Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T17:31:48.749Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Intergenerational attrition: direct or reverse language transmission?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 December 2024

Silvina Montrul*
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics/Department of Spanish and Portuguese, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, United States
*
Corresponding author: Silvina Montrul; Email: montrul@illinois.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

It has been suggested that the parents of heritage speakers (2nd generation immigrants), who are the main source of input to them, may exhibit first-language (L1) attrition in their language, thereby directly transmitting different structural properties or “errors” to the heritage speakers. Given the state of current knowledge of inconsistent input in L1 acquisition, age of acquisition effects in bilingualism, and how long it takes children to master different properties of their native language, it is highly unlikely that immigrant parents are directly transmitting patterns of language attrition to their heritage language children. The argument advanced in this article is that if the patterns evident in heritage speakers and first-generation immigrants are related, reverse transmission may be at play instead, when the heritage speakers might be influencing the language of the parents rather than the other way around. Theoretical and empirical evidence for this proposal may explain the emergence of the variety of Spanish spoken in the United States.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Highlights

  • It has been suggested that the parents of heritage speakers (2nd generation immigrants), who are the main source of input to them, may exhibit first-language (L1) attrition in their language, thereby directly transmitting different structural properties or “errors” to the heritage speakers.

  • This is unlikely: Language attrition in adults is rare, while structural changes in heritage speakers are very common.

  • Like L1 children, heritage speakers do not directly replicate the input from the parents.

  • Structural divergence between child and adult grammars are not necessarily related; they arise independently.

  • If the patterns evident in heritage speakers and first-generation immigrants are related, reverse transmission may be at play instead, when the heritage speakers might be influencing the language of the parents rather than the other way around.

1. Introduction

Heritage speakers are native bilingual individuals who acquired their native language naturalistically at home since birth in a societal context where their native language is a minority language. Some heritage speakers acquired the heritage language together with the majority language (simultaneous bilinguals) while others enjoyed a period of monolingualism; the majority language was learned after the heritage language (sequential bilinguals), most likely upon (pre)school entry by age 4 or 5 (Carreira & Kagan, Reference Carreira and Kagan2011; Montrul, Reference Montrul2016a; Polinsky, Reference Polinsky2018; Rothman, Reference Rothman2009). Although many multilingual situations give rise to heritage language acquisition, in this article, I exclusively discuss heritage languages in the context of immigration, as spoken in the diaspora. If heritage speakers were born into the majority language environment, or arrived very early in childhood, they are typically referred to as second-generation immigrants (Silva-Corvalán, Reference Silva-Corvalán1994). The parents of heritage speakers are first-generation immigrants, native speakers of standard and nonstandard languages who grew up in their homeland (in monolingual or multilingual environments) and immigrated in adulthood.

Heritage languages typically develop in a language-contact situation together with the majority language. This bilingual setting renders conditions of reduced exposure and use of the heritage language during late childhood and adolescence, which directly impact the development and ultimate attainment of the heritage language. Many heritage speakers are dominant in the heritage language in early childhood, but as they socialize more in the majority language as they get older, the heritage language progressively weakens (Carreira & Kagan, Reference Carreira and Kagan2011). Research in the last three decades has shown that young adult heritage speakers display variable levels of proficiency in the heritage language, ranging from receptive bilinguals (overhearers, Au et al., Reference Au, Knightly, Jun and Oh2002, receptive bilinguals, Sherkina-Lieber et al., Reference Sherkina-Lieber, Pérez-Leroux and Johns2011) to fully fluent (Kupisch et al., Reference Kupisch, Barton, Hailer, Klaschik, Stangen, Lein and van de Weijer2014; Rao, Reference Rao2015). Most heritage speakers, however, display low to advanced proficiency in their productive abilities, along with structural changes (also “divergences”, “errors” or “innovations” depending on theoretical stance) in morphology, syntax, semantics, discourse pragmatics and less so in phonetics/phonology, compared to age-matched monolingual individuals in the homeland and to their own parents (first-generation immigrants) residing in the same sociolinguistic environment (Cuza & Pérez-Tattam, Reference Cuza and Pérez-Tattam2016; Montrul, Reference Montrul2016a; Polinsky, Reference Polinsky2018; Aalberse et al., Reference Aalberse, Backus and Muysken2019; Montrul & Polinsky, Reference Montrul and Polinsky2021; Rao, Reference Rao2015).

Although heritage speakers with the lowest proficiency in the heritage language display more widespread structural changes in their grammars than speakers with higher proficiency (Giancaspro et al., Reference Giancaspro, Perez-Cortes and Higdon2022; Pérez Cortés, Reference PérezCortés2022; Polinsky, Reference Polinsky2006), this general finding does not imply that heritage speakers with lower proficiency have acquired a “rogue”, haphazard grammar, as Bayram et al. (Reference Bayram, Kupisch, Cabo and Rothman2019) have suggested, but quite the contrary (Holmes & Putnam, Reference Holmes and Putnam2020; Putnam et al., Reference Putnam, Kupisch, Pascual y Cabo, Miller, Bayram, Rothman and Serratrice2018). In fact, Sherkina-Lieber (Reference Sherkina-Lieber2020) provides compelling evidence that receptive bilinguals maintain a (high) degree of representational complexity in their morphosyntax. Many of the structural patterns exhibited by heritage speakers, such as tendencies toward reduction, simplification and overgeneralization of vocabulary and morphology that affect syntax and long-distance dependencies, are systematic and arise from normal and natural processes of language acquisition and language change in a bilingual situation (Polinsky, Reference Polinsky2018; Polinsky & Scontras, Reference Polinsky and Scontras2020). For example, Ivanova-Sullivan et al. (Reference Ivanova-Sullivan, Sekerina, Tofighi and Polinsky2022) show how overgeneralization of prosodic constraints by Bulgarian heritage speakers leads to a lack of discrimination in processing and equal acceptance of ungrammatical and grammatical clitic positions.

How these structural patterns arise in heritage language speakers has been an important question, and debate on the sources of grammatical changes in their grammars has been intense. My position is that the changes documented in heritage language speakers typically emerge during language development. Although the basic structure of language emerges early in childhood (ages 3–4), its further growth and consolidation must be supported with optimal input conditions. A child needs to be exposed to the language frequently, through multiple interlocutors, and in different environments during the entire language learning process, which goes beyond age 3 or 4 and lasts until adolescence, so that the richest possible experience is provided. Aspects of grammar acquired early (by age 5), like phonology, syntax and morphology, get entrenched and consolidated with sustained language exposure until at least puberty (Berman, Reference Berman2004; Bylund, Reference Bylund2009).

Because heritage speakers are bilingual, the input they are exposed to in the diaspora is different in quantity and nature from the input monolingual children receive in the homeland. Monolingual children are exposed to 100% of the input in one language. The input to bilingual children, in contrast, must be split between the two languages (50–50%). Most often, however, the proportions are uneven for heritage speakers (40–60%, 30–70%, 20–80%, 90–10%), and the majority language receives more exposure than the minority language. According to some accounts (Montrul, Reference Montrul2008, Reference Montrul2016a; O’Grady et al., Reference O’Grady, Kwak, Lee and Lee2011; Polinsky, Reference Polinsky2006; Silva-Corvalán, Reference Silva-Corvalán2014, Reference Silva-Corvalán2018), insufficient input in the heritage language during childhood and adolescence leads to partial or incomplete acquisition of the heritage language; that is, while heritage speakers acquire all the basic structural properties (e.g., word order, case, gender, agreement, tenses) they do not fully master morphology (Montrul, Reference Montrul, Cho, Judy and Mendez2018a).

It has also been argued that the patterns characteristic of the grammatical development of heritage speakers, such as variable gender agreement, erosion of case distinction, stricter word order, overuse of overt subject pronouns, confusion with aspectual forms and verbal tenses, to name just a few, can be directly linked to the language of their parents. That is, structural changes are also related to the nature of the input, which is already affected by contact-induced changes in the parental generation (first-generation immigrants) when heritage speakers receive it (Pascual y Cabo, Reference Pascual y Cabo2020; Sorace, Reference Sorace2011, Reference Sorace2020). Direct transmission is how native language acquisition is believed to happen, a model also assumed in theories of diachronic language change (Kerswill, Reference Kerswill1996; Sankoff, Reference Sankoff2019), when continuity between the child and adult grammars is the norm. A myriad of complex factors are involved and interact in language change across generations over time. However, in this study, I will focus only on how the timing of acquisition of the heritage speakers and the length of residence (LOR) of adult immigrants may relate to each other to transmit or effect changes in their grammars.

Although direct transmission of contact-induced changes from the first to the second generation is a logical possibility, I argue that it is highly unlikely conceptually, when we consider the developmental schedules of heritage language acquisition from childhood to young adulthood and potential L1 attrition in adults. Furthermore, the direct parental transmission of structural changes would also go against theoretical accounts of diachronic language change that assume that changes are driven by disruptions in transmission (i.e., acquisition failure, as in Lightfoot, Reference Lightfoot1991, or parsing, as in Lightfoot, Reference Lightfoot2020). The direct intergenerational transmission possibility is also not generally supported empirically because reports of L1 attrition in adults at the level of morphosyntax, the grammar module where heritage speakers display most changes, are uncommon, and when found, not very extensive. I discuss empirical findings from studies of child heritage speakers and their parents on a variety of morphosyntactic phenomena that found no evidence of direct transmission of attrited output as input to heritage language grammars. However, in the few cases where the parental generation has been found to exhibit L1 attrition in morphosyntax, I suggest that similar patterns in the second generation may be an entirely independent development. However, if related, they may have been reinforced by reverse transmission, such that the patterns emerging in the first-generation adults result from the children (the adolescent/young adult heritage speakers) affecting the language of the parents. This theoretical proposal might explain the emergence of US Spanish as a distinct language variety of Spanish spoken in the United States (see also Shin & Otheguy, Reference Shin and Otheguy2013). Granted, my argument at this stage rests on cases of potentially attrited immigrants who have children for which empirical evidence exists and leaves to be established by further research whether similar attrition effects would be detected in long-term immigrants with no children or infrequent interactions with heritage speakers.

