Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T18:50:45.718Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Decision-making difficulties mediate the association between poor emotion regulation and eating disorder symptoms in adolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 March 2022

Marta Francesconi*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Human Development, Institute of Education, University College London, 25 Woburn Square, London WC1H 0AA, UK
Eirini Flouri
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Human Development, Institute of Education, University College London, 25 Woburn Square, London WC1H 0AA, UK
Amy Harrison
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Human Development, Institute of Education, University College London, 25 Woburn Square, London WC1H 0AA, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Marta Francesconi, E-mail: m.francesconi@ucl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The emergence of eating problems during childhood increases the risk for eating disorders (EDs) during young adulthood. Previous studies highlight a relationship between poor self-regulation and onset of eating pathology. In this study, we investigated whether this association is mediated by decision-making difficulties.

Methods

To test this hypothesis, we used data from the Millennium Cohort Study. Decision-making performance was assessed with the Cambridge Gambling Task at age 11. Principal components analysis was used to derive an index of ED symptoms at age 14. The trajectories of scores of two subscales of the Child Social Behaviour Questionnaire, Independence and Self-Regulation (ISR) and Emotional Dysregulation (EmotDy), were modelled from ages 3 to 7 years in a latent growth curve analysis. The individual predicted values of the intercept (set at baseline, 3 years) and the slope (rate of annual change) were then used in the mediation analysis.

Results

In our sample of 11 303 individuals, there was evidence for mediation by three measures of decision-making at age 11 (poor quality of decision-making, delay aversion and low risk-adjustment) in the association between EmotDy across ages 3–7 and ED symptoms at age 14 even after the adjustment for relevant covariates. We found no evidence of association between ISR and ED symptoms.

Conclusion

Our findings suggest that emotion regulation processes during childhood may be relevant for the future onset of ED symptoms via their association with decision-making skills. These findings, obtained from a large, representative, sample, shed light on the relationship between self-regulation, decision-making and symptoms of EDs.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Eating disorders (EDs) are severe mental illnesses known to have physical, psychological and cognitive impacts (Smink, van Hoeken, & Hoek, Reference Smink, van Hoeken and Hoek2013; Smith, Xiao, & Bechara, Reference Smith, Xiao and Bechara2012). Individuals with EDs often report serious medical morbidity and psychiatric comorbidity (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Xiao and Bechara2012; Steinhausen, Reference Steinhausen2009) and the National Health Service (NHS) has recently estimated that costs for EDs across the UK are likely to be more than £6.8–8 billion per year (Beat, Reference Beat2015; Demmler, Brophy, Marchant, John, & Tan, Reference Demmler, Brophy, Marchant, John and Tan2020). ED symptomatology typically begins during early adolescence (Kotler, Cohen, Davies, Pine, & Walsh, Reference Kotler, Cohen, Davies, Pine and Walsh2001; Zipfel, Giel, Bulik, Hay, & Schmidt, Reference Zipfel, Giel, Bulik, Hay and Schmidt2015) and having problems with eating, weight and shape during childhood increases the risk for EDs developing in young adulthood 7.3 times (Kotler et al., Reference Kotler, Cohen, Davies, Pine and Walsh2001). Therefore, there is an urgent need to understand what early factors are involved in the onset of eating problems.

Self-regulation represents the capacity to adjust cognitive, emotional and behavioural reactions to reach a desired outcome. It includes being able to resist highly emotional reactions to upsetting situations, to calm yourself down when you get upset, to adjust to a change in expectations, and to handle frustration without an outburst. The strategies that are used to regulate these responses may be adaptive, such as reappraisal, problem-solving and acceptance or maladaptive, such as suppression, avoidance and rumination (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010). Self-regulation has been identified as a potential risk factor for EDs (Donofry, Roecklein, Wildes, Miller, & Erickson, Reference Donofry, Roecklein, Wildes, Miller and Erickson2016; Mallorquí-Bagué et al., Reference Mallorquí-Bagué, Vintró-Alcaraz, Sánchez, Riesco, Agüera, Granero and Fernández-Aranda2018; Prefit, Cândea, & Szentagotai-Tătar, Reference Prefit, Cândea and Szentagotai-Tătar2019). Recent evidence shows that self-regulation processes play a crucial role in many developmental outcomes and may contribute to the maintenance of EDs (Treasure & Schmidt, Reference Treasure and Schmidt2013). In particular, it has been found that patients with EDs show poorer behavioural control and difficulty in their overall ability to regulate emotions (Johnson & Wardle, Reference Johnson and Wardle2005; Ruscitti, Rufino, Goodwin, & Wagner, Reference Ruscitti, Rufino, Goodwin and Wagner2016).

