form scale indicated that it is invariant at the scalar level [CFIΔ<0.001; RMSEAΔ<0.001 (scalar versus metric); CFIΔ<0.001; RMSEAΔ=0.001 (metric versus configural)]. For factor loadings, see Supplementary material.

Establishing a scoring guide for the ČEFSA-14

The second-order factor IRT model showed good fit to the data (CFI=0.992; TLI=0.987; RMESA=0.0354; SRMR=0.172), indicating that calculating a total score from the 14-item ČEFSA scale is appropriate and that measurement invariance assessment for categorical variables could proceed. Using the coefficients from this model, expected test score was estimated for the full range of theta (FSA-dissociation severity) (Fig. 2). This indicated that an average level of FSA-dissociation (i.e. theta=0) would correspond to a score of 17.96 out of a maximum of 56 on the short-form ČEFSA. The expected score would be 26.42 at 0.5SD above the average (theta=0.5), 35.07 at 1SD (theta=1), 43.07 at 1.5SD, 49.55 at 2SD, 53.36 at 2.5SD, and 55.11 at 3SD. This latter score indicates that the maximum score on the short-form ČEFSA reflects levels of FSA-dissociation approximately three standard deviations above average levels.

Figure 2. Plot of expected test score (on the 14-item ČEFSA) across the range of theta (i.e. FSA-dissociation severity).

The correlation of the raw scores with the general factor scores from the IRT model were very high (0.978), indicating that it was appropriate to use expected test scores to establish scoring guidance. Therefore, we developed interpretive score guides (Table 4). Following Bird et al. (Reference Bird, Loe, Kirkham, Fergusson, Shearn, Stratford, Teale, Waite and Freeman2020), we set the ‘high’ or ‘severe’ range to incorporate test scores where dissociation is two standard deviations above the mean (i.e. theta=2.0), and the first clinically significant range (i.e. the end of the ‘average’ range) to begin at theta=0.7. The expected test score at theta=2.0 is 49.55, thus a scoring boundary of 49 and above was set for ‘severe’. The expected test score at theta=0.7 was 29.91, thus a scoring boundary of 28 and below was set for ‘average’.

Table 4. Interpretive score ranges for the 14-item short-form version of the Černis Felt Sense of Anomaly scale (ČEFSA-14) sum score

The range of scores between 0.7 and 2.0 theta were then divided equally to produce an ‘elevated’ and ‘moderately severe’ range. This resulted in a boundary at approximately theta=1.25, or an expected test score of 39.15: therefore, a score of 38 was judged to be the highest possible score of the ‘elevated’ category, and 39 the lowest boundary of the ‘moderately severe’ category. Table 4 also reports the proportions of these four categories in each of the participant groups.

Discussion

The precise measurement of dissociative phenomena is important. Conceptual confusion has been a key barrier to progress in this area. Here, we present the ČEFSA-14: a brief and psychometrically robust measure of a specific subtype of dissociative experience [felt sense of anomaly-type dissociation (FSA-dissociation)].

A key advantage of this short-form measure is its measure invariance. This means that there are no systematic differences in the way the ČEFSA-14 measures FSA-dissociation across gender (male/female), or clinical status (psychosis diagnosis/general population). Crucially, the scale is also invariant across age over the range of 13 years to approximately 65 years. Although participants slightly older than this were included in our analyses, caution is required at this upper age limit due to the rapid decline in model fit above 65 years resulting from insufficient data from older adults in our group. Nevertheless, this property of the ČEFSA-14 makes it a valuable tool for investigating dissociative experiences across the aforementioned traditional (age 18 years) boundary between ‘child and adolescent’ and ‘adult’ groups. This confers significant benefits for research studies and healthcare services serving groups that span this divide, and in contexts where a longer-term course of work may mean that the respondent crosses this age-boundary during the treatment or measurement period.

To clarify the benefits and limitations of both versions (the original 35- and short-form 14-item) of the ČEFSA scale, our analysis suggests that whilst the original 35-item version of the ČEFSA has been demonstrated to be psychometrically valid for ages 18 and above (Černis et al., Reference Černis, Beierl, Molodynski, Ehlers and Freeman2021), it is not as psychometrically robust as the interim 25-item model (Supplementary material) presented here, as 10 items in the original ČEFSA may be interpreted differently by different genders, ages, and by people with different clinical statuses. The original version is therefore a good option where the aim is to explore in further detail, with an adult respondent, the nature of their dissociative experiences, without the need to interpret their value score on the measure. Adding to the utility of the original scale as a tool to explore clients’ phenomenological experiences, the factors may be treated as separable constructs within the original version of the scale, whilst in the short form, they have not been validated in this way. The short-form version presented here may therefore be most appropriate to use with younger ages (i.e. from 13 upwards), in services or studies with age ranges spanning 18 years, or where it may be helpful to interpret the sum score (e.g. in clinical research or audit).

