INTRODUCTION
Difficulties concentrating and maintaining engagement, mental fatigue, and ‘brain fog’ are commonly reported in a wide range of psychiatric, neurodevelopmental, and neurological conditions, including depression (Deng, Li, & Tang, Reference Deng, Li and Tang2014), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) stroke (Rueckert & Grafman, Reference Rueckert and Grafman1996), Alzheimer’s disease (Perry, Watson, & Hodges, Reference Perry, Watson and Hodges2000), and, most recently, following COVID-19 (Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Lu, Chen, Wei, Wang and Lyu2020). Deficits in this fundamental facet of cognition can hinder recovery and exacerbate neurocognitive issues in at least two ways. Firstly, lapses in sustained attention compromise the ability to engage with the task at hand (Smilek, Carriere, & Cheyne, Reference Smilek, Carriere and Cheyne2010). Therefore, sustained attention deficits limit the extent to which patients can engage with and accordingly benefit from learning, intervention, and rehabilitation programmes (e.g., Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Corbett, Gaden, Grayson, Kril and Broe2002; Blanc-garin, Reference Blanc-garin1994; Robertson, Ridgeway, Greenfield, & Parr, Reference Robertson, Ridgeway, Greenfield and Parr1997; van Zandvoort, Kessels, Nys, de Haan, & Kappelle, Reference van Zandvoort, Kessels, Nys, de Haan and Kappelle2005). Second, the deployment of attention is known to be critical for gating plasticity processes (Polley, Steinberg, & Merzenich, Reference Polley, Steinberg and Merzenich2006; Recanzone, Schreiner, & Merzenich, Reference Recanzone, Schreiner and Merzenich1993). Plasticity processes in turn underlie cortical remapping and functional reorganisation of the brain that are characteristic of recovery (Di Lazzaro et al., Reference Di Lazzaro, Profice, Pilato, Capone, Ranieri and Pasqualetti2010; Fu & Zuo, Reference Fu and Zuo2011; Hallett, Reference Hallett2001; Phillips, Batten, Tremblay, Aldosary, & Blier, Reference Phillips, Batten, Tremblay, Aldosary and Blier2015; Qian et al., Reference Qian, Loo, Castellanos, Liu and Translationaln.d.). Thus, sustained attention deficits may also hinder the extent to which functional restoration can occur at the neural level.
Several studies in neurologically healthy participants have suggested that sustained attention is supported by a predominantly right-lateralised network (Johannsen et al., Reference Johannsen, Jakobsen, Bruhn, Hansen, Gee, Stødkilde-Jørgensen and Gjedde1997; Langner & Eickhoff, Reference Langner and Eickhoff2013; Singh-Curry & Husain, Reference Singh-Curry and Husain2009; Warm, Richter, Sprague, Porter, & Schumsky, Reference Warm, Richter, Sprague, Porter and Schumsky1980; Whitehead, Reference Whitehead1991), modulated by the locus coeruleus norepinephrine noradrenergic arousal system (Corbetta & Shulman, Reference Corbetta and Shulman2002; Reference Corbetta and Shulman2011; Posner & Petersen, Reference Posner and Petersen1990). In correspondence with findings highlighting a privileged role for the right hemisphere (RH) for sustained attention, stroke survivors with damage to RH networks show pronounced deficits in this capacity (Malhotra, Coulthard, & Husain, Reference Malhotra, Coulthard and Husain2009; Robertson, Manly, Beschin, & Daini, Reference Robertson, Manly, Beschin and Daini1997; Rueckert & Grafman, 1996; Reference Rueckert and Grafman1998; Wilkins, Shallice, & McCarthy, Reference Wilkins, Shallice and McCarthy1987), typically defined by an overall deficit in the ability to detect infrequently occurring visual targets when monitoring a continuous stream of stimuli.