One explanation for such findings may be that ED symptoms, like restricting, bingeing and purging, may function as coping mechanisms to unpleasant emotional states (Burns, Fischer, Jackson, & Harding, Reference Burns, Fischer, Jackson and Harding2012; Overton, Selway, Strongman, & Houston, Reference Overton, Selway, Strongman and Houston2005).

The ability to regulate emotional and behavioural responses starts developing during early childhood and its stability over time is well established (Edossa, Schroeders, Weinert, & Artelt, Reference Edossa, Schroeders, Weinert and Artelt2018; Raffaelli, Crockett, & Shen, Reference Raffaelli, Crockett and Shen2005). Given its developmental relevance, self-regulation has also been studied in the context of executive functioning skills, including attentional control, cognitive inhibition, inhibitory control, working memory and decision-making (Dohle, Diel, & Hofmann, Reference Dohle, Diel and Hofmann2018; Kittel, Schmidt, & Hilbert, Reference Kittel, Schmidt and Hilbert2017), but also decision-making under risk (Martin & Delgado, Reference Martin and Delgado2011). Decision-making skills represent the ability to select between two or more alternatives to reach the best outcome in the shortest time. In a recent study, the authors found attenuated blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signals in the striatum, a brain structure previously linked with reward-related processing, in those showing strengths in emotion regulation (Martin & Delgado, Reference Martin and Delgado2011). In turn, reward-related processing, risk-taking and decision-making under risk in general are related to EDs (Harrison, O'Brien, Lopez, & Treasure, Reference Harrison, O'Brien, Lopez and Treasure2010; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Xiao and Bechara2012). For example, we recently found in a large population sample that the risk of developing ED symptoms was higher in those who showed higher risk-taking and lower in those with better decision-making (Francesconi, Flouri, & Harrison, Reference Francesconi, Flouri and Harrison2020).

However, what we did not investigate, and is still unexplored, is the possible role played by self-regulation processes in the association between decision-making and ED symptoms. Increasing knowledge on the association between self-regulation, decision-making and EDs will help understanding of the development of eating pathology. Moreover, it will provide evidence for planning targeted interventions, which are greatly needed especially in young individuals. Thus, this study aims to be the first to test the hypothesis that decision-making skills (measured with a gambling task) in late childhood mediate the association between self-regulation abilities in early childhood and ED symptoms in early adolescence. We hypothesised that decision-making ability could play a mediation role, given its strong association with both ED pathology and self-regulation. To achieve this, we examined associations between the trajectories of two domains of children's self-regulation, (a) independence and self-regulation and (b) emotional dysregulation across ages 3–7 with decision-making abilities at age 11, in turn associated with ED symptoms at age 14 while adjusting for pubertal status, maternal psychological distress, family poverty, IQ and other relevant covariates. The ED symptoms that we considered in our study describe subtle, prodromal signs of eating pathology. Exploring the prodromal phase which precedes the onset of ED, rather than a full-blown syndrome, may provide helpful information to structure earlier intervention and thus achieve better outcomes.

Methods

Study sample

The Millennium Cohort Study (MCS) has been following the lives of 19 244 young people born across England, Scotland, Wales and Northern Ireland in 2000–2002 over a series of six sweeps. The study began with an original sample of 18 818 cohort members (Plewis, Calderwood, Hawkes, Hughes, & Joshi, Reference Plewis, Calderwood, Hawkes, Hughes and Joshi2007). The main data collection methods used during the study have included questionnaires, cognitive assessments and interviewer-administered physical measurements. The MCS sample was designed to provide a proper representation of the total population. In order to do so, certain subgroups of the population were intentionally over-sampled, namely children living in disadvantaged areas, children of ethnic minority backgrounds and children growing up in the three smaller nations of the UK. The disproportionate representation of these groups ensures that typically hard to reach populations are adequately represented and that sample sizes are sufficient for the analysis of ethnic minorities, those from disadvantaged backgrounds and children within each of the UK nations. In this study, we used data from sweep 2 (age 3), when self-regulation was first measured in the MCS, to sweep 6 (age 14), the last available sweep of data, and when eating, dieting and body image questions were asked in MCS. Our analytic sample included singletons and first-born twins or triplets with available information on eating, dieting or body image questions at age 14 and with available data on the Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT) at either age it was measured in MCS, i.e. at 11 or 14 years (n = 11 303).