However, it is also important to note an interesting finding raised by our age invariance analysis: that age is a moderating factor of dissociation across time. It was a common pattern for FSA-dissociation to be higher at younger ages, dropping rapidly between mid-teens and early-30s, before declining more slowly from this point until the upper age limit of the scale. This finding underscores the importance of validating the ČEFSA measure below age 18 years, and reflects previous similar results. For example, Ross et al. (Reference Ross, Ryan, Anderson, Ross and Hardy1989) report that in a group of 168 school and college students, Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES; Carlson and Putnam, Reference Carlson and Putnam1993) scores were higher in adolescents and appeared to decline with increasing age. More recently, Tolmunen et al. (Reference Tolmunen, Maaranen, Hintikka, Kylmä, Rissanen, Honkalampi, Haukijärvi and Laukkanen2007) reported a comparable finding using the adolescent version of the DES, noting that ‘although statistically significant, the difference may not have clinical relevance […] our findings may reflect [normative] development’ (p. 616).

For this reason, and due to the method used to develop them, the interpretive scoring guides for the ČEFSA-14 presented here should be used with clinical judgement, and should not be referred to as a ‘clinical cut-off’. Indeed, they may be better understood as a preliminary guide to be updated in futureFootnote 1 following the collection of data where dissociation levels have been clinically validated [e.g. via use of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Dissociative Disorders (SCID-D); Steinberg, Reference Steinberg1994]. Using high-quality data of this kind to establish a more robust scoring guide is the next step in validating this measure. Given the age-related trend, future further validation of this scale may also seek to include longitudinal data to determine how scoring ranges may be interpreted intra-individually over time.

There are further limitations to the work presented here. We did not have sufficient data to test for invariance across ethnicity, nor to include the ‘other’ gender group in our test for invariance across gender. Therefore, data with adequate representation of ethnic and gender minorities are also required for the next step in further developing and validating the ČEFSA-14 measure. As noted above, there were also less data available at the upper end of the age range, meaning that further data collection is required to confidently validate the observed continued downward trend in frequency of dissociative experiences in older adults. Additionally, future research may wish to explore the expression of dissociation at this age, to understand whether there are any qualitative differences, as well as quantitative, in comparison with dissociation in adults and adolescents.

Nonetheless, the brief scale presented here offers a valid and effective way for researchers to measure FSA-dissociation across the age-18 divide, and for clinicians to interpret the levels of such phenomena in their clients whilst opening up conversation about these difficult-to-describe, distressing experiences.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465823000498

Data availability statement

The authors do not have participant consent to share the data collected in this study.

Acknowledgements

We thank the staff and patients of the NHS trusts that participated in the ‘Exploring Unusual Feelings’ study, and the community groups and schools that took part in the adolescent survey.

Author contribution

Emma Černis: Conceptualization (lead), Data curation (equal), Formal analysis (supporting), Funding acquisition (equal), Investigation (lead), Methodology (equal), Project administration (lead), Visualization (equal), Writing – original draft (lead), Writing – review & editing (equal); Bao Loe: Conceptualization (supporting), Data curation (equal), Formal analysis (lead), Methodology (equal), Software (lead), Visualization (equal), Writing – original draft (supporting), Writing – review & editing (equal); Katie Lofthouse: Data curation (supporting), Funding acquisition (supporting), Investigation (supporting), Project administration (supporting), Resources (supporting), Writing – review & editing (equal); Polly Waite: Project administration (supporting), Resources (supporting), Writing – review & editing (equal); Andrew Molodynski: Project administration (supporting), Resources (supporting), Writing – review & editing (supporting); Anke Ehlers: Supervision (equal), Writing – review & editing (supporting); Daniel Freeman: Conceptualization (supporting), Funding acquisition (equal), Methodology (supporting), Resources (equal), Supervision (lead), Writing – original draft (supporting), Writing – review & editing (equal).