Early experimental tasks designed to capture sustained attention usually asked participants to monitor targets over tens of minutes. This would typically elicit performance decrements (so-called time-on-task effects) in healthy individuals (Mackworth & Taylor, Reference Mackworth and Taylor1963; Mackworth, Reference Mackworth1948; Parasuraman, Reference Parasuraman1979). Subsequent assessments defined sustained attention as the ability to ‘self-sustain mindful, conscious processing of stimuli’ in order to capture transient lapses in attention while performing more complex, goal-directed tasks of much shorter durations (e.g., the Sustained Attention to Response Test (Manly et al., Reference Manly, Owen, McAvinue, Datta, Lewis and Scott2003; Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley, & Yiend, Reference Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley and Yiend1997)). Counter-intuitively, when neurological healthy individuals perform such tasks for protracted periods of time, performance improvements as opposed to decrements are observed (Staub, Doignon-Camus, Marques-Carneiro, Bacon, & Bonnefond, Reference Staub, Doignon-Camus, Marques-Carneiro, Bacon and Bonnefond2015; Brosnan et al., Reference Brosnan, Arvaneh, Harty, Maguire, O’Connell, Robertson and Dockree2018). Moreover, experimental tasks assessing time on task effects in RH stroke often fail to show performance decrements over relatively short task durations (˜8–10 min; (Malhotra et al., Reference Malhotra, Coulthard and Husain2009; Rueckert & Grafman, 1996; Reference Rueckert and Grafman1998). This suggests that either RH stroke survivors do not show linear decrements in performance over time on these tasks (which is often at odds with anecdotal reports from both RH stroke survivors and their clinicians), or that sustained attention tasks may not be sufficiently sensitive to capture precise deficits in the temporal dynamics of sustained attention difficulties.
One limitation of traditional sustained attention tasks is that the discrete onset of sensory target stimuli (i.e., of either visual or auditory targets) may exogenously capture attention and inadvertently ‘support’ sustained attention, such that successful performance on the task is not uniquely contingent on endogenous deployment of attentional control. Moreover, the continuous mapping of a transiently presented target stimulus (e.g., number 3 from a stream of nine numbers; Manly et al., Reference Manly, Owen, McAvinue, Datta, Lewis and Scott2003; Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley, & Yiend, Reference Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley and Yiend1997) with a specific behaviour (respond or withhold a response) may result in automatic detection which is characteristic of consistent-mapping paradigms (Shiffrin & Schneider, Reference Shiffrin and Schneider1977). Thus, these tasks may fail to identify decrements in attentional engagement occurring over relatively shorter timescales.
The current study employed the Continuous Temporal Expectancy Task (CTET; (O’Connell et al., Reference O’Connell, Dockree, Robertson, Bellgrove, Foxe and Kelly2009) in which targets and non-targets differ only in temporal duration and are otherwise perceptually identical. This eliminates any potential for exogenous attentional capture by perceptual features of the stimuli, since targets and non-targets differ only in temporal duration. Moreover, previous work has shown that with mind-wandering, time ‘contracts’ (Terhune, Croucher, Marcusson-Clavertz, & Macdonald, Reference Terhune, Croucher, Marcusson-Clavertz and Macdonald2017); therefore, a task necessitating the detection of relatively longer temporal intervals might be particularly sensitive to states of inattention. In the CTET, participants monitor a continuous stream of patterned stimuli that are presented centrally and make a button press when they detect a longer-duration stimulus. This task is sensitive to within-block performance decrements (over approximately 3 min windows) in neurologically healthy individuals and here we assess its potential to elicit performance decrements in RH stroke survivors, a group who are clinically and experimentally characterised by sustained attention deficits (Malhotra et al., Reference Malhotra, Coulthard and Husain2009; Molenberghs, Gillebert, Schoofs, & Dupont, Reference Molenberghs, Gillebert, Schoofs, Dupont, Peeters and Vandenberghe2009; Robertson, Ridgeway, Greenfield, & Parr, Reference Robertson, Ridgeway, Greenfield and Parr1997; Rueckert & Grafman, 1996; Reference Rueckert and Grafman1998; Wilkins et al., Reference Wilkins, Shallice and McCarthy1987). We reasoned that the CTET will maximally tax sustained attention in RH stroke survivors and reveal performance decrements over short time windows.
METHODS
Participants
Fourteen chronic stroke survivors with unilateral damage to the RH were recruited through an existing database of patients (either in acute settings or through self-referrals) interested in research participation at the Cognitive Neuropsychology Centre, Department of Experimental Psychology, Oxford University. All patients were >1 year post-stroke (M = 2.15 years, SD = 1.07; note these data were not available for one self-referred patient). All study participants provided written informed consent, in compliance with relevant protocols approved by the University of Oxford Central University Research Ethics Committee, and the experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki. Eighteen neurologically healthy older adults were recruited through an existing database of healthy older adults interested in participating in research at the Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Australia. The experimental protocol was approved by Monash University’s Human Research Ethics Committee before testing and was carried out in accordance with the approved guidelines.