Measures

Decision-making under risky conditions

The CGT was developed to assess decision-making and risk-taking behaviour outside a learning context (Deakin, Aitken, Robbins, & Sahakian, Reference Deakin, Aitken, Robbins and Sahakian2004; Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Owen, Middleton, Williams, Pickard, Sahakian and Robbins1999). It is a subtest of the widely used and well-validated Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (Robbins et al., Reference Robbins, James, Owen, Sahakian, Lawrence, McInnes and Rabbitt1998). Participants are presented with a row of 10 boxes across the top of the screen, of which some are red and some are blue, and are told that a token is hidden behind one of them. They have to choose (a) which colour of box they believe the token is hidden behind (red or blue), and (b) the number of points they want to gamble. The proportion of red to blue boxes (box ratio) varies during the task pseudo-randomly to assess the influence of statistical risk (probability) on decision-making. The five CGT measures of decision-making that are used in this study include (1) the mean proportion of points bet on trials where the most probable colour was selected (risk-taking); (2) the mean proportion of trials where the most probable colour was selected (quality of decision-making); (3) the mean reaction time (in milliseconds) for making a selection (deliberation time); (4) the tendency to stake higher bets on favourable compared to unfavourable trials (risk adjustment); and (5) the time participants are prepared to wait in order to place a higher or lower bet (delay aversion). A sixth CGT measure, overall proportion bet (i.e. the mean proportion of points gambled across all trials), was excluded from our analysis in view of its very high correlation (>0.90) with risk-taking.

Eating disorder (ED) symptoms

In the MCS, a series of questions were asked at age 14 to assess eating and dieting attitudes and behaviours. These questions measured: body dissatisfaction (whether the participant reported a perception of their body as too overweight or not); intention to lose weight (the presence of a strong desire to weight); dietary restriction (whether the participant had ever actively eaten less to influence their shape/weight) and excessive exercise (whether the participant had ever exercised in a driven way in order to influence weight and shape). We also included in our analysis an objective measure of underweight and overweight based on the most widely-used reference panel, the UK90, which is sensitive to gender and age and developed for the British population. Cut-offs in our sample were based on the age of the cohort member at the time of interview. The underweight cut-off point was the second centile and the overweight cut-off point was the 85th centile, as suggested by the UK90 (Cole, Freeman, & Preece, Reference Cole, Freeman and Preece1995). Online Supplementary Table S3 reports the list of questions used to assess ED symptoms in this study.

Self-regulation

During the self-completion module of the parent-interview at ages 3, 5 and 7, parents completed 10 items from the Child Social Behaviour Questionnaire (CSBQ) (Hogan, Scott, & Bauer, Reference Hogan, Scott and Bauer1992). The main respondent completed two CSBQ subscales: the Independence and Self-Regulation (ISR) subscale which contains five items related to cognitive self-regulation (e.g. ‘persists in the face of difficult tasks’) and the Emotional Dysregulation (EmotDy) subscale which contains five items related to emotional self-regulation (e.g. ‘is easily frustrated’). Responses were on a three-point scale (not true, somewhat true and certainly true). Higher scores indicate more independence on the ISR subscale and more emotional dysregulation on the EmotDy subscale. All items were averaged, creating a total score ranging from 1 to 3. The full list of items can be found in online Supplementary Table S3.

Covariates

We controlled for a number of covariates known to be associated with exposure and outcome, including gender, ethnicity (according to the UK census groups of white, black, Indian, Pakistani/Bangladeshi, mixed or other), family poverty when the cohort child was aged 3 (below the poverty line or not), maternal mental health, assessed with the six-item Kessler scale of psychological distress (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Barker, Colpe, Epstein, Gfroerer, Hiripi and Zaslavsky2003) when the cohort child was aged 3, IQ, measured in the MCS at age 3 with the Naming Vocabulary subscale of the British Ability Scales (BAS) (Elliott, Smith, & McCulloch, Reference Elliott, Smith and McCulloch1996) and pubertal status at age 11 (breast growth or menstruation or hair on body for females, and voice change or facial hair or hair on body for males).

