Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T01:42:33.831Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Has maternal sensitivity been comprehensively investigated in sub-Saharan Africa? A narrative scoping review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 March 2023

A. Prag*
Affiliation:
ACSENT Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
K.A. Donald
Affiliation:
Neuroscience Institute, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa Division of Developmental Paediatrics, Department of Paediatrics and Child Health, Red Cross War Memorial Children’s Hospital, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
E. Weldon
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology Bath University, England
S.L. Halligan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology Bath University, England Department of Psychiatry and Neuroscience Institute, Groote Schuur Hospital, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
D.J. Stein
Affiliation:
Neuroscience Institute, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa Department of Psychiatry and Neuroscience Institute, Groote Schuur Hospital, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa MRC Unit on Risk & Resilience in Mental Disorders, Cape Town, South Africa
S. Malcolm-Smith
Affiliation:
ACSENT Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa Neuroscience Institute, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
*
Author for correspondence: A. Prag, Email: praganita@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Child development is strongly influenced by maternal characteristics. Maternal sensitivity, as well as risks to and outcomes of sensitive maternal style, are well studied in industrialised western contexts, but it is unclear if this is the case for other contexts. Sub-Saharan Africa has been subjected to and continues to negotiate socio-economic and psychological sequelae of colonial and race-based politics: exploring the nature and outcomes of early caregiver input in such challenging conditions is imperative. This scoping review thus aims to 1) evaluate the nature and extent of quantified observational assessments of dyadic interactions, with a focus on maternal sensitivity, in Sub-Saharan Africa and 2) ascertain which risk and outcome factors have been examined in relation to maternal sensitivity. Study quality and cross-cultural appropriateness will also be considered. The search using expanded search terms yielded 20 papers –four characterizing maternal sensitivity or style, eight examining maternal sensitivity in relation to risks and outcomes, and eight intervention studies examining efforts to improve maternal sensitivity. Most research was conducted in South Africa – only seven studies were conducted in four other countries. Researchers used a wide array of coding schemes, mostly developed in the west. Ten studies made some adaptations to measures. Language issues and cultural considerations were often not explicitly addressed. Taken together, very limited research on this important topic exists. For the work that does exist, questions around westernized assumptions, language, and appropriateness of measures remain. Substantially more research, informed by both culturally flexible conceptualizations and methodological rigour, is required.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology

Summations

  • A search using all the core foci search terms yielded zero papers; therefore, a single standard PRISMA compliant search method could not be used. Six searches were conducted (Ebscohost – Academic Search Premier, MEDLINE, APA PsychArticles, APA PsychInfo, and APA PsychTests) using various combinations of the core foci for a yield of relevant research data.

  • The reviewed research shows some findings consistent with research from HICs. However, this small set of studies from a limited number of contexts cannot be considered to provide a full articulation of the nature of maternal style or sensitive caregiving for sub-Saharan Africa, let alone factors promoting or disrupting it, or the child developmental outcomes associated with variability in caregiving.

  • More research is needed to understand the emerging child influenced by maternal sensitivity, child temperament, along with cultural and environmental factors, to foster optimally targeted interventions for mothers and children in low- to middle-income countries contexts, and to avoid biased interpretation of behaviours occurring in non-western environments.

Considerations

  • Maternal sensitivity was not defined or operationalized uniformly enough to allow for a meta-analysis to be conducted.

  • The heterogeneity in research foci and methods also made descriptive comparisons across studies difficult.

  • A hopeful sign is that a number of recently published papers have focused on maternal sensitivity in LMICs – suggesting that researchers are recognising the need to address the dearth of knowledge on the fundamental functioning and ongoing impact of this phenomenon in non-western contexts.

Introduction

Parenting practices vary across countries, socio-economic circumstances, parental psychological resources, child characteristics, and contextual sources of stress and support (Fish, Reference Fish2001; Whittaker et al., Reference Whittaker, Harden, See, Meisch and Westbrook2011). At present, most theorising and empirical findings regarding maternal style and characterising the nature of the primary caregiver–infant dyad seem to be found in relation to North American, Australian and European samples (Mesman, Minter, et al., Reference Mesman, Minter, Angnged, Cisse, Salali and Migliano2018; Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, van Ijzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg2012), where it is widely understood that improving the mother (primary caregiver)–child relationship can have significantly positive effects on child development (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, van and Juffer2003; Joosen et al., Reference Joosen, Mesman, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn2012). In an African context of growing dissatisfaction with imported ideas and knowledge and increased questioning of the validity of such knowledge (Kiguwa & Segalo, Reference Kiguwa and Segalo2018), it is necessary to delineate and interrogate the knowledge base that exists for African dyads in the psychological literature. Many have argued that a more critical approach to psychology research in sub-Saharan Africa, challenging in particular neo-colonial ideas of individualism, can better guide the field in terms of understanding and responding to the mental health challenges created in large part by historically colonial economic, social, and political contexts (Cullen et al., Reference Cullen, Rhodes, Brockman, Hunt and Langtiw2021). Any unquestioning adoption of western methods and constructs is considered inappropriate for contexts that have divergent conceptualisations of what it means to be a psychological being (Kiguwa & Segalo, Reference Kiguwa and Segalo2018; Seehawer, Reference Seehawer2018) and of parenting skills and goals within a more collectivistic society (Seehawer, Reference Seehawer2018). We thus need to carefully consider the evidence for potentially universal principles regarding sensitive caregiving and its outcomes and examine the methods and models that underlie such findings as well as those that diverge from accepted norms.

Research from low- to middle-income countries (LMICs) reported in a series of reviews exploring inequality in early childhood proposes that a conservative estimate of 200 million children under the age of 5 fail to reach their potential in development due to poverty, poor health and nutrition, and inadequate care (Grantham-McGregor et al., Reference Grantham-McGregor, Cheung, Cueto, Glewwe, Richter and Strupp2007; Walker et al., Reference Walker, Wachs, Meeks Gardner, Lozoff, Wasserman, Pollitt and Carter2007; Walker et al., Reference Walker, Wachs, Grantham-McGregor, Black, Nelson, Huffman, Baker-Henningham, Chang, Hamadani, Lozoff, Gardner, Powell, Rahman and Richter2011). In sub-Saharan Africa, a long history of colonial influence has had deleterious consequences on its citizens, often further exacerbated by subsequent mal-administration and high levels of internal conflict (Luiz, Reference Luiz2006, Reference Luiz2013). Social welfare systems and psychological health development are regarded as having struggled to evolve from imported colonial ideologies and have failed to serve the needs of disenfranchised citizens (Nsamenang & Dawes, Reference Nsamenang and Dawes1998; Luiz, Reference Luiz2006; Nsamenang & Dawes, Reference Nsamenang and Dawes1998). South Africa, for example, has one of the highest Gini coefficients’ (0.65 in 2015 – Galal, Reference Galal2021) indicating vast economic disparities between communities. This is known to drive high levels of violence, crime, and specifically intimate partner violence (Yapp & Pickett, Reference Yapp and Pickett2019), while social support for citizens who have experienced the worst consequences is limited (Maluleke, Reference Maluleke2018). Existing research has identified multiple risk factors in LMICs that negatively impact child development, including, inter alia, maternal depression, inadequate cognitive stimulation, and exposure to societal violence (Grantham-McGregor et al., Reference Grantham-McGregor, Cheung, Cueto, Glewwe, Richter and Strupp2007; Walker et al., Reference Walker, Wachs, Meeks Gardner, Lozoff, Wasserman, Pollitt and Carter2007; Walker et al., Reference Walker, Wachs, Grantham-McGregor, Black, Nelson, Huffman, Baker-Henningham, Chang, Hamadani, Lozoff, Gardner, Powell, Rahman and Richter2011). Among the identified risk factors, several are known to affect the kind of care the child receives, and/or that the mother is able to provide.