Demographics
All participants provided their age, gender, and level of education (Table 1). Differences in demographic factors between healthy older adults and the stroke survivors were assessed using one way between-subject ANOVAs with ‘Group’ as the between-subject factor. There was no evidence to suggest that the stroke survivors and healthy older adults differed in age (F 1,30 = 2.99; p = .09, Table 1). In an effort to ensure that our results were generalisable to both genders, we aspired to obtain an equal number of male and female participants in both groups. Binomial tests conducted separately for the healthy older controls and the stroke survivors indicated that the proportion of male to female participants did not differ significantly from the expected .50 (p > .41 for both groups, Table 1). The RH stroke survivors had spent fewer years in formal education (F 1,30 = 8.98, p = .005, η p 2 = .23, Table 1).
Note. Years in education was not available for one stroke participant.
* A significant difference between patients and controls values denote mean (SD).
Lesions
Magnetic resonance images (MRIs) were acquired on a 3-T TIM Trio scanner at the Oxford Centre for Clinical Magnetic Resonance Research (OCMR) for N = 7 patients. These scans were acquired at the chronic stage (i.e., >6 months post-stroke onset). High-resolution 3D whole-brain T1-weighted MRI scans were acquired using a magnetisation-prepared rapid gradient-echo sequence (TR 3000 ms, TE 4.7 ms, flip angle 8°, 1 mm isotropic resolution) and fluid rapid attenuated inversion recovery images were also obtained acquired (TR 5000 ms, TE 397, in-plane resolution 1 mm, slice thickness 1.5 mm). For the remaining patients (n = 7), clinical imaging was acquired at the acute stage (i.e., <3 weeks post-stroke onset) as part of their hospital admission diagnostics. Of these patients, lesions were delineated using either a clinical T1-weighted MRI (n = 1, voxel resolution 1.78 mm × 1.78 mm × 1 mm, 192 axial slices) or clinical computerised tomography (CT) (n = 6, slice thickness 5 mm, 34.17 (2.40; range 30–36) axial slices). Lesions were manually delineated in native structural space slice by slice in the axial plane (MRIcron; McCausland Center for Brain Imaging, Columbia, SC, USA). Lesion masks were smoothed at 5 mm full width at half maximum in the z-direction and binarised using a .5 intensity threshold. Native space structural scans and lesion masks were aligned to an Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) equivalent stereotactic space (2 mm3) specific for each modality and generated from a healthy elderly population. The template spaces and normalisation algorithms were performed using ‘Clinical Toolbox’ (Rorden et al., 2012), implemented with Statistical Parametric Mapping 8 (SPM8: Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, UK; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk). For two patients, lesions were not yet visible at the time of their clinically acquired acute CT scans. However, both patients’ medical presentation and diagnosis indicated RH stroke. The lesion distributions are shown in Figure 2a.
Assessment
The Continuous Temporal Expectancy Task (CTET)
The CTET (O’Connell, et al., Reference O’Connell, Dockree, Robertson, Bellgrove, Foxe and Kelly2009), a temporal judgement task designed to elicit frequent lapses of attention was employed to measure sustained attention. In this task, a patterned stimulus was presented centrally on a grey background and was constantly rotated at 90° angles (see Figure 1). The pattern stimulus consisted of a single 8 cm2 large square divided into a 10 × 10 grid of identical square tiles (0.8 mm2), each one diagonally split into black and white halves. The tile orientation shifted by 90° in a random direction (clockwise or counterclockwise) on each frame change, yielding four distinct patterns. In the ‘standard’ trials (˜90% of trials), the stimuli were presented for a temporal duration of 690 ms. The participant’s task was to identify the infrequent ‘target’ trials by a button press using their right index finger, where the stimulus was presented for a longer temporal duration. Stimuli were pseudo-randomly presented such that there were between 7 and 15 (average of 11) standard trials between each target presentation.