Statistical analysis

We performed a mediation analysis to test the association between the trajectories of self-regulation (measured at ages 3, 5 and 7) and ED symptoms at age 14, and the intervening role of decision-making ability in this association. All analyses were performed in STATA 16.0. In all models, the MCS sampling stratum was controlled to account for the disproportionate stratification of the MCS survey design. To better understand the participants’ ED symptoms, and to simplify results, we used principal components analysis (PCA) to combine into a single index the five items assessing eating and dieting attitudes and behaviour. We then fitted latent growth curve models to estimate the longitudinal trajectories of self-regulation from ages 3 to 7 years for both ISR and EmotDy subscales separately. The individual predicted values of the intercept (set at baseline at age 3 years) and the slope (rate of annual change) were then used in the mediation models. Using the predict command in STATA, we generated predictions for the out-of-sample cases, i.e. the cases that were not used in the original estimation. This command uses the maximum likelihood with missing values (MLMV) estimation for those who had data on at least one time-point and single imputation for those missing information on all time-points. In this way, both the intercepts of ISR and EmotDy and their slopes were estimated for our whole analytic sample (n = 11 303). The individual predicted values of the intercepts and the slopes were then saved and used in regression models testing for mediation. Missingness ranged between 0.1% (ethnicity) and 28.7% (risk adjustment at age 11), and, to handle it, we used multiple imputation by chained equations (MICE) (20 imputed datasets) (Royston & White, Reference Royston and White2011). To predict missing data, we used all variables selected for analysis models. During the imputation process, the MCS sampling stratum was controlled to account for the disproportionate stratification of the MCS sample. Mediation models were fitted using seemingly unrelated regression analysis.

Results

Principal components analysis (PCA)

The PCA confirmed the presence of a general underlying factor for the assessment of ED risk symptoms. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) test value was 0.75 indicating that the sampling was adequate. The component extracted showed positive loadings of roughly equal size on all variables which can be interpreted as an overall indicator of ED risk symptoms. The component accounted for 47% of the total variance. The loadings of the items on the underlying factor were as follows: body dissatisfaction 0.24, intention to lose weight 0.53, dietary restriction 0.50, excessive exercise 0.47 and the UK90 cut-offs 0.40.

Descriptive analysis

A total of 11 303 participants had at least one available measure of ED symptoms at age 14 and a valid measure of CGT at age 11. Multicollinearity among CGT measures was assessed by inspection of the variance inflation factor (VIF) values. The risk-taking and the overall proportion bet variables were very highly intercorrelated as discussed (r > 0.90, p < 0.001). They showed a VIF of 13.82, and we therefore excluded the overall proportion bet score from further analyses. Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics for the study variables including means and proportions for exposures, outcomes and covariates. The majority of our sample was not underweight or overweight according to the UK90 reference panel. A small proportion of our sample reported a perception of their body as very overweight whereas almost half of our sample reported dietary restraint and dieting behaviours. Finally, the majority of our analytic sample reported the use of exercise to influence body weight. Tables 2 and 3 show the correlation among exposures, outcomes and mediators. Correlations were generally low to moderate and the EmotDy subscale had higher correlations with both the CGT measures and the ED risk symptoms compared to the ISR subscale. EmotDy and ISR subscales were inversely related at all time-points, albeit weakly (−0.09 at age 3, −0.25 at age 5 and −0.32 at age 7).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics in the analytic sample (n = 11 303)

CGT, Cambridge Gambling Task; CSBQ, Child Social Behaviour Questionnaire.

Table 2. Pearson's correlations of the main study variables in the analytic sample (n = 11 303)

Note: ED, eating disorder; CGT, Cambridge Gambling Task; CSBQ, Child Social Behaviour Questionnaire; ISR, Independence Self-Regulation.

*p<0.05; **p<0.01.

Table 3. Pearson's correlations of the main study variables in the analytic sample (n = 11 303)

Note: ED, eating disorder; CGT, Cambridge Gambling Task; CSBQ, Child Social Behaviour Questionnaire; EMotDy, Emotional Dysregulation.

*p<0.05; **p<0.01.

Mediation models

We tested the effect of the intercept and the slope of EmotDy and ISR on ED risk symptoms via the CGT measures. We found no association between the intercept and the slope of ISR and ED symptoms (Table 4). However, we found an association between both the intercept and the slope of EmotDy and ED symptoms. This association was mediated by three out of the five CGT measures, as shown in Table 5. Quality of decision-making mediated the association between both the intercept and the slope of EmotDy and ED symptoms whereas risk adjustment and delay aversion mediated the association between only the intercept of EmotDy and ED symptoms. These results were robust to adjustment for covariates. Tables 4 and 5 present the results (direct, indirect and total effects) of our mediation models after adjustment in the imputed cases. We obtained the same results in the complete cases analysis, as displayed in online Supplementary Tables S1 and S2.

Table 4. Mediation (by CGT) models of independence-self regulation and ED risk symptoms

b, Unstandardised regression coefficient; s. e., standard error; CI, confidence interval; ED, eating disorder; CGT, Cambridge Gambling Task; ISR, Independence Self-Regulation.