In socio-economic contexts, where health and community resources are scarce, individual differences in a broader framework of maternal characteristics would constitute a central factor in determining the child’s adequate physical growth, cognitive, and socio-emotional development (Fraley et al., Reference Fraley, Roisman and Haltigan2013; Harris et al., Reference Harris, Sideris, Serpell, Burchinal and Pickett2014; Harmeyer et al., Reference Harmeyer, Ispa, Palermo and Carlo2016). However, maternal competence/style (including sensitivity) may be adversely affected in challenging contexts (Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, van Ijzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg2012; Hosokawa & Katsura, Reference Hosokawa and Katsura2017). For example, the potential negative effect maternal depression has on a child’s socio-emotional development – mediated through maternal sensitivity – is well documented in high-income countries (Murray & Cooper, Reference Murray and Cooper1997; Trapolini et al., Reference Trapolini, Ungerer and McMahon2008; Gummerum et al., Reference Gummerum, Hanoch, Keller, Parsons and Hummel2010; Gelaye et al., Reference Gelaye, Rondon, Araya and Williams2016).

Maternal sensitivity has been shown to be central in creating a secure mother–child bond, and critically development of this construct was based on research conducted in sub-Saharan Africa (Uganda) and the USA. Mary Ainsworth’s definition of a sensitive mother obtained through her seminal research included three components – 1) observation of child cues; 2) correct perception and interpretation of child cues, and 3) appropriate behavioural and emotional responses to child cues (Ainsworth et al., Reference Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall1978). Sensitive responsiveness has been shown to cultivate a secure and trusting relationship, with caregivers being experienced as available and reliable. Maternal sensitivity is understood to encompass the attuned (timely, contingent, and appropriate) manner in which mother as primary caregiver attends to her child (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Fiori-Cowley and Hooper1996; Whittaker et al., Reference Whittaker, Harden, See, Meisch and Westbrook2011), keeping in mind individual temperament and other nuances of her child’s needs. Parental characteristics, such as sensitivity, including the ability to scaffold and structure activities, along with a lack of hostility and intrusiveness in particular, are considered important in creating an emotionally open and available relationship with the child (Biringen & Robinson, Reference Biringen and Robinson1991; Bornstein & Manian, Reference Bornstein and Manian2013; Biringen et al., Reference Biringen, Derscheid, Vliegen, Closson and Easterbrooks2014). Furthermore, accurate identification of negative infant emotions, emotional responses to distress, parenting goals, and emotional efficacy (inclusive or exclusive of warmth) influence the quality of dyadic interactions (Lohaus et al., Reference Lohaus, Keller, Ball, Elban and Voelker2001).

Since the original maternal sensitivity construct emerged as pivotal to understanding the nature of parent–child relationships, non-gendered descriptive terms such as “parental sensitivity” have been introduced into the literature in HICs (Broom, Reference Broom1994) and has been incorporated by experts in the field (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, van and Juffer2003; Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, van Ijzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg2012). However, because most early research has focussed on the mother–infant bond (Cooke et al., Reference Cooke, Deneault, Devereux, Eirich, Fearon and Madigan2022), we elected to focus on this original terminology.

Maternal warmth has been understood to entail an expression of either smiling or empathy during positive and negative infant emotions, respectively. Talking to the infant in engaging age-appropriate tones and language is also considered a component of warmth (Lohaus et al., Reference Lohaus, Keller, Ball, Elban and Voelker2001). Since Mary Ainsworth’s Sensitivity/Insensitivity Scale, which excluded overt expressions of warmth, several sensitivity instruments developed since then have included aspects of warmth and variations of positive maternal affect as integral components of the sensitivity construct. In a review of sensitivity observational instruments used since Ainsworth’s scale, Mesman and Emmen found that of the eight main instruments, seven included warmth/positive affect (or a variation of the construct) as a significant component in measuring maternal sensitivity. Studies of the role warmth plays in maternal sensitivity have yielded varied results almost exclusively from samples in the global north (Mesman & Emmen, Reference Mesman and Emmen2013). Rural samples in sub-Saharan Africa have in the past been misunderstood to be insensitive in part due to their lack of overtly expressed warmth (Kermoian & Leidermann, Reference Kermoian and Leidermann1986). The role of warmth in characterising maternal sensitivity should be examined in sub-Saharan African and possibly other LMIC research.

The relationship between maternal sensitivity and child outcomes continues to be a relevant avenue of research. A recent global review of maternal sensitivity and its impact on child outcomes (Deans, Reference Deans2018) highlighted several domains of functioning directly (k = 46) influenced by maternal sensitivity. The areas tested were varied but included cognitive (language acquisition, general cognitive and executive functioning, attentional control) and physical factors (obesity, sleep). Broadly, socio-emotional factors included – behavioural problems, social competence, emotionality, and temperament. The precise characteristics investigated within each domain were numerous and heterogenous.

Deans noted a caveat – great heterogeneity in study methods and the definition of maternal sensitivity informed a broad summary rather than a detailed analysis of studies reviewed. All of the domains of functioning directly impacted by maternal sensitivity represented in this list were researched in a HIC context.

The extent to which maternal sensitivity directly impacts or mediates various child outcomes in sub-Saharan Africa as has been demonstrated in HIC’s is unclear. Socio-economic disparities can have a synergistic influence on what takes place in parenting and child outcomes (Walker et al., Reference Walker, Wachs, Meeks Gardner, Lozoff, Wasserman, Pollitt and Carter2007; Walker et al., Reference Walker, Wachs, Grantham-McGregor, Black, Nelson, Huffman, Baker-Henningham, Chang, Hamadani, Lozoff, Gardner, Powell, Rahman and Richter2011). Varying levels of socio-economic status within a region could further inform developmental expectations where middle class and affluent families that make up a smaller part of the region’s population may be more acculturated to western norms (Grantham-McGregor et al., Reference Grantham-McGregor, Cheung, Cueto, Glewwe, Richter and Strupp2007; Walker et al., Reference Walker, Wachs, Grantham-McGregor, Black, Nelson, Huffman, Baker-Henningham, Chang, Hamadani, Lozoff, Gardner, Powell, Rahman and Richter2011). In a country such as South Africa, where majority of households are economically disadvantaged ( Stats SA General Household Survey, 2021), different risks or contexts to those found in HIC’s could apply. The implications of stressful factors such as compromised maternal mental health and socio-emotional competence, limited social and economic support, inadequate living conditions (intense daily threats to personal safety), substance abuse and community, and personal trauma interacting with various child characteristics are fairly unclear. Comprehensive evidence of what these factors are in sub-Saharan Africa and how they impact child development is not yet known.

Following Mary Ainsworth’s original Ugandan research, there appears to be a gap in the knowledge production from sub-Saharan Africa as compared to elsewhere in the world based on the fundamental principles of primary caregiver/s and sensitivity. Therefore, the aims of this review are 1) to scope and clarify precisely what quantitative, objectively coded research has been conducted on maternal sensitivity in Sub-Saharan Africa and 2) to identify which risk factors and child outcomes related to maternal sensitivity have been investigated in this region. The central question of the quality and appropriateness of methods will be considered in reporting the results.

Methods

Search method

The search aimed to find research on observational studies of maternal sensitivity. The decision to review research that employed videoed observed dyadic interactions was made due to its relative methodological strength as compared to maternal self-report measures. Observed interactions allow for objectivity in coding that can be verified by a team of coders, reducing bias in both the participant (social desirability bias in self-report) and observer/coder (Althubaiti, Reference Althubaiti2016). Considering the synergistic nature of maternal characteristics and child development, we also included a search of risks and outcomes of maternal sensitivity assessed in such observational studies.

The first author and a subject librarian at the University of Cape Town set up key concept search terms and included terms using the expanded thesaurus function for the databases on Ebscohost – Academic Search Premier, MEDLINE, APA PsychArticles, APA PsychInfo, and APA PsychTests (see search terms in Table 1).

Table 1. Search terms

Inclusion criteria

To be included, articles needed at minimum to include research on observed videoed and coded maternal sensitivity or style for dyads in sub-Saharan Africa. Only English language, published, peer-reviewed papers were included. No date restriction was applied.

This review initially aimed to determine the extent of data available addressing the full combination of research foci. However, conducting a full key concept search (maternal sensitivity, videoed interactions, dyad, risks to maternal sensitivity, outcomes of maternal sensitivity, sub-Saharan Africa), yielded one article which did not fulfil inclusion criteria, therefore several subset concept searches were required (see Table 1).