The CTET was designed such that temporal judgements were perceptually undemanding for participants but challenging when tasked to continuously perform the judgements over sustained periods of time. Prior to the experimental blocks, a practice block was administered, where all participants were required to demonstrate 100% accuracy, before advancing to the experimental blocks. For the practice block, three targets were randomly interspersed among 25 standard stimuli. Target stimuli were presented at a target duration of 1020 ms, that is, 330 ms /47.83% longer than standard trials. If participants missed one or more target stimuli, the block was performed again. If the participant still failed to identify all the targets, the duration of the target stimulus was adapted such that all participants demonstrated 100% accuracy on two consecutive blocks. For the healthy older adults, all participants identified the target stimuli at 100% accuracy during the practice blocks at the target duration of 1020 ms. Of the stroke survivors, seven identified the target stimulus at a duration of 1020 ms at 100% accuracy. For the other seven patients, the duration of the target stimulus was adapted as follows such that all participants demonstrated 100% accuracy on two consecutive practice blocks: if the patient did not achieve 100% accuracy during two consecutive blocks within the first three practice blocks, the duration of the target stimulus was increased to 1140 ms. Participants were then given two practice blocks with this target duration. If 100% accuracy was achieved during these blocks, then this target duration was set at 1140 ms for the rest of the experiment. If the patient did not exhibit 100% accuracy at this duration, the target duration was subsequently increased again to 1500 ms, and following this to 1800 ms. In order to ensure the stroke survivors fully understood the task requirements, all patients were required to complete two consecutive practice blocks at 100% accuracy before proceeding to the experimental trials. For the experimental trials, the target durations for the stroke survivors were either 1020 ms (N = 7), 1140 ms (N = 2), 1500 ms (N = 4), or 1800 ms (N = 1).
This tailored approach was adopted to ensure that any observed differences in attention could not be attributed to the perceptual demands of the task. Participants completed five blocks of the task and were given a break (˜1 min) in between all blocks. Each block consisted of 225 stimulus rotations with a total duration of between 3 min and 5 s and 3 min and 30 s. The number of targets varied between 18 and 22 per block.
Analyses
Sustained attention was expressed as the percentage of correct target detections. A target was considered correctly identified if a participant responded within 2.07 seconds (i.e., three standard trials) of a target trial. One stroke patient presented with working memory problems, identified by a previous study in the centre using the Oxford Cognitive Screen (Demeyere, Riddoch, Slavkova, Bickerton, & Humphreys, Reference Demeyere, Riddoch, Slavkova, Bickerton and Humphreys2015). In an effort to exclude the possibility that these deficits were contributing to performance on the task, this patient was required to recall the task instructions at the end of each block. Outliers were defined as participants whose overall accuracy levels were greater than three times the interquartile range, which was determined separately for the healthy older adults and stroke survivors.
We examined overall accuracy across all blocks of the task (Figure 2b), and we also evaluated changes in accuracy both within each block and across blocks (Figure 2c). Differences in accuracy between the stroke survivors and healthy older adults were evaluated using a repeated-measures mixed ANOVA with ‘Performance Decrement Within Blocks’ (within four quartiles of each block) and ‘Performance Decrement Across Blocks’ (five blocks) as within subject factors. ‘Group’ (RH stroke, healthy ageing) was included as a between-subject factor. In order to assess the putatively restorative effect of task break on performance, the average of the fourth quartile computed across blocks 1, 2, 3, and 4 was compared against the average of the first quartile across blocks 2, 3, 4, and 5 using a repeated-measures ANOVA with break as a within-subjects and group as a between-subjects factor.
The verify that any of the observed effects could not be attributed to group differences in levels of formal eduction, all significant ANOVAs were repeated years in formal education included as a covariate.
RESULTS
Overall Target Detection (Accuracy)
The rate of false alarms (i.e., the number of button presses in the absence of a target stimulus) was low on the CTET (M = 3.36, SD = 2.03, range = 1.2–9.4%) indicating that all participants were performing well above chance level and had correctly understood the instructions. In line with previous reports investigating overall performance levels in RH stroke survivors (Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley, & Yiend, Reference Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley and Yiend1997; Rueckert & Grafman, 1996; Reference Rueckert and Grafman1998), a main effect of Group indicated that the patients were significantly less accurate at identifying target stimuli, compared with the healthy older controls (F 1,29 = 25.34, p < .0005, η p 2 = .47, Figure 2b; RH stroke M(SD) 64.27% (19.17); healthy older controls M(SD) 89.29% (6.56)). This main effect remained significant after controlling for years of education (F 1,28 = 16.70, p < .0005, ηp 2 = .37).