Adjusted for: pubertal status, gender, ethnicity, maternal psychological distress at age 3, family poverty at age 3, IQ at age 3.

*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

Table 5. Mediation (by CGT) models of emotional dysregulation and ED risk symptoms

b, Unstandardised regression coefficient; s.e., standard error; CI, confidence interval; ED, eating disorder; CGT, Cambridge Gambling Task; EmotDy, Emotional Dysregulation.

Adjusted for: pubertal status, gender, ethnicity, maternal psychological distress at age 3, family poverty at age 3, IQ at age 3.

*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

Discussion

The aim of the study was to test for the first time the mediator effect of decision-making abilities at age 11 (delay aversion, risk adjustment, risk-taking, quality of decision-making and deliberation time) on the longitudinal association between self-regulation across ages 3–7 (ISR and EmotDy) and ED symptoms measured at age 14. We found, in a large population sample of 11 303 individuals, that poor quality of decision-making, poor risk adjustment and delay aversion mediated the positive association between EmotDy and ED symptoms and this result was robust to adjustment for covariates.

With respect to the trajectories of ISR and EmotDy from age 3 to 7 that we estimated, our sample showed a small increase over time in ISR, and a small decline over time in EmotDy. These results support previous findings describing self-regulation as a stable ability (Edossa et al., Reference Edossa, Schroeders, Weinert and Artelt2018; Raffaelli et al., Reference Raffaelli, Crockett and Shen2005). We found that both the intercept (set at age 3) and the slope (which measured the rate of change from age 3 to 7 years) of EmotDy were associated with ED symptoms at age 14 and this finding is in line with previous evidence that identified this specific construct of self-regulation as a risk factor for the development and maintenance of all forms of eating pathology (Mallorquí-Bagué et al., Reference Mallorquí-Bagué, Vintró-Alcaraz, Sánchez, Riesco, Agüera, Granero and Fernández-Aranda2018; Prefit et al., Reference Prefit, Cândea and Szentagotai-Tătar2019). It has been hypothesised that ED symptoms may be maladaptive strategies used to manage negative emotions, hence the expression of poor emotion regulation (Burns et al., Reference Burns, Fischer, Jackson and Harding2012; Fairburn, Reference Fairburn2008; Harrison, Sullivan, Tchanturia, & Treasure, Reference Harrison, Sullivan, Tchanturia and Treasure2009; Overton et al., Reference Overton, Selway, Strongman and Houston2005; Treasure & Schmidt, Reference Treasure and Schmidt2013). Following this hypothesis, dysfunctional eating behaviours such as vomiting, excessive exercise, eating restriction and binge eating would emerge to regulate unpleasant emotional states (Evers, Marijn Stok, & de Ridder, Reference Evers, Marijn Stok and de Ridder2010; Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Sullivan, Tchanturia and Treasure2009). This explanation indicates that the emotion regulation processes may have a central role in the aetiology of ED symptom development as is also suggested by our findings. In view of the evidence that emotion regulation strategies can be modified (Mallorquí-Bagué et al., Reference Mallorquí-Bagué, Vintró-Alcaraz, Sánchez, Riesco, Agüera, Granero and Fernández-Aranda2018; Preyde, Watson, Remers, & Stuart, Reference Preyde, Watson, Remers and Stuart2016), this in turn would suggest that a clear implication of these findings is the need for emotional skill interventions in early childhood. Mounting evidence supports the efficacy of early, comprehensive and developmentally based prevention programmes, such as The Incredible Years programmes, for reducing early risk factors related to several adverse outcomes in adolescence (Marcynyszyn, Maher, & Corwin, Reference Marcynyszyn, Maher and Corwin2011; Webster-Stratton, Jamila Reid, & Stoolmiller, Reference Webster-Stratton, Jamila Reid and Stoolmiller2008). Our results indicate that such programmes may be used to target dysfunctional emotion regulation during childhood with the final aim of reducing the risk for ED symptoms during adolescence.

The longitudinal association between decision-making abilities and ED symptoms identified in this study also corroborates previous findings regarding the role of cognition in the development of EDs (Garrido & Subirá, Reference Garrido and Subirá2013; Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, O'Brien, Lopez and Treasure2010; Kittel et al., Reference Kittel, Schmidt and Hilbert2017). Recent studies found that reduced functionality of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex was associated with worse decision-making performance in ED patients, suggesting a common substrate for impaired decision-making in EDs (Reiter, Heinze, Schlagenhauf, & Deserno, Reference Reiter, Heinze, Schlagenhauf and Deserno2017).