Six separate searches across databases were conducted – see Fig. 1 below. Initially, search 1 yielded k = 54 papers, search 2: k = 41; search 3: k = 5; search 4: k = 7 and search 5: k = 63. Titles and abstracts were screened and k = 22 remained. Of these, only k = 13 were eligible for inclusion due to article duplication across search results. We hand-searched the reference lists of those that met inclusion criteria and found k = 4 additional papers. The first author had in her possession three articles (Klein and Rye, Reference Klein and Rye2004; Broesch et al., Reference Broesch, Rochat, Olah, Broesch and Henrich2016; Murray et al., Reference Murray, De Pascalis, Tomlinson, Vally, Dadomo, MacLachlan, Woodward and Cooper2016) which were not found in the search process. Broesch et al. (Reference Broesch, Rochat, Olah, Broesch and Henrich2016) had no key words listed. As these are of significance to this review, they were included. To be thorough, a separate search using the key search term listed in the remaining two papers “parent child interaction” with the countries was conducted. These two papers were then found. Importantly, this additional search/search term yielded no other new papers. In total, 20 articles were found.

Fig. 1 Search Flow Diagram.

Evaluation of study quality

The quality of the reviewed papers was assessed in two ways: firstly via a standard empirical quality assessment tool (Downs & Black, Reference Downs and Black1998), and secondly, because this tool does not include evaluation of cross-cultural methodology, we also examined consideration of key factors in evaluating the appropriateness of such research (Aival-Naveh et al., Reference Aival-Naveh, Rothschild-Yakar and Kurman2019).

The Downs and Black methodological quality assessment tool was developed for randomised and non-randomised studies of healthcare interventions (see Table 2). The tool addresses quality of reporting, external validity (generalisability), and internal validity (bias and confounding) of the study. Each paper was rated as either “excellent” (24–28 points), “good” (19–23 points), “fair” (14–18 points), or “poor” (<14 points). This tool has been reported to be a valid, reliable, and commonly used scale (Downs & Black, Reference Downs and Black1998; O'Connor et al., Reference O'Connor, Tully, Ryan, Bradley, Baxter and McDonough2015). Given its widely accepted use and that eight papers were intervention studies, this tool was used despite its inability to capture other key aspects needing consideration in social science research.

Table 2. Maternal sensitivity observation coding methods and key results

Note *Downs and Black rating scale- “excellent” (24–28 points), “good”(19–23 points), “fair” (14–18 points) or “poor” (<14 points).

Therefore, given that our focus was research in sub-Saharan Africa, further evaluation was undertaken in relation to three factors pertaining to important considerations in cross-cultural research – 1) translation of measures, 2) validation of measure for context, and 3) treatment of language during maternal sensitivity video coding (i.e. coded by trained first language speakers or translated and subtitled for expert researchers/clinicians to code) (Beaton et al., Reference Beaton, Bombardier, Guillemin and Ferraz2000; Boer et al., Reference Boer, Hanke and He2018; Aival-Naveh et al., Reference Aival-Naveh, Rothschild-Yakar and Kurman2019).

Results

Twenty articles reported observational research investigating maternal sensitivity or style within the dyad. Papers reported original research, with the exception of two articles by Judi Mesman (Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Bain and Mesman2021; Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2021) found in search 1 who re-analysed original articles (Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Bain and Mesman2018; Mesman, Basweti, et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2018) to accommodate newer insights and a recent paper by Mark Tomlinson (Tomlinson et al., Reference Tomlinson, Rabie, Skeen, Hunt, Murray and Cooper2020) using data from an earlier body of work (Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Landman, Tomlinson, Molteno, Swartz and Murray2002). Numerous measures were used to assess differing components of maternal sensitivity making a meta-analysis for a systematic review impossible. There are 51 countries in sub-Saharan Africa (Anderson & Connor, Reference Anderson and Connor2018), yet published research on this topic was found for only five of these: single studies from Mali, Ethiopia, and Sierra Leone, 4 from Kenya, and 13 from South Africa.

Key factors and findings in relation to maternal sensitivity and risks and outcomes associated with the variable were tabulated. Three types of research studies were found – 1) papers characterising maternal sensitivity or style (k = 4); 2) papers examining maternal sensitivity in relation to risks and outcomes (k = 8); and 3) papers examining the impact of interventions to improve maternal style (k = 8). Table 2 provides details on the maternal sensitivity observations. Table 3 shows the measures and results of risks and outcomes assessed. Also reported in Table 3 is maternal sensitivity as a variable for intervention in relation to risks and outcomes.

Table 3. Risks and outcome measures and results

Note R: Risks and O: Outcomes.

Maternal sensitivity

Maternal sensitivity was broadly defined by authors as the mother’s ability to perceive and respond to various cues attending to her infant. See Table 2 and Table 3 for details. A number of points deserve emphasis. First, where sensitivity was measured as an independent variable through observation and characterised, even though infants were handled somewhat roughly at times, the authors reported that sensitivity was not completely compromised (Dixon et al, Reference Dixon, LeVine, Richman and Berry-Brazelton1984; Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2018; Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2021). Secondly, in these African mothers, sensitivity was often expressed non-verbally (Broesch et al., Reference Broesch, Rochat, Olah, Broesch and Henrich2016; Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Bain and Mesman2018, Reference Dawson, Bain and Mesman2021; Mesman, Basweti, et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2018; Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2021).

Maternal sensitivity in relation to risks and outcomes

Studies that focused on risks to and outcomes (Table 3) suggested that maternal sensitivity and maternal mental health were compromised by a lack of social support (Tomlinson et al., Reference Tomlinson, Cooper and Murray2005; Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Landman, Molteno, Stein, McPherson and Murray2009). Poorer mothers with anaemia but adequate partner support were sensitive in their behaviours (Perez et al., Reference Perez, Hendricks, Beard, Murray-Kolb, Berg, Tomlinson, Irlam, Isaacs, Njengele, Sive and Vernon-Feagans2005); although otherwise iron-deficient mothers were less sensitive and emotionally available than healthy control group mothers (Perez et al., Reference Perez, Hendricks, Beard, Murray-Kolb, Berg, Tomlinson, Irlam, Isaacs, Njengele, Sive and Vernon-Feagans2005; Murray-Kolb & Beard, Reference Murray-Kolb and Beard2009). High maternal education levels did not always predict adequate sensitivity scores (Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Bain and Mesman2018, Reference Dawson, Bain and Mesman2021). No clear evidence was presented to confirm western findings of sensitivity positively predicting secure attachment status (McMahan-True et al., Reference McMahan-True, Pisani and Oumar2001; Tomlinson et al., Reference Tomlinson, Cooper and Murray2005) except in one sub-analysis of the Tomlinson et al. (Reference Tomlinson, Cooper and Murray2005) paper, where adequate concurrent sensitivity and secure attachment at 18 months was demonstrated. Sensitivity had a positive relationship with one passive infant emotion regulation technique (passive gaze) in three communities of varying SES (Bozicevic et al., Reference Bozicevic, De Pascalis, Schuitmaker, Tomlinson, Cooper and Murray2016).

Maternal sensitivity as a variable in intervention studies

In intervention studies (Table 3), improved sensitivity was associated with positive physical growth of infant (Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Landman, Tomlinson, Molteno, Swartz and Murray2002), child cognitive development (Klein & Rye, Reference Klein and Rye2004; Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Vally, Cooper, Radford, Sharples, Tomlinson and Murray2013; Murray et al., Reference Murray, De Pascalis, Tomlinson, Vally, Dadomo, MacLachlan, Woodward and Cooper2016), and child socio-emotional development (Murray et al., Reference Murray, De Pascalis, Tomlinson, Vally, Dadomo, MacLachlan, Woodward and Cooper2016; Pitillas et al., Reference Pitillas, Berastegui, Halty, Rodriguez, Kamara and Mesman2021). Cooper et al. (Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Landman, Molteno, Stein, McPherson and Murray2009) found that even with improved sensitivity and less intrusiveness there was no significant association between these maternal variables and attachment status.