Time on Task Decrements: Performance Decrement Within Blocks
A main effect of Quartile (F 3,87 = 15.58, p < .0005, η p 2 = .35) demonstrated that accuracy on the task decreased within each block (see Figure 2c), as confirmed by a significant linear contrast (F 1,29 = 42.36, p < .0005, η p 2 = .59). Critically, this was qualified by a Group × Quartile interaction (F 3,87 = 3.21, p = .03, η p 2 = .10). Follow-up repeated-measures ANOVAs with Quartile as the within-subjects factor were conducted separately for the neurologically healthy older group and the RH stroke survivors. For both the healthy group (F 3,48 = 3.58, p = .02, η p 2 = .18) and the stroke survivors (F 3,39 = 11.19, p < .0005, η p 2 = .46), there was a main effect of Quartile, best captured by a linear fit (healthy; F 1,16 = 7.41, p = .02, η p 2 = .32; stroke: F 1,13 = 38.37, p < .0005, η p 2 = .75). Thus, the significant Group × Quartile interaction was driven by a significantly steeper within-block decrement in the accuracy of the RH stroke survivors relative to the healthy older controls. When controlling for years of education, the main effect of Quartile was no longer significant (F 3,84 = 1.14, p = .34). Crucially, however, the Group × Quartile interaction remained significant (F 3,84 = 3.73, p = .01, η p 2 = .12).
Time on Task Decrements: Performance Decrement Across Blocks
There was no main effect of Block (F 4,116 = 1.78, p = .14), but there was a Group × Block interaction (F 4,116 = 2.75, p = .03, η p 2 = .09) which remained significant after controlling for education (F 4,112 = 2.66, p = .04, η p 2 = .09) indicating that the two groups differed in the way their performance changed across blocks. Follow-up repeated-measures analyses conducted for each group separately failed to resolve the source of this interaction with no main effect of Block for either the stroke (F 4,52 = 2.31, p = .07, η p 2 = .15) or the healthy older cohort (F 4,64 = .85, p = .5). As such, these results suggest that while the stroke survivors exhibit particularly sharp decrements in their ability to sustain attention within blocks, we found no evidence to suggest that the stroke survivors differed from controls in their capacity to sustain attentional engagement over coarser timescales (i.e., across blocks). Finally, there were no other significant interaction terms (Quartile × Block, and Quartile × Block × Group both p > .72).
Break Periods to Restore Performance
The findings thus far indicate that deficits in attention arise from difficulties in the capacity to maintain engagement over short time periods (within block). This suggests that the short break periods in between blocks exerted a restorative effect over performance. Formal analyses supported this hypothesis. A main effect of Break was observed (F 1,29 = 36.76, p < .0005, η p 2 = .56), and there was no significant Break × Group interaction effect (F 1,29 = 3.30, p = .08, η p 2 = .10). As such, for both the healthy older adults and the RH stroke survivors, performance during the fourth quartiles (healthy older M = 85.83%, SD = 12.07, RH stroke M = 58.45%, SD = 23.78) was significantly lower than during the first quartile (healthy older M = 94.63%, SD = 7.68; RH stroke M = 74.79%, SD = 21.23), demonstrating that irrespective of group, the performance deficit was offset by rest periods between blocks.
DISCUSSION
Here we show that monitoring attention in the temporal domain captures clinical deficits in maintaining attention within a shorter time window (a linear decline in performance within ˜3 ½ min) than previously noted with RH stroke survivors (Robertson, Ridgeway, Greenfield, & Parr, Reference Robertson, Ridgeway, Greenfield and Parr1997; Rueckert & Grafman, 1996; Reference Rueckert and Grafman1998). Our results indicate that well-established clinical deficits in sustained attention (overall target detection accuracy) following RH stroke (Robertson et al., Reference Robertson, Ridgeway, Greenfield and Parr1997; Rueckert & Grafman, 1996; Reference Rueckert and Grafman1998) may arise from a specific impairment in the ability to maintain engagement over relatively short temporal windows. As such, these results suggest that the CTET (O’Connell, et al., Reference O’Connell, Dockree, Robertson, Bellgrove, Foxe and Kelly2009) is a sensitive tool for capturing and examining performance decrements in sustained attention over short temporal windows.