Financial support

The work was supported by the Wellcome Trust via a Clinical Doctoral Fellowship to E.Č. (grant number 102176/B/13/Z). Additionally, the community adolescent data were collected as part of K.L.’s undergraduate degree and was supported by funding from the University of Oxford Department of Experimental Psychology and University College, University of Oxford. P.W. is funded by an NIHR Development and Skills Enhancement Award (NIHR 302843). D.F. is an NIHR Senior Investigator and supported by the Oxford Health NIHR Biomedical Research Centre. A.E. is funded by the Wellcome Trust (200796) and supported by the Oxford Health NIHR Biomedical Research Centre and a NIHR Senior Investigator Award. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the National Health Service, the NIHR, or Department of Health and Social Care.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethical standard

Authors abided by the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct as set out by the BABCP and BPS. Ethical approval was obtained for non-clinical data collection from the University of Oxford Central University Research Ethics Committee (adult study: REF: R61315/RE001; adolescent study: R71497/RE001), and for clinical data collection from the NHS London (City & East) Research Ethics Committee (REF: 19/LO/1394).

Footnotes

1 To easily communicate updates to the ČEFSA scale and scoring guide, we have created an Open Science Framework page. Please see https://osf.io/t2re7/ for the most up-to-date information.

References

Allemand, M., Olaru, G., & Hill, P. L. (2021). Age-related psychometrics and differences in gratitude and future time perspective across adulthood. Personality and Individual Differences, 182, 111086. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111086 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechdolf, A., Nelson, B., Cotton, S. M., Chanen, A., Thompson, A., Kettle, J., Conus, P., Amminger, G. P., Yung, A. R., Berk, M., & McGorry, P. D. (2010). A preliminary evaluation of the validity of at-risk criteria for bipolar disorders in help-seeking adolescents and young adults. Journal of Affective Disorders, 127, 316320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2010.06.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentler, P. M., & Chou, C.-P. (1987). Practical issues in structural modeling. Sociological Methods & Research, 16, 78117. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124187016001004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, J. C., Loe, B. S., Kirkham, M., Fergusson, E. C., Shearn, C., Stratford, H., Teale, A.-L., Waite, F., & Freeman, D. (2020). The assessment of paranoia in young people: item and test properties of the Bird Checklist of Adolescent Paranoia. Schizophrenia Research, 220, 116122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2020.03.046 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briere, J., Weathers, F. W., & Runtz, M. (2005). Is dissociation a multidimensional construct? Data from the Multiscale Dissociation Inventory. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18, 221231. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, E. B., & Putnam, F. W. (1993). An update on the Dissociative Experiences Scale. Dissociation, 6, 1627.Google Scholar
Černis, E., Beierl, E., Molodynski, A., Ehlers, A., & Freeman, D. (2021). A new perspective and assessment measure for common dissociative experiences: ‘Felt Sense of Anomaly’. PLOS One, 16, e0247037. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0247037 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Černis, E., Ehlers, A., & Freeman, D. (2022a). Psychological mechanisms connected to dissociation: generating hypotheses using network analyses. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 148, 165173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.01.049 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Černis, E., Molodynski, A., Ehlers, A., & Freeman, D. (2022b). Dissociation in patients with non-affective psychosis: prevalence, symptom associations, and maintenance factors. Schizophrenia Research, 239, 1118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2021.11.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chalmers, R. P. (2012). mirt: a Multidimensional Item Response Theory package for the R environment. Journal of Statistical Software, 48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i06 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14, 464504. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701301834 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hildebrandt, A., Lüdtke, O., Robitzsch, A., Sommer, C., & Wilhelm, O. (2016). Exploring factor model parameters across continuous variables with local structural equation models. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 51, 257258. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2016.1142856 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, E., Brown, R., Mansell, W., Fearon, R., Hunter, E., Frasquilho, F., & Oakley, D. (2005). Are there two qualitatively distinct forms of dissociation? A review and some clinical implications. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2004.08.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Amminger, G. P., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Lee, S., & Ustun, T. B. (2007). Age of onset of mental disorders: a review of recent literature: Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 20, 359364. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0b013e32816ebc8c CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koğar, H. (2022). Which scale short form development method is better? A comparison of ACO, TS, and SCOFA. International Journal of Assessment Tools in Education, 9, 583592. https://doi.org/10.21449/ijate.946231 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lofthouse, M. K., Waite, P., & Černis, E. (2023). Developing an understanding of the relationship between anxiety and dissociation in adolescence. Psychiatry Research, 324, 115219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2023.115219 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olaru, G., & Jankowsky, K. (2022). The HEX-ACO-18: developing an age-invariant HEXACO short scale using ant colony optimization. Journal of Personality Assessment, 104, 435446. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2021.1934480 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olaru, G., Schroeders, U., Hartung, J., & Wilhelm, O. (2019). Ant colony optimization and local weighted structural equation modeling. a tutorial on novel item and person sampling procedures for personality research. European Journal of Personality, 33(3), 400419. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2195 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnick, D. L., & Bornstein, M. H. (2016). Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: the state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Developmental Review, 41, 7190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qualtrics (2019). [Computer software]. www.qualtrics.com Google Scholar
Qualtrics (2020). [Computer software]. www.qualtrics.com Google Scholar
R Core Team (2022). R: A language and enivronment for statistical computing. [Computer software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. www.R-project.org Google Scholar
Revelle, W. (2022). psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research (2.2.9) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych Google Scholar
Rhemtulla, M., Brosseau-Liard, P. É., & Savalei, V. (2012). When can categorical variables be treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychological Methods, 17, 354373. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029315 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rijmen, F. (2009). Three multidimensional models for Testlet-based tests: formal relations and an empirical comparison. ETS Research Report Series, 2009, i–13. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2333-8504.2009.tb02194.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robitzsch, A. (2019). sirt: Supplementary item response theory models (version 3) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=sirt Google Scholar
Ross, C. A., Ryan, L., Anderson, G., Ross, D., & Hardy, L. (1989). Dissociative experiences in adolescents and college students. Dissociation, 2, 239242.Google Scholar
Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: an R Package for Structural Equation Modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, M. (1994). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV dissociative disorders (SCID-D) (rev). American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Tolmunen, T., Maaranen, P., Hintikka, J., Kylmä, J., Rissanen, M.-L., Honkalampi, K., Haukijärvi, T., & Laukkanen, E. (2007). Dissociation in a general population of Finnish adolescents. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 195, 614617. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e318093f487 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uhlhaas, P. J., Davey, C. G., Mehta, U. M., Shah, J., Torous, J., Allen, N. B., Avenevoli, S., Bella-Awusah, T., Chanen, A., Chen, E. Y. H., Correll, C. U., Do, K. Q., Fisher, H. L., Frangou, S., Hickie, I. B., Keshavan, M. S., Konrad, K., Lee, F. S., Liu, C. H., … & Wood, S. J. (2023). Towards a youth mental health paradigm: a perspective and roadmap. Molecular Psychiatry, 111. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-023-02202-z Google ScholarPubMed
van Buuren, S., & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v045.i03 Google Scholar
Waite, F., Kabir, T., Johns, L., Mollison, J., Tsiachristas, A., Petit, A., Černis, E., Maughan, D., & Freeman, D. (2020). Treating sleep problems in young people at ultra-high-risk of psychosis: study protocol for a single-blind parallel group randomised controlled feasibility trial (SleepWell). BMJ Open, 10, e045235. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2020-045235 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, E. J., Harrington, K. M., Clark, S. L., & Miller, M. W. (2013). Sample size requirements for structural equation models: an evaluation of power, bias, and solution propriety. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 73, 913934. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164413495237 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yung, A. R., Yung, A. R., Pan Yuen, H., Mcgorry, P. D., Phillips, L. J., Kelly, D., Dell’olio, M., Francey, S. M., Cosgrave, E. M., Killackey, E., Stanford, C., Godfrey, K., & Buckby, J. (2005). Mapping the onset of psychosis: the comprehensive assessment of at-risk mental states. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 39, 964971. https://doi.org/10.1080/j.1440-1614.2005.01714.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic details and difference tests for the final group (n=3196)