Sample SES

Across all reviewed studies, researchers focussed on samples from economically disadvantaged or rural populations, except where the Bozicevic et al. (Reference Bozicevic, De Pascalis, Schuitmaker, Tomlinson, Cooper and Murray2016) study sampled two contrasting SES populations in Cape Town, and two studies (Dixon et al., Reference Dixon, LeVine, Richman and Berry-Brazelton1984; Broesch et al., Reference Broesch, Rochat, Olah, Broesch and Henrich2016) included samples from the USA as part of their analysis.

The small number of studies, representing only five countries, and the limited risk and outcome factors examined, over short follow-up periods, must be noted. Given these cautions, the reviewed findings seem to suggest that maternal sensitivity and its effects, to the extent that they have been investigated, are consistent with what is demonstrated in western literature.

Downs and black assessment tool

On the basis of the Downs and Black rating, the methods used in the reviewed papers appear satisfactory. Only one older paper was deemed fair, while all others were either classified as good or excellent. Despite this positive outcome, it should be noted that this assessment tool does not address whether cross-cultural factors have been considered during recruitment of sample, data collection, or analysis and interpretation of results. Factors relating to cross-cultural methodological rigour that were not considered in the Downs and Black methodological assessment tool can be referenced in Table 4.

Table 4. Cross-cultural research considerations in data collection and coding not assessed by downs and black rating

*Note It is possible that in some of the research reviewed in this paper these considerations were made but not reported.

Cross-cultural research considerations in data collection and coding

The information in Table 4 suggests that researchers reported limited implementation of important cross-cultural methodological considerations. There were no reports of formal translation of sensitivity measures or questionnaires, limited translation of risk and outcome measures, and critically, no formal validation of these measures for context, and no report of observational video translations for coding by researchers. The (McMahan-True et al., Reference McMahan-True, Pisani and Oumar2001) sensitivity coding was based on written records of the home observation videotape. No details about coders or translation have been provided. One study (Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Woolgar, Murray and Molteno1999) reported that an appropriately trained first language coder was used.

Discussion

A total of 20 English language, peer-reviewed papers on maternal sensitivity were found, from only 5 of 55 Sub-Saharan countries. The majority (13 papers) stemmed from areas around two metro centres in South Africa (Cape Town and Johannesburg). Kenya produced four papers, and single studies are available for Mali, Ethiopia, and Sierra Leone. In addition, besides one Cape Town intervention study (Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Landman, Molteno, Stein, McPherson and Murray2009), the sample sizes for the included studies are small. The lack of sufficient literature to represent this vast and diverse region is evident. This problematic dearth of knowledge production from Africa could be attributed to there being less available funding and fewer research groups to do the work (Jeenah & Pouris, Reference Jeenah and Pouris2008; Cooper & Nicholas, Reference Cooper and Nicholas2012). To date, most funding to the region has been allocated to the fields of malaria, HIV/AIDS, and tuberculosis research (Asante et al., Reference Asante, Wasike and Ataguba2020).

Few studies focussed entirely on maternal sensitivity (four papers). Eight focussed on risks to maternal sensitivity or child outcomes related to the construct; while another eight reported on interventions aimed at improving maternal sensitivity. The results of these limited investigations seem to indicate comparable effects to those reported in HICs (maternal education positively impacts sensitivity; poor support for mother has negative effect on sensitivity, interventions targeted at maternal sensitivity have positive impact) with the following possible exceptions: generally low sensitivity ratings in some studies; evidence of rough handling not received negatively by the child; sensitivity often expressed predominantly non-verbally; and the predictive association of sensitivity with secure attachment not clearly established.

Evaluation of studies using a standard, western developed quality assessment tool gave very positive results regarding research quality; however, detailed investigation of the extent to which central cross-cultural methodological principles were employed gave a different picture. Researchers either failed to report their methods fully or neglected to consider vital aspects of cross-cultural research (see Methodological Considerations below) – the review cannot establish which is the case. The following considerations were reported:

All but two studies (Murray-Kolb & Beard, Reference Murray-Kolb and Beard2009; Tomlinson et al., Reference Tomlinson, Rabie, Skeen, Hunt, Murray and Cooper2020) reportedly engaged local authorities or community members during recruitment and consent for research. Trained community members were reported to be employed to assist in five intervention studies (Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Landman, Tomlinson, Molteno, Swartz and Murray2002; Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Landman, Molteno, Stein, McPherson and Murray2009; Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Vally, Cooper, Radford, Sharples, Tomlinson and Murray2013; Murray et al., Reference Murray, De Pascalis, Tomlinson, Vally, Dadomo, MacLachlan, Woodward and Cooper2016; Tomlinson et al., Reference Tomlinson, Rabie, Skeen, Hunt, Murray and Cooper2020). Two intervention studies (Klein & Rye, Reference Klein and Rye2004; Pitillas et al., Reference Pitillas, Berastegui, Halty, Rodriguez, Kamara and Mesman2021) reported using local first-language paraprofessionals to conduct the research. Ten studies reported modified measures to accommodate the cultural nuances of the population.

As reflected in the focus of several reviewed studies, the primary reason maternal sensitivity is of interest because of its demonstrated effect on child outcomes, at least in western literature. It appears that the quantity and quality of focus on maternal input as it relates to child development in sub-Saharan Africa does not match that of western, educated, industrialised, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) countries (Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010).

This dearth of peer-reviewed scientific psychological studies in sub-Saharan Africa since Ainsworth’s original work (Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, van Ijzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg2012; Mesman & Emmen, Reference Mesman and Emmen2013) is disturbing because imported notions of child development and parenting potentially skew perceptions and risk impeding respect for people who do not fit criteria of HIC contexts (Nsamenang & Dawes, Reference Nsamenang and Dawes1998; Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010). Broesch et al. (Reference Broesch, Rochat, Olah, Broesch and Henrich2016) make an important point: The way western mothers respond to their infants may be unique when compared to majority of the world’s population which is in fact rural and non-westernised, arguing therefore that more research must be conducted to understand maternal responsiveness in contexts other than HICs (Broesch et al., Reference Broesch, Rochat, Olah, Broesch and Henrich2016).

Also of concern is that to date, in sharp contrast to reported work in the (Deans, Reference Deans2018) systematic literature review, an extremely limited set of risks (social support, maternal frightening behaviours, maternal iron deficiency, postpartum depression, reflective functioning, maternal education, parent stress, child problems) and outcomes (attachment status, child physical growth, language development, attention, emotion regulation strategies, and helping behaviour) has been examined. Given the number of challenges present across the continent, this seems inadequate. Of further concern is that the reviewed studies include only short-term follow up of one or two areas of function. The variability of child-rearing practices, the multiplicity of cultures, and the range of socio-economic and related psychosocial factors likely to impact on mothers and developing infants and toddlers across Sub-Saharan Africa have yet to be adequately explored and articulated.

Issues that have received scant consideration include the following: With the exception of the work conducted recently in Kenya by Mesman where alloparenting informed a reformulation of maternal sensitivity to “infant received sensitivity”, the role of extended family networks in sensitive caregiving is seldom considered. Extended family caregiving practices are common and valued in non-western contexts and are pivotal to the well-being of family systems largely unlike the primary dyad and nuclear family units in individualistic western contexts. Research from other LMICs suggests this may constitute a resilience factor (Fourment et al., Reference Fourment, Noblega, Conde, Del Prado and Mesman2018) and warrants more attention in African studies in the progress towards decolonising psychology on the continent. This reality is key to building a knowledge base that is representative of the varied parenting context in Sub-Saharan Africa.

The differing roles mothers assume appear to vary across cultures, and this is not considered in the studies reviewed. Evidence from other LMICs is suggestive – for example, Valenzuela (Reference Valenzuela1997) in Chile found that sensitivity in mothers functioning as caregivers, attachment figures, and teachers could be considered universal, while mother as playmate was a foreign concept. Similarly, mothers in Yemen were unfamiliar with interacting with their children (face-to-face interactive play) in the manner required for observation (Alsarhi et al., Reference Alsarhi, Rahma, M.J., Alink and Mesman2018). Face-to-face interaction is a key component when coding for sensitivity in several commonly used measures which may not be appropriate in non-western contexts (Mesman, Reference Mesman2018). The degree to which mothers are comfortable with being observed and video-recorded is seldom considered: only four studies (two of which were reanalysis in 2021) specifically reported that their participants reacted mostly with ease to being observed and videoed (Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Bain and Mesman2018, Reference Dawson, Bain and Mesman2021; Mesman, Basweti, et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2018; Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2021). Notably, the role a child plays in the dynamic interaction of the dyad in Africa is crucial and has been neglected as a focus of study in relation to the quality of maternal characteristics.