There are several aspects of the CTET that make it appealing for assessing attentional deficits, particularly in patient populations. Firstly, the target stimulus is adapted such that 100% accuracy is exhibited during the practice trials preceding the experimental blocks. Thus, the task is not confounded by an overall perceptual deficit in the stroke survivors. Secondly, unlike many sustained attention tasks (e.g., Malhotra et al., Reference Malhotra, Coulthard and Husain2009; Manly et al., Reference Manly, Owen, McAvinue, Datta, Lewis and Scott2003; Rueckert & Grafman, 1996; Reference Rueckert and Grafman1998), identification of the target stimulus in the CTET is facilitated by continuously monitoring time as opposed identifying distinct perceptual characteristics of targets relative to non-targets. This is potentially particularly advantageous in producing a performance decrement as visual search processes which may help with the identification of target stimuli, such as stimulus-driven attentional capture and stimulus-response mapping, are reduced in the CTET, thus placing greater demands on the continuous monitoring process and taxing the sustained attention system.
Despite the stroke survivors demonstrating steep within-block performance declines, they also exhibited the capacity to temporarily revive their sustained attention following the brief within-block rest periods, an observation supported by significantly higher performance during the first quartiles of blocks 2, 3, 4, and 5, as compared with the last quartiles of blocks 1, 2, 3, and 4. In the current study, the within-block performance decrement observed in the RH stroke survivors did not get worse with time (i.e., there was no interaction between this effect and the overall performance decrement over the course of the experiment). This suggests that the sustained attention system can be revived within intervals as short as the between block breaks of approximately 1 min. This is in line with work showing that sustained attention can be modulated by gaining volitional control over biophysiological markers of arousal (O’Connell, Bellgrove, Dockree, & Lau, Reference O’Connell, Bellgrove, Dockree and Lau2008; Robertson, Tegnér, Tham, & Lo, Reference Robertson, Tegnér, Tham and Lo1995) or by presenting salient sensory stimuli which act as external alerting cues (Finke, Matthias, Keller, Müller, & Schneider, Reference Finke, Matthias, Keller, Müller and Schneider2012; Robertson, Mattingley, Rorden, & Driver, Reference Robertson, Mattingley, Rorden and Driver1998).
In this study, we observed large interindividual differences in sustained attention capacity on the CTET, particularly in the patient cohort. An avenue for future work will be to highlight neural sources of variability on the CTET along with the functional, neuropsychological, and clinical implications of deficits in the capacity to sustain engagement. While our study highlights the feasibility of the CTET to effectively elicit sustained attention deficits in a relatively small cohort that is characterised by known difficulties in this capacity, future confirmation of our results will be required in larger-scale clinical studies. In particular, it will be important to assess whether the CTET is sensitive to attention deficits following exclusively right hemisphere infarcts or whether this task is sensitive to other neurological events, including brainstem damage, left hemisphere lesions, traumatic brain injury, and neuroinflammation. Moreover, our groups differed both in terms of geographical location and levels of educational attainment, and future research should address whether cultural, environmental, and cognitively enriching factors including education may mitigate deficits in the capacity to sustain engagement.
One likely explanation for these sharp decreases in arousal following RH damage is dysfunction of the noradrenergic system. Noradrenaline (norepinephrine), a neurotransmitter emanating from the locus coeruleus with widespread projections throughout the cortex, plays a well-established role in maintaining arousal (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002; Reference Corbetta and Shulman2011; Posner & Petersen, Reference Posner and Petersen1990), for a review see Sara, Reference Sara2009. RH dominance of noradrenaline is evident from asymmetric organisation of the noradrenergic system in rats (Robinson, Reference Robinson1979; Robinson & Coyle, Reference Robinson and Coyle1980), higher noradrenergic concentrations in the right thalamus in post-mortem human neurochemical studies (Oke, Keller, Mefford, & Adams, Reference Oke, Keller, Mefford and Adams1978), and increased blood-oxygen-level-dependent imaging (BOLD) activation exclusively within the RH during pharmacological upregulation of noradrenaline (Grefkes, Wang, Eickhoff, & Fink, Reference Grefkes, Wang, Eickhoff and Fink2010). Pharmacological interventions such as noradrenergic agonists and methylphenidate that modulate noradrenergic levels within