Finally, our results show, for the first time, that the association between EmotDy during early childhood and the expression of ED symptoms during adolescence was mediated by three out of five measures of decision-making. In particular, we found that (i) higher EmotDy was associated with lower quality of decision-making and with lower risk adjustment in turn associated with higher levels of ED symptoms, and that (ii) higher EmotDy was associated with more delay aversion in turn associated with higher ED symptomatology. The ability to regulate emotion is an essential coping mechanism and its development precedes the development of higher cognitive functions such as decision-making (Gilmore et al., Reference Gilmore, Shi, Woolson, Knickmeyer, Short, Lin and Shen2012; Martin & Delgado, Reference Martin and Delgado2011). In fact, from a neurodevelopmental perspective, the development of subcortical brain structures involved in the emotional processes, such as the amygdala, predates and may influence the development of cortical brain regions engaged with decision-making abilities such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Delgado, Nearing, LeDoux, & Phelps, Reference Delgado, Nearing, LeDoux and Phelps2008; Gilmore et al., Reference Gilmore, Shi, Woolson, Knickmeyer, Short, Lin and Shen2012). These cortical areas also showed reduced functionality in patients with EDs (Goodkind et al., Reference Goodkind, Eickhoff, Oathes, Jiang, Chang, Jones-Hagata and Korgaonkar2015; Reiter et al., Reference Reiter, Heinze, Schlagenhauf and Deserno2017; Voon et al., Reference Voon, Derbyshire, Rück, Irvine, Worbe, Enander and Sahakian2015). This interconnected neurobiological background could explain our findings.

Quality of decision-making mediated the association between both the intercept and the slope of EmotDy and ED symptoms, whereas risk adjustment and delay aversion were mediators of the association between the intercept, but not the slope, of EmotDy and ED symptoms. This result highlights the relevance of identifying early dysfunctional emotional regulation. We did not find any association between the ISR subscale and ED symptoms. This is in contrast with other studies which found impaired behavioural and cognitive regulation in individuals with EDs (Crino, Touyz, & Rieger, Reference Crino, Touyz and Rieger2019; Johnson & Wardle, Reference Johnson and Wardle2005). However, the behavioural and cognitive regulation domain was assessed in our study with a scale of five items, the ISR subscale, measuring the ability to focus and sustain attention and the ability or tendency to do things independently. The lack of specificity and the limited number of items may be an explanation for our null finding.

There are several important study limitations. First, given the observational design, causality cannot be inferred. Second, given the multidisciplinary nature of the MCS, we did not have a clinical interview for EDs available. However, it is also important to note that clinical diagnosis was not the focus of this work and that, as mentioned earlier, this study aimed to explore ED symptoms during early adolescence which as previously discussed are strongly related to high risk for EDs later in adolescence and early adulthood. Third, ED symptoms in the MCS were measured for the first time at age 14 so we could not control for them at age 11, when decision-making was first measured. Fourth, self-regulation was measured in the MCS with two scales, ISR and EmotDy, of five items each. As described earlier, self-regulation is a complex construct that includes cognitive, emotional and behavioural processes. In our study, we were not able to cover this complexity fully. However, we explored several important aspects of children's self-regulation. Finally, our sample showed different amounts of missing data across the CGT outcomes. There are two reasons that may explain this. First, in MCS, the CGT derived measures only included data when the most likely outcome was chosen, therefore some outcome measures do not include the rest of trials. Second, our analytic sample included singletons and first-born twins or triplets having at least one available CGT outcome and that answered to at least one eating-related question. This can explain the heterogeneous missingness rate across CGT outcomes. However, our sample selection strategy was chosen in order to be neither too conservative nor too liberal and we used a strong and reliable imputation strategy that relies on available data to replace missing data (MICE).

These limitations notwithstanding, our findings suggest that emotional dysregulation during early childhood is related to the emergence of ED symptoms during adolescence via suboptimal decision-making. These findings, obtained from a large, general population sample, provide an additional step towards understanding the relationship between self-regulation processes and symptoms of eating pathology.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172200037X

Data

We used data from Millennium Cohort Study, which are publicly available and can be downloaded from the UK data service website (https://ukdataservice.ac.uk/).

Acknowledgements

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study and the whole MCS team.

Financial support

This project is funded by a Medical Research Council awarded to Dr Amy Harrison, Principal Investigator and Professor Eirini Flouri, Co-Investigator, ref: MR/SO19707/1.

Conflict of interest

The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Ethical standards

The Millennium Cohort Study (MCS) received the ethics approval from the National Health Service (NHS) Research Ethics Committee (REC) system.