2. Direct language transmission?

To what extent can many of the divergent structural patterns observed in heritage speakers be related to similar patterns in the first-generation immigrants who are the parental generation? The idea seems to be that many first-generation immigrant adults who are living in a language contact situation show native language attrition and interact with other bilinguals in their communities, including their children. To give a concrete example, the distribution of null and overt subjects has been extensively discussed in L1 attrition studies (Domínguez & Hicks, Reference Domínguez, Hicks, Cuza, Czerwionka and Olson2016; Gürel, Reference Gürel2004; Tsimpli et al., Reference Tsimpli, Sorace, Heycock, Filiaci and Bouba2003), and the main finding has been that speakers of null subject languages overextend overt subjects in pragmatic contexts where null subjects are more felicitous, such as in topic continuity or same reference contexts (Sorace, Reference Sorace2011). If adult immigrants who speak a null subject language (Spanish, Greek, Italian) show, say, 5% overproduction of overt pronominal subjects in same reference contexts due to attrition, heritage speakers are likely to show twice or thrice that rate, to give an estimate, of the same divergent pattern (see Montrul, Reference Montrul2018b for an example with null and overt subjects in Spanish; see also Labov, Reference Labov2007 for a similar explanation for sound changes). The generalized interpretation has been that first-generation immigrants’ attrition patterns are transmitted directly to the second-generation speakers (the heritage speakers), who in turn amplify the extent and magnitude of the changes.

For a language to be transmitted across generations, an obvious relationship must exist between the language of the parents (or adult caregivers) and the language of the children. For instance, sociolinguistic studies have linked the speech of adults and children, as with the pronunciation of /ou/ in southern British English, where young children model the pronunciation of one or both parents (Kerswill, Reference Kerswill1996). In Lightfoot’s (Reference Lightfoot1991) and Yang’s (Reference Yang2000) accounts of language acquisition and language change, there is a direct link between the language produced by the parents (E-language) and the internalized grammar (I-language) of the children to account for language transmission, as in Figure 1. E-language has no internal structure; structure is assigned to it through parsing by the child’s I-language (Lightfoot, Reference Lightfoot2020).

Figure 1. The dynamics of language acquisition and change (Yang, Reference Yang2000, p. 232).

A well-known proposal in diachronic change is that when young children somehow fail to parse the parental input, a process of reanalysis and restructuring that gives rise to a different, divergent grammar ensues (Lightfoot, Reference Lightfoot1991, Reference Lightfoot2020; Pires & Thomason, Reference Pires, Thomason, Ferraresi and Goldbach2008). In essence, change occurs because there is a break in transmission, and the language of the parents and the language of the children diverge with respect to a particular property (e.g., case marking, V2, null subjects). “When E-language shifts, children may parse differently, and thus a new I-language emerges” (Lightfoot, Reference Lightfoot2020, p. 29.)Footnote 1

However, child monolingual (L1) learners have been shown to be very conservative during early development and make very few errors (Aitchison, Reference Aitchison2003; Snyder, Reference Snyder2017). So, they are unlikely to experience the amount of parsing failure leading to language change. For this reason, both Meisel et al. (Reference Meisel, Martin and Rinke2013) and Westergaard (Reference Westergaard, Janda, Joseph and Vance2021) have raised credible objections on the purported link between monolingual acquisition and diachronic language change, maintaining instead that language contact through second (L2) acquisition or bilingualism in the parental generation brings about changes in the E-language that is the input to the I-language of their children. Their position bodes well with the consensus in sociolinguistics (Bentz & Winter, Reference Bentz, Winter, Wichmann and Good2014; McWhorter, Reference McWhorter2007; Meisel et al., Reference Meisel, Martin and Rinke2013; Trudgill, Reference Trudgill2001): that adult L2 learners are the main agents of language change because L2 acquisition generally results in non-native attainment after puberty (Johnson & Newport, Reference Johnson and Newport1989; Hartshorne et al., Reference Hartshorne, Tenenbaum and Pinker2018).

Leaving aside for now the issue of diachronic language change, I want to bring attention to the idea of successful transmission (Figure 1); that is, when the output of the parents’ grammars feeds the developing system of the children’s grammars, and the children and the parents end up sharing approximately the same I-grammars across generations. A child exposed to English acquires English; a child exposed to Mandarin acquires Mandarin. This is what happens in typical language acquisition in general, when new, different patterns do not emerge in the younger generation. While heritage speakers exposed to Spanish, or Russian, or Korean acquire Spanish, Russian or Korean, respectively, it has been repeatedly claimed that the heritage speakers who show errors, divergent attainment, innovations, or changes in their heritage language do so because of direct transmission (and supposedly exact “replication”, as in Sankoff, Reference Sankoff2019) of structural patterns from the parental generation, as in Figure 1. For example, responding to Montrul and Polinsky’s (Reference Polinsky2011) commentary on the extension of the Interface Hypothesis to study heritage speakers, Sorace (Reference Sorace2012, pp. 213–214) writes:

individual speakers removed from their original community … provide input affected by attrition to second generation speakers (i.e., heritage speakers), who may acquire a different grammar from that of their parents: this is a different stage of the attrition process and does not present the same characteristics as those identified by the IH (see also Sorace, Reference Sorace2004; Rothman Reference Rothman2007, Rothman, Reference Rothman2009). My claim that heritage speakers are exposed to qualitatively different input (Sorace, Reference Sorace2005) is now beginning to be supported by a number of studies (e.g. Pires & Rothman, Reference Pires and Rothman2009; Place & Hoff, Reference Place and Hoff2011; Unsworth et al., Reference Unsworth, Argyri, Cornips, Hulk, Sorace and Tsimpli2011).Footnote 2

And in a commentary on Hicks and Domínguez’ (Reference Hicks and Domínguez2020) model of L1 attrition, Sorace (Reference Sorace2020), pp. 203–204) amplifies the same claim:

individual attrition involves no ‘erosion’ or ‘permanent loss’ but rather fluctuations and increasing optionality: this is because attrition in this sense crucially does not affect the grammar itself but rather how the grammar is accessed (Sorace, Reference Sorace2011, Reference Sorace2016). Parental input affected by attrition is then transmitted to the next generation of heritage speakers (emphasis mine), who regularize variable input as part of their grammar. The degree of parental attrition has generally not been studied as a variable in heritage language development, but it is crucial for an understanding of the diachronic dimension of language change.

That heritage speakers may be exposed to qualitatively different input compared to monolingual children is possible and true, but Sorace’s claim that such qualitatively different input “is transmitted” and becomes intake to heritage speakers’ developing systems (i.e., my interpretation of direct transmission) is an entirely different possibility. The studies cited by Sorace (Reference Sorace2011)—Pires and Rothman (Reference Pires and Rothman2009), Place and Hoff (Reference Place and Hoff2011) and Unsworth et al. (Reference Unsworth, Argyri, Cornips, Hulk, Sorace and Tsimpli2011)—provide evidence that heritage speakers may be generally exposed to qualitatively different input, but nothing else: They are not about how the language of the parents affects the heritage language of their children.

Still, theoretically, direct transmission of grammatical changes is in principle possible, but how common is it, and has it been attested? After many years of living in the United States, some first-generation immigrants can also exhibit signs of language attrition in some aspects of their grammars. Although most studies of attrition have found optionality due to lexical access and processing effects, as the quote from Sorace (Reference Sorace2020) above suggests, in my work with Spanish speakers in the United States, I have found evidence of attrition at the level of linguistic representations, not just processing, with differential object marking (DOM) and related syntactic structures, as measured by a variety of comprehension, production and judgment tasks (Montrul, Reference Montrul2019, Reference Montrul2022). I assume that attrition in heritage speakers and in the first generation of immigrants leads to structural changes and different endstate grammars. DOM is the phenomenon by which animate, specific direct objects are overtly marked with the preposition “a” in Spanish. Several studies have found that heritage speakers of Spanish largely omit this marker (Arechabaleta Regulez, Reference Arechabaleta Regulez2019; Cuza et al., Reference Cuza, Miller, Pérez Tattam and Ortiz Vergara2019; Montrul, Reference Montrul2004; Montrul & Bowles, Reference Montrul and Bowles2009; Thane, Reference Thane2024). Montrul and Sánchez-Walker (Reference Montrul and Sánchez-Walker2013) compared child heritage speakers to age-matched monolingual children in Mexico, and adult heritage speakers to age-matched monolingual native speakers in Mexico and first-generation immigrants in the United States. There was a significant omission of DOM (35%) by the bilingual children, whereas the monolingual children and adults tested in Mexico were at ceiling (above 98%). The adult heritage speakers omitted more DOM (20%) than age-matched native speakers in Mexico. As for the bilingual immigrant adults, about half of them omitted DOM like the heritage speakers (ranging from 10% to 70% omission). It is easy to conclude from these findings that many adult heritage speakers may be receiving attrited input from the adult immigrants because the same phenomenon was found in the two groups. In fact, this is the conclusion reached by Grosjean and Py’s (Reference Grosjean and Py1991) studies of first and second-generation Spanish-speaking immigrants in French Switzerland, who also found incorrect acceptance of omission of DOM and other structures in a grammaticality judgment task and suggested that morphosyntactic changes in the first generation pass over to the second generation.

Montrul (Reference Montrul and Pascual y Cabo2016b) is another study of the same adult participants in Montrul and Sánchez-Walker (Reference Montrul and Sánchez-Walker2013), about the preposition “a” as an obligatory dative marking with dative experiencer verbs like gustar “like” ( A Juan le gusta cantar “Juan likes to sing”). Spanish heritage speakers have been found to omit the dative case marking with these verbs, producing and preferring nominative experiencers as in English (*Juan (le) gusta cantar) (Montrul, Reference Montrul2014; de Prada Pérez & Pascual y Cabo, Reference de Prada Pérez and y Cabo2011; Toribio & Nye, Reference Toribio and Nye2006). Montrul (Reference Montrul and Pascual y Cabo2016b) used an acceptability judgment task and a written production task. Ungrammatical sentences without the preposition “a” were judged as statistically more acceptable than other ungrammatical sentences. In the written production task, some participants omitted the “a” with gustar-type verbs between 15% (the adult immigrants) and 20% (the heritage speakers), whereas the native speakers from Mexico did not omit a-marking with the psych verbs in written production. Montrul (Reference Montrul and Pascual y Cabo2016b) concluded that because the first-generation immigrants are the main source of input to the heritage speakers, the structural properties of the output of the first generation may reinforce, not necessarily feed directly, the mental representations of the individual grammars of the second generation, which may also be affected by majority language transfer.