Methodological considerations

It would seem imperative that the method and interpretive gaze used when studying non-western samples considers and is sensitive to nuances of context. An important question when reading the literature reviewed above is whether or to what extent the theoretical perspectives and hence methods employed permit observation of divergence from high-income country models of ideal parenting.

Quality assessment of papers

The Downs and Black quality assessment tool was used to rate each paper (Downs & Black, Reference Downs and Black1998; O'Connor et al., Reference O'Connor, Tully, Ryan, Bradley, Baxter and McDonough2015). Quality assessment of only one older paper was deemed fair, while all others were either classified as good or excellent. Despite this positive outcome, it should be noted that the assessment tool does not address whether cultural factors have been considered during the recruitment of sample, data collection or analysis, and interpretation of results. Meaningful cross-cultural research should include proper translation of measures, validation of measures for the particular context, and participant responses appropriately translated especially when coded by experts who are not first-language speakers. However in this review, we found only one study (Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Woolgar, Murray and Molteno1999) used a first language trained lay person for coding and no one translated videos for English speaking coders. Nine did some measure translation (Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Woolgar, Murray and Molteno1999; McMahan-True et al., Reference McMahan-True, Pisani and Oumar2001; Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Landman, Tomlinson, Molteno, Swartz and Murray2002; Perez et al., Reference Perez, Hendricks, Beard, Murray-Kolb, Berg, Tomlinson, Irlam, Isaacs, Njengele, Sive and Vernon-Feagans2005; Tomlinson et al., Reference Tomlinson, Cooper and Murray2005; Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Landman, Molteno, Stein, McPherson and Murray2009; Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Vally, Cooper, Radford, Sharples, Tomlinson and Murray2013; Murray et al., Reference Murray, De Pascalis, Tomlinson, Vally, Dadomo, MacLachlan, Woodward and Cooper2016; Pitillas et al., Reference Pitillas, Berastegui, Halty, Rodriguez, Kamara and Mesman2021), and eight made slight adaptations to measures for context (McMahan-True et al., Reference McMahan-True, Pisani and Oumar2001; Klein & Rye, Reference Klein and Rye2004; Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Landman, Molteno, Stein, McPherson and Murray2009; Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Vally, Cooper, Radford, Sharples, Tomlinson and Murray2013; Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Bain and Mesman2018; McMahan-True et al., Reference McMahan-True, Pisani and Oumar2001; Mesman, Basweti, et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2018; Murray et al., Reference Murray, De Pascalis, Tomlinson, Vally, Dadomo, MacLachlan, Woodward and Cooper2016; Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2021; Pitillas et al., Reference Pitillas, Berastegui, Halty, Rodriguez, Kamara and Mesman2021). In addition, establishing measure invariance would strengthen the quality of data (Beaton et al., Reference Beaton, Bombardier, Guillemin and Ferraz2000; Boer et al., Reference Boer, Hanke and He2018; Aival-Naveh et al., Reference Aival-Naveh, Rothschild-Yakar and Kurman2019).

Employing local vs research/clinical experts

A primary consideration, increasingly evident to us in working with diverse populations in South Africa, centres on who does the research and what impact this may have on results. Many measures of maternal sensitivity and dyadic interaction require a high level of (often postgraduate) psychological training along with adequate clinical experience to administer and code reliably. However, in many contexts such expertise is extremely scarce, thus impeding contextually sensitive cross-cultural investigations. Researchers are often faced with a choice: employ lay research assistants with limited training, but with local language and community expertise versus highly qualified researchers who often have to work with translators. Each option has marked benefits and disadvantages. While those embedded in the community will have insight into norms and practices, hence providing the research team with an invaluable perspective, social desirability bias or perceived demand characteristics may impede objectivity rendering results unreliable. Where research/clinical experts are employed, we must recognise the power differentials such a research team entering a community invariably introduces. Moreover, highly trained coders may have a limited, theoretically determined gaze and may not recognise nuanced meaningful data, rendering them blind to the emergence of subtle differences not otherwise expected. There is much room for more emphasis and funding to develop local experts to engage in knowledge creation.

Language considerations

Notably, even though it is relevant for coding of sensitivity and dyadic interactions, only one study reported utilising a first-language speaker to code sensitivity (Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Woolgar, Murray and Molteno1999) and one other was coded by a clinician with assistance from a first-language research assistant (Tomlinson et al., Reference Tomlinson, Cooper and Murray2005) – the implications of this should be considered. When videos are not translated, coding relies on non-verbal behaviour. Three studies acknowledge this (Bozicevic et al., Reference Bozicevic, De Pascalis, Schuitmaker, Tomlinson, Cooper and Murray2016; Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2021) when videos are coded solely on observations of non-verbal interactions, coders risk missing nuanced incongruences between verbal and non-verbal communication. On the other hand, as noted in the Malian study (McMahan-True et al., Reference McMahan-True, Pisani and Oumar2001) where coding solely occurred based on written reports of videoed interactions, non-verbal communication could have been missed. Furthermore, if videos are not translated and subtitled, language could be variably interpreted and therefore coded differently by different interpreters or coders.

Concepts and limiting lenses

Most measures included in the review above have been developed in contexts that do not mirror LMIC environments and we must consider the potential of them providing misleading negative characterisations about behaviour, which can influence public opinion about cultures foreign to individualistic western developed ones. For example, anthropological studies of the Gusii in Kenya (between the 1960s and 1990s), who have complex cultural practices in relation to marriage affecting child rearing, missed nuanced maternal sensitive behaviours. The belief that Gusii mothers are highly insensitive in their caregiving has been refuted in subsequent observations (Mesman, Minter, et al., Reference Mesman, Minter, Angnged, Cisse, Salali and Migliano2018).

The papers reviewed gave some consideration to the appropriateness of their measures. For example, Cooper et al. (Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Woolgar, Murray and Molteno1999) noted that despite being developed in the UK their maternal sensitivity measure was reliable in the context of Khayelitsha. The measure was utilised in a variety of contexts where meaningful group differences emerged allaying concerns about the Khayelitsha sample’s very high prevalence of maternal insensitive behaviours. The authors concluded that even though the overall mean ratings were low, many mothers were rated as highly sensitive suggesting that the measure was sensitive to variations in the sample. However, the only researchers to formally investigate measures assessed the performance of two established measures of maternal sensitivity and cautioned against accepting sensitivity to variation as evidence of validity (Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Bain and Mesman2018, Reference Dawson, Bain and Mesman2021). These authors found that the MBQS did result in some mothers being rated as highly sensitive; however, these profiles were not consistent with those obtained for the same sample using the Ainsworth Sensitivity Scale, with the latter resulting in a far larger proportion of interactions being coded as sensitive. The difficulty of being unable to capture something a tool is not designed to measure remains a critical issue with which researchers must engage. Damaging theories based on biased measures and interpretation have been dangerous in the past (Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010; Mesman, Minter, et al., Reference Mesman, Minter, Angnged, Cisse, Salali and Migliano2018). Researchers have a social, psychological, and ethical responsibility to produce scientific knowledge that does not harm but rather advances understanding of the nuanced social and ecological context of a child.