Consent to participate

In MCS written consent has been sought from parents for their participation and the participation of their child(ren). Written consent has also been sought for gathering information from health, education and economic records and from teachers.

Consent for publication

Written informed consent for publication was obtained from all the authors.

References

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beat, . (2015). The cost of eating disorders: Social, health and economic impacts. UK: PricewaterhouseCoopers LLP.Google Scholar
Burns, E. E., Fischer, S., Jackson, J. L., & Harding, H. G. (2012). Deficits in emotion regulation mediate the relationship between childhood abuse and later eating disorder symptoms. Child Abuse & Neglect, 36(1), 3239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, T. J., Freeman, J. V., & Preece, M. A. (1995). Body mass index reference curves for the UK, 1990. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 73(1), 2529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crino, N., Touyz, S., & Rieger, E. (2019). How eating disordered and non-eating disordered women differ in their use (and effectiveness) of cognitive self-regulation strategies for managing negative experiences. Eating and Weight Disorders-Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 24(5), 897904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deakin, J., Aitken, M., Robbins, T., & Sahakian, B. J. (2004). Risk taking during decision-making in normal volunteers changes with age. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(4), 590598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delgado, M. R., Nearing, K. I., LeDoux, J. E., & Phelps, E. A. (2008). Neural circuitry underlying the regulation of conditioned fear and its relation to extinction. Neuron, 59(5), 829838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demmler, J. C., Brophy, S. T., Marchant, A., John, A., & Tan, J. O. (2020). Shining the light on eating disorders, incidence, prognosis and profiling of patients in primary and secondary care: National data linkage study. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 216(2), 105112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dohle, S., Diel, K., & Hofmann, W. (2018). Executive functions and the self-regulation of eating behavior: A review. Appetite, 124, 49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donofry, S. D., Roecklein, K. A., Wildes, J. E., Miller, M. A., & Erickson, K. I. (2016). Alterations in emotion generation and regulation neurocircuitry in depression and eating disorders: A comparative review of structural and functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 68, 911927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edossa, A. K., Schroeders, U., Weinert, S., & Artelt, C. (2018). The development of emotional and behavioral self-regulation and their effects on academic achievement in childhood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 42(2), 192202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliott, C.D., Smith, P., & McCulloch, K. (1996). British ability scales (BAS II): Full age range. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson Ed.Google Scholar
Evers, C., Marijn Stok, F., & de Ridder, D. T. (2010). Feeding your feelings: Emotion regulation strategies and emotional eating. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(6), 792804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairburn, C. G. (2008). Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders. New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Francesconi, M., Flouri, E., & Harrison, A. (2020). Change in decision-making skills and risk for eating disorders in adolescence: A population based study. European Psychiatry, 63(1), 126. doi:10.1192/j.eurpsy.2020.92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrido, I., & Subirá, S. (2013). Decision-making and impulsivity in eating disorder patients. Psychiatry Research, 207(1–2), 107112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilmore, J. H., Shi, F., Woolson, S. L., Knickmeyer, R. C., Short, S. J., Lin, W., … Shen, D. (2012). Longitudinal development of cortical and subcortical gray matter from birth to 2 years. Cerebral Cortex, 22(11), 24782485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodkind, M., Eickhoff, S. B., Oathes, D. J., Jiang, Y., Chang, A., Jones-Hagata, L. B., … Korgaonkar, M. S. (2015). Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness. Jama Psychiatry, 72(4), 305315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, A., O'Brien, N., Lopez, C., & Treasure, J. (2010). Sensitivity to reward and punishment in eating disorders. Psychiatry Research, 177(1–2), 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, A., Sullivan, S., Tchanturia, K., & Treasure, J. (2009). Emotion recognition and regulation in anorexia nervosa. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy: An International Journal of Theory & Practice, 16(4), 348356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogan, A. E., Scott, K. G., & Bauer, C. R. (1992). The adaptive social behavior inventory (ASBI): A new assessment of social competence in high-risk three-year-olds. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 10(3), 230239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, F., & Wardle, J. (2005). Dietary restraint, body dissatisfaction, and psychological distress: A prospective analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(1), 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Barker, P. R., Colpe, L. J., Epstein, J. F., Gfroerer, J. C., Hiripi, E., … Zaslavsky, A. M. (2003). Screening for serious mental illness in the general population. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(2), 184189. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.2.184CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kittel, R., Schmidt, R., & Hilbert, A. (2017). Executive functions in adolescents with binge-eating disorder and obesity. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 50(8), 933941.