Pascual y Cabo (Reference Pascual y Cabo2013, Reference Pascual y Cabo2020) also argued that linguistic innovations in heritage speaker grammars with gustar-type verbs can be traced back to subtle changes in the input received from first-generation immigrants, especially when production data are considered. The hypothesis investigated was that heritage speakers have reanalyzed dative experiencer verbs as nominative experiencer verbs, which would lead them to accept passive structures with gustar-type verbs. Pascual y Cabo found that only the heritage speakers accepted ungrammatical passives with gustar-like verbs in the grammaticality judgment task. In the production task, some omission of “a” was produced by monolingual adult (13.31%), bilingual adults (23.17%), advanced proficiency heritage speakers (29.21%) and intermediate proficiency heritage speakers (48.37%). Because all groups omitted “a” to some extent, Pascual y Cabo concluded that heritage language acquisition outcomes could result from differences in the input heritage speakers receive, following Pires and Rothman (Reference Pires and Rothman2009). However, Pascual y Cabo also considered that passivization of gustar-verbs can come from direct transfer from English, which affects the heritage speaker groups more than the first-generation bilingual adults.

Despite these assertions, direct transmission of attrited patterns from the parents to the children is highly unlikely, for the following reasons:

  1. 1) Language attrition in adults at the level of linguistic representations, especially at the morphosyntactic level, has been very hard to find (Schmid, Reference Schmid2013). There are some reported cases (Kasparian et al., Reference Kasparian, Vespignani and Steinhauer2017; Kasparian & Steinhauer, Reference Kasparian and Steinhauer2017; Montrul et al., Reference Montrul, Bhatt and Girju2015, Reference Montrul and Pascual y Cabo2016b), but they are very few. Most studies have found L1 attrition in lexical access, phonetics/phonology, and some syntactic preferences (Schmid, Reference Schmid, Schmid and Köpke2019). In general, language attrition is a highly individual phenomenon and quite rare (Hicks & Domínguez, Reference Hicks and Domínguez2020, cf. Schmid & Köpke, Reference Schmid and Köpke2017). By contrast, grammatical changes in heritage speakers are very extensive and remarkably common.

  2. 2) Related to (1), most studies of intergenerational transmission comparing heritage speakers and first-generation immigrants show a break in transmission. First-generation speakers are at ceiling and do not differ from speakers in the homeland; heritage speakers are statistically different from first-generation immigrants, showing changes in lexical access in Dutch (Hulsen, Reference Hulsen2000), evidentiality markers in Turkish (Karayayla, Reference Karayayla2020), VOTs in Russian and Ukrainian (Nagy & Kochetov, Reference Nagy, Kochetov, Siemund, Gogolin, Schulz and Davydova2013), verb tenses, copulas, null and overt pronouns in Spanish (Silva-Corvalán, Reference Silva-Corvalán1994), DOM and dative clitics in Spanish (Irizarri van Suchtelen, Reference Irizarri van Suchtelen2016); accusative clitics in European Portuguese (Flores & Rinke, Reference Flores and Rinke2020; Rinke & Flores, Reference Rinke and Flores2014); case in Polish, Russian and Ukrainian (Łyskawa & Nagy, Reference Łyskawa and Nagy2020), to name just a few.

  3. 3) Some of the patterns found in heritage language speakers are also found in second language (L2) learners, even those L2 learners exposed to native input. For example, L2 learners of Spanish omit DOM in obligatory contexts as much as heritage speakers of Spanish (Arechabaleta Regulez, Reference Arechabaleta Regulez2019; Guijarro-Fuentes & Marinis, Reference Guijarro-Fuentes and Marinis2007; Guijarro-Fuentes, Reference Guijarro-Fuentes2012). Case marking in general is difficult for heritage speakers of Hindi, Korean and Samoan; it is also difficult for L2 learners of Hindi (Montrul, Reference Montrul2019), Korean (Chung, Reference Chung2020) and Samoan (Muāgututi’a, Reference Muāgututi’a2018). This suggests internally driven developments in different bilingual grammars converging on a common result, independently of input quality.

  4. 4) During early stages of language development, children make errors (overgeneralizations of morphology, case omissions, under or overproduction of null/overt subjects, determiners or other pronouns, for instance) that are internally motivated by the current state of the children’s developing grammars and other cognitive resources (working memory, parsing routines, etc.) because their caregivers do not produce such errors (Crain, Reference Crain1991; Guasti, Reference Guasti2002; Lidz & Gagliardi, Reference Lidz and Gagliardi2015; Pinker, Reference Pinker1989). If heritage speakers are native speakers and learn their heritage language like L1-acquiring children (Montrul, Reference Montrul2013, Reference Montrul2022; Rothman & Treffers-Daller, Reference Rothman and Treffers-Daller2014), why would they be different?

Rather than parental transmission of structural patterns, I argue instead for a break in transmission, as in diachronic language change: In essence, structural changes in heritage grammars arise mostly (but not exclusively) from internal reanalysis of the heritage language grammar due to insufficient input (Montrul, Reference Montrul2016a, Reference Montrul2022; Polinsky, Reference Polinsky2018). Under Lightfoot’ (Reference Lightfoot2020) proposal that parsing and acquisition are the same construct, the changes arise because the I-language of the heritage speakers may not fully parse the E-language from the parents with all its finer morphosyntactic details, not necessarily because the E-language provided by the parents may have changed, but because the quantity of input is insufficient to internally trigger certain parsing routines. Insufficient input during a critical age in language development leads to psycholinguistic effects such as fuzzy linguistic representations and difficulty accessing the linguistic representations of the heritage language for production and comprehension (Putnam & Sánchez, Reference Putnam and Sánchez2013; Montrul, Reference Montrul2021). In recent studies of school-age heritage speakers, we have found that when the heritage language is the medium of instruction at school, such exposure enhances cue integration during processing and acquisition (Armstrong, Reference Armstrong2024; Montrul & Armstrong, Reference Montrul, Armstrong and Batbasouli2024).

As for the very few instances where we see incipient morphosyntactic changes in the first generation that are amplified in the heritage speakers (Montrul & Sánchez-Walker, Reference Montrul and Sánchez-Walker2013; Montrul, Reference Montrul2016a, Reference Montrul2022; Pascual y Cabo, Reference Pascual y Cabo2020), these changes most likely develop independently in the two groups (see also Putnam et al. Reference Putnam, Kupisch, Pascual y Cabo, Miller, Bayram, Rothman and Serratrice2018), because the age of acquisition and the flexibility of the linguistic competence after a certain age play a critical role in language change (Kerswill, Reference Kerswill1996; Montrul, Reference Montrul2022; Sankoff, Reference Sankoff2019). If one generation affects the other, I suggest it is the adult heritage speakers who may contribute to changes in the language of the parents. For parents to directly transmit L1 attrition patterns to their children, the age of the children is critical to parse the input, as I discuss next.

3. Interrupted transmission

Both generative approaches to language acquisition and the variationist sociolinguistic concept of generational change share the assumption that the linguistic knowledge of the individual stabilizes and becomes firmly established and consolidated after childhood (Labov, Reference Labov2001, pp. 448–552). Before puberty, the linguistic knowledge of children is nimble. After puberty, linguistic patterns become entrenched. For this reason, if linguistic innovations occur in the parental generation during the heritage speakers’ childhood years (pre-adolescence), language change is more likely because children’s linguistic ability is malleable and flexible (see Mayberry & Kluender, Reference Mayberry and Kluender2018 and Birdsong, Reference Birdsong2018 for the relationship between language development, age and brain plasticity). For instance, Lightfoot (Reference Lightfoot2020) claims that changes in reduced verb morphology in the primary linguistic data (adult E-language) led 3-year-old children in the Middle English period to parse English verbs into different categories. Meisel et al. (Reference Meisel, Martin and Rinke2013) also contend that the age of the language learner and the psycholinguistic mechanisms that mediate between the I-language and the E-language is what links L2 acquisition and language change.

As discussed earlier, the literature on diachronic linguistics has made the monolingual child the prototypical agent of language change (Lightfoot, Reference Lightfoot1991, Reference Lightfoot2020; Lightfoot & Westergaard, Reference Lightfoot and Westergaard2007). While children do acquire the language of their environment and do not stray very far from what they hear, they do not always reproduce what they hear in the input faithfully (Shin & Miller, Reference Shin and Miller2022) because they are constrained by their cognitive and psycholinguistic capacities. Unlike their parents and adult interlocutors, children simplify phonological clusters, omit required morphology (Snyder, Reference Snyder2021), overgeneralize regular morphology to irregular forms, and overgeneralize other syntactic patterns with different verb placement (Westergaard, Reference Westergaard, Manaffert, Bull and Killie2005) or wh-words (Thornton, Reference Thornton1995). For example, Hudson Kam and Newport (Reference Hudson Kam and Newport2005) taught children ages 5–7 an artificial language. They then presented the children with input for that language with either consistent use of the determiner (i.e., nouns occurring with the same determiner 100% of the time) or inconsistent use (i.e., nouns occurring with different determiners at different frequencies). Unlike the adults tested who reproduced the input veridically, many children regularized the language, imposing patterns that were not the same as what they had been exposed to in their input. These results suggest that children and adults do not learn from variable input in the same way and that children do not faithfully reproduce the input.

In situations where children could be exposed to inconsistent or variable input, such as when the parents are non-native speakers of the majority language, children do not always adapt toward the non-native patterns of their parents. Creolization studies have shown that children who receive only pidgin input develop a more complex and regularized grammar (Bickerton, Reference Bickerton1984; Senghas, Reference Senghas1995), providing some of the strongest evidence that children go beyond their input in the process of acquiring a native language. Other compelling examples come from sign languages, where children surpass their parents’ L2-accented input and become native signers (Singleton & Newport, Reference Singleton and Newport1994). At the same time, this empirical evidence does not deny altogether a potential link between child language and the caregiver model, including some changes that may be going on in the adult model; my claim is simply that not all changes or differences in the adult parental model are adopted by the children.

Heritage speakers also surpass the input from their parents in their command of the majority language. In most of the studies I have conducted on heritage speakers to date, my participants have had very advanced to native-like command of English, reflected both in proficiency measures and linguistic tasks on unaccusativity (Montrul, Reference Montrul2006) and the semantics of definite determiners in Spanish and English (Montrul & Ionin, Reference Montrul and Ionin2012). In the studies reported in Montrul (Reference Montrul2022), the Romanian heritage speakers and the Spanish heritage speakers had higher self-rated proficiency in English than the first-generation immigrants. Surpassing the adult non-native model is possible in these cases because heritage speakers are exposed to the majority language in childhood, they have access to the language in the community, which includes many other native speakers and are schooled in the majority language.