Careful detailed attention must thus be paid to interpretation of behaviours in non-western samples as this can affect coding. The Ethiopian intervention study (Klein & Rye, Reference Klein and Rye2004) was careful to use local cultural norms and values in relation to the cognitive and emotional needs of the child during parental sensitization. The Weigh-In measure in the Malian study is the only novel non-western measure developed for this context. This study also documented potential culture-specific frightened/frightening behaviour and its impact on maternal sensitivity. These authors (McMahan-True et al., Reference McMahan-True, Pisani and Oumar2001) deliberately accounted for the possibility that Dogon infants would show behaviour patterns not captured by traditional attachment classifications. Importantly, they were able to work in collaboration with a local psychiatrist well versed in mother–infant relations. More recently, Mesman has worked in close collaboration with local researchers in various contexts to address the need for nuanced knowledge of maternal sensitive behaviours (Alsarhi et al., Reference Alsarhi, Rahma, M.J., Alink and Mesman2018; Asanjarani et al., Reference Asanjarani, Abadi, Ghomi and Mesman2018; Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Bain and Mesman2018; Fourment et al., Reference Fourment, Noblega, Conde, Del Prado and Mesman2018; Fourment et al., Reference Fourment, Noblega, Conde, Del Prado and Mesman2018; Mesman, Basweti, et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2018; Ribeiro-Accioly et al., Reference Ribeiro-Accioly, Seidl-De-Moura, Mendes and Mesman2018; Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2021; Pitillas et al., Reference Pitillas, Berastegui, Halty, Rodriguez, Kamara and Mesman2021). Sensitivity measures, such as the MBQS mini which set relatively rigid definitions for sensitive responding appropriate for WEIRD countries, are being challenged (Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Bain and Mesman2018, Reference Dawson, Bain and Mesman2021; Mesman, Basweti, et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2018; Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2021).

Defining the difference between sensitivity and warmth

In Ainsworth’s original work on attachment and sensitivity in Uganda, it was clear that dyads were more securely attached, and a higher degree of sensitive behaviours were displayed in comparison to her Baltimore sample (MacDonald, Reference MacDonald1992). MacDonald (Reference MacDonald1992) proposed that there is a difference between warmth-seeking as a positive social reward system versus security-seeking in attachment which is fear or anxiety motivated. Ugandan mothers may not have displayed warm behaviours in the form of hugging and kissing but were highly attuned to their infants needs and signals. Two general conceptions of infant care can be observed cross-culturally (Keller et al., Reference Keller, Lohaus, Völker, Cappenberg and Chasiotis1999; Keller et al., Reference Keller, Bard, Morelli, Chaudhary, Vicedo, Rosabal-Coto, Scheidecker, Murray and Gottlieb2018) – 1) fostering close proximity to primary caregivers through extended body contact via carrying and co-sleeping arrangements, with indirect dyadic interactions, seems prevalent in non-western environments and in contrast 2) the dominant concept of care in western environments comprises mostly direct dyadic and short-term face to face interactive episodes. Three studies reviewed here (Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Woolgar, Murray and Molteno1999; Tomlinson et al., Reference Tomlinson, Cooper and Murray2005; Bozicevic et al., Reference Bozicevic, De Pascalis, Schuitmaker, Tomlinson, Cooper and Murray2016) have reported that warmth was included in their maternal sensitivity measure. No further expansion on the particular role and effects of warmth was reported. In this review, only one study (Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Basweti and Misati2021) of the Gusii addresses warmth as a discreet separate component for discussion. Mesman notes that while less frequent than has been observed in western samples, behavioural expressions of warmth through hugging, kissing, smiling, and touch were present. There has been a tendency to generalise western notions and expectations of maternal competencies to the rest of the world. Assessing warmth as part of sensitivity is questionable when in some languages no words exist for parental warmth, love, and affection (Keller et al., Reference Keller, Bard, Morelli, Chaudhary, Vicedo, Rosabal-Coto, Scheidecker, Murray and Gottlieb2018). As mentioned previously, this should be cautioned against and a nuanced understanding of the different components of maternal in/sensitivity should be allowed to emerge. Whether warmth is necessary to foster emotional and physical safety for the survival of the child has been questioned (MacDonald, Reference MacDonald1992). A caregiver could display warm characteristics, yet attune inappropriately to the emotional and physical needs of their child. Potentially, differing parenting goals between a predominantly individualistic western reward-driven context and historically community-driven non-western rural societies could motivate divergent approaches to creating healthy bonds between caregiver and infant.

Limitations

A conventional PRISMA compliant search method could not be used because a search using all the key search terms turned up no papers. For a yield of pertinent study data, six searches employing different combinations of the core foci were made. Highly heterogenous research foci and variable methods prohibited any direct or statistical comparison of findings and made it challenging to descriptively compare studies.

Sub-Saharan Africa has several Francophone countries, but French language papers were not considered for this review.

This review focussed on research using objective quantitative coding of observed maternal sensitivity. The use of maternal sensitivity exclusively rather than including non-gendered terms such as parental sensitivity (Broom, Reference Broom1994; Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, van and Juffer2003; Mesman & Emmen, Reference Mesman and Emmen2013) as a search term can be considered a limitation; however, sensitivity research is still in its infancy in SSA. In very recent studies of family roles undertaken in South Africa (Hatch & Posel, Reference Hatch and Posel2018; Adebiyi et al., Reference Adebiyi, Goldschmidt, Benjamin, Sonn and Roman2021), females are still primary caregivers. In contrast, maternal sensitivity research has been extensive in the global north, and the limited focus of sensitivity in relation to mother as primary caregiver has expanded its gaze to fathers as primary caregivers (West et al., Reference West, Lewis, Ram, Barnes, Leach, Sylva and Stein2009; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Foley and Golombok2022). SSA sensitivity research should explore if and how such expansion is applicable. In South Africa, for example, many children are raised by their grandmothers or are from female-headed single parent households ( Stats SA General Household Survey, 2021). We note however that the exclusion of non-gendered search terms in our search may have resulted in some papers being missed in this review.

Additionally, although these were not included in our review, theoretical (critical literature reviews) and qualitative papers have added to the characterisation and reconceptualization of the concept in sub-Saharan Africa (Dawson, Reference Dawson2022).

Conclusion

Maternal sensitivity is an important construct that was developed including seminal evidence from Africa. However, there is relatively little work on it since Ainsworth’s research in sub-Saharan Africa. The review highlights the limited research available, along with extremely limited knowledge regarding risk factors and child outcomes related to maternal sensitivity. Considering the importance of understanding sensitive caregiving in aid of fostering emotionally and physically healthy children, additional research in this area should be encouraged. The positive influence of reviewed interventions on maternal sensitivity, albeit small and modest, could be encouraging for further implementation. Critically, what has emerged from this review are some interesting and important reflections on the landscape of maternal sensitivity research. Fundamental methodological principles such as translation and validation of measures for non-western contexts must be implemented. Issues such as researcher gaze and its impact on the nature and quality of knowledge created must be taken into account. Researcher awareness of potentially unique caregiving systems and role of mother/caregiver within the sample must inform future responsible research design. It is hopeful that within the last 5 years nuanced sensitivity knowledge from LMICS has been generated (Alsarhi et al., Reference Alsarhi, Rahma, M.J., Alink and Mesman2018; Asanjarani et al., Reference Asanjarani, Abadi, Ghomi and Mesman2018; Fourment et al., Reference Fourment, Noblega, Conde, Del Prado and Mesman2018; Mesman, Reference Mesman2018; Ribeiro-Accioly et al., Reference Ribeiro-Accioly, Seidl-De-Moura, Mendes and Mesman2018), and we hope for significant growth and expansion as seen in the global north.