prefrontal regions have been shown to improve sustained attention performance on the CTET in healthy younger adults (Dockree et al., Reference Dockree, Barnes, Matthews, Dean, Abe and Nandam2017), and preliminary evidence suggests that this may be a useful approach to improve sustained attention deficits associated with damage to the RH (Malhotra, Parton, Greenwood, & Husain, Reference Malhotra, Parton, Greenwood and Husain2006; Singh-Curry, Malhotra, Farmer, & Husain, Reference Singh-Curry, Malhotra, Farmer and Husain2011). Future work should directly explore whether noradrenergic dysfunction underlies the pronounced decrements in sustained attention observed in the current study, and whether manipulating activity within this right-lateralised system, for example, through pharmacological interventions (Dockree et al., Reference Dockree, Barnes, Matthews, Dean, Abe and Nandam2017), behavioural training paradigms (Milewski-Lopez et al., Reference Milewski-Lopez, Greco, van den Berg, McAvinue, McGuire and Robertson2014), functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) neurofeedback training techniques (deBettencourt, Cohen, Lee, Norman, & Turk-Browne, Reference deBettencourt, Cohen, Lee, Norman and Turk-Browne2015; Habes et al., Reference Habes, Rushton, Johnston, Sokunbi, Barawi and Brosnan2016), or brain stimulation (Brosnan et al., Reference Brosnan, Demaria, Petersen, Dockree, Robertson and Wiegand2017; Reference Brosnan, Arvaneh, Harty, Maguire, O’Connell, Robertson and Dockree2018), might ameliorate these performance decrements.
Finally, although specific functional impairments resulting from a stroke have often been considered in the context of modular damage (e.g., Broca, Reference Broca1865), there is a growing consensus that it is not only damage to specific regions but rather disruptions to inter-connected neural networks which are associated with functional impairments (e.g., Forkel et al., Reference Forkel, Thiebaut de Schotten, Dell’Acqua, Kalra, Murphy, Williams and Catani2014; cf Baldassarre, Ramsey, Siegel, Shulman, & Corbetta, Reference Baldassarre, Ramsey, Siegel, Shulman and Corbetta2016; Corbetta & Shulman, Reference Corbetta and Shulman2011). With regard to the sustained attention system, recent multivariate analyses of fMRI data have identified a widespread network of cortical, subcortical, and cerebellar regions underlying the capacity to maintain attentional engagement (deBettencourt et al., Reference deBettencourt, Cohen, Lee, Norman and Turk-Browne2015; Rosenberg et al., Reference Rosenberg, Finn, Scheinost, Papademetris, Shen, Constable and Chun2016). Rosenberg and colleagues recently showed that only 27% of a multivariate pattern analysis-derived network which successfully predicted the ‘high’ attention state both in healthy younger adults and children and adults with attentional deficits (ADHD symptoms) included prefrontal and parietal nodes. These findings do not discount fronto-parietal networks contributions to maintaining sustained attention (Rueckert & Grafman, 1996; Reference Rueckert and Grafman1998; Wilkins et al., Reference Wilkins, Shallice and McCarthy1987) but rather demonstrate the role of these regions within a widespread network (Shalev, Humphreys, & Demeyere, Reference Shalev, Humphreys and Demeyere2016). The heterogeneity of the lesions in the current cohort support these findings and suggest damage to widespread cortical regions within the right-lateralised noradrenergic network results in deficits maintaining sustained attention over time (cf, (Corbetta & Shulman, Reference Corbetta and Shulman2011).
The current findings hold important translational implication for informing cognitive assessment and rehabilitation strategies for individuals who have experienced RH stroke, and in particular, they speak to the need for employing assays that are sensitive to detecting deficits over short time intervals when appraising the integrity of sustained attention in this cohort.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We would like to sincerely thank the late Prof Glyn Humphreys for his valuable contributions to the design and implementation of this project. We wish to thank Tara Anne Maguire for her assistance with data collection, and our participants in Melbourne and Oxford for generously volunteering their time. This work was supported by the European Union FP7 Marie Curie Initial Training Network Individualised Diagnostics & Rehabilitation of Attention Disorders (grant number 606901), a Marie Skłodowska-Curie Fellowship from the European Commission (AGEING PLASTICITY; grant number 844246), and supported by the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre. The Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome Trust (203139/Z/16/Z). CR Gillebert was supported by an Odysseus grant of the Research Foundation Flanders (G0H7718N). Paul Dockree was supported by an Irish Research Council Laureate grant: IRCLA/2017/306.
CONFLICTS OF INTEREST
The authors have nothing to disclose.