But let us consider the possible developmental scenarios of the minority, heritage language and the issue of timing of variable, inconsistent input, if such inconsistent input were available to the heritage speakers and they replicated it veridically (which they typically do not). We will see that when we compare the language learning and forgetting trajectories of children and their parents, assuming families are having children within the first 10 years postmigration, the timing of attrition in adults and the timing when developing child grammars are malleable do not match in such a way that the parental generation could possibly directly influence the children in the manner that it has been suggested.Footnote 3

L1 attrition in adult immigrants at the level of linguistic representations (Hicks & Domínguez, Reference Hicks and Domínguez2020; Schmid, Reference Schmid2011; Schmid & Köpke, Reference Schmid and Köpke2017) is rare, and when it occurs, it only affects a handful of individuals. Typically, studies have detected measurable effects of attrition in morphosyntax with immigrants with a LOR beyond 10 years (Gürel, Reference Gürel2004, cf. Chamorro et al., Reference Chamorro, Sturt and Sorace2015). The Mexican immigrants who showed morphosyntactic attrition reported in Montrul et al. (Reference Montrul, Bhatt and Girju2015) and Montrul (Reference Montrul2022) (n = 21) had a mean LOR in the United States of 19.5 years compared to 9.12 years in the Romanian immigrants. Those Mexican immigrants who showed omission of DOM were older (mean 48.3 versus mean 43.2), had been in the United States longer than the ones who did not omit DOM (27.3 years versus 23.1 years) and had acquired English later in life (22 versus 20.2). The two immigrants with the lowest accuracy on DOM (both 33%) had been in the United States for 33 and 37 years, respectively. Figures 2 and 3 show their acceptability of ungrammatical sentences in Spanish, with the omission of DOM (Figure 2) and acceptance of accusative clitic doubling (Figure 3), which is ungrammatical in Mexican Spanish (but grammatical in other varieties).

Figure 2. Relationship between length of residence in the U.S. and acceptability of ungrammatical animate specific direct objects without DOM by the adult Mexican immigrants (Montrul, Reference Montrul2022). A linear mixed-effects model revealed that this correlation was significant (SE = .01, β = .03, p < .05).

Figure 3. Relationship between length of residence in the U.S. and acceptability ratings on animate definite objects with clitic doubling by the adult Mexican immigrants (Montrul, Reference Montrul2022). The output of the model showed that there was a significant correlation between LOR and acceptability of the target construction (SE = .01, β = .02, p < .05).

These data show that it takes several years of immersion in the second language and restricted input and use of the native language for attrition to be detected in oral production and grammaticality judgment tasks. However, some native speakers may start to show changes in lexical access as soon as they start L2 acquisition (Bice & Kroll, Reference Bice and Kroll2015), suggesting that changes in processing before changes in production are possible but hardly permanent (see also Chamorro et al., Reference Chamorro, Sorace and Sturt2016). By the time adult immigrants start showing non-native patterns in their language production, comprehension and grammaticality judgments of morphosyntax, as in the studies reported in Montrul (Reference Montrul2022), their children have probably well passed the period when they were most likely to be influenced by the parental input, i.e., preadolescence. Of course, this proposal raises several questions: When exactly do heritage speakers begin influencing their parents? Do heritage speakers initiate the change in the parental generation, or do they simply reinforce incipient variability in the parental generation? Is there a way to tease apart changes in the parental generation due to LOR in the host country versus heritage-speaker-initiated changes?

Considering studies on DOM, monolingual children show some variability with DOM (about 20–25% omission depending on the language), before age two or three (Coşkun Kunduz & Montrul, Reference Montrul2021; Ticio & Avram, Reference Ticio and Avram2015). Studies of DOM with bilingual children have shown that DOM lags in development very early; the earliest data we have is from 3-year-olds (Ticio, Reference Ticio2015), who produce DOM about 25% of the time (i.e., 75% omission) (but see Requena, Reference Requena2023’s reanalysis of these data). Montrul and Sánchez-Walker (Reference Montrul and Sánchez-Walker2013) showed that children ages 6–14 (mean 11) continue to show very high rates of DOM omission (almost 40%). This means that children are omitting DOM when they are younger, and during that time, the parents may be producing DOM at ceiling. Some variability and DOM instability before age three is part of the developmental picture in monolingual and bilingual acquisition. The problem is what happens after age three, when bilingual development now appears to lag. So, the developmental trajectories of partial, interrupted acquisition in children (heritage speakers) and attrition in adults (parents or other caregivers) do not match, as illustrated in Figure 4. Therefore, as bilingual children are growing up, if they show nontarget development between preschool and middle school, it is highly unlikely a result of their parents’ input.

Figure 4. Possible developmental trajectories of L1 acquisition, L1 attrition and acquisition in heritage speakers and their caregivers.

One way to test this hypothesis is to directly compare the linguistic knowledge of heritage language children and that of their parents. If the parents show almost ceiling production while the children show extensive variability, then it is difficult to link the grammars of the children (I-language) to the parents’ production (E-language).

4. Empirical evidence

Recent studies have focused on how children acquire the heritage language across the school-age years (Chondrogianni & Schwartz, Reference Chondrogianni and Schwartz2020; Daskalaki et al., Reference Daskalaki, Blom, Chondrogianni, Paradis and Daskalaki2020; Flores et al., Reference Flores, Santos, Jesus and Marques2017; Jia & Paradis, Reference Jia and Paradis2015). Some have tested heritage language children and their parents. Cuza et al. (Reference Cuza, Miller, Pérez Tattam and Ortiz Vergara2019) elicited the oral production of DOM in SVO matrix sentences (Llamé a mi abuela, “I called my grandmother”) and in sentences with clitic left dislocations ( A mi abuela la llamé, “My grandmother, I called”) in Spanish-speaking monolingual and bilingual children ages 5–11 (mean 8;2), and in the Mexican parents of the bilingual children with a mean LOR in the United States of 13.8 years. The monolingual children and the bilingual parents were at ceiling, but the heritage language children omitted DOM 35% of the time. Cuza et al. (Reference Cuza, Miller, Pérez Tattam and Ortiz Vergara2019) did not find L1 attrition of DOM in the parents tested; DOM was present in the children’s input 99% of the time. There were no age differences within the bilingual children: older children did not omit DOM more than younger children, which also supports the observed “stability” of this phenomenon in different bilingual samples (Montrul & Sánchez-Walker, Reference Montrul and Sánchez-Walker2013).

Similar findings are reported by Coşkun Kunduz and Montrul (Reference Coşkun Kunduz and Montrul2022), who investigated DOM in Turkish heritage language children (ages 7–14) and their parents in the United States. Turkish native speakers in Turkey were tested to establish whether the adult Turkish first- generation immigrants in the United States manifested L1 attrition. A group of adult heritage speakers in the United States (second-generation immigrants) were included to see whether omission of DOM could be attributed to L1 attrition in the adult heritage speakers (as in Polinsky, Reference Polinsky2011). The Turkish immigrants performed at ceiling in two oral tasks, like the native speakers in Turkey. However, both the child and adult heritage speakers showed extensive variability and omission of accusative case marking with specific direct objects, which is the way that Turkish instantiates DOM. Correlation analyses of child–parent dyads found no relationship between the language of the parents and the language of the children, suggesting no evidence of L1 attrition directly impacting the omission patterns found in the children and in the adult heritage speakers. In a related study with the same participants and tasks, Coşkun Kunduz and Montrul (Reference Coşkun Kunduz and Montrul2023) tested the production and comprehension of evidentiality markers –DI and–mIş Footnote 4, another feature that has been found to be vulnerable in adult heritage speakers of Turkish (Karayayla, Reference Karayayla2020). Another group of very young Turkish speakers in Turkey (3–6 year-olds) was included to establish when in monolingual acquisition Turkish children know the difference between the two evidentiality markers depending on whether the source of information is direct (-DI) or indirect (–mIş). Results revealed weak mappings between the indirect evidential marker –mIş and its pragmatic use in storytelling as opposed to intact knowledge of the Turkish evidentiality system in the first-generation immigrant group. The child and adult heritage speakers, on the other hand, were the least accurate groups in both tasks, performing even more variably than the 3–6-year-old monolingual children in Turkey. The individual analysis of the child heritage speaker data as well as a comparison of the child–parent dyads confirmed that the nature of the parental input is an unlikely source of the variability observed in children and eventually in adult Turkish heritage speakers.

Another study showing little relationship between the linguistic knowledge of first-generation immigrants and their children (second-generation) is Daskalaki et al. (Reference Daskalaki, Blom, Chondrogianni, Paradis and Daskalaki2020), who studied subject positions in Greek heritage language children in Canada (most second-generation but a few third-generation immigrants) and their parents, as well as an age-matched group of monolingual children and parents in Greece. Postverbal subjects are obligatory in Greek interrogative sentences, while in declarative sentences and in wide focus contexts, postverbal subjects are variable and depend on semantic and pragmatic conditions. The child monolinguals and their parents were all at ceiling (99%) in an elicited production task. Whereas the heritage children showed an overall accuracy of 45% and produced more infelicitous preverbal subjects than postverbal subjects in wide focus than in embedded questions, the first-generation immigrant parents were very accurate. Results from second-generation parents and third-generation children were different because the parents did show some variability and their children even more. This study shows that the parental input to monolingual children can be different from the parental input to bilingual children for the parents who are themselves heritage speakers, but most importantly, they also refute the hypothesis that second-generation heritage speakers receive attrited input from their parents.

Requena and Dracos (Reference Requena and Dracos2021) examined second-generation Spanish-English bilingual children ages 5–14 (heritage speakers) to determine whether they interpret Spanish copulas ser and estar with adjectives like first-generation adults in their community. Some adjectives can be used with either copula, but there is a semantic and pragmatic aspectual distinction. Estar is specified for aspectual properties (Schmitt & Miller, Reference Schmitt and Miller2007), while ser is unspecified for aspect. By age 4 monolingual Spanish speakers have acquired the semantic and pragmatic interpretation of the two copulas (Requena, Reference Requena2021). Adult heritage speakers of Spanish have been shown to extend estar to uses of ser (Silva-Corvalán, Reference Silva-Corvalán1994; Geeslin & Guijarro-Fuentes, Reference Geeslin and Guijarro-Fuentes2008), and the two Spanish-English bilingual children followed longitudinally by Silva-Corvalán (Reference Silva-Corvalán2014) from ages 1;00 to 6;00 also showed extensions of estar to ser contexts. Requena and Dracos (Reference Requena and Dracos2021) investigated the effects of age, language exposure/use, and proficiency on copula selection. Some of the adults were the parents of the children, so their language can be considered representative of the input to the children. In a picture selection task, the adults showed much higher selection of the temporary picture with estar (92%) compared to the children (67%). For the children, higher Spanish proficiency increased the selection of the picture with estar and decreased the preference for ser. The low-proficiency children were at chance like the 3-year-old children in Requena’s (Reference Requena2021) L1 acquisition study. Even though they did not exhibit exact adult-like usage, high-proficiency children did exhibit knowledge of the semantics and pragmatics of the copulas. There is no basis to conclude that the low proficiency children are receiving attrited input from the parents.