References

Adebiyi, BO, Goldschmidt, T, Benjamin, F, Sonn, IK and Roman, NV (2021) Exploring the perspectives of south african parents and primary caregivers living in low-income communities on what children need to thrive within the first 1000 days of life. Children (Basel) 8(6), 483. doi: 10.3390/children8060483.Google ScholarPubMed
Ainsworth, MDS, Blehar, MC, Waters, E and Wall, S (1978) Patterns of attachment: a psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Aival-Naveh, E, Rothschild-Yakar, L and Kurman, J (2019) Keeping culture in mind: a systematic review and initial conceptualization of mentalizing from a cross-cultural perspective. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 26(4). doi: 10.1111/cpsp.12300.Google Scholar
Alsarhi, K, Rahma, Prevoo, M.J., L, Alink, LRA and Mesman, J (2018) Observing sensitivity in slums in Yemen: the veiled challenge. Attachment & Human Development 23, 176187. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1454058.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Althubaiti, A (2016) Information bias in health research: definition, pitfalls, and adjustment methods. Journal of Multidisciplinary Healthcare 9, 211217. doi: 10.2147/JMDH.S104807.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, M and Connor, P (2018) Sub-Saharan African immigrants in the US are often more educated than those in top European destinations. Pew Research Center.Google Scholar
Asanjarani, F, Abadi, FD, Ghomi, M and Mesman, J (2018) Video observations of maternal sensitivity in urban and rural Iran. Attachment & Human Development 23, 164175. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1454057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asante, A, Wasike, WSK and Ataguba, JE (2020) Health financing in Sub-Saharan Africa: from analytical frameworks to empirical evaluation. Applied Health Economics and Health Policy 18(6), 743746. doi: 10.1007/s40258-020-00618-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakermans-Kranenburg, MJ, van, IMH and Juffer, F (2003) Less is more: meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin 129(2), 195215. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaton, D, Bombardier, C, Guillemin, F and Ferraz, MB (2000) Guidelines for the process of cross-cultural adaptation of self-report measures. Spine 25(24), 31863191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biringen, Z, Derscheid, D, Vliegen, N, Closson, L and Easterbrooks, MA (2014) Emotional availability (EA): theoretical background, empirical research using the EA Scales, and clinical applications. Developmental Review 34(2), 114167. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2014.01.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biringen, Z and Robinson, J (1991) Emotional availability in mother-child interactions: a reconceptualization for research. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 61(2), 258271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boer, D, Hanke, K and He, J (2018) On detecting systematic measurement error in cross-cultural research: a review and critical reflection on equivalence and invariance tests. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 49(5), 713734. doi: 10.1177/0022022117749042.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, MH and Manian, N (2013) Maternal responsiveness and sensitivity reconsidered: some is more. Development and Psychopathology 25(4 Pt 1), 957971. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bozicevic, L, De Pascalis, L, Schuitmaker, N, Tomlinson, M, Cooper, PJ and Murray, L (2016) Longitudinal association between child emotion regulation and aggression, and the role of parenting: a comparison of three cultures. Psychopathology 49(4), 228235. doi: 10.1159/000447747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broesch, T, Rochat, P, Olah, K, Broesch, J and Henrich, J (2016) Similarities and differences in maternal responsiveness in three societies: evidence from Fiji, Kenya, and the United States. Child Development 87(3), 700711. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broom, BL (1994) Impact of marital quality and psychological well being on parental sensitivity. Nursing Research 43(3), 138143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooke, JE, Deneault, AA, Devereux, C, Eirich, R, Fearon, RMP and Madigan, S (2022) Parental sensitivity and child behavioral problems: a meta-analytic review. Child Development 93(5), 12311248. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, PJ, Landman, M, Tomlinson, M, Molteno, C, Swartz, L and Murray, L (2002) Impact of a mother infant intervention in an indigent peri-urban South Africa context. British Journal of Psychiatry 180(1), 7681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, PJ, Tomlinson, M, Swartz, L, Landman, M, Molteno, C, Stein, A, McPherson, K and Murray, L (2009) Improving quality of mother-infant relationship and infant attachment in socioeconomically deprived community in South Africa: randomised controlled trial. BMJ 338(apr14 2), b974b974. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, PJ, Tomlinson, M, Swartz, L, Woolgar, M, Murray, L and Molteno, C (1999) Postpartum depression and the mother infant relationship. British Journal of Psychiatry 175(6), 554558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, PJ, Vally, Z, Cooper, H, Radford, T, Sharples, A, Tomlinson, M and Murray, L (2013) Promoting mother-infant book sharing and infant attention and language development in an impoverished South African Population: a pilot study. Early Childhood Education Journal 42(2), 143152. doi: 10.1007/s10643-013-0591-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, S and Nicholas, L (2012) An overview of South African psychology. International Journal of Psychology 47(2), 89101. doi: 10.1080/00207594.2012.660160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cullen, K, Rhodes, P, Brockman, R, Hunt, C and Langtiw, CL (2021) Decolonising clinical psychology: national and international perspectives. Clinical Psychologist 24(3), 211222. doi: 10.1111/cp.12228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, N (2022) Contextual ghosts in the nursery: systemic influences on sensitive maternal responsiveness in a low to middle income country. Psychoanalytic Social Work 29(1), 126. doi: 10.1080/15228878.2021.1878044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, N, Bain, K and Mesman, J (2018) Comparing two measures of maternal sensitivity: goodness of fit with a South African cultural context. Attachment & Human Development, 19. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1454056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, N, Bain, K and Mesman, J (2021) Observing maternal sensitivity in a South African township: an exploratory study into behavioral features using different measures. Attachment & Human Development 23(2), 150163. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2020.1828531.Google Scholar
Deans, CL (2018) Maternal sensitivity, its relationship with child outcomes, and interventions that address it: a systematic literature review. Early Child Development and Care 190(2), 252275. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2018.1465415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, SD, LeVine, RA, Richman, A and Berry-Brazelton, T (1984) Mother child interaction around a teaching task: an African-American comparison. Child Development 55(4), 12521264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Downs, SH and Black, N (1998) The feasibility of creating a checklist for the assessment of the methodological quality both of randomised and non-randomised studies of health care interventions. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 52(6), 377384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fish, M (2001) Attachment in low SES rural Appalacian infants: contexual, infant and maternal interaction risk and protective factors. Infant Mental Health Journal 22, 641664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fourment, K, Noblega, M, Conde, G, Del Prado, JN and Mesman, J (2018) Maternal sensitivity in rural Andean and Amazonian Peru. Attachment & Human Development 23, 134149. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1454055.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraley, RC, Roisman, GI and Haltigan, JD (2013) The legacy of early experiences in development: formalizing alternative models of how early experiences are carried forward over time. Developmental Psychology 49(1), 109126. doi: 10.1037/a0027852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galal, S (2021) Gini coefficient in South Africa 2006-2015. https://www.statista.com/statistics/1127860/gini-coefficient-in-south-africa/ Google Scholar
Gelaye, B, Rondon, MB, Araya, R and Williams, MA (2016) Epidemiology of maternal depression, risk factors, and child outcomes in low-income and middle-income countries. The Lancet Psychiatry 3(10), 973982. doi: 10.1016/s2215-0366(16)30284-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grantham-McGregor, S, Cheung, YB, Cueto, S, Glewwe, P, Richter, L and Strupp, B (2007) Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. The Lancet 369(9555), 6070. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(07)60032-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gummerum, M, Hanoch, Y, Keller, M, Parsons, K and Hummel, A (2010) Preschoolers’ allocations in the dictator game: the role of moral emotions. Journal of Economic Psychology 31(1), 2534. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2009.09.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmeyer, E, Ispa, JM, Palermo, F and Carlo, G (2016) Predicting self-regulation and vocabulary and academic skills at kindergarten entry: the roles of maternal parenting stress and mother-child closeness. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 37, 153164. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.05.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, T, Sideris, J, Serpell, Z, Burchinal, M and Pickett, C (2014) Domain-specific cognitive stimulation and maternal sensitivity as predictors of early academic outcomes among low-income African American preschoolers. Journal of Negro Education 83(1), 1528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatch, M and Posel, D (2018) Who cares for children? A quantitative study of childcare in South Africa. Development Southern Africa 35(2), 267282. doi: 10.1080/0376835x.2018.1452716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J, Heine, SJ and Norenzayan, A (2010) The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33(2-3), 61–83; discussion 83-135. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosokawa, R and Katsura, T (2017) A longitudinal study of socioeconomic status, family processes, and child adjustment from preschool until early elementary school: the role of social competence. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 11(1), 62. doi: 10.1186/s13034-017-0206-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeenah, M and Pouris, A (2008) South African research in the context of Africa and globally. South African Journal of Science 104, 351354.Google Scholar