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotler, L. A., Cohen, P., Davies, M., Pine, D. S., & Walsh, B. T. (2001). Longitudinal relationships between childhood, adolescent, and adult eating disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(12), 14341440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mallorquí-Bagué, N., Vintró-Alcaraz, C., Sánchez, I., Riesco, N., Agüera, Z., Granero, R., … Fernández-Aranda, F. (2018). Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic feature among eating disorders: Cross-sectional and longitudinal approach. European Eating Disorders Review, 26(1), 5361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcynyszyn, L. A., Maher, E. J., & Corwin, T. W. (2011). Getting with the (evidence-based) program: An evaluation of the Incredible Years Parenting Training Program in child welfare. Children and Youth Services Review, 33(5), 747757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, L. N., & Delgado, M. R. (2011). The influence of emotion regulation on decision-making under risk. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(9), 25692581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overton, A., Selway, S., Strongman, K., & Houston, M. (2005). Eating disorders – The regulation of positive as well as negative emotion experience. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 12(1), 3956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plewis, I., Calderwood, L., Hawkes, D., Hughes, G., & Joshi, H. (2007). Millennium Cohort Study: Technical report on sampling. London: Centre for Longitudinal Studies.Google Scholar
Prefit, A.-B., Cândea, D. M., & Szentagotai-Tătar, A. (2019). Emotion regulation across eating pathology: A meta-analysis. Appetite, 143, 104438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preyde, M., Watson, J., Remers, S., & Stuart, R. (2016). Emotional dysregulation, interoceptive deficits, and treatment outcomes in patients with eating disorders. Social Work in Mental Health, 14(3), 227244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raffaelli, M., Crockett, L. J., & Shen, Y.-L. (2005). Developmental stability and change in self-regulation from childhood to adolescence. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166(1), 5476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reiter, A. M., Heinze, H.-J., Schlagenhauf, F., & Deserno, L. (2017). Impaired flexible reward-based decision-making in binge eating disorder: Evidence from computational modeling and functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42(3), 628637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robbins, T. W., James, M., Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., Lawrence, A. D., McInnes, L., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1998). A study of performance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volunteers: Implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(5), 474490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M., Middleton, H. C., Williams, E. J., Pickard, J. D., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (1999). Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 19(20), 90299038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royston, P., & White, I. R. (2011). Multiple imputation by chained equations (MICE): Implementation in Stata. Journal of Statistical Software, 45(4), 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruscitti, C., Rufino, K., Goodwin, N., & Wagner, R. (2016). Difficulties in emotion regulation in patients with eating disorders. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation, 3(1), 3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smink, F. R., van Hoeken, D., & Hoek, H. W. (2013). Epidemiology, course, and outcome of eating disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 26(6), 543548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, D. G., Xiao, L., & Bechara, A. (2012). Decision making in children and adolescents: Impaired Iowa Gambling Task performance in early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), 1180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinhausen, H.-C. (2009). Outcome of eating disorders. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 18(1), 225242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treasure, J., & Schmidt, U. (2013). The cognitive-interpersonal maintenance model of anorexia nervosa revisited: A summary of the evidence for cognitive, socio-emotional and interpersonal predisposing and perpetuating factors. Journal of Eating Disorders, 1(1), 13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voon, V., Derbyshire, K., Rück, C., Irvine, M. A., Worbe, Y., Enander, J., … Sahakian, B. J. (2015). Disorders of compulsivity: A common bias towards learning habits. Molecular Psychiatry, 20(3), 345352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webster-Stratton, C., Jamila Reid, M., & Stoolmiller, M. (2008). Preventing conduct problems and improving school readiness: Evaluation of the incredible years teacher and child training programs in high-risk schools. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(5), 471488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zipfel, S., Giel, K. E., Bulik, C. M., Hay, P., & Schmidt, U. (2015). Anorexia nervosa: Aetiology, assessment, and treatment. The Lancet Psychiatry, 2(12), 10991111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics in the analytic sample (n = 11 303)

Figure 1

Table 2. Pearson's correlations of the main study variables in the analytic sample (n = 11 303)

Figure 2

Table 3. Pearson's correlations of the main study variables in the analytic sample (n = 11 303)

Figure 3

Table 4. Mediation (by CGT) models of independence-self regulation and ED risk symptoms

Figure 4

Table 5. Mediation (by CGT) models of emotional dysregulation and ED risk symptoms

Supplementary material: File

Francesconi et al. supplementary material

Francesconi et al. supplementary material

Download Francesconi et al. supplementary material(File)
File 34.2 KB