Finally, Mai et al. (Reference Mai, Zhao and Yip2022) investigated knowledge and use of the Mandarin ba-construction through elicited narration in heritage Mandarin children ages 4–14 and their parents in the UK. Mandarin and English have basic SVO order, but in Mandarin SOV order is possible with the -ba construction, which takes the form ([(NP1)-ba-NP2-VP]) and is the preferred way native speakers express statements in disposal and causative contexts, as in (1). NP1 is the subject/topic and can be overt or null based on syntactic, semantic and discourse conditions independent of this construction. NP2 (qingua) is the object.

In monolingual acquisition, the ba-construction emerges around age 2 and becomes highly productive by 2;6 (Deng et al., Reference Deng, Mai and Yip2018). Adult heritage speakers have been found to underuse the ba-construction, preferring SVO sentences instead. Mai et al. (Reference Mai, Zhao and Yip2022) investigated the extent to which school-age Mandarin heritage children in the UK produced the ba-construction and how their productions compared to that of their parents, the input providers, in terms of (i) structural frequency, (ii) the linear structure of [(NP1)-ba-NP2-VP], and (iii) the types of nominal and verbal phrases used in NP2 and VP. All heritage language children and the parents narrated a frog story. No correlation was found between the children and the parents’ productions because the heritage children produced the ba-construction less frequently than their parents and used a more limited set of nominal and verbal phrases. None of the child–adult differences in the UK groups were attested in the children and adults tested in Beijing. Mai et al. concluded that the lower frequency of the structure and reduced diversity of the nominal and verbal phrases in the heritage children’s construction can only be attributed to the diasporic context in the UK, where input in the heritage language is significantly less abundant.

To summarize, all these recent studies comparing heritage language children and their parents on different linguistic phenomena converge on the same outcome: by school-age, structural patterns and frequency in the parental input no longer determine the children’s output. In all these cases, the parents are at or close to ceiling in their linguistic performance, as in the idealized grey solid line in Figure 4, while their children are not (dashed line). For school-age heritage children, hearing more utterances with DOM, evidential markers, subject position, copulas and the ba-construction from the parents does not necessarily boost their production or comprehension of all these structures. All these studies point to the conclusion that simplification in the children’s grammars most likely arises by how their internal grammars process and make do with insufficient input in the heritage language and cross-linguistic influence from the societal majority language before the closure of the critical period, when their native language representations are still nimble and malleable.Footnote 5

Finally, Requena and Dracos (Reference Requena and Dracos2021) also suggest that low-proficiency second-generation children are a catalyzing locus of accelerated language change observed by Silva-Corvalán (Reference Silva-Corvalán1994). Because there are a few reported cases of first-generation immigrants displaying some of the same patterns as second-generation immigrants (heritage speakers) (Montrul et al., Reference Montrul, Bhatt and Girju2015; Montrul, Reference Montrul2016a, Reference Montrul2022; Pascual y Cabo, Reference Pascual y Cabo2020), I suggest that attrition in the first generation of adults who have children may arise through reverse language transmission.

5. Reverse language transmission and language change

Montrul (Reference Montrul2014, Reference Montrul2022) made the novel proposal that heritage speakers are active agents in the diachronic evolution of Spanish as a distinct language variety of the United States. Although Meisel et al. (Reference Meisel, Martin and Rinke2013) argue that simultaneous bilingual children are like monolingual children and cannot be agents of language change, studies of child and adult heritage speakers show that simultaneous bilinguals can and do in fact fail to replicate the parental input, not because their language learning mechanisms are inefficient, as claimed to be for adult L2 acquisition past a critical period (Meisel, Reference Meisel2011), but because heritage speakers do not receive sufficient exposure to the heritage language during the critical period. Like Birdsong (Reference Birdsong2018), I use “critical period” as a generic term that also subsumes sensitive periods. Hartshorne et al.’s (Reference Hartshorne, Tenenbaum and Pinker2018, p. 12) results of L2 acquisition point to “a grammar-learning ability that is preserved throughout childhood and declines rapidly in late adolescence.”

Heritage speakers use the heritage language less frequently in late childhood and early adolescence than in early childhood, before the closure of the critical period, when the language capacity is still flexible and malleable. Less frequent use and activation of the heritage language leads to slow and taxing processing and parsing of the heritage language, under cognitive and social pressure from the majority language. Among the several factors that could explain the critical period, Hartshorne et al. also consider age-dependent changes in neural plasticity (see also Birdsong, Reference Birdsong2018; Mayberry & Kluender, Reference Mayberry and Kluender2018). Finally, Hartshorne et al. (Reference Hartshorne, Tenenbaum and Pinker2018) further found that bilinguals from birth were not entirely monolingual-like, which could also be attributed to the amount of language exposure rather than to timing of exposure, as I defend in this article. Birdsong (Reference Birdsong2018, p. 10) even writes: “Among international adoptees and heritage speakers, dominance shifts involve attrition of the L1, a representational and functional loss which likewise reflects neural plasticity.” Therefore, under certain circumstances, heritage speakers are driving the changes of their heritage language (not the majority language) which, when diffused or spread to other generations and speakers with different proficiency in the heritage language, creates a new variety of the language, in the same manner proposed for the diachronic evolution of standard languages with many non-native speakers (McWhorter, Reference McWhorter2007).

My claim that heritage speakers can be agents of language change because of the timing of acquisition (Figure 4) and when changes may spread is entirely consistent with sociolinguistics proposals that pre-adolescents are the main agents of language change (Tagliamonte & D’Arcy, Reference Tagliamonte and D’Arcy2009). Socio-psychological factors involving linguistic maturation and evolving social relationships have an impact on the way in which language change is transmitted (Fagyal et al., Reference Fagyal, Swarup, Escobar, Gasser and Lakkaraju2010). A recurrent finding from dialectal research is that children acquire the vernacular of their primary caretaker, most often the mother (Kerswill, Reference Kerswill1996; Kerswill & Williams, Reference Kerswill and Williams2000a; Labov, Reference Labov2001). Hence, the vernacular that children first speak is that of their female caregivers, and women have also been portrayed as leading linguistic changes (Labov, Reference Labov1989; Shin, Reference Shin2013). But upon school entry, children quickly adopt the vernacular of their peers, a critical stage in the incrementation of linguistic change (Labov, Reference Labov2001): the speech of 8-year-olds shows a striking departure from the speech of their parents in phonological processes, morphologically conditioned phenomena, and lexical rules (Kerswill & Williams, Reference Kerswill and Williams2000b). There is a weak correlation between linguistic features of 4-year-olds and their caregivers, but for 6–12-year-olds there is no correlation between caregiver and children’s features because the speech of the 6–12 year-olds accommodates to that of their peers, not their parents’. From this point on, social pressures encouraging conformity to peer group norms interact strongly with individuals’ declining ability to accommodate linguistically, at least in lexically complex processes, and probably, morphosyntax.

Between ages 12 and 17, past the critical period for language, children are essentially adults in terms of their abilities to acquire language, but in sociolinguistic variation, it is at this stage when some of the most dramatic changes seem to come about: “Adolescents are clearly significant agents of change; their social networks allow them to have wider contact than younger children, and their desire for a distinct social identity means that they are willing to modify their speech. At the same time, during this period (at age 16 at the very latest), they can no longer have the ability to acquire lexically complex rules, new oppositions or new intonational systems.” (Kerswill, Reference Kerswill1996, p. 198).

In situations of societal bilingualism where the heritage language is always threatened, as in Ireland (Gathercole & Thomas, Reference Gathercole and Thomas2009; Mueller Gathercole, Reference Mueller Gathercole2007), Fhlannchadha and Hickey (Reference Fhlannchadha and Hickey2021) found that changes in gender marking in Irish-speaking children (ages 6–13) were reflected in the adult input. But at this time in their linguistic development, heritage speakers of Irish have extensive exposure to input from L2 learners from-pre-school onwards in most educational settings. Jones (Reference Jones1998) found that Welsh children in immersion schools adopted the errors of the L2 learners of Welsh rather than influencing the L2 learners to use the correct form, or following the model provided by their parents, which did not show changes. Therefore, as heritage language children grow older, they do not necessarily adopt the speech patterns of their parents, if their social networks in the heritage language include other peers.

If adolescents are agents of language change, they provide us with a new way of understanding the mechanisms of language change in heritage languages, which points to different patterns of directionality of change and diffusion: parent-to-child (direct transmission), child-to-adult (backward transmission) and adult-to-adult. The studies found that both first-generation immigrants and second-generation heritage speakers omit DOM and dative case with psych verbs (Montrul & Sánchez-Walker, Reference Montrul and Sánchez-Walker2013; Montrul et al., Reference Montrul, Bhatt and Girju2015; Montrul, Reference Montrul2016a, Reference Montrul and Pascual y Cabob; Montrul, Reference Montrul2022; Pascual y Cabo, Reference Pascual y Cabo2020), overextend pronominal subjects in same reference contexts and preverbal subjects (Domínguez & Hicks, Reference Domínguez, Hicks, Cuza, Czerwionka and Olson2016; Montrul & Sánchez-Walker, Reference Montrul, Sánchez-Walker, Carvalho, Orozco and Shin2015; Otheguy & Zentella, Reference Otheguy and Zentella2012) and extend the copula estar to ser contexts (Requena & Dracos, Reference Requena and Dracos2021; Silva-Corvalán, Reference Silva-Corvalán2014) support the possibility of backward transmission in Spanish with respect to ongoing changes in the language. My proposed backward transmission (child-to-parent) is based on relating the age of the child during heritage language development, when input to the heritage language becomes progressively reduced, with how long it takes for L1 attrition to manifest in the parental generation (see Figure 2). A study comparing mothers and child heritage speakers as well as mothers and adult heritage speakers (Montrul, Reference Montrul2022, pp. 277–279) found that the mothers of both child and adult heritage speakers were largely at ceiling with the production of DOM in two oral tasks, and statistically more accurate than their own children. Accuracy on DOM in the child and adult heritage language groups ranged from 0% to 100%. There was incipient variability with DOM in 4 of 14 (28.5%) mothers of the adult heritage speakers, compared to 1 of 20 (0.5%) mothers of the child heritage speakers. So, the age of the children and the LOR of the parents must be considered in studies of child–parent dyads to understand this phenomenon.