Figure 1

Table 2. Showing the model fit indices for all invariance models for the demographics variables

Figure 2

Figure 1. Factor means of the 25-item version of the ČEFSA scale. The continuous black line shows point estimates at each age point. The dashed grey lines represent the 95% confidence interval for each point estimate. Blue lines show the linear approximation.Key: ‘Self’, Anomalous Experience of the Self factor; ‘Body’, Anomalous Experience of the Body factor; ‘Unfamiliar’, Altered Sense of Familiarity factor; ‘Emotion’, Anomalous Experience of Emotion factor; ‘Detached’, Altered Sense of Connection factor; ‘Passive’, Altered Sense of Agency factor; ‘Unreal’, Altered Sense of Reality factor.

Figure 3

Table 3. Permutation test for LSEM (conducted with h=2)

Figure 4

Figure 2. Plot of expected test score (on the 14-item ČEFSA) across the range of theta (i.e. FSA-dissociation severity).

Figure 5

Table 4. Interpretive score ranges for the 14-item short-form version of the Černis Felt Sense of Anomaly scale (ČEFSA-14) sum score

Supplementary material: File

Černis et al. supplementary material

Černis et al. supplementary material 1

Download Černis et al. supplementary material(File)
File 45.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Černis et al. supplementary material

Černis et al. supplementary material 2

Download Černis et al. supplementary material(File)
File 9.2 MB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.