Jones, C, Foley, S and Golombok, S (2022) Parenting and child adjustment in families with primary caregiver fathers. Journal of Family Psychology 36(3), 406415. doi: 10.1037/fam0000915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joosen, KJ, Mesman, J, Bakermans-Kranenburg, MJ and van Ijzendoorn, MH (2012) Maternal sensitivity to infants in various settings predicts harsh discipline in toddlerhood. Attachment & Human Development 14(2), 101117. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2012.661217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, H, Bard, K, Morelli, G, Chaudhary, N, Vicedo, M, Rosabal-Coto, M, Scheidecker, G, Murray, M and Gottlieb, A (2018) The myth of universal sensitive responsiveness: comment on Mesman et al. (2017). Child Development 89(5), 19211928. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, H, Lohaus, A, Völker, S, Cappenberg, M and Chasiotis, A (1999) Temporal contingency as an independent component of parenting behavior. Child Development 70(2), 474485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kermoian, R and Leidermann, P (1986) Infant attachment to mother and child caretaker in an East African community. International Journal of Behavioral Development 9, 455469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiguwa, P and Segalo, P (2018) Decolonising psychology in residential and open distance e-learning institutions: critical reflections. South African Journal of Psychology 48(3), 310318. doi: 10.1177/0081246318786605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, PS and Rye, H (2004) Interaction orientated early intervention in Ethiopia - the MISC approach. Infants and Young Children 18, 340354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lohaus, A, Keller, H, Ball, J, Elban, C and Voelker, S (2001) Maternal sensitivity: components and relations to warmth and contingency. Parenting: Science and Practice 1(4), 267284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luiz, JM (2006) The wealth of some and the poverty of Sub Saharan Africa. International Journal of Social Economics 33(9), 625648. doi: 10.1108/03068290610683422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luiz, JM (2013) A review of social welfare in Sub-Saharan Africa: from the colonial legacy to the millennium development goals. Economic Papers: A Journal of Applied Economics and Policy 32(1), 110121. doi: 10.1111/1759-3441.12013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, K (1992) Warmth as a developmental construct: an evolutionary analysis. Child Development 63(4), 753773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maluleke, R (2018) Men Women and Children, Findings of the Living Conditions Survey. Available at https://www.statssa.gov.za/?page_id=1854&PPN=Report-03-10-02.Google Scholar
McMahan-True, M, Pisani, L and Oumar, F (2001) Infant mother attachment among the Dogon of Mali. Child Development 72(5), 14511466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesman, J (2018) Video observations of sensitive caregiving “off the beaten track”: introduction to the special issue. Attachment & Human Development 23, 115123. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1454052.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesman, J, Basweti, N and Misati, J (2018) Sensitive infant caregiving among the rural Gusii in Kenya. Attachment & Human Development 23, 124133. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1454053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesman, J, Basweti, N and Misati, J (2021) Sensitive infant caregiving among the rural Gusii in Kenya. Attachment & Human Development 23(2), 124133. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2020.1828512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesman, J and Emmen, RA (2013) Mary Ainsworth’s legacy: a systematic review of observational instruments measuring parental sensitivity. Attachment & Human Development 15(5-6), 485506. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2013.820900.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesman, J, Minter, T, Angnged, A, Cisse, IAH, Salali, GD and Migliano, AB (2018) Universality without uniformity: a culturally inclusive approach to sensitive responsiveness in infant caregiving. Child Development 89(3), 837850. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12795.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesman, J, van Ijzendoorn, MH and Bakermans-Kranenburg, MJ (2012) Unequal in opportunity, equal in process: parental sensitivity promotes positive child development in ethnic minority families. Child Development Perspectives 6(3), 239250. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00223.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, L and Cooper, PJ (1997) Postpartum depression and child development. Psychological Medicine 27(2), 253260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, L, De Pascalis, L, Tomlinson, M, Vally, Z, Dadomo, H, MacLachlan, B, Woodward, C and Cooper, PJ (2016) Randomized controlled trial of a book-sharing intervention in a deprived South African community: effects on carer-infant interactions, and their relation to infant cognitive and socioemotional outcome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 57(12), 13701379. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, L, Fiori-Cowley, A and Hooper, R (1996) The impact of postnatal depression and associated adversity on early mother-infant interactions and later infant outcome. Child Development 2512(5), 19962526.Google Scholar
Murray-Kolb, LE and Beard, JL (2009) Iron deficiency and child and maternal health. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 89(3), 946S950S. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2008.26692D.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nsamenang, AB and Dawes, A (1998) Developmental psychology as political psychology in Sub-Saharan Africa: the challenge of Africanisation. Applied Psychology: An International Review 47(1), 7387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, SR, Tully, MA, Ryan, B, Bradley, JM, Baxter, GD and McDonough, SM (2015) Failure of a numerical quality assessment scale to identify potential risk of bias in a systematic review: a comparison study. BMC Research Notes 8(1), 224. doi: 10.1186/s13104-015-1181-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perez, EM, Hendricks, MK, Beard, JL, Murray-Kolb, LE, Berg, A, Tomlinson, M, Irlam, J, Isaacs, W, Njengele, T, Sive, A, Vernon-Feagans, L (2005) Mother infant interactions and infant development are altered by maternal iron deficiency anemia. The Journal of Nutrition 135(4), 850855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitillas, C, Berastegui, A, Halty, A, Rodriguez, P, Kamara, M and Mesman, J (2021) Enhancing maternal sensitivity in contexts of urban extreme poverty in Sierra Leone: a pilot study. Infant Mental Health Journal 42(6), 812822. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21945.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ribeiro-Accioly, ACL, Seidl-De-Moura, ML, Mendes, D and Mesman, J (2018) Maternal sensitivity in mother-infant interactions in Rio de Janeiro. Attachment & Human Development 23, 761770. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1454059.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seehawer, MK (2018) Decolonising research in a Sub-Saharan African context: exploring Ubuntu as a foundation for research methodology, ethics and agenda. International Journal of Social Research Methodology 21(4), 453466. doi: 10.1080/13645579.2018.1432404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stats SA General Household Survey (2021) https://www.statssa.gov.za/?page_id=1854&PPN=P0318&SCH=73293.Google Scholar
Tomlinson, M, Cooper, PJ and Murray, L (2005) The mother-infant relationship and infant attachment in a South African peri-urban settlement. Child Development 76(5), 10441054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomlinson, M, Rabie, S, Skeen, S, Hunt, X, Murray, L and Cooper, PJ (2020) Improving mother-infant interaction during infant feeding: a randomised controlled trial in a low-income community in South Africa. Infant Mental Health Journal 41(6), 850858. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21881.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trapolini, T, Ungerer, JA and McMahon, CA (2008) Maternal depression: relations with maternal caregiving representations and emotional availability during the preschool years. Attachment & Human Development 10(1), 7390. doi: 10.1080/14616730801900712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valenzuela, M (1997) Maternal sensitivity in developing society: the context of urban poverty and infant chronic undernutrition. Developmental Psychology 33(5), 845855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, SP, Wachs, TD, Grantham-McGregor, S, Black, MM, Nelson, CA, Huffman, SL, Baker-Henningham, H, Chang, SM, Hamadani, JD, Lozoff, B, Gardner, JMM, Powell, CA, Rahman, A, Richter, L (2011) Inequality in early childhood: risk and protective factors for early child development. The Lancet 378(9799), 13251338. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(11)60555-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, SP, Wachs, TD, Meeks Gardner, J, Lozoff, B, Wasserman, GA, Pollitt, E and Carter, JA (2007) Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. The Lancet 369(9556), 145157. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(07)60076-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, AF, Lewis, S, Ram, B, Barnes, J, Leach, P, Sylva, K and Stein, A (2009) Why do some fathers become primary caregivers for their infants? A qualitative study. Child: Care, Health and Development 35(2), 208216. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2008.00926.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whittaker, JEV, Harden, BJ, See, HM, Meisch, AD and Westbrook, TPR (2011) Family risks and protective factors: pathways to Early Head Start toddlers’ social-emotional functioning. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26(1), 7486. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.04.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yapp, E and Pickett, KE (2019) Greater income inequality is associated with higher rates of intimate partner violence in Latin America. Public Health 175, 8789. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2019.07.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Search terms

Figure 1

Fig. 1 Search Flow Diagram.

Figure 2

Table 2. Maternal sensitivity observation coding methods and key results

Figure 3

Table 3. Risks and outcome measures and results

Figure 4

Table 4. Cross-cultural research considerations in data collection and coding not assessed by downs and black rating