Spanish-speaking adolescent and young adult heritage speakers influencing their parents would now be a case of adult-to-adult transmission since linguistically speaking, 17-year-olds and older are adults, as in Figure 5.

Figure 5. The dynamics of language acquisition and change in Spanish in the United States.

The teenage years are the time when many heritage speakers, who may now willingly embrace their heritage identity and are eager to regain contact with their language and culture, begin to use the language more with the parents at home. While progression of linguistic change slows in early adulthood, there are also suggestions that speakers participate in ongoing developments throughout the life cycle (Labov, Reference Labov2001; Shin & Otheguy, Reference Shin and Otheguy2013; Tagliamonte & D’Arcy, Reference Tagliamonte and D’Arcy2009), and this may be the reason why some of their adult parents’ speech (first-generation immigrants) starts to converge with the speech of their children, who show variability with respect to DOM, overt pronouns, dative case, copula selection and so forth. As pointed out by an anonymous reviewer, limited literacy may also contribute to accelerating language change in heritage speakers. Many, but certainly not all, heritage speakers are illiterate in their heritage language; their grammars being unconstrained by the effects of orthography and standardization efforts. Previous literature suggests that literacy safeguards against attrition, impeding or at least de-accelerating it in heritage language children (Zaretsky & Bar-Shalom, Reference Zaretsky and Bar-Shalom2010), and possibly in adulthood (Armstrong, Reference Armstrong2024). An input environment restricted to mainly the family supports a feedback loop leading to the possibility of the linguistic output of the second generation feeding the input to the first generation.

Therefore, Spanish heritage speakers contribute to shaping the variety of Spanish spoken in the United States. U.S. Spanish is made up of bilingual speakers with different bilingual profiles and different levels of proficiency: these include speakers of historical Spanish spoken in the region before the arrival of the English colonizers (e.g., New Mexico and other parts of the Southwest), Spanish-dominant speakers from Spain and Latin America, U.S.-born balanced Spanish-English bilinguals, English-dominant bilinguals and speakers of Spanish as a second language. Spanish-speaking families increasingly speak more English at home as children grow up but do not abandon Spanish (Hurtado & Vega, Reference Hurtado and Vega2004). As shift in language happens from Spanish to English and from one generation to the next, different levels of language use occur within the home domain, allowing exposure of Spanish to children and exposure of English to parents. Therefore, there are different levels or degrees of language use and exposure falling under the use of Spanish only or the use of English only, and the continuum in between. Heritage language speakers are an important segment of this population, and they contribute to shaping the variety of Spanish spoken in the United States. Increased use of subject pronouns (Otheguy & Zentella, Reference Otheguy and Zentella2012) and variable use of DOM with animate and inanimate objects are “stable” defining feature of U.S. Spanish, different from the variable use of null and overt subjects and DOM in other varieties of Spanish. A prediction this proposal makes, worthy of future research, is that adult Spanish-speaking immigrants in the United States who do not have children and do not frequently interact as much with heritage speakers should be spared from L1 attrition.

6. Conclusion

Heritage language studies afford a unique and exciting opportunity to understand the nature of linguistic competence in a language acquired under different sociolinguistic circumstances, how it is transmitted from one generation to the next, and how it can change across generations. I have argued that many of the linguistic patterns typical of heritage language speakers are unlikely to arise from direct transmission of the attrited language from their parents, for both theoretical and empirical reasons. Rather, they are more likely to arise independently, through internal reanalysis from insufficient input during the critical period for language acquisition. I have also proposed that in the few cases where attrition effects have been documented in the parental generation, these may be related to backward transmission: rather than parents transmitting changes to the heritage language children, adolescents and adult heritage speakers may reinforce patterns of incipient attrition in the parents, that may have arisen independently. Admittedly, the argument advanced here is restricted to a particular age group for which data are available. It is still possible that immigrants who have children at around age 35, or 20 years after migration, and may themselves be attrited, may contribute to pass down linguistic patterns to their children, but this is an empirical question.

I have presented two maximally contrastive positions to account for intergenerational attrition: direct versus reverse intergenerational transmission. Strong positions generate hypotheses that are easily confirmed or falsified. There is room for other possible intermediate possibilities. For example, the grammars of the parents and the children may undergo structural changes independently: Children show developmental changes triggered by misparsing insufficient input, cross-linguistic influence from the majority language, peer influence, and so forth and L1 attrition in the parental generation could be solely related to LOR in the host country and influence from the majority language, which may increase with increased LOR and majority language use. The children and their parents interact linguistically, without necessarily initiating or reinforcing innovative structural changes in each other’s grammars. The other possibility is that parent–child (Figure 1) and child–parent transmission (Figure 5) are both at play: Children may initiate changes first in their extensive grammatical variability, which over time becomes consistent and stabilizes, and eventually triggers changes in the parents’ language, who then start to show the same patterns as well, although less than the children. When parents start showing some L1 attrition, their patterns might contribute to the stabilization of the observed variability in heritage grammars.

It is possible to think about a continuum where the children are the initiators of the process, but eventually children and their parents organically start affecting each other. The effects are amplified in heritage children while their grammars are still flexible, whereas first-generation grammars are still resisting change given that their grammars have already been established. In conclusion, the possibility that the process may be bidirectional instead of unidirectional is not entirely ruled out.

To return to the questions posed at the end of Section 3, When exactly do heritage speakers begin influencing their parents? Do heritage speakers initiate the change in the parental generation, or do they simply reinforce incipient variability in the parental generation? Is there a way to tease apart changes in the parental generation due to LOR in the host country versus heritage-speaker-initiated changes? We covered in Section 4 one possible way to address these questions, which is by conducting experimental studies of child and adult heritage language speakers (second-generation immigrants) and their parents (first-generation immigrants). All these studies have been cross-sectional; ideally, 5-to-10-year longitudinal studies testing child–mother dyads performing an interactive oral task (as in Luo et al., Reference Luo, Escobar and Tamis-LeMonda2020) could provide more direct evidence of potential attrition and acquisition over time in the same dyads. Another possibility is to do an analysis of heritage speakers and their social networks to get more detailed and precise information on the contexts of heritage language use with different interlocutors daily (Sharma, Reference Sharma2017; Sharma & Dodsworth, Reference Sharma and Dodsworth2019; Tiv et al., Reference Tiv, Gullifer, Feng and Titone2020; Titone & Tiv, Reference Titone and Tiv2022). Finally, computational modelling of attrition and language transmission with an agent-based simulation of individuals who are having interactions (Fagyal et al., Reference Fagyal, Swarup, Escobar, Gasser and Lakkaraju2010; Stanford & Kenny, Reference Stanford and Kenny2013) may be another way to possibly test these possibilities. However, as with most statistical models, while relationships will be uncovered, causality will be difficult to establish.

Acknowledgement

I am grateful to Alejandro Cuza and Aylin Coşkun Kunduz for reading and commenting on earlier drafts of this article and for the stimulating questions posed by three anonymous reviewers who helped sharpen the arguments presented here. All remaining errors are my own.

Footnotes

1 In my reading of Lightfoot (Reference Lightfoot1991), I do not see explicitly mention of bilingualism. Lightfoot (Reference Lightfoot2020) suggests that the changes in the E-language of monolingual three-year-old children were coming from bilingual adults but, again, he do not explain this in any detail, nor does he explicitly state it or connect it to the main reason why the child parses the input differently.

2 Unsworth et al. (Reference Unsworth, Argyri, Cornips, Hulk, Sorace and Tsimpli2011) is in fact Unsworth et al. (Reference Unsworth, Argyri, Cornips, Hulk, Sorace and Tsimpli2011) as cited in the references of Sorace’ (Reference Sorace2012) response.

3 To my knowledge, no studies in the L1 attrition field have looked at whether the attriters have children, how many they have, how old they were when they had the children and the social networks of the attriters. I concur with the sentiment of one anonymous reviewer that previous published data on L1 attrition is of limited use to further corroborate the hypothesis and that futures studies should control for these factors.

4 The vowels are in capital letters because they stand for a variable sound, since Turkish has vowel harmony. Once the affix attaches to the root, the vowel of the affix matches the vowel of the root in height and roundness: e.g., gör-dü/ gör-müş “saw.”

5 However, as Daskalaki et al. (Reference Daskalaki, Blom, Chondrogianni, Paradis and Daskalaki2020) found, the situation may be different for parents who are themselves heritage speakers (second-generation immigrants) and their children (third-generation immigrants). Their study found that the second-generation parents also had higher preference for SVO patterns, and this pattern was amplified in their third-generation children (see also Labov, Reference Labov2007). More studies of second- and third-generation heritage speakers are needed to understand whether this is in fact the case.

References

Aalberse, S., Backus, A., & Muysken, P. (2019). Heritage languages: A language contact approach. John Benjamins Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aitchison, J. (2003). Psycholinguistic perspectives of language change. In Blackwell handbook of historical linguistics (pp. 736743).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arechabaleta Regulez, B. (2019). Tracing language variation in Spanish: A multidisciplinary approach. Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.Google Scholar
Armstrong, A. (2024). The impact of literacy on L1 morphosyntactic development and the production of verbal passives in school-age heritage speakers of Spanish L1 Spanish adults. Doctoral dissertation. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.Google Scholar
Au, T. K.-f., Knightly, L. M., Jun, S.-A., & Oh, J. S. (2002). Overhearing a language during childhood. Psychological science, 13(3), 238243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayram, F., Kupisch, T., y Cabo, D. P., & Rothman, J. (2019). Terminology matters on theoretical grounds too!: Coherent grammars cannot be incomplete. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41(2), 257264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentz, C., & Winter, B. (2014). Languages with more second language learners tend to lose nominal case. In Wichmann, S. & Good, J. (Eds.), Quantifying language dynamics (pp. 96124). Leiden: Brill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A. (2004). Between emergence and mastery. The long developmental route. Language development across childhood and adolescence (pp. 934). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bice, K., & Kroll, J. F. (2015). Native language change during early stages of second language learning. NeuroReport, 26(16), 966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bickerton, D. (1984). The language bioprogram hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7(2), 173188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birdsong, D. (2018). Plasticity, variability and age in second language acquisition and bilingualism. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00081CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bylund, E. (2009). Maturational constraints and first language attrition. Language Learning, 59(3), 687715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carreira, M., & Kagan, O. (2011). The results of the national heritage language survey: Implications for teaching, curriculum design, and professional development. Foreign Language Annals, 44(1), 4064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chamorro, G., Sturt, P., & Sorace, A. (2015). Selectivity in L1 attrition: Differential object marking in Spanish near-native speakers of English. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 45, 697715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chamorro, G., Sorace, A., & Sturt, P. (2016). What is the source of L1 attrition? The effect of recent L1 re-exposure on Spanish speakers under L1 attrition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(3), 520532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chondrogianni, V., & Schwartz, R. G. (2020). Case marking and word order in Greek heritage children. Journal of Child Language, 47(4), 766795.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chung, E. S. (2020). Acquisition of differential object marking in Korean. The Acquisition of Differential Object Marking, 26, 343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coşkun Kunduz, A., & Montrul, S. (2022). Sources of variability in the acquisition of differential object marking by Turkish heritage language children in the United States. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25(4), 603616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coşkun Kunduz, A., & Montrul, S. (2023). The role of input in the acquisition of evidentiality by Turkish heritage language children in the United States. Language Acquisition, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crain, S. (1991). Language acquisition in the absence of experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14(4), 597612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A., & Pérez-Tattam, R. (2016). Grammatical gender selection and phrasal word order in child heritage Spanish: A feature re-assembly approach. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 5068.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A., Miller, L., Pérez Tattam, R., & Ortiz Vergara, M. (2019). Structure complexity effects in child heritage Spanish: The case of the Spanish personal a. International Journal of Bilingualism, 23(6), 13331357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deng, X., Mai, Z., & Yip, V. (2018). An aspectual account of ba and bei constructions in child Mandarin. First Language, 38(3), 243262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daskalaki, E., Blom, E., Chondrogianni, V., Paradis, J., & Daskalaki, E. (2020). Effects of parental input quality in child heritage language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 47(4), 709736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Domínguez, L., & Hicks, G. (2016). Synchronic change in a multidialectal Spanish community. In Cuza, A., Czerwionka, L., & Olson, D. (Eds.), Inquiries in hispanic linguistics: From theory to empirical evidence, 12, 53. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fagyal, Z., Swarup, S., Escobar, A. M., Gasser, L., & Lakkaraju, K. (2010). Centers and peripheries: Network roles in language change. Lingua, 120(8), 20612079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fhlannchadha, S. N., & Hickey, T. M. (2021). Where are the goalposts? Generational change in the use of grammatical gender in Irish. Languages, 6(1), 33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flores, C., & Rinke, E. (2020). The relevance of language-internal variation in predicting heritage language grammars. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(1), 2526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flores, C., Santos, A. L., Jesus, A., & Marques, R. (2017). Age and input effects in the acquisition of mood in Heritage Portuguese. Journal of Child Language, 44(4), 795828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, V. C. M., & Thomas, E. M. (2009). Bilingual first-language development: Dominant language takeover, threatened minority language take-up. Bilingualism, 12(2), 213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geeslin, K. L., & Guijarro-Fuentes, P. (2008). Variation in contemporary Spanish: Linguistic predictors of estar in four cases of language contact. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(3), 365380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giancaspro, D., Perez-Cortes, S., & Higdon, J. (2022). (Ir) regular mood swings: Lexical variability in heritage speakers’ oral production of subjunctive mood. Language Learning, 72(2), 456496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F. & Py, B. (1991). La restructuration d’une première langue: l’intégration de variantes de contact dans la compétence de migrants bilingues. La Linguistique, 27, 3560.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T. (2002). Language acquisition: A linguistic perspective. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Guijarro-Fuentes, P. (2012). The acquisition of interpretable features in L2 Spanish: Personal a. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(4), 701720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guijarro-Fuentes, P., & Marinis, T. (2007). Acquiring phenomena at the syntax/semantics interface in L2 Spanish: The personal preposition a. Eurosla Yearbook, 7(1), 6788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gürel, A. (2004). Selectivity in L2-induced L1 attrition: A psycholinguistic account. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17(1), 5378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartshorne, J. K., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Pinker, S. (2018). A critical period for second language acquisition: Evidence from 2/3 million English speakers. Cognition, 177, 263277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hicks, G., & Domínguez, L. (2020). A model for L1 grammatical attrition. Second Language Research, 36(2), 143165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, B., & Putnam, M. T. (2020). Mental representations in receptive multilingual grammars. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 10(3), 309314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson Kam, C. L., & Newport, E. L. (2005). Regularizing unpredictable variation: The roles of adult and child learners in language formation and change. Language Learning and Development, 1(2), 151195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulsen, M. E. H. (2000). Language loss and language processing: Three generations of Dutch migrants in New Zealand. Doctoral dissertation Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen.Google Scholar
Hurtado, A., & Vega, L. A. (2004). Shift happens: Spanish and English transmission between parents and their children. Journal of Social Issues, 60(1), 137155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irizarri van Suchtelen, P. (2016). Spanish as a heritage language in the Netherlands. A cognitive linguistic exploration. Doctoral Dissertation, Universiteit Utrecht: LOT.Google Scholar
Ivanova-Sullivan, T., Sekerina, I. A., Tofighi, D., & Polinsky, M. (2022). Language-Internal Reanalysis of Clitic Placement in Heritage Grammars Reduces the Cost of Computation: Evidence from Bulgarian. Languages, 7(1), 24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jia, R., & Paradis, J. (2015). The use of referring expressions in narratives by Mandarin heritage language children and the role of language environment factors in predicting individual differences. Bilingualism, 18(4), 737.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21 ( 1), 6099.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, M. C. (1998). Language obsolescence and revitalization: Linguistic change in two sociolinguistically contrasting Welsh communities. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karayayla, T. (2020). Effects of first language attrition on heritage language input and ultimate attainment. In Lost in transmission: The role of attrition and input in heritage language development (pp. 3369).Google Scholar
Kasparian, K., & Steinhauer, K. (2017). When the second language takes the lead: Neurocognitive processing changes in the first language of adult attriters. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 236344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasparian, K., Vespignani, F., & Steinhauer, K. (2017). First language attrition induces changes in online morphosyntactic processing and re-analysis: An ERP study of number agreement in complex Italian sentences. Cognitive Science, 41(7), 17601803.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerswill, P. (1996). Children, adolescents, and language change. Language Variation and Change, 8(2), 177202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerswill, P., & Williams, A. (2000a). Creating a new town koine: Children and language change in Milton Keynes. Language in Society, 29(1), 65115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerswill, P., & Williams, A. (2000b). “Salience” as an explanatory factor in language change: Evidence from dialect levelling in urban England. Reading Working Papers in Linguistics 4, 6394. Department of Linguistic Science. The University of Reading.Google Scholar
Kupisch, T., Barton, D., Hailer, K., Klaschik, E., Stangen, I., Lein, T., & van de Weijer, J. (2014). Foreign accent in adult simultaneous bilinguals. Heritage Language Journal, 11(2), 123150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W. (1989). The child as linguistic historian. Language Variation and Change, 1(1), 8597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W. (2001). Principles of linguistic change. 2: Social factors. In Language in society. Oxford.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (2007). Transmission and diffusion. Language, 83(2), 344387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lidz, J., & Gagliardi, A. (2015). How nature meets nurture: Universal grammar and statistical learning. Annual Review of Linguistics, 1(1), 333353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, D. (1991). How to set parameters: Arguments from language change. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lightfoot, D. (2020). Born to parse: How children select their languages. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, D., & Westergaard, M. (2007). Language acquisition and language change: Inter-relationships. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1(5), 396415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luo, R., Escobar, K., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2020). Heterogeneity in the trajectories of US Latine mothers’ dual-language input from infancy to preschool. First Language, 40(3), 275299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Łyskawa, P., & Nagy, N. (2020). Case marking variation in heritage Slavic languages in Toronto: Not so different. Language Learning, 70, 122156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mai, Z., Zhao, L., & Yip, V. (2022). The Mandarin ba-construction in school-age heritage speakers and their parental input. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 12(3), 377405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayberry, R. I., & Kluender, R. (2018). Rethinking the critical period for language: New insights into an old question from American Sign Language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(5), 886905.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McWhorter, J. (2007). Language interrupted: Signs of non-native acquisition in standard language grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (2011). First and second language acquisition: Parallels and differences. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J., Martin, E., & Rinke, E.. (2013). Language acquisition and change: A morphosyntactic perspective. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2004). Subject and object expression in Spanish heritage speakers: A case of morphosyntactic convergence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7(2), 125142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2006). On the bilingual competence of Spanish heritage speakers: Syntax, lexical-semantics and processing. International Journal of Bilingualism, 10(1), 3769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2008). Incomplete Acquisition in Bilingualism: Reexamining the Age Factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2013). How native are heritage speakers? A look at gender agreement in Spanish. The Heritage Language Journal, 10(2), 1539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2014). Structural changes in Spanish in the United States: Differential object marking in Spanish heritage speakers across generations. Lingua, 151, 177196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2016a). The acquisition of heritage languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2016b). Losing your case? Dative experiencers in Mexican Spanish and heritage speakers in the United States. In Pascual y Cabo, D. (Ed.), Advances in Spanish as a heritage language(pp. 99124). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2018a). The Bottleneck Hypothesis extends to heritage language acquisition. In Cho, J., Judy, T., & Mendez, T. L. (Eds.), Meaning and structure in second language acquisition, 149177. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2018b). Heritage language development. Connecting the dots. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22(5), 530546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2019). Attrition, addition and age. Critical commentary. Second Language Research, 36(2), 213218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2021). Representational and computational changes in heritage language grammars. Heritage Language Journal, 18(2), 130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2022). Native speakers interrupted: Differential object marking and language change in heritage grammars. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. & Armstrong, A. (2024). The literacy enhancement hypothesis in bilingual language development. In Batbasouli, E. (Ed.), Multilingual acquisition and learning: An ecosystemic view to diversity (pp. 6089). Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Ionin, T. (2012). Dominant language transfer in Spanish heritage speakers and L2 learners in the interpretation of definite articles Modern Language Journal, 96 ( 1), 7094.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., & Polinsky, M. (2021). The Cambridge handbook of heritage languages and linguistics. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Bowles, M. (2009). Back to basics: Differential object marking under incomplete acquisition in Spanish heritage speakers. Bilingualism, 12(3), 363383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Sánchez-Walker, N. (2013). Differential object marking in child and adult Spanish heritage speakers. Language Acquisition, 20(2), 109132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Sánchez-Walker, N. (2015). Subject expression in bilingual school-age children in the United States. In Carvalho, A., Orozco, R., & Shin, N. (Eds.), Subject pronoun expression in Spanish: A cross-dialectal perspective (pp. 231248). Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Bhatt, R., & Girju, R. (2015). Differential object marking in Spanish, Hindi, and Romanian as heritage languages. Language, 91(3), 564610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muāgututi’a, G. T. (2018). Recovering Ergativity in Heritage Samoan (Doctoral dissertation,University of Hawaiʻi at Mānoa).Google Scholar
Mueller Gathercole, V. (2007). Miami and North Wales, so far and yet so near: A constructivist account of morphosyntactic development in bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10(3), 224247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagy, N., & Kochetov, A. (2013). Voice onset time across the generations: A cross-linguistic study of contact-induced change. In Siemund, P., Gogolin, I., Schulz, M., & Davydova, J. (Eds.), Multilingualism and language contact in urban areas: Acquisition—development—teaching—communication (pp. 1938). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Grady, W., Kwak, H.-Y., Lee, O.-S., & Lee, M. (2011). An emergentist perspective on heritage language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33(2), 223245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otheguy, R., & Zentella, A. C. (2012). Spanish in New York: Language contact, dialectal leveling, and structural continuity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascual y Cabo, D. (2013). Knowledge of gustar-like verbs in Spanish heritage speakers. In 12th Generative approaches to second language acquisition (pp. 162169). Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Pascual y Cabo, D. (2020). Examining the role of cross-generational attrition in the development of Spanish as a heritage language: Evidence from gustar-like verbs. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 10(1), 86108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PérezCortés, S. (2022). On complexity and divergence in heritage language grammars: The case of double mood selection in reported speech contexts. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 44(3), 818842.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E. (2011). Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82(6), 18341849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pires, A., & Rothman, J. (2009). Disentangling sources of incomplete acquisition: An explanation for competence divergence across heritage grammars. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(2), 211238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pires, A., & Thomason, S. G. (2008). How much syntactic reconstruction is possible. In Ferraresi, G., & Goldbach, M. (Eds.), Principles of syntactic reconstruction (Vol.27, p. 72). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
de Prada Pérez, A., & y Cabo, D. P. (2011, March). Invariable gusta in the Spanish of Heritage Speakers in the US. In Proceedings of the 11th generative approaches to second language acquisition conference (Vol. 1991, pp. 110120). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2006). Incomplete acquisition: American Russian. Journal of Slavic Linguistics, 14(2),191262.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2011). Reanalysis in adult heritage language: A case for attrition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33(2), 305328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2018). Heritage languages and their speakers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M., & Scontras, G. (2020). Understanding heritage languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(1), 420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, M. T., & Sánchez, L. (2013). What’s so incomplete about incomplete acquisition?: A prolegomenon to modeling heritage language grammars. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3(4), 478508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, M. T., Kupisch, T., & Pascual y Cabo, D. (2018). Different situations, similar outcomes: Heritage grammars across the lifespan. In Miller, D., Bayram, F., Rothman, J., & Serratrice, L., Bilingual cognition and language: The state of the science across its subfields (pp. 251281). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rao, R. (2015). Manifestations of/bdg/in heritage speakers of Spanish. Heritage Language Journal, 12(1), 4874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Requena, P. (2021). Spanish copula selection with adjectives at age three. Journal of Child Language, 48(3), 621633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Requena, P. (2023). Variation versus deviation: Early bilingual acquisition of Spanish differential object marking. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 13(6), 801829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Requena, P., & Dracos, M. (2021). Spanish copula selection with adjectives in school-aged bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 25(3), 548567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rinke, E., & Flores, C. (2014). Morphosyntactic knowledge of clitics by Portuguese heritage bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(4), 681699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J. (2007). Heritage speaker competence differences, language change, and input type: Inflected infinitives in Heritage Brazilian Portuguese. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11(4), 359389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J. (2009). Understanding the nature and outcomes of early bilingualism: Romance languages as heritage languages. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(2), 155163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J., & Treffers-Daller, J. (2014). A prolegomenon to the construct of the native speaker: Heritage speaker bilinguals are natives too!. Applied Linguistics, 35(1), 9398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sankoff, G. (2019). Language change across the lifespan: Three trajectory types. Language, 95(2), 197229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, M. (2011). Language attrition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, M. S. (2013). First language attrition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3(1), 94115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, M. S. (2019). The impact of frequency of use and length of residence on L1 attrition. In Schmid, M. S., & Köpke, B., (Eds), The Oxford handbook on language attrition (pp. 288303). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, M., & Köpke, B. (2017). The relevance of first language attrition to theories of bilingual development. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7(6), 637667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt, C., & Miller, K. (2007). Making discourse-dependent decisions: The case of the copulas ser and estar in Spanish. Lingua, 117(11), 19071929.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Senghas, A. (1995). The development of Nicaraguan Sign Language via the language acquisition process. In Proceedings of the 19th annual Boston university conference on language development (pp. 543552). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Sharma, D. (2017). Scalar effects of social networks on language variation. Language Variation and Change, 29(3), 393418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharma, D, & Dodsworth, R. (2019). Language variation and social networks. Annual Review of Linguistics, 6(1) 121.Google Scholar
Sherkina-Lieber, M., Pérez-Leroux, A. T., & Johns, A. (2011). Grammar without speech production: The case of Labrador Inuttitut heritage receptive bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 14(3), 301317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherkina-Lieber, M. (2020). A classification of receptive bilinguals. Why we need to distinguish them, and what they have in common. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 10(3), 412440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shin, N. L. (2013, April). Women as leaders of language change: A qualification from the bilingual perspective. In Selected proceedings of the 6th Workshop on Spanish sociolinguistics (pp. 135147). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Shin, N., & Miller, K. (2022). Children’s acquisition of morphosyntactic variation. Language Learning and Development, 126.Google Scholar
Shin, N. L., & Otheguy, R. (2013). Social class and gender impacting change in bilingual settings: Spanish subject pronoun use in New York. Language in Society, 42(4), 429452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. (1994). Language contact and change: Spanish in Los Angeles. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. (2014). Bilingual language acquisition: Spanish and English in the first six years. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. (2018). Simultaneous bilingualism: Early developments, incomplete later outcomes? International Journal of Bilingualism, 22(5), 497512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singleton, J., & Newport, E. (1994). When learners surpass their models: The acquisition of ASL from impoverished input. Unpublished Manuscript. University of Illinois.Google Scholar
Snyder, W. (2021). A parametric approach to the acquisition of syntax. Journal of Child Language, 48(5), 862887.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A. (2004). Native language attrition and developmental instability at the syntax-discourse interface: Data, interpretations and methods. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7(2), 143145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A. (2005). Syntactic optionality at interfaces. In Syntax and variation: Reconciling the biological and the social (pp. 46111). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Sorace, A. (2012). Pinning down the concept of interface in bilingual development: A reply to peer commentaries. Linguistic approaches to bilingualism, 2(2), 209217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A. (2011). Pinning down the concept of “interface” in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1 ( 1), 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A. (2016). Referring expressions and executive functions in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(5), 669684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A. (2020). L1 attrition in a wider perspective. Second Language Research, 36(2), 203206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyder, W. (2017). Child language: The parametric approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stanford, J. N., & Kenny, L. A. (2013). Revisiting transmission and diffusion: An agent-based model of vowel chain shifts across large communities. Language Variation and Change, 25(2), 119153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tagliamonte, S. A., & D’Arcy, A. (2009). Peaks beyond phonology: Adolescence, incrementation, and language change. Language, 85(1), 58108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thane, P. D. (2024). On the Acquisition of differential object marking in child heritage spanish: Bilingual education, exposure, and age effects (in memory of phoebe search). Languages, 9(1), 26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, R. (1995). Referentiality and wh-movement in child English: Juvenile D-Linkuency. Language Acquisition, 4(1–2), 139175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ticio, E. (2015). Differential object marking in Spanish-English early bilinguals. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 5(1), 6290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ticio, E., & Avram, L. (2015). The acquisition of differential object marking in Spanish and Romanian: Semantic scales or semantic features. Revue Romaine de Linguistique, 4, 383402.Google Scholar
Titone, D. A., & Tiv, M. (2022). Rethinking multilingual experience through a systems framework of bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 26(1) 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiv, M., Gullifer, J. W., Feng, R. Y., & Titone, D. (2020). Using network science to map what Montréal bilinguals talk about across languages and communicative contexts. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56, 100913CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toribio, A. J., & Nye, C. (2006). Restructuring of reverse psychological predicate. Amsterdam Studies in the Theory and History of Linguistic Science Series, 4, 275–263.Google Scholar
Trudgill, P. (2001). Contact and simplification: Historical baggage and directionality in linguistic change. Linguistic Typology, 5(2/3), 371374.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I., Sorace, A., Heycock, C., Filiaci, F., & Bouba, M. (2003, April). Subjects in L1 attrition: Evidence from Greek and Italian near-native speakers of English. In Proceedings of the 27th annual Boston University conference on language development (Vol. 2, pp. 787797). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S., Argyri, F., Cornips, L., Hulk, A., Sorace, A., & Tsimpli, I. (2011). Bilingual acquisition of Greek voice morphology and Dutch gender: What do they have in common?. In BUCLD 35 proceedings of the 35th annual Boston University conference on language development (pp. 590602). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Westergaard, M. R. (2005). Norwegian child language and the history of English: The interaction of syntax and information. In Manaffert, K., Bull, T., & Killie, K. (Eds.), Contexts-Historical, social, linguistic: Studies in celebration of toril swan, 293310. Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Westergaard, M. (2021). Language acquisition, microcues, parameters, and morphosyntactic change. In Janda, R. D., Joseph, B. D., & Vance, B S. (Eds.), Handbook of historical linguistics, (Vol.2, Ch. 17, pp. 357374). Wiley Publishers.Google Scholar
Yang, C. D. (2000). Internal and external forces in language change. Language Variation and Change, 12(3), 231250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaretsky, E., & Bar-Shalom, E. G. (2010). Does reading in shallow L1 orthography slow attrition of language-specific morphological structures?. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 24(4–5), 401415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. The dynamics of language acquisition and change (Yang, 2000, p. 232).

Figure 1

Figure 2. Relationship between length of residence in the U.S. and acceptability of ungrammatical animate specific direct objects without DOM by the adult Mexican immigrants (Montrul, 2022). A linear mixed-effects model revealed that this correlation was significant (SE = .01, β = .03, p < .05).

Figure 2

Figure 3. Relationship between length of residence in the U.S. and acceptability ratings on animate definite objects with clitic doubling by the adult Mexican immigrants (Montrul, 2022). The output of the model showed that there was a significant correlation between LOR and acceptability of the target construction (SE = .01, β = .02, p < .05).

Figure 3

Figure 4. Possible developmental trajectories of L1 acquisition, L1 attrition and acquisition in heritage speakers and their caregivers.

Figure 4

Figure 5. The dynamics of language acquisition and change in Spanish in the United States.